Abstract
Growing evidence suggests that childhood socioeconomic status (SES) influences neural development, which may contribute to the well-documented SES-related disparities in academic achievement. However, the particular aspects of SES that impact neural structure and function are not well understood. Here, we investigate associations of childhood SES and a potential mechanism—degree of cognitive stimulation in the home environment—with cortical structure, white matter microstructure, and neural function during a working memory (WM) task across development. Analyses included 53 youths (age 6–19 years). Higher SES as reflected in the income-to-needs ratio was associated with higher parent-reported achievement, WM performance, and cognitive stimulation in the home environment. Although SES was not significantly associated with cortical thickness, children raised in more cognitively stimulating environments had thicker cortex in the frontoparietal network. Higher family SES was associated with white matter microstructure and neural activation in the frontoparietal network during a WM task, including greater fractional anisotropy (FA) in the right and left superior longitudinal fasciculi (SLF), and greater BOLD activation in multiple regions of the prefrontal cortex during WM encoding and maintenance. Greater FA and activation in these regions was associated higher parent-reported achievement. Together, cognitive stimulation, WM performance, FA in the SLF, and prefrontal activation during WM encoding and maintenance significantly mediated the association between SES and parent-reported achievement. These findings highlight potential neural, cognitive, and environmental mechanisms linking SES with academic achievement.
Keywords: functional MRI, white matter, socioeconomic status, academic achievement, cognitive stimulation, frontoparietal network
1. Introduction
Growing evidence indicates that brain development varies as a function of family socioeconomic status (SES; Brito & Noble, 2014; Noble et al., 2015; Ursache & Noble, 2016). These neural differences may play a role in the well-documented academic achievement gap between children raised in high- compared to low-SES households (Baydar, 1993; Brooks-Gunn & Duncan, 1997). SES-related differences in cognitive and brain outcomes are particularly pronounced in the domains of executive functions (EF) and language (Noble et el., 2005; 2007). EF are a set of cognitive functions including inhibition, cognitive flexibility, and working memory (Miyake & Friedman, 2012). Here, we focus specifically on SES-related differences in working memory (WM), which involves the ability to hold in mind, manipulate, and update information in memory.
Children growing up in low-SES households exhibit worse WM than children raised in higher-SES families; these differences have been observed across the SES gradient and are not limited to children living in poverty (Hackman & Farah, 2009; Noble et al., 2005; 2007). EF abilities broadly, and WM specifically, are strongly associated with academic achievement (Best et al., 2011; Blair & Diamond, 2008; Finn et al., 2016). Understanding how the neural networks that support WM vary as a function of SES may shed light on neural pathways that explain the achievement gap. To that end, the present study uses a multi-modal approach to investigate SES-related differences in brain structure and function in neural systems involved in WM and the links between these neural systems and cognitive and academic performance in children.
Why might childhood SES influence brain development? It is likely that many aspects of SES produce differences in neural structure and function, and ultimately academic achievement, including cognitive and social stimulation, environmental predictability, parenting, exposure to toxins, nutrition, and exposure to violence (Johnson et al., 2016). Here, we focus on whether SES-related differences in neural structure and function are driven by differences in the degree of cognitive stimulation in the home environment.
Children raised in low-SES families experience lower levels of cognitive stimulation, interaction with adults, linguistic complexity, and access to enriching experiences at home and school than children raised in high-SES families (Hart & Risley, 1995; Bradley et al., 2001; Bradley & Corwyn, 2002; Hackman et al., 2015). This reduction in cognitive and social stimulation is argued to constrain early forms of learning that rely on rich sensory, linguistic, and social input, resulting in accelerated synaptic pruning throughout the cortex (McLaughlin, et al., 2017; McLaughlin & Sheridan, 2016; Sheridan & McLaughlin, 2016). These patterns could produce age-specific reductions in cortical thickness and surface area among children from low-SES backgrounds.
Existing research on SES and neural structure is consistent with these predictions (Jednoróg et al., 2012; Lawson et al., 2013; Mackey et al., 2015; Noble et al., 2015; 2012; Piccolo et al., 2016). Lower parental education and family income are associated with reduced cortical thickness and surface area throughout the cortex (Mackey et al., 2015; Noble et al., 2015), with the strongest association between SES and cortical surface area at the lowest levels of income (Noble et al., 2015). SES-related differences are particularly pronounced in regions underlying WM and language, including dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC), superior temporal cortex, and superior parietal cortex (Noble et al., 2012; 2015). Cortical thickness in dlPFC and superior temporal cortex decreases more rapidly in children from low-SES backgrounds followed by an attenuation of this thinning in adolescence (Piccolo et al., 2016), consistent with the idea that children in low-SES environments exhibit accelerated pruning of synaptic connections early in life that produces more rapid declines in cortical thickness and surface area across childhood (McLaughlin et al., 2017; Sheridan & McLaughlin; 2014; 2016; McLaughlin et al., 2014). Although accumulating evidence demonstrates reductions in cortical thickness and surface area among children raised in low-SES environments, we are unaware of prior research directly examining the hypothesis that these differences are driven by the reductions in cognitive stimulation experienced by low-SES children. We provide the first empirical test of this hypothesis in the current paper.
Moreover, differences in cortical structure may have implications for cognitive and academic outcomes. Longitudinal data indicate that accelerated cortical thinning in childhood followed by attenuated thinning in adolescence—a pattern observed among low-SES children (Piccolo et al., 2016) is associated with lower cognitive ability (Shaw et al., 2006). Similarly, greater thickness in temporal, parietal, and occipital cortex is associated with better standardized test scores in adolescents (Mackey et al., 2015). Thus, reductions in cortical thickness may be a mechanism linking low-SES with poor academic outcomes.
Although SES-related differences in cortical structure are well documented, few studies have examined the associations of SES with white matter microstructure, particularly in children. White matter microstructure in fronto-striatal and fronto-temporal tracts is reduced in children who have experienced adverse environments characterized by deprivation in cognitive and social stimulation, such as institutional rearing and neglect (Eluvathingal et al., 2006; Kumar et al., 2014; Hanson et al., 2013; Bick et al., 2015). Because children raised in low-SES families also experience lower levels of cognitive stimulation than children raised in high-SES families (Bradley & Corwyn, 2002), it is possible that SES may have similar influences on white matter microstructure, although this is largely untested. Evidence from adults is somewhat consistent with this hypothesis: global white matter integrity follows an SES gradient in adults, such that greater education, income, and community-level SES are associated with higher fractional anisotropy (FA) across the entire brain (Gianaros et al., 2013). Variability in white matter structure, in turn, influences cognitive outcomes. In particular, greater integrity of the superior longitudinal fasiculus (SLF), a tract that connects the lateral PFC to the parietal cortex, has been linked to better WM (Mabbott et al., 2006; Vestergaard et al., 2011) and higher educational attainment in adolescents (Noble et al., 2013), suggesting that this tract may play an important role in SES-related variation in WM and academic achievement.
Few studies have examined SES-related differences in neural function during WM tasks in children, although such differences in neural function have been observed in relation to language processing (e.g. Raizada et al., 2008), emotion regulation (e.g. Kim et al., 2013), and academic subjects including mathematics and reading (e.g. Demir-Lira et al., 2016; and Noble et al., 2006). Existing evidence suggests that SES is associated with PFC function during multiple forms of EF. During novel rule-learning, low-SES children perform more poorly and exhibit a more diffuse pattern of PFC activation than higher-SES children (Sheridan et al., 2012). Additionally, SES is related to reduced inhibitory control in adolescents and greater recruitment of the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex, coupled with reduced connectivity between the dorsal anterior cingulate and dorsolateral PFC specifically among low SES girls (Spielberg et al., 2015). During a WM task, children from lower-income backgrounds exhibit reduced PFC and superior parietal recruitment than children from higher-SES families (Finn et al., 2016). In that study, WM capacity and fronto-parietal recruitment mediated the association between SES and performance on a statewide mathematics exam. These findings suggest that differences in neural recruitment in the frontoparietal network observed among low-SES children may contribute directly to academic performance.
In the present study we used a multi-modal neuroimaging approach to investigate the associations of parental SES with brain structure and function, including cortical structure, white matter microstructure, and neural function during a WM task. We focused on SES-related differences in the frontoparietal network because of its known role in WM (Corbetta & Shulman, 2002; Curtis & D’Esposito, 2003), which was the focus of our behavioral and fMRI task. Moreover, previous studies have found SES-related differences in neural structure and function in the frontoparietal network (Sheridan et al., 2012; Noble et al., 2013; Finn et al., 2016). For structural region of interest (ROI) analyses, we focused on the middle frontal gyrus (MFG) and superior parietal lobule / intraparietal sulcus (SPL/IPS), key frontoparietal regions that are recruited during WM tasks (Corbetta & Shulman, 2002). For white matter analyses, we examined the superior longitudinal fasciculus (SLF) because it connects the prefrontal and parietal cortices and is associated with WM performance (Mabbott et al., 2006; Vestergaard et al., 2011). In our fMRI approach, we used a conservative whole brain approach to examine SES-related differences in neural recruitment, which demonstrated clear differences in our frontoparietal regions. We then extracted ROIs from frontoparietal regions to examine associations with task performance and academic achievement.
