Abstract
Within each sensory modality, age-related deficits in temporal perception contribute to the difficulties older adults experience when performing everyday tasks. Since perceptual experience is inherently multisensory, older adults also face the added challenge of appropriately integrating or segregating the auditory and visual cues present in our dynamic environment into coherent representations of distinct objects. As such, many studies have investigated how older adults perform when integrating temporal information across audition and vision. This review covers both direct judgments about temporal information (the sound-induced flash illusion, temporal order, perceived synchrony, and temporal rate discrimination) and judgments regarding stimuli containing temporal information (the audiovisual bounce effect and speech perception). Although an age-related increase in integration has been demonstrated on a variety of tasks, research specifically investigating the ability of older adults to integrate temporal auditory and visual cues has produced disparate results. In this short review, we explore what factors could underlie these divergent findings. We conclude that both task-specific differences and age-related sensory loss play a role in the reported disparity in age-related effects on the integration of auditory and visual temporal information.
Keywords: temporal perception, multisensory, audiovisual, integration, audition, vision, aging
Introduction
The normal aging process degrades both auditory and visual temporal perception, contributing to the difficulties older adults encounter with everyday tasks. For example, age-related impairments in speech comprehension and driving performance are associated with temporal processing deficits within audition (Gordon-Salant and Fitzgibbons, 1993; Füllgrabe et al., 2014; Babkoff and Fostick, 2017) and vision (Wood, 2002; Conlon and Herkes, 2008; Lacherez et al., 2014), respectively. However, many tasks stimulate both audition and vision. Therefore, this review targets how older adults combine these two sources of temporal information.
Integration binds auditory and visual stimuli into a unified percept, altering their perception relative to how they are perceived in isolation (Ernst and Bülthoff, 2004; Stein et al., 2009). Correctly inferring whether audiovisual integration is appropriate poses a behaviorally relevant challenge. Integration occurs when stimuli seem to share the same origin and one factor influencing this is close temporal correspondence between auditory and visual stimuli, such as similar onset (Lewald and Guski, 2003) and correlated temporal structure (Parise et al., 2012; Denison et al., 2013). Recent reviews indicate that aging increases integration across a range of perceptual domains and behaviors (Freiherr et al., 2013; de Dieuleveult et al., 2017). An age-related increase in integration of congruent auditory and visual cues may be beneficial, for example improving driving performance (Ramkhalawansingh et al., 2016). Conversely, integration of incongruent cues achieves a coherent percept at the cost of veridical perception and so greater integration could hinder everyday function in older adults. Interestingly, review of the literature specifically concerning the integration of temporal information indicates varied age-related effects, with reports of unaltered (e.g., Sommers et al., 2005; McGovern et al., 2014; Brooks et al., 2015) and decreased integration in older adults (e.g., Tye-Murray et al., 2011; Roudaia et al., 2013; Brooks et al., 2015). The perceptual change induced by integration depends on unisensory reliability (the precision of a sensory estimate of a property, as given by the inverse of its variance) and the brain’s tendency to integrate auditory and visual cues (Ernst and Bülthoff, 2004; Odegaard and Shams, 2016). In this minireview, we consider whether age-related sensory loss (which affects unisensory reliability) and task-related differences (which may impact the integration process) explain the diversity of study findings.
Age-Related Changes in Unisensory Temporal Perception
Physiological aging affects the auditory and visual pathways from the sensory periphery to the cortex, resulting in a decline in many aspects of temporal perception. Within audition, older age reduces sensitivity to temporal fine structure (Grose and Mamo, 2010; Moore et al., 2012; Füllgrabe, 2013; Füllgrabe et al., 2014), amplitude modulation (Takahashi and Bacon, 1992; He et al., 2008; Kumar and Sangamanatha, 2011; Füllgrabe et al., 2014; Wallaert et al., 2016) and frequency modulation (He et al., 2007; Grose and Mamo, 2012; Wallaert et al., 2016). Older adults exhibit impaired auditory gap detection (Snell, 1997; Strouse et al., 1998; Heinrich and Schneider, 2006; Humes et al., 2009; Kumar and Sangamanatha, 2011) and duration discrimination (Fitzgibbons and Gordon-Salant, 1994; Fitzgibbons and Gordon-Salant, 1995; Gordon-Salant and Fitzgibbons, 1999; Kumar and Sangamanatha, 2011), as well as impaired temporal order judgments (Gordon-Salant and Fitzgibbons, 1999; Fitzgibbons et al., 2006; Ulbrich et al., 2009) and temporal sequencing (Trainor and Trehub, 1989).