We hypothesized that SES would be associated with frontoparietal structure and function, including positive associations with cortical thickness in the MFG and SPL/IPS; positive associations with white matter integrity in the SLF; and positive associations with BOLD signal in the prefrontal and parietal cortex during a WM task. We expected that cortical thickness, white matter microstructure, and BOLD signal in these frontparietal regions would be associated not only with SES, but also with parent-reported academic achievement. Importantly, we also hypothesized that SES-related differences in neural structure and function would be driven by differences in the degree of cognitive stimulation in the home environment. Together, we expected that WM performance, cognitive stimulation, and neural structure and function would be mechanisms explaining the income-achievement gap. Critically, low-SES is associated not only with lower levels of cognitive stimulation but also higher levels of exposure to violence, which has distinct influences on neural development (McLaughlin et al., 2014). To date, only one study examining SES-related differences in neural measures has measured and controlled for violence exposure (Sheridan et al., 2017). We did so in the present study to isolate the links between SES and neural structure and function that are not explained by exposure to violence.
2. Materials and Methods
2.1 Participants
A sample of 66 participants aged 6 to 19 years (M=13.68 years, SD=3.23 years; 35 male) participated. The sample was recruited in Seattle, WA between February 2014 and February 2015. Youths were recruited at schools, after-school and prevention programs, medical clinics, and in the general community. Half of the sample was recruited based on exposure to violence in order to test additional questions about how environmental experience is associated with neural processes involved in memory for emotional information (Lambert et al., 2017). We controlled for violence exposure by including it as a covariate of non-interest in all analyses that included SES or cognitive enrichment as the predictor variable. The study sample was racially and ethnically diverse (53.5% White, 6.25% Black, 14.55% Hispanic, 2.1% Asian, 23.6% Multiracial or Other). The Institutional Review Board at the University of Washington approved all procedures. Participants were compensated and written informed consent was obtained from legal guardians; youths provided written assent.
Five participants were excluded from all analyses. One subject (female, 15 years) had an incidental neurological finding, one subject got out of the scanner (female 8 years), and three subjects had excessive motion for all three scan types (female, 6 years, male 6 years, male, 9 years). For functional MRI analyses, seven additional participants (5 female, mean age: 11.43 ±3.11 years) were excluded from analyses due to below-chance performance on the task and two participants (females, 9 and 12 years) were excluded due to excessive motion (>20% TRs with framewise displacement outliers). For DTI analyses, nine additional subjects (6 female, mean age 10.92±2.76 years) were excluded due to poor quality assurance on DTI images. The final analytic sample after the above exclusions and missing data was 49 for structural MRI analyses, 43 for DTI, and 47 for fMRI. Importantly, exclusion from analyses did not vary as a function of income-to-needs ratio (ps >.250).
2.2 Measures
2.2.1 Socioeconomic Status
We used the income-to-needs ratio as a measure of SES. The income-to-needs ratio captures the amount of annual income that a family earns relative to the federally-defined poverty threshold for a family of that size. Income-to-needs is a widely used measure of SES that allows associations to be examined across the entire SES gradient. Parents reported annual income in 10 bins, and the median of the income bins was used except for the lowest and highest bins, which were assigned $5,000 and $200,000 respectively. The median income was $42,500. Income-to-needs ratio was calculated by dividing the total household income by the 2014 U.S. census-defined poverty line for a family of that size, with a value of 1 or less indicating income below the poverty line. The range of income-to-needs was 0.13 to 8.33. Median income-to-needs was 1.77, and 21 participants (38.18%) were living in poverty. Six participants (mean age 16.85 ±2.32 years, 4 female) had missing income data. These participants were included for analyses where they had available data (e.g., those examining cognitive stimulation and neural structure and function). The income-to-needs ratio was log-transformed for all analyses, following prior work documenting that associations between income and neural measures exist across the SES distribution but are stronger at lower levels of income (Noble et al., 2015). Additionally, we examined whether the best fitting model included income-to-needs as a linear or log-transformed variable, and the logarithmic model was a better fit to the data for all analyses.
2.2.2 Parent-reported academic achievement
To assess academic achievement, parents completed the Child Behavior Check List (CBCL; Achenbach, Howell, Quay, & Conners, 1991). Parents reported their child’s performance in four academic subjects (Reading, English, or Language Arts, History or Social Studies, Arithmetic or Math, and Science) choosing from four options. Each of these options was assigned a numerical value (1= Failing, 2=Below Average, 3= Average, 4 = Above Average) and we used this value to compute a composite score for each participant by taking the mean performance level for all four academic subjects. Three subjects were missing parent-reported achievement data (2 males 18 years, 1 female 18 years).
2.2.3 Cognitive Stimulation
To assess the degree of cognitive stimulation in the home environment, parents completed the Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment-Short Form (HOME-SF; Mott, 2004). The HOME-SF has slightly different versions for children aged 6–9 and 10–15 years, with 16 items that are identical across these age ranges. We used only the 16 questions that are present in the HOME-SF for both younger and older children. This assessment included items that assess cognitive stimulation and exposure to varied learning experiences. Example items included: “About how many books does your child have?”; “‘How many times does your child get out of the house per week for activities (e.g., sports, extracurricular activities, activities with the family)?”; and “Did you and/or your partner teach your child numbers at home?”. The measure was scored using the cut-offs used in the original HOME assessment, where one point is assigned for each item where age-appropriate experiences are met (e.g., 1 point is assigned if a child leaves the house at least once a week for an activity; 0 is assigned if the child leaves the house less than once a week for an activity); for a total possible score of 16. While this assessment has not been validated in youths over 15, it has been used in older adolescents in prior studies (Cleveland et al., 2000). In the present study, the HOME-SF had good internal consistency (α = .75) and the subscale assessing cognitive stimulation used in the present study has adequate internal consistency (α = .62). The mean score on the cognitive stimulation sub-scale was 13.05 ±2.15, and scores were normally distributed. Seven subjects were missing cognitive stimulation data (4 male, mean age = 14.5 years).
2.2.4 Violence Exposure
Violence exposure was assessed with the Childhood Experiences of Care and Abuse (CECA) interview (Bifulco et al., 1997), the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire (CTQ) (Bernstein et al., 1997) and the UCLA PTSD Reaction Index (PTSD-RI) trauma screen (Steinberg, Brymer, Decker, & Pynoos, 2004). Each of these measures assesses exposure to violence, including child abuse and domestic violence. Participants who reported physical abuse, sexual abuse, witnessing domestic violence, or directly experiencing other violence, or who had a score on the CTQ physical or sexual abuse sub-scales above a validated threshold (Walker et al., 1999) were classified as exposed to violence and this dichotomous variable was included as a covariate in all analyses investigating SES-related differences including behavioral, parent-reported achievement, cognitive stimulation, cortical thickness, white matter integrity, and functional MRI analyses. Because this study included youths exposed to different types of violence (e.g., abuse, domestic violence, community violence) and no single scale captures all of these exposures, a dichotomous variable for violence exposure was used. As a sensitivity analysis, we constructed a continuous variable reflecting a sum of the standardized scores of multiple violence measures as a covariate rather than a dichotomous variable. Using this continuous measure of violence exposure as a covariate in our analyses did not alter any of our results.
2.2.5 Working Memory Task
Participants completed a delayed-match to sample WM task (Supplemental Figure 1) using emotional faces as stimuli. Faces were drawn from a standardized stimulus set (Tottenham et al., 2009). Stimuli were neutral, happy, and angry faces, distributed evenly across trials and presented in a counter-balanced order across participants. Participants were instructed to attend and respond to the faces and their emotional expressions. The delayed-match-to-sample (DMS) task consisted of two runs of 50 trials. Each trial involved Encoding (2000 ms), Delay (1000 or 5000 ms), and Probe (2000 ms) and an inter-trial interval (ITI) of 500 ms (67% of trials) or 2000 ms (33% of trials). Each actor was presented 6–7 times for each facial expression. During the Encoding, facial stimuli were embedded in realistic background scenes to investigate implicit context encoding for another study that involved a memory test outside the scanner for contextual information (Lambert et al., 2017). This made encoding more similar to real-world facial encoding. During the Probe, an image of a face without a background scene was presented, and participants were asked to indicate whether the Probe face matched the Encoding face. On 1/3 of trials, the Probe face presented matched the Encoding face (i.e. was the same person showing the same emotion) and on the other 2/3 of trials, the Probe did not match. There were two types of mismatches: Emotion Mismatch (same person, different emotion), Identity Mismatch (different person). Each subject had 30 trials of each mismatch type (16–17 per run, for a total of 100 trials). All trial types were interspersed throughout the 2 task runs. Subjects completed two runs of the WM task, with the exception of one subject that completed only one run.
Behavioral performance was assessed using d′, which was calculated using the following formula:
where z is the standardized score, as a measure of the sensitivity to detect matches.