Within vision, many age-related temporal processing deficits have been documented, including reduced flicker sensitivity (Mayer et al., 1988; Tyler, 1989; Kuyk and Wesson, 1991; Kim and Mayer, 1994), reduced critical flicker frequency (Misiak, 1947, 1951; Coppinger, 1955; McFarland et al., 1958; Lachenmayr et al., 1994) and some impairments in motion perception (Trick and Silverman, 1991; Tran et al., 1998; Habak and Faubert, 2000; Snowden and Kavanagh, 2006; Bennett et al., 2007; Billino et al., 2008). Similar to deficits in auditory processing, there is an age-related decline in visual gap detection (Humes et al., 2009) and temporal order judgments (Ulbrich et al., 2009; Busey et al., 2010; de Boer-Schellekens and Vroomen, 2014).
Furthermore, sensory decline is typically uneven across vision and audition, though the more adversely affected sensory modality varies between tasks (Čeponienė et al., 2008; Guerreiro and Van Gerven, 2011; Lustig and Meck, 2011; Cliff et al., 2013; Diaconescu et al., 2013; Guerreiro et al., 2013). For example, older age impairs visual duration judgments more than auditory ones (Lustig and Meck, 2011). In contrast, auditory but not visual temporal rate discriminability is poorer in older adults (Brooks et al., 2015). Consequently, for a given audiovisual task, auditory and visual cues are not expected to be equally degraded by age-related sensory changes.
Age-Related Changes in Audiovisual Temporal Perception
Studies have investigated age-related changes to various measures of audiovisual temporal perception presumed to reflect multisensory integration, with disparate results for transient stimuli like flashes and beeps. Older adults are more susceptible to the sound-induced flash illusion (Setti et al., 2011a; DeLoss et al., 2013; McGovern et al., 2014). In this illusory fission effect, the integration of two beeps and a solitary flash results in the perception of two flashes (Shams et al., 2000). Older adults also exhibit an enhanced temporal ventriloquist effect (de Boer-Schellekens and Vroomen, 2014), in which the presentation of a click before and after a sequence of two flashes increases visual temporal order sensitivity (Morein-Zamir et al., 2003). Conversely, the integration of two flashes with a single beep results in the perception of a single flash in an illusory fusion effect (Andersen et al., 2004) that does not change in aging (McGovern et al., 2014). In the audiovisual bounce effect, an auditory beep influences a visual spatiotemporal illusion, in which two disks moving toward each other appear either to stream past one another, or less frequently, to bounce (Sekuler and Sekuler, 1999). The beep, sounding as the disks overlap, causes an increase in the frequency of perceived bouncing (Sekuler et al., 1997). This effect is reduced in older adults, suggestive of decreased integration (Roudaia et al., 2013).
Age-related effects on the integration of temporally modulated stimuli depend on audiovisual congruency. Partial integration of non-identical auditory and visual temporal rates distorts perceived rate such that the auditory or visual rate subjectively equivalent to a reference is non-veridical (Roach et al., 2006). This effect is equivalent in younger and older adults (Brooks et al., 2015). However, integration of identical auditory and visual temporal rates improves temporal rate discrimination in a statistically optimal fashion in younger (Koene et al., 2007; Brooks et al., 2015) but not older adults, who fail to show the √ 2 improvement predicted by maximum likelihood estimation (Brooks et al., 2015).
Research on audiovisual speech perception in aging is also relevant to this review, since the temporal relationships between auditory and visual speech cues (Chandrasekaran et al., 2009)facilitate both auditory speech detection (Grant and Seitz, 2000) and recognition (ten Oever et al., 2013; Jaekl et al., 2015). Visual facilitation of auditory speech detection is reduced in older adults (Tye-Murray et al., 2011). The effect of aging on audiovisual gains in speech recognition is more complex, with no age-related changes evident when auditory and visual speech cues are clear and congruent (Ballingham and Cienkowski, 2004; Sommers et al., 2005; Spehar et al., 2008; Gordon and Allen, 2009; Legault et al., 2010; Tye-Murray et al., 2010; Winneke and Phillips, 2011; Huyse et al., 2014; Smayda et al., 2016; Sommers and Phelps, 2016). However, audiovisual gains are reduced in older adults when auditory and visual speech cues are degraded (Tye-Murray et al., 2008, 2010, 2011; Gordon and Allen, 2009; Huyse et al., 2014; Stevenson et al., 2015) or asynchronous (Gordon-Salant et al., 2017).