2.3 Image Acquisition and Processing
2.3.1 Image Acquisition
Before undergoing scanning, children 12 years and younger and older children exhibiting anxiety about the scan were trained to minimize head movements in a mock scanner with a motion tracker that stopped playing a movie if a movement of >2 mm occurred. This method has been shown to significantly reduce head motion once children are in the scanner (Raschle et al., 2012). In the scanner, we used a head-stabilizing pillow to further restrict movement.
Scanning was performed on a 3T Phillips Achieva scanner at the University of Washington Integrated Brain Imaging Center using a 32-channel sensitivity-encoding (SENSE) head coil. T1-weighted multi-echo MPRAGE volumes were acquired (TR=2530ms, TE=1.64–7.04µs, flip angle=7°, FOV=256mm2, 176 slices, in-plane voxel size=1mm3). Blood oxygenation level dependent (BOLD) signal during functional runs was acquired using a gradient-echo T2*-weighted EPI sequence. Thirty-two 3mm thick slices were acquired parallel to the AC-PC line (TR=2000ms, TE=30ms, flip angle=90degrees, bandwidth=2300, echo spacing=0.5, FOV=256×256, matrix size=64×64). Prior to each scan, four images were acquired and discarded to allow longitudinal magnetization to reach equilibrium. DTI was acquired using a single-shot echoplanar imaging sequence (TR=8165ms, TE=75ms, flip angle=90°, FOV=256×256mm, 72 slices, in-plane voxel size=2mm3). Diffusion-weighted images were acquired along 64 non-collinear and non-coplanar directions with a b value of 1000 s/mm2 and 1 image with a b value of 0 s/mm2.
2.3.2 Structural Image Processing
Cortical surface of each hemisphere was computationally reconstructed using FreeSurfer software (Dale, Fischl, & Sereno, 1999; Fischl, Sereno, & Dale, 1999). ROI analyses of the prefrontal and parietal cortex were conducted by extracting the mean cortical thickness value for each subject for predetermined regions in the frontoparietal network defined by the FreeSurfer 2005 parcellation (left and right MFG and SPL/IPS). We focus here on hypothesized differences in the frontoparietal network, but present a whole-cortex exploratory analysis in the Supplemental Materials.
2.3.3 DTI Processing
DTI pre-processing including skull-stripping and correction for distortion due to eddy currents in FSL and registration using non-linear symmetric diffeomorphic transformation in Advanced Normalization Tools (ANTS) (Avants et al., 2011). Head motion and eddy current correction were conducted with the ‘eddy’ tool in FSL (Andersson and Sotiropoulos, 2016). The diffusion tensor was calculated per voxel using conventional reconstruction methods in FSL’s dtifit. From these maps, FA was calculated. The temporal signal-to-noise ratio and estimates of subject movement (from the eddy tool) were calculated for quality assurance purposes (Roalf et al., 2016). Subjects with values greater than 2.5 standard deviations from the mean of these metrics were excluded from the analyses.
We then examined FA in the SLF. To extract FA values, we used a standardized protocol developed by the ENIGMA consortium; this protocol is described in detailed elsewhere (Jahanshad et al., 2013). Briefly, FA images were nonlinearly registered to the ENIGMA-DTI target brain using FNIRT. The data were then processed using a modified version of FSL’s tract-based spatial statistics (TBSS) (Smith et al., 2006) to project individual FA values on the hand-segmented ENIGMA-DTI skeleton mask rather than the TBSS skeleton. After extracting the skeletonized white matter and the projection of individual FA values, ENIGMA tract-wise regions of interest, derived from the Johns Hopkins University white matter parcellation atlas (Mori et al., 2008), were transferred to extract the average FA values for the SLF tract for each hemisphere. The protocol, target brain, ENIGMA-DTI skeleton mask, source code and executables are all publicly available (http://enigma.ini.usc.edu/ongoing/dti-working-group/). We focus here on hypothesized differences in the frontoparietal network, but present a whole-brain exploratory analysis in the Supplemental Materials.
2.3.4 fMRI Processing
Preprocessing and statistical analysis of fMRI data was performed in a pipeline using Make, a software development tool that can be used to create neuroimaging workflows that rely on multiple software packages (Askren et al., 2016). Simultaneous motion and slice-time correction was performed in NiPy (Roche, 2011). Spatial smoothing with a Gaussian kernel (6mm full width at half maximum [FWHM]) was performed in FSL (Jenkinson et al., 2012). Data were inspected for artifacts, and volumes with motion >2-mm or >3-SD change in signal intensity were regressed out using volume-specific covariates of non-interest. For subjects who were not specifically excluded for motion, motion was extremely low. Those with the highest motion had fewer than 10% of volumes with framewise displacement outliers across both runs, with the next highest being 3.6% of volumes with outliers. Six rigid-body motion regressors were included in person-level models. A component-based anatomical noise correction method (Behzadi, Restom, Liau, & Liu, 2007) was used to reduce noise associated with physiological fluctuations. Person- and group-level models were estimated in FSL. Following estimation of person-level models, the resulting contrast images were normalized into standard space, and anatomical co-registration of the functional data with each participant’s T1-weighted image was performed using surface-based registration in FreeSurfer version 5.3 (Dale et al., 1999), which provides better alignment than other methods in children (Ghosh et al., 2010). Normalization was implemented in Advanced Normalization Tools (ANTs) software, version 2.1.0 (Avants et al., 2011). Each participant’s MRI data were first warped to a pediatric template (NIH Pediatric MRI Data Repository: https://pediatricmri.nih.gov/nihpd/info/index.html), then from the pediatric template to MNI space. The pediatric template was originally developed from a sample of 500 youths aged 6–18 years with a mean age of 14 years, similar to the present study.
fMRI analysis was performed using FEAT (FMRI Expert Analysis Tool) Version 6.00, part of FSL (FMRIB’s Software Library, www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl). Regressors were created by convolving a boxcar function of phase duration with the standard double-gamma hemodynamic response function for each phase of the task (Encoding, Delay, and Probe). A general linear model (GLM) was constructed for each participant. Higher-level analysis was carried out using FLAME (FMRIB’s Local Analysis of Mixed Effects) stage 1, (Woolrich et al., 2004). Individual-level estimates of BOLD activity were submitted to group-level random effects models of Encoding, Delay, and Probe periods, each compared to Baseline (ITI). Whole-brain analyses were conducted using only correct trials.
We performed cluster-level correction (z > 2.3, p < .01) to our models run in FSL FLAME. This combination of correction and model is associated with relatively low risk of both false positive and false negative findings in recent simulations (see Eklund et al., 2016, Figure 1). Results were then projected onto the cortical surface for visualization purposes using Connectome Workbench (Washington University, St. Louis; Marcus et al., 2013).
Figure 1.
Associations of SES with parent-reported achievement (A), and working memory performance (B).
Results of whole-brain group average activation are displayed in Supplemental Figure 2. ROI analyses were conducted to examine associations of neural activation with performance on the task and parent-reported achievement. ROIs were created by masking functional activation for each period of the task (i.e. Encoding, Delay, and Probe) in the group average for correct trials only and intersecting this mask with an anatomical mask (20% threshold) from the Harvard-Oxford atlas in FSL. This produced an anatomical ROI that included only task-active regions. Importantly, we used a mask based on recruitment across the whole sample to avoid double-dipping when investigating the association of activation with task performance (Vul et al., 2009).
2.4 Data Analysis
Bivariate correlations between all study variables are presented in Table 2.
Table 2.
Bivariate correlations
Sex | Age | Violence | Log Income- to-Need |
Cognitive Enrichmenst |
d′ | Academic Achievement |
|
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Sex | |||||||
Age | .085, | ||||||
Violence | .086 | .101 | |||||
Log Income-to-Needs | −.125 | .061 | −.479** | ||||
Cognitive Enrichment | .062 | −.085 | −.471** | .419** | |||
d′ | −.105 | .322* | −.259 † | .380** | .070 | ||
Academic Achievement | −.070 | −.065 | −.352** | .546** | .423** | .397** | |
LH MFG thick | .137 | −.364** | −.075 | .084 | .343* | −.090 | −.60 |
RH MFG thick | .062 | −.350** | −.142 | .138 | .302* | −.227 | .063 |
LH SPL thick | −.140 | −.555** | −.121 | .033 | .349** | −.262† | −.020 |
RH SPL thick | −.080 | −.637** | −.104 | .098 | .203 | −.250† | −.020 |
L SLF | −.134 | .051 | −.368** | .451** | .320* | .318* | .332* |
R SLF | −.243† | .151 | −.412** | .459** | .143 | .212 | .191 |
LSFG Cue | .148 | .124 | −.165 | .418** | .310* | .394** | .367* |
RSFG Cue | −.019 | .065 | −.160 | .291* | .168 | .317* | .217 |
LMFG Cue | .078 | .224 | −.260† | .438** | .210 | .421** | .312* |
L OccFusCue | −.169 | .344* | −.262† | .314* | −.019 | .500** | .346* |
LOFC Delay | .122 | .182 | −.103 | .434** | .297* | .186 | .340* |
p<.05
p<.01
p<.1
Associations of the income-to-needs ratio with cognitive stimulation, WM, and parent-reported achievement were assessed using linear regression, controlling for age, sex, and violence exposure. As noted earlier, the income-to-needs ratio was log-transformed in all analyses, as this provided a better fit to the data than a linear variable in every model. We use the term SES throughout results to refer to this log-transformed variable. The logarithmic association of income-to-needs with academic achievement and WM and FA in the SLF can be viewed in Supplemental Figure 3A–B and Supplemental Figure 4. We investigated whether the association between SES and these outcomes was moderated by age and found no evidence for moderation.