In the McGurk effect, integration of incongruent auditory and visual speech results in a fused percept that matches neither cue (McGurk and Macdonald, 1976). Most studies report a similar proportion of fused responses in younger and older adult groups for syllables (Cienkowski and Carney, 2002; Ballingham and Cienkowski, 2004; Huyse et al., 2014; Stothart and Kazanina, 2016). Discrepant findings reported in the literature likely reflect differences in study design, with an age-related increase in fused responses found when speech stimuli were words (Setti et al., 2013) and an age-related decrease in fused responses found when auditory syllables were presented in modulated noise (Huyse et al., 2014). Comparison between studies using different experimental stimuli is also complicated by known variability in the McGurk effect with different speakers (Jiang and Bernstein, 2011; Mallick et al., 2015) and different auditory and visual syllable pairings (Jiang and Bernstein, 2011).
Altogether, research indicates an age-related increase in the integration of temporally offset auditory and visual events. Older adults perceive the sound-induced flash illusion at larger temporal offsets between auditory and visual stimuli than younger adults (Setti et al., 2011a; McGovern et al., 2014). When judging synchrony, older adults perceive temporally offset auditory and visual events as simultaneous over a broader (Hay-McCutcheon et al., 2009; Noel et al., 2016), narrower (Alm and Behne, 2013) or similar (Baskent and Bazo, 2011; Bedard and Barnett-Cowan, 2016) temporal window compared to younger adults. However, the temporal window of perceived synchrony is broader in older adults when individual differences in stimulus detectability and response criterion are normalized, indicative of enhanced integration (Chan et al., 2014). Similarly, in judgments of temporal order, older adults need a larger temporal gap between an auditory and visual stimulus to distinguish which appeared first (Virsu et al., 2003; Setti et al., 2011b; de Boer-Schellekens and Vroomen, 2014; Bedard and Barnett-Cowan, 2016), but one study found no age-related differences in audiovisual temporal order judgments (Fiacconi et al., 2013). All of these temporal order studies used stimuli that were not individually scaled to detectability, and Fiacconi et al tested observers of at least 70 years of age, which is older than the inclusion criteria of the other above cited studies (at least 60 years).
In summary, integration of temporal auditory and visual information may be increased, decreased or unchanged by physiological aging (see Tables 1, 2). This suggests that age-related effects on integration may be task-specific, as discussed in the next section.
Table 1.
Age-related changes in the perception of audiovisual temporal information consistent with decreased, unchanged, or increased integration in older relative to younger adults.
See text for discussion of how closely changes in perceptual measures reflect the underlying ability to integrate.
Table 2.
Age-related changes in the temporal binding window of auditory and visual events, indicating whether older adults were found to have narrower, unchanged, or wider temporal binding windows relative to younger adults.
Audiovisual task | Age-related changes in the temporal binding window: | ||
---|---|---|---|
Narrower | Unchanged | Wider | |
Temporal order judgments | – | Fiacconi et al., 2013 | Virsu et al., 2003; Setti et al., 2011b; de Boer-Schellekens and Vroomen, 2014; Bedard and Barnett-Cowan, 2016 |
Synchrony judgments | Alm and Behne, 2013 | Baskent and Bazo, 2011; Bedard and Barnett-Cowan, 2016 | Hay-McCutcheon et al., 2009; Chan et al., 2014; Noel et al., 2016 |
Task-Specific Integration Processes
An outstanding question is how closely audiovisual integration of temporal information is linked across different tasks. On the one hand, performance across different temporal audiovisual tasks is often correlated within individuals (Tremblay et al., 2007; Stevenson et al., 2012; Stevenson and Wallace, 2013), suggesting that different tasks can index common processes. Accordingly, older adults exhibit both increased susceptibility to the sound induced flash illusion (Setti et al., 2011a; DeLoss et al., 2013; McGovern et al., 2014) and broader temporal binding windows (Chan et al., 2014; Noel et al., 2016), consistent with the correlation between performance on these tasks in younger adults (Stevenson et al., 2012). However, within-subject correlations in younger adults have a limited potential to connect age-related effects on integration across different tasks. Correlations between tasks may reflect unisensory reliability, rather than a shared tendency to integrate, as both factors contribute to observed audiovisual interactions (Odegaard and Shams, 2016). Preliminary evidence suggests that the brain’s tendency to integrate auditory and visual cues is task-specific, though it remains unclear how tightly integration tendency is linked across tasks within the same perceptual domain (Odegaard and Shams, 2016). Furthermore, while low level stimulus characteristics such as temporal correspondence and unisensory reliability modulate the strength of audiovisual interactions, the integration process is also influenced by contextually-driven factors such as task goal, attention, and learned audiovisual associations (for review see van Atteveldt et al., 2014; Tye-Murray et al., 2016).
Different tasks likely index different perceptual processes. For example, research suggests distinctions between the perception of audiovisual temporal rate and relative timing (Fujisaki and Nishida, 2005). Moreover, a complex task like audiovisual speech perception is influenced by higher level factors, such as semantic congruence and linguistic meaning, not just lower level stimulus characteristics like temporal structure (Eskelund et al., 2011; Lee and Noppeney, 2011; Stekelenburg and Vroomen, 2012; ten Oever et al., 2013; Stevenson et al., 2014). In older adults, audiovisual gains in speech recognition decrease with task complexity, such as lexical difficulty (Dey and Sommers, 2015) or whole-word compared to phoneme recognition (Stevenson et al., 2015).
Additionally, audiovisual temporal tasks may not index the same perceptual processes even with comparable stimuli, such as audiovisual speech detection and recognition(Eskelund et al., 2011) and audiovisual temporal order and synchrony judgments (Love et al., 2013). In fact, estimates of the temporal binding window from temporal order and synchrony tasks are not correlated in older adults (Bedard and Barnett-Cowan, 2016). The McGurk effect is experienced at temporal offsets between auditory and visual syllables where observers are aware of the asynchrony (Soto-Faraco and Alsius, 2009), in line with differences in the neural substrate processing asynchrony and perceptual fusion for audiovisual speech (Stevenson et al., 2011). Furthermore, evidence points toward distinct cortical mechanisms of illusory flash fusion and fission (Mishra et al., 2007, 2008) that may be differentially vulnerable to age-related effects given differences in older adult susceptibility to each illusion type (McGovern et al., 2014). Likewise, older adults exhibit impaired integration of identical but not conflicting auditory and visual temporal rates, suggesting the presence of separate integration mechanisms that differ in their susceptibility to age-related decline (Brooks et al., 2015).
Lastly, there are concerns regarding the adequacy of some study measures as indices of integration. Though some argue that decisional processes drive the audiovisual bounce effect (Grove et al., 2016; Zeljko and Grove, 2016), most studies indicate that the effect is perceptual (Watanabe and Shimojo, 2001; Sanabria et al., 2004; Dufour et al., 2008; Meyerhoff and Scholl, 2018). Multiple studies suggest that the McGurk effect is an unreliable measure of integration, due to substantial individual variability (Mallick et al., 2015), underestimation of visual modulation of the auditory cue (Brancazio and Miller, 2005; Tiippana, 2014) and the contribution of individual differences in sensitivity to incongruity and lipreading skill to illusion susceptibility (Strand et al., 2014). As for audiovisual gains in speech recognition, recent analysis argues improved performance does not result from integration and that different integration measures indicate opposing age-related effects (Tye-Murray et al., 2016). Greater understanding of the relationships between audiovisual tasks and how closely perceptual changes reflect integration is needed to clarify current research.
The Role of Age-Related Changes in Unisensory Processing
Age-related unisensory changes are a potential confounding factor in the interpretation of age-related changes in audiovisual perception. According to the principle of inverse effectiveness, if auditory and visual cues are less salient, there will be a greater proportionate change in neural responses upon their integration (Meredith and Stein, 1983). Since this principle can also apply to perception (Stein et al., 1988), apparently enhanced integration in older age may reflect reduced stimulus saliency due to age-related sensory loss (Mozolic et al., 2012; Freiherr et al., 2013). However, a relationship between sensory reliability and integration does not necessarily mean that age-related sensory loss will explain age-related differences. While lower stimulus intensities broaden temporal binding windows in younger adults (Krueger Fister et al., 2016), older adults demonstrated broader temporal binding windows than younger adults when stimulus intensity was scaled to individual detection thresholds, indicating the effect was independent of age-related declines in sensory sensitivity (Chan et al., 2014). Furthermore, counter to the predictions of inverse effectiveness, there is an age-related increase in audiovisual enhancement of word recognition at intermediate auditory signal to noise ratios (Stevenson et al., 2015) and an age-related decrease in audiovisual enhancement of degraded sentences (Tye-Murray et al., 2010). This is consistent with evidence that audiovisual gains in speech recognition are greatest for intermediate auditory clarity (Ross et al., 2007; cf. Stevenson et al., 2015).
The reliability of auditory relative to visual sensory estimates of a property (where reliability is defined as the estimate’s precision, as given by the inverse of its variance) also influences audiovisual interactions (Ernst and Bülthoff, 2004). In contrast to the historical view that audition dominates temporal perception due to its superior temporal resolution (Welch and Warren, 1980), current theory holds that the brain weights a pair of sensory cues according to their relative reliability to derive the most precise multisensory representation possible from available information (Ernst and Bülthoff, 2004; Witten and Knudsen, 2005; Fetsch et al., 2013). Unisensory reliability plays a role in susceptibility to audiovisual illusions, in which information from one sensory modality modulates perception in another (e.g., Sekiyama and Tohkura, 1991; Andersen et al., 2005; Roach et al., 2006; Kumpik et al., 2014; Strand et al., 2014). Consequently, uneven age-related sensory decline could alter the relative contribution of audition and vision to the audiovisual percept, resulting in an apparent change in illusion susceptibility. Age-related shifts in the weighting of auditory and visual cues have been documented for audiovisual speech. When studies employed audiovisual speech cues that gave rise to age-related differences in unisensory performance, older adults gave more weight than younger adults to auditory (Huyse et al., 2014) or visual information (Cienkowski and Carney, 2002; Sekiyama et al., 2014; Festa et al., 2017), in accordance with the more reliable sensory modality. However, it is possible to probe age-related differences in integration capacity by individually balancing the relative reliability of auditory and visual cues, as was shown for temporal rate perception (Brooks et al., 2015).