We applied the same analysis approach to each of our measures of neural structure and function. For neural structure, we investigated the association of SES and cognitive stimulation with cortical thickness and FA in our ROIs of interest. We then determined whether regions that were associated with SES were also associated with WM performance and parent-reported achievement. For neural function, we estimated a model with a mean-centered regressor for log income-to-needs and cognitive stimulation for each contrast of interest (Encoding, Delay, Probe). We then examined whether neural recruitment in regions that were associated with SES were also associated with WM and parent-reported achievement using the ROI approach described above (i.e., ROIs defined by intersecting a structural mask with task-related group average activation for the contrast of interest, to avoid double-dipping). All analysis controlled for age, sex, and violence exposure.
We evaluated whether age moderated the association of SES and cognitive stimulation with neural structure and function and found no evidence for interactions with age for any outcome.
We tested two mechanistic hypotheses using standard tests of statistical mediation. First, we tested the hypothesis that cognitive stimulation in the home environment is a mechanism explaining the association between SES and neural structure and function; we examined this model for neural measures that were associated with cognitive stimulation. Second, we investigated potential mechanisms explaining the link between SES and parent-reported achievement; we included cognitive, neural, and environmental factors that were significantly associated with both SES and parent-reported achievement as mediators. For both models, we used the PROCESS macro to perform a test of statistical mediation that allows multiple mediators to be examined simultaneously and that uses a bootstrapping approach that provides confidence intervals for the indirect effects (Hayes, 2013). Confidence intervals that do not include 0 reflect statistically significant indirect (i.e., mediated) effects.
3. Results
3.1 SES, Academic Achievement, and Working Memory
Consistent with prior work, we found that SES was positively associated with parent-reported achievement (β = .51, p < .0001, Figure 1A) and WM performance (β = .29, p = .042, Figure 1B), such that children with higher SES had better parent-reported achievement and WM. WM performance was also positively associated with achievement (β = .51, p = .001).
3.2 Cognitive Stimulation
Next, we investigated our hypothesis that cognitive stimulation in the home would be associated with SES as well as WM and achievement. As predicted, higher SES was associated with higher levels of cognitive stimulation (β = .33, p = .034), and cognitive stimulation was positively associated with parent-reported achievement (β = .35, p = .022). Cognitive stimulation was not associated with WM performance (p > .8).
3.3 SES, Cognitive Stimulation, and Neural Structure
We next tested the hypothesis that SES and cognitive stimulation would be associated with cortical thickness and FA in the frontoparietal network. With regard to cortical thickness, we observed no association between SES and cortical thickness in the MFG or SPL/IPS. In contrast, cognitive stimulation was significantly associated with cortical thickness in both left MFG (β = .31, p = .011) and SPL/IPS (β = .29, p = .045; Figure 2), but not right MFG or SPL/IPS (ps > .05).
Figure 2.
Associations between cognitive stimulation in the home environment and cortical thickness in the left frontoparietal network.
With regard to white matter microstructure, SES was positively associated with FA in both the left (β = .37, p = .021) and right (β = .32, p = .032) SLF (Figure 3). There were no significant associations between cognitive stimulation and FA (ps > .27).
Figure 3.
A) Definitions of left and right superior longitudinal fasciculus. B) Associations between SES and fractional anisotropy in the superior longitudinal fasciculi.
We examined whether cognitive stimulation was a mechanism linking SES and cortical thickness in the frontoparietal network. Although the association between SES and cortical thickness was not statistically significant, cognitive stimulation was associated with both SES and cortical thickness, as noted above. It is well-established that requiring a significant association in the direct path in order to test statistical mediation reduces power to detect mediation, and that is appropriate to examine a mediation analysis even when the direct effect does not reach conventional thresholds of statistical significance (Hayes, 2009; MacKinnon, Fairchild, and Fritz, 2010). Here, we observed a significant indirect effect of SES on cortical thickness in the left MFG (95% CI: .0001–.0872) and left SPL (95% CI: .0052–.0765) through cognitive stimulation.
3.4 SES, Cognitive Stimulation, and Neural Function
We next investigated whether SES was associated with neural recruitment during the WM task in a whole-brain analysis. SES was positively associated with BOLD signal in left MFG, left occipital fusiform gyrus, and medial superior frontal gyrus (SFG) during WM Encoding, such that youths with higher SES exhibited greater activation in these regions (Table 3, Figure 4A). During the Delay (i.e., WM maintenance), SES was positively associated with activation in left MFG, left orbitofrontal cortex (OFC), and left posterior middle temporal gyrus, again reflecting greater activation among youth with higher SES (Table 3, Figure 4B). During the Probe, SES was positively associated with activation in left posterior middle temporal gyrus (Table 3, Figure 4C). There were no areas where SES was negatively associated with activation. Cognitive stimulation was not associated with neural activation in frontoparietal regions.
Table 3.
Log income-to-needs. MNI coordinates reflect the peak of each cluster.
Encoding > ITI | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Anatomical Region | x | y | z | voxels | z-max | p-value |
Left middle frontal gyrus, | −40 | 10 | 40 | 199 | 3.32 | .0005 |
Left occipital fusiform cortex | −14 | −84 | −22 | 148 | 3.85 | .006 |
Bilateral medial superior frontal gyrus | 0 | 10 | −54 | 140 | 3.29 | .009 |
| ||||||
Delay > ITI | ||||||
Anatomical Region | x | y | z | voxels | z-max | p-value |
| ||||||
Left posterior middle temporal gyrus | −62 | −52 | 2 | 195 | 4.85 | .002 |
Left orbitofrontal cortex | −40 | −26 | −4 | 165 | 3.47` | .007 |
Left middle frontal gyrus | −58 | 18 | 20 | 153 | 3.77 | .009 |
| ||||||
Probe > ITI | ||||||
Anatomical Region | x | y | z | voxels | z-max | p-value |
| ||||||
Left posterior middle temporal gyrus | −54 | −48 | 16 | 175 | 3.74 | .004 |
Figure 4.
Whole brain activation associations during a working memory paradigm using mean-centered predictor of log income-to-needs during: A) Encoding, B) Delay, and C) Probe.
3.5 Brain-Behavior Associations
Given widespread associations of SES and cognitive stimulation with frontoparietal network structure and function, we examined whether any of these neural measures were associated with WM and academic performance.
With regard to neural structure, we found no associations of cortical thickness in left MFG and SPL with WM or parent-reported achievement (ps > .30). FA in left SLF was positively associated with WM performance (β = .31, p = .042) and parent-reported achievement (β = .34, p = .034; Figure 5A), but FA in the right SLF was not associated with either (β = .21–.26, p = .124–.192).
Figure 5.
Associations between FA in left SLF and parent-reported achievement (A) and between activation in regions that show whole-brain association with SES and parent-reported achievement (B and C).
For neural function, we examined regions where children with higher SES had heightened activation during the WM task and where we observed significant task-related activation within the frontoparietal network in the group average (Encoding: left MFG, left and right SFG; Delay: left MFG). See Methods for ROI definition. Given that we examined multiple regions, including those outside of frontoparietal regions (see below), we applied FDR correction to all tests. WM performance was positively associated with neural activation in left MFG (β = .38, p = .008), left SFG (β = .40, p = .008), and right SFG (β = .29, p = .038) during Encoding, but not left MFG during the Delay (β = .26, p = .076). Academic performance was positively associated with activation during Encoding in left MFG (β = .35, p = .040), left SFG (β = .36, p = .032; Figure 5B and C), but not right SFG (β = .20, p = .120) nor in left MFG during the Delay (β = .21, p = .102)
A significant association between SES and neural activation emerged in several regions outside of the frontoparietal network, including the left occipital fusiform cortex during Encoding, left OFC and left middle temporal gyrus during the Delay, and left middle temporal gyrus during the Probe. As a final step, we investigated whether any of these regions were significantly associated with WM performance or parent-reported achievement after FDR correction. We found a significant association between activation in the occipital fusiform cortex and both WM performance (β = .442, p = .022; Supplemental Figure 5) and parent-reported achievement (β = .509, p = .006; Supplemental Figure 6), and between activation in the left OFC during the Delay and parent-reported achievement (β = .320, p = .047; Supplemental Figure 6).
3.6 Mechanisms linking SES and Academic achievement
Finally, we examined whether the environmental, cognitive, and neural factors that were related to both SES and parent-reported achievement mediated this association. Specifically, we examined cognitive stimulation, WM performance, FA in left SLF, and activation in left MFG and left SFG during WM Encoding as mediators. Jointly, these factors significantly mediated the association between SES and parent-reported achievement, and the strong association between SES and achievement was no longer significant in the final model (c path, B =.439, p = .007; c′ path, B =.068, p = .67; Figure 6). We conducted an additional mediation analysis that included all functional ROIs that were significantly associated with both SES and parent-reported achievement (i.e., including those outside the frontoparietal network) and this mediation analysis also produced a significant indirect effect (95% CI: .105 to .800, c path, B =.439, p = .007; c′ path, B =.021, p = .89; Supplemental Figure 7).
Figure 6.
Mediation model. Cognitive stimulation, WM performance, white matter connectivity in the frontoparietal network, and neural recruitment during the working memory task mediate the association between SES and parent-reported achievement (95% CI: .089 to .766).
4. Discussion
The present study extends the burgeoning literature on SES-related neural differences that may contribute to the achievement gap by documenting differences in brain structure and function as a function of family SES that, in turn, are related to WM and academic performance. First, although we did not find differences in cortical thickness as a function of SES, we provide novel evidence for a link between low cognitive stimulation in the home environment—which has frequently been documented in low-SES households—and thinner cortex in frontoparietal regions. Second, we extend prior work that has focused primarily on cortical and sub-cortical structure by documenting SES-related differences in white matter microstructure in the SLF, a frontoparietal tract involved in WM (Mabbott et al., 2006; Vestergaard et al., 2011). Moreover, greater white matter integrity in this tract was associated with better academic achievement. Third, we demonstrate SES-related differences in brain activation in PFC and occipital-temporal cortex during WM encoding, maintenance, and retrieval, such that children from families with higher SES showed greater task-related activation. Greater activation in these regions was associated with improved performance on the WM task as well as better academic achievement. Finally, we found that cognitive stimulation, WM performance, and structure and function in the frontoparietal network fully mediated the association of SES with parent-reported achievement. Taken together, these findings highlight multiple links between SES and frontoparietal network structure and function and provide evidence for several environmental, neural, and cognitive factors that contribute to the SES-achievement gap.
4.1 Cognitive Stimulation and Cortical Thickness
We extend prior work on SES and cortical thickness by documenting associations between cognitive stimulation and thickness in key nodes of the frontoparietal network and demonstrating that the degree of cognitive stimulation is a mechanism linking SES and cortical thickness. Cognitive stimulation is a key dimension of environmental experience that varies by SES (Bradley et al., 2001) and is argued to influence cortical thinning (McLaughlin et al., 2017; Sheridan & McLaughlin, 2014, 2016). Prior studies have found that low-SES and institutionalization, which are associated with low levels of cognitive simulation, are associated with cortical thickness in the frontoparietal network (McLaughlin et al., 2014; Mackey et al., 2015; Piccolo et al., 2016; Brito et al., 2017). However, this is the first study to our knowledge to demonstrate direct associations between cognitive stimulation in the home and cortical structure in children and adolescents as well as a significant indirect effect of SES on cortical thickness that operates through cognitive stimulation. These findings provide some support for theoretical models arguing that some forms of adversity characterized by environmental deprivation are associated with accelerated pruning of the cortex due to a lack of cognitive stimulation (McLaughlin, Sheridan, & Nelson, 2017; Sheridan & McLaughlin, 2014; Sheridan & McLaughlin, 2016). Perhaps most importantly, these findings provide a direct target for interventions aimed at reducing the income-achievement gap, as cognitive stimulation was also related to academic achievement. Enhancing cognitive stimulation in the home environment might be one effective strategy for reducing SES-related disparities in academic outcomes.
4.2 SES, White Matter Microstructure, and Academic Achievement
Our findings also provide novel evidence for SES-related differences in white matter microstructure in the frontoparietal network. Specifically, we show that higher SES is associated with greater FA in the SLF, a tract that connects the dorsolateral PFC and posterior parietal cortex. Moreover, greater FA in the left SLF was associated with higher levels of academic achievement. Higher levels of connectivity in the SLF have been associated with better EF in children and adolescents (Urger et al., 2015), although in one study this was true only among low-SES youths (Ursache & Noble, 2016). Together, our findings suggest that greater white matter integrity in the frontoparietal network among children from higher-SES backgrounds may contribute to better WM and academic achievement and are broadly consistent with prior work demonstrating positive associations between FA in the SLF and educational attainment in late adolescence (Noble et al., 2013).
4.3 SES, Neural Function, and Academic Achievement
Our fMRI results replicate a previous study that found greater activation in the MFG during WM among higher- as compared to lower-SES children (Finn et al., 2016), although a recent study found that lower SES was associated with greater activation in the IPS during WM (Sheridan et al., 2017). Future studies should investigate the possibility that different regions of the frontoparietal network are differentially influenced by early SES. Also consistent with prior work (Finn et al., 2016), we found that greater left MFG activation during WM encoding was associated with both higher performance on the WM task and higher achievement. We extend this prior work in several ways. First, we show that higher SES is associated with greater activation during WM encoding in the medial SFG, a key node in the cognitive control network (Yeo et al., 2011) that is recruited for a wide range of EF tasks (e.g., Church et al., 2017; Verbruggen & Logan, 2008). Greater activation in this region was associated with better performance on the task as well as higher academic achievement. We additionally document greater recruitment of the fusiform gyrus during WM encoding among children from families with higher SES; greater activation in this region is also associated with better WM performance and academic achievement. Here, children raised in low-SES environments exhibit reduced activation in the visual association region involved in the initial encoding of the to-be-remembered stimulus—this could reflect reduced depth of perceptual processing or reduced visual attention (see Amso & Scerif, 2015; Rosen et al, in press).
4.4 Environmental, Cognitive, and Neural, Mechanisms in the SES-Achievement Gap
Critically, we found that cognitive stimulation, WM performance, frontoparietal white matter structure, and frontoparietal recruitment during a WM task jointly mediated the association between SES and parent-reported achievement. These findings are broadly consistent with prior work showing that neural recruitment during a WM task could be a mechanism explaining the income-achievement gap (Finn et al., 2016) and that enrichment and stimulation in the home environment may explain the association between SES and EF (Hackman et al., 2015). Here, we provide evidence that multiple environmental, cognitive, and neural factors are involved in the association between SES and parent-reported achievement.
Cognitive stimulation was not associated with white matter microstructure or functional recruitment during the WM task, although each of these measures was associated with SES. Recent work has suggested that many aspects of the home environment not measured in the present study including environmental predictability, exposure to toxins, nutrition, and parenting, may contribute SES-related neural differences (Johnson et al., 2016). It is possible that in low-SES environments, which are often characterized by lower levels of predictability (Deater-Deckard et al., 2012; Evans & Wachs, 2009), the environment produces an information processing style that prioritizes short-term goals over longer-term goals (Liu, Feng et al., 2012; Shah, Mullainathan, and Shafir, 2012). This shift, coupled with fewer opportunities to engage in cognitively stimulating activities, may result in chronic underutilization of the frontoparietal network, which in turn may result in reduced ability to engage this network when circumstances demand it. Importantly, this strategy and resulting neural changes could be adaptive for children growing up in unpredictable environments (Ellwood-Lowe et al., 2016). However, when children encounter environments that require long-term sustained attention (e.g. school), they may be less equipped to deal with the required focus on long-term goals and associated EFs required to obtain those goals, resulting in poor performance.
Moreover, exposure to complex language is a key domain of cognitive stimulation that we did not measure directly. Differences in language exposure may have contributed to the left-lateralized pattern of SES-related differences observed here, consistent with work showing a reduced left lateralization among low compared to high SES children during a language rhyming task (Raizada et al., 2008). Language complexity and child-directed speech is reduced among children raised in low-SES environments (Hart & Risley, 1995; Walker, Greenwood, Hart, & Carta, 1994; Fernald et al., 2013). While reduced language complexity and contingency has an obvious link to SES-related differences in language performance (Noble et al., 2005; Noble et al., 2007) and neural recruitment during language tasks (Romeo et al., 2017), a previous study found that reduced complexity of language in the home environment was associated with differential recruitment of the PFC during an EF task among low-SES children (Sheridan et al., 2012). More complex language environments may provide greater opportunities for children to build and practice EF skills (McLaughlin, 2016) as well as early opportunities to disambiguate perceptually similar information that scaffold the development of the PFC and more complex forms of EF (see Amso & Scerif, 2015). Indeed, the PFC computations necessary for EF skills emerge much earlier in development than previously thought through the process of learning language (Werchan, Collins, Frank, & Amso, 2016). Lower levels of caregiver interactions in low-SES households may contribute to reduced opportunities for learning categories of stimuli processed in the ventral visual stream; these differences may ultimately influence the ability to encode and sustain representations of complex visual stimuli (Amso & Scerif, 2015). Our finding that SES was positively associated with fusiform activation during WM encoding, which in turn was related to both WM and academic performance, is consistent with this possibility, and indicates that differences in early visual and linguistic processing may have meaningful implications for more complex cognitive functions. Higher SES environments, perhaps because of their higher levels of cognitive and caregiver stimulation, may enhance visual attention or perceptual processing during encoding and increase the ability to sustain these representations during maintenance, resulting in better performance. Future research should more closely examine language exposure and visual processing as potential mechanisms explaining SES related differences in neural, cognitive, and academic outcomes.