Unfortunately, studies of audiovisual integration in older adults have not employed a consistent definition of what constitutes normal hearing and vision, and may not screen both sensory modalities for age-abnormal changes. Even so, screening measures commonly used in older adults such as audiometric thresholds and visual acuity are poor indicators of other aspects of auditory (e.g., Gordon-Salant and Fitzgibbons, 1999; Plack et al., 2014) or visual perception (e.g., Haegerstrom-Portnoy et al., 1999), respectively. The issue is further complicated by the often indistinct boundary between age-related and pathological changes (Owsley, 2011). Older age increases the prevalence of ocular diseases such as glaucoma (Quigley and Broman, 2006) and age-related macular degeneration (Friedman et al., 2004) that can further impair temporal perception even in early stages (e.g., Ansari et al., 2002; Phipps et al., 2004; Spry et al., 2005; Dimitrov et al., 2011; Gin et al., 2011). Age-related sensorineural hearing loss contributes to impaired auditory temporal perception independently from physiological aging (Gallun et al., 2014) and for stimuli at low frequencies despite normal audiometric thresholds (Feng et al., 2010). Age-related high frequency hearing loss also increases the intrinsic functional connectivity between auditory and visual cortical regions, which may increase audiovisual interactions in older adults (Puschmann and Thiel, 2017). Before drawing conclusions on how integration may change with age, studies should carefully account for age-related changes in both auditory and visual saliency on an individual basis (e.g., Chan et al., 2014; Brooks et al., 2015).
Conclusion
Audiovisual integration may not necessarily provide older adults with an effective compensatory mechanism for age-related unisensory decline in temporal information. While older adults often benefit from the provision of complimentary auditory and visual cues, it can be to a reduced extent compared to younger adults. Older adults sometimes demonstrate an increased tendency to synthesize conflicting temporal auditory and visual cues into a unified percept but this is not a universal finding. Further research elucidating how closely integration is linked across different audiovisual tasks is needed to clarify this pattern of results. Currently, it remains unclear to what extent age-related changes in audiovisual temporal perception reflect true changes in integration rather than sequential effects of age-related sensory loss. Future research should account for individual differences in unisensory reliability to distinguish age-related sensory loss from age-related effects on integration.
Author Contributions
CB: conception and planning of review and wrote the manuscript. YC: planning of review and contributed to the writing of Section “Age-Related Changes in Audiovisual Temporal Perception.” AM and AA: conception of review and revision of the manuscript. All authors approved the manuscript.
Conflict of Interest Statement
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Footnotes
Funding. This study was supported by Australian Research Council (FT0990930 to AM).
References
- Alm M., Behne D. (2013). Audio-visual speech experience with age influences perceived audio-visual asynchrony in speech. 134 3001–3010. 10.1121/1.4820798 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Andersen T. S., Tiippana K., Sams M. (2004). Factors influencing audiovisual fission and fusion illusions. 21 301–308. 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2004.06.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Andersen T. S., Tiippana K., Sams M. (2005). Maximum likelihood integration of rapid flashes and beeps. 380 155–160. 10.1016/j.neulet.2005.01.030 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ansari E. A., Morgan J. E., Snowden R. J. (2002). Psychophysical characterisation of early functional loss in glaucoma and ocular hypertension. 86 1131–1135. 10.1136/bjo.86.10.1131 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Babkoff H., Fostick L. (2017). Age-related changes in auditory processing and speech perception: cross-sectional and longitudinal analyses. 14 269–281. 10.1007/s10433-017-0410-y [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ballingham T., Cienkowski K. M. (2004). Visual enhancement in consonant identification by younger and older adults. 37 11–21. [Google Scholar]
- Baskent D., Bazo D. (2011). Audiovisual asynchrony detection and speech intelligibility in noise with moderate to severe sensorineural hearing impairment. 32 582–592. 10.1097/AUD.0b013e31820fca23 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bedard G., Barnett-Cowan M. (2016). Impaired timing of audiovisual events in the elderly. 234 331–340. 10.1007/s00221-015-4466-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bennett P. J., Sekuler R., Sekuler A. B. (2007). The effects of aging on motion detection and direction identification. 47 799–809. 10.1016/j.visres.2007.01.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Billino J., Bremmer F., Gegenfurtner K. R. (2008). Differential aging of motion processing mechanisms: evidence against general perceptual decline. 48 1254–1261. 10.1016/j.visres.2008.02.