4.5 Limitations and Future Directions
The present findings have several notable strengths. Importantly, previous studies have used proxies for SES, including whether the child received free or reduced lunch (Finn et al., 2016), while in the present study we directly assess family SES as well as a parent report measure of cognitive stimulation. Furthermore, unlike previous studies we measure and control for childhood violence exposure to identify differences in brain structure and function that are driven by SES-related differences that are independent of violence exposure, which is known to impact neural development (McLaughlin et al., 2014). However, there are also several key limitations. First, the sample was relatively small, particularly given the large age range, which limited statistical power. Indeed, cortical thickness and white matter connectivity results only emerged in ROI analyses and not in the whole brain. A limited sample size increases the risk of both Type I and Type II errors and therefore, results should be interpreted with caution. Future studies should seek to replicate these findings with a larger sample size.
Second, unlike other studies that have found a link between SES, brain structure and function, and academic achievement (Mackey et al., 2015; Finn et al., 2016), we did not have access to statewide standardized test results, as such tests are not performed at every grade level in Washington State. Nor did we focus on actual grades, as the children in our sample came from a wide range of schools. Instead we relied on parent reports of children’s academic performance across a range of subjects, which are strongly correlated with academic performance and do not vary by demographics (Maguin & Loeber, 1996). However, it is possible that SES may be related to differences in how parents perceive their children’s academic performance. Parent-reports of academic achievement have been shown to be significantly related to objectively measured achievement, but are not a perfect proxy (Maguin & Loeber, 1996; Quiroga et al., 2013) and lower SES parents may be less accurate (e.g., Huston et al., 2005). Therefore, it will be critical for future studies to replicate the present findings using more objective measures of academic achievement. Additionally, while there is reason to believe that cognitive stimulation would mediate the link between SES and neural measures and those neural measures would in turn mediate the link between SES and academic achievement, the data did not support moving forward with a multilevel mediation analysis because cognitive stimulation was associated only with cortical thickness and not the other neural measures. Any future studies that find links between SES, cognitive stimulation, neural measures, and academic achievement should explore this possibility. Furthermore, although our motion correction threshold remains common in pediatric fMRI studies, including recent studies on the development of the frontoparietal network and on SES-related differences in neural function (e.g., Peters et al., 2016; Romeo et al., 2017), even small amounts of motion can impact fMRI results (Siegel et al., 2014) and therefore results from the current study should be interpreted with caution. Additionally, it should be noted that the regions we refer to as the frontoparietal network overlap considerably with both the dorsal attention and cognitive control networks (Yeo, Krienen et al., 2011). While findings for both of these networks would generate similar interpretations, future studies should focus on more fine-grained parcellation of regions within these networks. Finally, because of the cross-sectional nature of the study, replication of our findings with regard to environmental, cognitive, and neural mediators in longitudinal studies is warranted.
4.6 Conclusions
The present study adds to the growing body of literature that highlights SES-related differences in neural structure and function in children. We document multiple links between childhood SES and frontoparietal network structure and function and provide novel evidence that low cognitive stimulation, a common characteristic of low-SES households, is associated with thinner cortex in the frontoparietal network. Perhaps most importantly, we show that low cognitive stimulation, poor WM performance, reduced white matter microstructure in the fronto-parietal network, and reduced PFC recruitment during a WM task are mechanisms underlying the association between SES and academic achievement. Together, these findings suggest novel targets for reducing SES-related disparities in academic performance.
Supplementary Material
Table 1.
Income-to-Needs Ratio
| ||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Age | ||||||||||
| ||||||||||
<0 | 1–2 | 2–3 | 3–4 | 4–5 | 5–6 | 6–7 | 7–8 | >8 | Total | |
8 | 3 | 2 | 1 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 6 |
9 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 1 | 0 | 1 | 2 |
10 | 2 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 1 | 0 | 0 | 3 |
11 | 3 | 1 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 2 | 0 | 0 | 6 |
12 | 2 | 0 | 1 | 1 | 0 | 0 | 1 | 1 | 0 | 5 |
13 | 2 | 1 | 0 | 1 | 0 | 0 | 1 | 1 | 0 | 6 |
14 | 1 | 0 | 1 | 0 | 1 | 0 | 1 | 0 | 1 | 5 |
15 | 3 | 1 | 2 | 0 | 0 | 1 | 1 | 0 | 2 | 10 |
16 | 2 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 2 | 0 | 4 |
17 | 2 | 1 | 0 | 0 | 1 | 0 | 2 | 0 | 0 | 6 |
18 | 0 | 1 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 1 |
19 | 1 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 1 |
Acknowledgments
Funding: This work was supported by the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development at the National Institute of Health [F32 HD089514 to MR], National Institute of Mental Health at the National Institutes of Health [R01-MH103291 and R01-MH106482 to KM], the Brain and Behavior Foundation NARSAD Early Investigator Award, an Early Career Research Fellowship from the Jacobs Foundation, and the IMHRO Rising Star Award to KM.
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
References
- Achenbach TM, Howell CT, Quay HC, Conners CK. National survey of problems and competencies among four- to sixteen-year-olds: parents’ reports for normative and clinical samples. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development. 1991;56(3):1–131. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Amso D, Scerif G. The attentive brain: insights from developmental cognitive neuroscience. Nature Reviews: Neuroscience. 2015;16:606–619. doi: 10.1038/nrn4025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Andersson JLR, Sotiropoulos SN. An integrated approach to correction for off-resonance effects and subject movement in diffusion MR imaging. NeuroImage. 2016;125:1063–1078. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.10.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Askren MK, McAllister-Day TK, Koh N, Mestre Z, Dines JN, Korman BA, et al. Using Make for Reproducible and Parallel Neuroimaging Workflow and Quality-Assurance. Frontiers in Neuroinformatics. 2016;10(13):2. doi: 10.3389/fninf.2016.00002. http://doi.org/10.3389/fninf.2016.00002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Avants BB, Tustison NJ, Song G, Cook PA, Klein A, Gee JC. A reproducible evaluation of ANTs similarity metric performance in brain image registration. NeuroImage. 2011;54(3):2033–2044. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.09.025. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.09.025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baydar N, Brooks-Gunn J, Furstenberg FF. Early warning signs of functional illiteracy: predictors in childhood and adolescence. Child Development. 1993;64(3):815–829. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1993.tb02945.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Behzadi Y, Restom K, Liau J, Liu TT. A component based noise correction method (CompCor) for BOLD and perfusion based fMRI. NeuroImage. 2007;37(1):90–101. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.04.042. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.04.042. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bernstein DP, Ahluvalia T, Pogge D, Handelsman L. Validity of the childhood trauma questionnaire in an adolescent psychiatric population. 1997;36(3):340–348. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199703000-00012. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00004583-199703000-00012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Best JR, Miller PH, Naglieri JA. Relations between Executive Function and Academic Achievement from Ages 5 to 17 in a Large, Representative National Sample. Learning and Individual Differences. 2011;21(4):327–336. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2011.01.007. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2011.01.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bick J, Zhu T, Stamoulis C, Fox NA, Zeanah C, Nelson CA. Effect of early institutionalization and foster care on long-term white matter development. Journal of the American Medical Association, Pediatrics. 2015;169(3):211–219. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2014.3212. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bifulco A, Brown GW, Lillie A, Jarvis J. Memories of childhood neglect and abuse: corroboration in a series of sisters. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 1997;38(3):365–375. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1997.tb01520.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blair C, Diamond A. Biological processes in prevention and intervention: the promotion of self-regulation as a means of preventing school failure. Development and Psychopathology. 2008;20(3):899–911. doi: 10.1017/S0954579408000436. http://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579408000436. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bradley RH, Corwyn RF. Socioeconomic status and child development. Annual Review of Psychology. 2002;53(1):371–399. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135233. http://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135233. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bradley RH, Corwyn RF, McAdoo HP, Coll CG. The home environments of children in the United States part I: variations by age, ethnicity, and poverty status. Child Development. 2001;72(6):1844–1867. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.t01-1-00382. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brito NH, Noble KG. Socioeconomic status and structural brain development. Frontiers in Neuroscience. 2014;8:276. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2014.00276. http://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2014.00276. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brito NH, Piccolo LR, Noble KM. Associations between cortical thickness and neurocognitive skills during childhood vary by socioeconomic status. Brain and Cognition. 2017;116:54–62. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2017.03.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brooks-Gunn J, Duncan GJ. The effects of poverty on children. The Future of Children / Center for the Future of Children, the David and Lucile Packard Foundation. 1997;7(2):55–71. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Church JA, Bunge SA, Petersen SE, Schlaggar BL. Preparatory Engagement of Cognitive Control Networks Increases Late in Childhood. Cerebral Cortex (New York, N.Y. : 1991) 2017;27(3):2139–2153. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhw046. http://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhw046. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cleveland HH, Jacobson KC, Lipinski JJ, Rowe DC. Genetic and shared environmental contributions to the relationship between the HOME environment and child and adolescent achievement. Intelligence. 2000;28(1):69–86. doi: 10.1016/S0160-2896(99)00029-X. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Corbetta M, Shulman GL. Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience. 2002;3(3):201–215. doi: 10.1038/nrn755. http://doi.org/10.1038/nrn755. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Curtis CE, D’Esposito M. Persistent activity in the prefrontal cortex during working memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2003;7(9):415–423. doi: 10.1016/s1364-6613(03)00197-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dale AM, Fischl B, Sereno MI. Cortical surface-based analysis. I. Segmentation and surface reconstruction. NeuroImage. 