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brancazio L., Miller J. L. (2005). Use of visual information in speech perception: evidence for a visual rate effect both with and without a McGurk effect. 67 759–769. 10.3758/BF03193531 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brooks C. J., Anderson A. J., Roach N. W., McGraw P. V., McKendrick A. M. (2015). Age-related changes in auditory and visual interactions in temporal rate perception. 15:2. 10.1167/15.16.2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Busey T., Craig J., Clark C., Humes L. (2010). Age-related changes in visual temporal order judgment performance: relation to sensory and cognitive capacities. 50 1628–1640. 10.1016/j.visres.2010.05.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Čeponienė R., Westerfield M., Torki M., Townsend J. (2008). Modality-specificity of sensory aging in vision and audition: evidence from event-related potentials. 1215 53–68. 10.1016/j.brainres.2008.02.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chan Y. M., Pianta M. J., McKendrick A. M. (2014). Older age results in difficulties separating auditory and visual signals in time. 14:13. 10.1167/14.11.13 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chandrasekaran C., Trubanova A., Stillittano S., Caplier A., Ghazanfar A. A. (2009). The natural statistics of audiovisual speech. 5:e1000436. 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000436 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cienkowski K. M., Carney A. E. (2002). Auditory-visual speech perception and aging. 23 439–449. 10.1097/01.aud.0000034781.95122.15 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cliff M., Joyce D. W., Lamar M., Dannhauser T., Tracy D. K., Shergill S. S. (2013). Aging effects on functional auditory and visual processing using fMRI with variable sensory loading. 49 1304–1313. 10.1016/j.cortex.2012.04.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Conlon E., Herkes K. (2008). Spatial and temporal processing in healthy aging: implications for perceptions of driving skills. 15 446–470. 10.1080/13825580701878008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Coppinger N. W. (1955). The relationship between critical flicker frequency and chronologic age for varying levels of stimulus brightness. 10 48–52. 10.1093/geronj/10.1.48 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Boer-Schellekens L., Vroomen J. (2014). Multisensory integration compensates loss of sensitivity of visual temporal order in the elderly. 232 253–262. 10.1007/s00221-013-3736-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Dieuleveult A. L., Siemonsma P. C., van Erp J. B. F., Brouwer A.-M. (2017). Effects of aging in multisensory integration: a systematic review. 9:80 10.3389/fnagi.2017.00080 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- DeLoss D. J., Pierce R. S., Andersen G. J. (2013). Multisensory integration, aging, and the sound-induced flash illusion. 28 802–812. 10.1037/a0033289 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Denison R. N., Driver J., Ruff C. C. (2013). Temporal structure and complexity affect audio-visual correspondence detection. 3:619. 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00619 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dey A., Sommers M. S. (2015). Age-related differences in inhibitory control predict audiovisual speech perception. 30 634–646. 10.1037/pag0000033 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Diaconescu A., Diaconescu L., Hasher A., McIntosh A. R. (2013). Visual dominance and multisensory integration changes with age. 65 152–166. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.09.057 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dimitrov P. N., Robman L. D., Varsamidis M., Aung K. Z., Makeyeva G. A., Guymer R. H., et al. (2011). Visual function tests as potential biomarkers in age-related macular degeneration. 52 9457–9469. 10.1167/iovs.10-7043 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dufour A., Touzalin P., Moessinger M., Brochard R., Després O. (2008). Visual motion disambiguation by a subliminal sound. 17 790–797. 10.1016/j.concog.2007.09.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ernst M. O., Bülthoff H. H. (2004). Merging the senses into a robust percept. 8 162–169. 10.1016/j.tics.2004.02.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eskelund K., Tuomainen J., Andersen T. S. (2011). Multistage audiovisual integration of speech: dissociating identification and detection. 208 447–457. 10.1007/s00221-010-2495-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Feng Y., Yin S., Kiefte M., Wang J. (2010). Temporal resolution in regions of normal hearing and speech perception in noise for adults with sloping high-frequency hearing loss. 31 115–125. 10.1097/AUD.0b013e3181bb69be [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Festa E. K., Katz A. P., Ott B. R., Tremont G., Heindel W. C. (2017). Dissociable effects of aging and mild cognitive impairment on bottom-up audiovisual integration. 59 155–167. 10.3233/JAD-161062 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fetsch C. R., DeAngelis G. C., Angelaki D. E. (2013). Bridging the gap between theories of sensory cue integration and the physiology of multisensory neurons. 14 429–442. 10.1038/nrn3503 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fiacconi C. M., Harvey E. C., Sekuler A. B., Bennett P. J. (2013). The influence of aging on audiovisual temporal order judgments. 