1999;9(2):179–194. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1998.0395. http://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.1998.0395. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Deater-Deckard K, Chen N, Wang Z, Bell MA. Socioeconomic risk moderates the link between household chaos and maternal executive function. Journal of Family Psychology : JFP : Journal of the Division of Family Psychology of the American Psychological Association (Division 43) 2012;26(3):391–399. doi: 10.1037/a0028331. http://doi.org/10.1037/a0028331. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Demir-Lira ÖE, Prado J, Booth JR. Neural correlates of math gains vary depending on parental socioeconomic status (SES) Frontiers In Psychology. 2016;7(892) doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00892. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eklund A, Nichols TE, Knutsson H. Cluster failure: Why fMRI inferences for spatial extent have inflated false-positive rates. 2016;113(28):7900–7905. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1602413113. http://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1602413113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ellwood-Lowe ME, Sacchet MD, Gotlib IH. The application of neuroimaging to social inequity and language disparity: A cautionary examination. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience. 2016;22:1–8. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2016.10.001. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2016.10.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eluvathingal TJ, Chugani HT, Behen ME, Juhász C, Muzik O, Maqbool M, et al. Abnormal brain connectivity in children after early severe socioemotional deprivation: a diffusion tensor imaging study. Pediatrics. 2006;117(6):2093–2100. doi: 10.1542/peds.2005-1727. http://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2005-1727. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Evans GW, Wachs T. Chaos and its influence on children’s development: An ecological perspective. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 2009. [Google Scholar]
- Fernald A, Marchman VA, Weisleder A. SES differences in language processing skill and vocabulary are evident at 18 months. Developmental Science. 2013;16(2):234–248. doi: 10.1111/desc.12019. http://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Finn AS, Minas JE, Leonard JA, Mackey AP, Salvatore J, Goetz C, et al. Functional brain organization of working memory in adolescents varies in relation to family income and academic achievement. Developmental Science. 2016 doi: 10.1111/desc.12450. http://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12450. [DOI] [PubMed]
- Fischl B, Sereno MI, Dale AM. Cortical surface-based analysis. II: Inflation, flattening, and a surface-based coordinate system. NeuroImage. 1999;9(2):195–207. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1998.0396. http://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.1998.0396. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ghosh SS, Kakunoori S, Augustinack J, Nieto-Castanon A, Kovelman I, Gaab N, et al. Evaluating the validity of volume-based and surface-based brain image registration for developmental cognitive neuroscience studies in children 4 to 11 years of age. NeuroImage. 2010;53(1):85–93. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.05.075. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.05.075. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gianaros PJ, Marsland AL, Sheu LK, Erickson KI, Verstynen TD. Inflammatory pathways link socioeconomic inequalities to white matter architecture. Cerebral Cortex (New York, N.Y. : 1991) 2013;23(9):2058–2071. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs191. http://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhs191. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hackman DA, Farah MJ. Socioeconomic status and the developing brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2009;13(2):65–73. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.11.003. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2008.11.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hackman DA, Gallop R, Evans GW, Farah MJ. Socioeconomic status and executive function: developmental trajectories and mediation. Developmental Science. 2015;18(5):686–702. doi: 10.1111/desc.12246. http://doi.org/10.1111/desc.1224. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hanson JL, Adluru N, Chung MK, Alexander AL, Davidson RJ, Pollak SD. Early neglect is associated with alterations in white matter integrity and cognitive functioning. Child Development. 2013;84(5):1566–1578. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12069. http://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12069. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hart B, Risley TR. Meaningful differences in the everyday experiences of young American children. Baltimore, MD: Brookes; 1995. [Google Scholar]
- Hayes AF. Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach. New York, NY, US: Guilford Press; 2013. [Google Scholar]
- Huston AC, Duncan GJ, McLoyd VC, Crosby DA, Ripke MN, Weisner TS, Eldred CA. Impacts on children of a policy to promote employment and reduce poverty for low-income parents: new hope after 5 years. Developmental Psychology. 2005;41(6):902–918. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.41.6.902. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jahanshad N, Kochunov PV, Sprooten E, Mandl RC, Nichols TE, Almasy L, et al. Multi-site genetic analysis of diffusion images and voxelwise heritability analysis: a pilot project of the ENIGMA-DTI working group. NeuroImage. 2013;81:455–469. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.04.061. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.04.061. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jednoróg K, Altarelli I, Monzalvo K, Fluss J, Dubois J, Billard C, et al. The influence of socioeconomic status on children’s brain structure. PloS One. 2012;7(8):e42486. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0042486. http://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0042486. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jenkinson M, Beckmann CF, Behrens TEJ, Woolrich MW, Smith SM. FSL. NeuroImage. 2012;62(2):782–790. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.09.015. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.09.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnson SB, Riis JL, Noble KG. State of the art review: poverty and the developing brain. Pediatrics. 2016 doi: 10.1542/peds.2015-3075. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kim P, Evans GW, Angstadt M, Ho SS, Sripada CS, Swain JE, Phan KL. Effects of childhood poverty and chronic stress on emotion regulatory brain function in adulthood. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2013;110(46):18442–18447. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1308240110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kumar A, Behen ME, Singsoonsud P, Veenstra AL, Wolfe-Christensen C, Helder E, Chugani HT. Microstructural abnormalities in language and limbic pathways in orphanage-reared children: a diffusion tensor imaging study. Journal of Child Neurology. 2014;29(3):318–325. doi: 10.1177/0883073812474098. http://doi.org/10.1177/0883073812474098. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lambert HK, Sheridan MA, Sambrook K, Rosen M, Askren MK, McLaughlin KA. Hippocampal Contribution to Context Encoding across Development is Disrupted following Early-Life Adversity. The Journal of Neuroscience : the Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience. 2017;37(7) doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2618-16.2017. 2618-16-1934. http://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2618-16.2017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lawson GM, Duda JT, Avants BB, Wu J, Farah MJ. Associations between children’s socioeconomic status and prefrontal cortical thickness. Developmental Science. 2013;16(5):641–652. doi: 10.1111/desc.12096. http://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12096. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liu L, Feng T, Suo T, Lee K, Li H. Adapting to destitute situations: poverty cues lead to short-term choice. PLOS One. 2012 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0033950. htpps://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0033950. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Mackey AP, Finn AS, Leonard JA, Jacoby-Senghor DS, West MR, Gabrieli CFO, Gabrieli JDE. Neuroanatomical correlates of the income-achievement gap. Psychological Science. 2015;26(6):925–933. doi: 10.1177/0956797615572233. http://doi.org/10.1177/0956797615572233. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mabbot DJ, Rovet J, Noseworthy MD, Smith ML, Rockel C. The relations between white matter and declarative memory in older children and adolescents. Brain Research. 2009;1294:80–90. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2009.07.046. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maguin E, Loeber R. How well do ratings of academic performance by mothers and their sons correspond to grades, academic test scores, and teachers’ ratings? Journal of Behavioral Education. 1996;6(4):405–425. [Google Scholar]
- Marcus DS, Harms MP, Snyder AZ, Jenkinson M, Wilson JA, Glasser MF, et al. Human Connectome Project informatics: quality control, database services, and data visualization. NeuroImage. 2013;80:202–219. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.077. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.077. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- MacKinnon DP, Fairchild AJ, Fritz MS. Mediation analysis. Annual Review of Psychology. 2007;58:593–614. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085542. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McLaughlin KA. Future directions in childhood adversity and youth psychopathology. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology. 2016;45(3):361–382. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2015.1110823. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McLaughlin KA, Sheridan MA. Beyond Cumulative Risk: A Dimensional Approach to Childhood Adversity. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2016;25(4):239–245. doi: 10.1177/0963721416655883. http://doi.org/10.1177/0963721416655883. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McLaughlin KA, Sheridan MA, Lambert HK. Childhood adversity and neural development: deprivation and threat as distinct dimensions of early experience. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 2014;47:578–591. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.10.012. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.10.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McLaughlin KA, Sheridan MA, Nelson CA. Neglect as a Violation of Species-Expectant Experience: Neurodevelopmental Consequences. Biological Psychiatry. 2017 doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2017.02.1096. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2017.02.1096. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Miyake A, Friedman NP. The nature and organization of individual differences in executive functions: Four general conclusions. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2012;21(1):8–14. doi: 10.1177/0963721411429458. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721411429458. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mori S, Oishi K, Jiang H, Jiang L, Li X, Akhter K, Mazziotta J. Stereotaxic white matter atlas based on diffusion tensor imaging in an ICBM template. Neuroimage. 2008;40(2):570–582. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.12.035. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mott FL. The Utility of the HOME-SF Scale for Child Development Research in a Large National Longitudinal Survey: The National Longitudinal Survey of Youth 1979 Cohort. Parenting: Science and Practice. 2004;4(2–3) https://doi.org/10.1080/15295192.2004.9681273. [Google Scholar]