39 179–193. 10.1080/0361073X.2013.761896 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fitzgibbons P. J., Gordon-Salant S. (1994). Age effects on measures of auditory duration discrimination. 37 662–670. 10.1044/jshr.3703.662 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fitzgibbons P. J., Gordon-Salant S. (1995). Age effects on duration discrimination with simple and complex stimuli. 98 3140–3145. 10.1121/1.413803 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fitzgibbons P. J., Gordon-Salant S., Friedman S. A. (2006). Effects of age and sequence presentation rate on temporal order recognition. 120 991–999. 10.1121/1.2214463 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Freiherr J., Lundström J. N., Habel U., Reetz K. (2013). Multisensory integration mechanisms during aging. 7:863. 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00863 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Friedman D. S., Wolfs R. C., O’Colmain B. J., Klein B. E., Taylor H. R., West S., et al. (2004). Prevalence of age-related macular degeneration in the United States. 122 564–572. 10.1001/archopht.122.4.564 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fujisaki W., Nishida S. (2005). Temporal frequency characteristics of synchrony–asynchrony discrimination of audio-visual signals. 166 455–464. 10.1007/s00221-005-2385-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Füllgrabe C. (2013). Age-dependent changes in temporal-fine-structure processing in the absence of peripheral hearing loss. 22 313–315. 10.1044/1059-0889(2013/12-0070 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Füllgrabe C., Moore B. C. J., Stone M. A. (2014). Age-group differences in speech identification despite matched audiometrically normal hearing: contributions from auditory temporal processing and cognition. 6:347. 10.3389/fnagi.2014.00347 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gallun F. J., McMillan G. P., Molis M. R., Kampel S. D., Dann S. M., Konrad-Martin D. L. (2014). Relating age and hearing loss to monaural, bilateral, and binaural temporal sensitivity. 8:172. 10.3389/fnins.2014.00172 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gin T. J., Luu C. D., Guymer R. H. (2011). Central retinal function as measured by the multifocal electroretinogram and flicker perimetry in early age-related macular degeneration. 52 9267–9274. 10.1167/iovs.11-8517 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gordon M. S., Allen S. (2009). Audiovisual speech in older and younger adults: integrating a distorted visual signal with speech in noise. 35 202–219. 10.1080/03610730902720398 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gordon-Salant S., Fitzgibbons P. J. (1993). Temporal factors and speech recognition performance in young and elderly listeners. 36 1276–1285. 10.1044/jshr.3606.1276 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gordon-Salant S., Fitzgibbons P. J. (1999). Profile of auditory temporal processing in older listeners. 42 300–311. 10.1044/jslhr.4202.300 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gordon-Salant S., Yeni-Komshian G. H., Fitzgibbons P. J., Willison H. M., Freund M. S. (2017). Recognition of asynchronous auditory-visual speech by younger and older listeners: a preliminary study. 142 151–159. 10.1121/1.4992026 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grant K. W., Seitz P.-F. (2000). The use of visible speech cues for improving auditory detection of spoken sentences. 108 1197–1208. 10.1121/1.1288668 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grose J. H., Mamo S. K. (2010). Processing of temporal fine structure as a function of age. 31 755–760. 10.1097/AUD.0b013e3181e627e7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grose J. H., Mamo S. K. (2012). Frequency modulation detection as a measure of temporal processing: age-related monaural and binaural effects. 294 49–54. 10.1016/j.heares.2012.09.007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grove P. M., Robertson C., Harris L. R. (2016). Disambiguating the stream/bounce illusion with inference. 29 453–464. 10.1163/22134808-00002524 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guerreiro M. J. S., Murphy D. R., Van Gerven P. W. M. (2013). Making sense of age-related distractibility: the critical role of sensory modality. 142 184–194. 10.1016/j.actpsy.2012.11.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guerreiro M. J. S., Van Gerven P. W. M. (2011). Now you see it, now you don’t: evidence for age-dependent and age-independent cross-modal distraction. 26 415–426. 10.1037/a0021507 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Habak C., Faubert J. (2000). Larger effect of aging on the perception of higher-order stimuli. 40 943–950. 10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00235-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haegerstrom-Portnoy G., Schneck M. E., Brabyn J. A. (1999). Seeing into old age: vision function beyond acuity. 76 141–158. 10.1097/00006324-199903000-00014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hay-McCutcheon M. J., Pisoni D. B., Hunt K. K. (2009). Audiovisual asynchrony detection and speech perception in hearing-impaired listeners with cochlear implants: a preliminary analysis. 48 321–333. 