- Noble KG, Grieve SM, Korgaonkar MS, Engelhardt LE, Griffith EY, Williams LM, Brickman AM. Hippocampal volume varies with educational attainment across the life-span. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 2012;6:307. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00307. http://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2012.00307. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Noble KG, Houston SM, Brito NH, Bartsch H, Kan E, Kuperman JM, et al. Family income, parental education and brain structure in children and adolescents. Nature Neuroscience. 2015;18(5):773–778. doi: 10.1038/nn.3983. http://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3983. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Noble KG, Houston SM, Kan E, Sowell ER. Neural correlates of socioeconomic status in the developing human brain. Developmental Science. 2012:1–12. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2012.01147.x. doi:10:1111/j.1467-7687.2012.01147.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Noble KG, Korgaonkar MS, Grieve SM, Brickman AM. Higher education is an age-independent predictor of white matter integrity and cognitive control in late adolescence. Developmental Science. 2013;16(5):653–664. doi: 10.1111/desc.12077. http://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12077. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Noble KG, McCandliss BD, Farah MJ. Socioeconomic gradients predict individual differences in neurocognitive abilities. Developmental Science. 2007;10(4):464–480. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00600.x. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00600.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Noble KG, Norman MF, Farah MJ. Neurocognitive correlates of socioeconomic status in kindergarten children. Developmental Science. 2005;8(1):74–87. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2005.00394.x. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2005.00394.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Noble KG, Wolmetz ME, Ochs LG, Farah MJ, McCandliss BD. Brain-behavior relationships in reading acquisition are modulated by socioeconomic factors. Developmental Science. 2006;9(6):642–654. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2006.00542.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peters S, Van Duijvenvoorde AC, Koolschijn PC, Crone EA. Longitudinal development of frontoparietal activity during feedback learning: contribitions of age, performance, working memory and cortical thickness. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience. 2016;19:211–22. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2016.04.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Piccolo LR, Merz EC, He X, Sowell ER, Noble KG Pediatric Imaging, Neurocognition, Genetics Study. Age-Related Differences in Cortical Thickness Vary by Socioeconomic Status. PloS One. 2016;11(9):e0162511. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0162511. http://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0162511. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Quiroga CV, Janosz M, Bisset S, Morin AJS. Early adolescent depression symptoms and school dropout: Mediating processes involving self-reported academic competence and achievement. Journal of Educational Psychology. 2013;105(2):552–560. [Google Scholar]
- Raizada RDS, Richards TL, Meltzoff A, Kuhl PK. Socioeconomic status predicts hemispheric specialisation of the left inferior frontal gyrus in young children. NeuroImage. 2008;40(3):1392–1401. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.01.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Raschle N, Zuk J, Ortiz-Mantilla S, Sliva DD, Franceschi A, Grant PE, et al. Pediatric neuroimaging in early childhood and infancy: challenges and practical guidelines. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2012;1252(1):43–50. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2012.06457.x. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.2012.06457.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roalf DR, Quarmley M, Elliott MA, Satterthwaite TD, Vandekar SN, Ruparel K, Prabhakaran K. The impact of quality assurance assessment on diffusion tensor imaging outcomes in a large-scale population-based cohort. Neuroimage. 2016;125:903–919. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.10.068. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roche A. A four-dimensional registration algorithm with application to joint correction of motion and slice timing in fMRI. IEEE Transactions on Medical Imaging. 2011;30(8):1546–1554. doi: 10.1109/TMI.2011.2131152. http://doi.org/10.1109/TMI.2011.2131152. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Romeo RR, Leonard JA, Robinson ST, West MR, Mackey AP, Rowe ML, Gabrieli JDE. “Beyond the ‘30 million word gap:’ Children’s conversational exposure is associated with language-related brain function”. Psychological Science. 2017 doi: 10.1177/0956797617742725. Epub ahead of print. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rosen ML, Sheridan MA, Sambrook KA, Peverill MR, Meltzoff AN, McLaughlin KA. The role of visual association cortex in associative memory formation across development. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_01202. Epub ahead of print. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_01202. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Siegel JS, Power JD, Dubis JW, Vogel AC, Church JA, Schlaggar BL, Petersen SE. Statistical improvements in functional magnetic resonance imaging analyses produced by censoring high-motion data points. Human Brain Mapping. 2014;35(5):1981–96. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22307. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shah AK, Mullainathan S, Shafir E. Some consequences of having too little. Science. 2012;338:682–685. doi: 10.1126/science.1222426. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shaw P, Greenstein D, Lerch J, Clasen L, Lenroot R, Gogtay N, et al. Intellectual ability and cortical development in children and adolescents. Nature. 2006;440(7084):676–679. doi: 10.1038/nature04513. http://doi.org/10.1038/nature04513. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sheridan MA, McLaughlin KA. Dimensions of early experience and neural development: deprivation and threat. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2014;18(11):580–585. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2014.09.001. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2014.09.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sheridan MA, McLaughlin KA. Neurobiological models of the impact of adversity on education. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences. 2016;10:108–113. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2016.05.013. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2016.05.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sheridan MA, Peverill M, McLaughlin KA. Deprivation, but not trauma, influences prefrontal recruitment during a working memory filtering task. Development and Psychopathology. 2017;29:1777–1794. doi: 10.1017/S0954579417001390. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sheridan MA, Sarsour K, Jutte D, D’Esposito M, Boyce WT. The Impact of Social Disparity on Prefrontal Function in Childhood. PloS One. 2012;7(4):e35744. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0035744. http://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0035744. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith SM, Jenkinson M, Johansen-Berg H, Rueckert D, Nichols TE, Mackay CE, et al. Tract-based spatial statistics: voxelwise analysis of multi-subject diffusion data. NeuroImage. 2006;31(4):1487–1505. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.02.024. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.02.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spielberg JM, Galarce EM, Ladouceur CD, McMakin DL, Olino TM, Forbes EE, Dahl RE. Adolescent development of inhibition as a function of SES and gender: Converging evidence from behavior and fMRI. Human Brain Mapping. 2015;36(8):3194–3203. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22838. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Steinberg AM, Brymer MJ, Decker KB, Pynoos RS. The University of California at Los Angeles Post-traumatic Stress Disorder Reaction Index. Current Psychiatry Reports. 2004;6(2):96–100. doi: 10.1007/s11920-004-0048-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tottenham N, Tanaka JW, Leon AC, McCarry T, Nurse M, Hare TA, et al. The NimStim set of facial expressions: judgments from untrained research participants. Psychiatry Research. 2009;168(3):242–249. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2008.05.006. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2008.05.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Urger SE, De Bellis MD, Hooper SR, Woolley DP, Chen SD, Provenzale J. The superior longitudinal fasciculus in typically developing children and adolescents: diffusion tensor imaging and neuropsychological correlates. Journal of Child Neurology. 2015;30(1):9–20. doi: 10.1177/0883073813520503. http://doi.org/10.1177/0883073813520503. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ursache A, Noble KG. Neurocognitive development in socioeconomic context: Multiple mechanisms and implications for measuring socioeconomic status. Psychophysiology. 2016;53(1):71–82. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12547. http://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.12547. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Verbruggen F, Logan GD. Response inhibition in the stop-signal paradigm. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2008;12(11):418–424. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.07.005. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2008.07.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vestergaard M, Madsen KS, Baaré WF, Skimminge A, Ejersbo LR, Ramsoy TZ, Jernigan TL. White matter microstructure in the superior longtudinal fasciculus associated with spatiatl working memory performance in children. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2011;23:2135–2146. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2010.21592. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vul E, Harris C, Winkielman P, Pashler H. Puzzlingly High Correlations in fMRI Studies of Emotion, Personality, and Social Cognition. Perspectives on Psychological Science : a Journal of the Association for Psychological Science. 2009;4(3):274–290. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6924.2009.01125.x. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-6924.2009.01125.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Walker D, Greenwood C, Hart B, Carta J. Prediction of school outcomes based on early language production and socioeconomic factors. Child Development. 1994;65(2):606–621. doi: 10.2307/1131404. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Walker EA, Unutzer J, Rutter C, Gelfand A, Saunders K, VonKorff M, Katon W. Costs of health care use by women HMO members with a history of childhood abuse and neglect. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1999;56(7):609–613. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.56.7.609. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Werchan D, Collins AG, Frank MJ, Amso D. Role of prefrontal cortex in learning and generalizing hierarchical rules in 8-month-old infants. The Journal of Neurosciene. 2016;36(40):10314–10322. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1351-16.2016. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1351-16.2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Woolrich MW, Behrens TEJ, Beckmann CF, Jenkinson M, Smith SM. Multilevel linear modelling for FMRI group analysis using Bayesian inference. NeuroImage. 2004;21(4):1732–1747. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.12.023. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.12.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yeo BTT, Krienen FM, Sepulcre J, Sabuncu MR, Lashkari D, Hollinshead M, et al. The organization of the human cerebral cortex estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2011;106(3):1125–1165. doi: 10.1152/jn.00338.2011. http://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00338.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.