10.1080/14992020802644871 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- He N. J., Mills J. H., Ahlstrom J. B., Dubno J. R. (2008). Age-related differences in the temporal modulation transfer function with pure-tone carriers. 124 3841–3849. 10.1121/1.2998779 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- He N. J., Mills J. H., Dubno J. R. (2007). Frequency modulation detection: effects of age, psychophysical method, and modulation waveform. 122 467–477. 10.1121/1.2741208 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heinrich A., Schneider B. (2006). Age-related changes in within- and between-channel gap detection using sinusoidal stimuli. 119 2316–2326. 10.1121/1.2173524 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Humes L. E., Busey T. A., Craig J. C., Kewley-Port D. (2009). The effects of age on sensory thresholds and temporal gap detection in hearing, vision, and touch. 71 860–871. 10.3758/APP.71.4.860 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huyse A., Leybaert J., Berthommier F. (2014). Effects of aging on audio-visual speech integration. 136 1918–1931. 10.1121/1.4894685 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jaekl P., Pesquita A., Alsius A., Munhall K., Soto-Faraco S. (2015). The contribution of dynamic visual cues to audiovisual speech perception. 75 402–410. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.06.025 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jiang J., Bernstein L. E. (2011). Psychophysics of the McGurk and other audiovisual speech integration effects. 37 1193–1209. 10.1037/a0023100 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kim C. B., Mayer M. J. (1994). Foveal flicker sensitivity in healthy aging eyes. II. Cross-sectional aging trends from 18 through 77 years of age. 11 1958–1969. 10.1364/JOSAA.11.001958 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koene A., Arnold D., Johnston A. (2007). Bimodal sensory discrimination is finer than dual single modality discrimination. 7:14. 10.1167/7.11.14 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krueger Fister J., Stevenson R. A., Nidiffer A. R., Barnett Z. P., Wallace M. T. (2016). Stimulus intensity modulates multisensory temporal processing. 88 92–100. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.02.016 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kumar A. U., Sangamanatha A. V. (2011). Temporal processing abilities across different age groups. 22 5–12. 10.3766/jaaa.22.1.2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kumpik D. P., Roberts H. E., King A. J., Bizley J. K. (2014). Visual sensitivity is a stronger determinant of illusory processes than auditory cue parameters in the sound-induced flash illusion. 14:12. 10.1167/14.7.12 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kuyk T. K., Wesson M. D. (1991). Aging-related foveal flicker sensitivity losses in normal observers. 68 786–789. 10.1097/00006324-199110000-00005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lachenmayr B. J., Kojetinsky S., Ostermaier N., Angstwurm K., Vivell P. M., Schaumberger M. (1994). The different effects of aging on normal sensitivity in flicker and light-sense perimetry. 35 2741–2748. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lacherez P., Turner L., Lester R., Burns Z., Wood J. M. (2014). Age-related changes in perception of movement in driving scenes. 34 445–451. 10.1111/opo.12140 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee H., Noppeney U. (2011). Physical and perceptual factors shape the neural mechanisms that integrate audiovisual signals in speech comprehension. 31 11338–11350. 10.1523/jneurosci.6510-10.2011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Legault I., Gagné J. P., Rhoualem W., Anderson-Gosselin P. (2010). The effects of blurred vision on auditory-visual speech perception in younger and older adults. 49 904–911. 10.3109/14992027.2010.509112 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lewald J., Guski R. (2003). Cross-modal perceptual integration of spatially and temporally disparate auditory and visual stimuli. 16 468–478. 10.1016/S0926-6410(03)00074-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Love S. A., Petrini K., Cheng A., Pollick F. E. (2013). A psychophysical investigation of differences between synchrony and temporal order judgments. 8:e54798. 10.1371/journal.pone.0054798 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lustig C., Meck W. H. (2011). Modality differences in timing and temporal memory throughout the lifespan. 77 298–303. 10.1016/j.bandc.2011.07.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mallick D. B., Magnotti J. F., Beauchamp M. S. (2015). Variability and stability in the McGurk effect: contributions of participants, stimuli, time, and response type. 22 1299–1307. 10.3758/s13423-015-0817-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mayer M. J., Glucs A., Kim C. B. Y., Svingos A. (1988). Foveal flicker sensitivity in healthy aging eyes. I. Compensating for pupil variation. 5 2201–2209. 10.1364/JOSAA.5.002201 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McFarland R. A., Warren A. B., Karis C. (1958). Alterations in critical flicker frequency as a function of age and light: dark ratio. 56 529–538. 10.1037/h0049128 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McGovern D. P., Roudaia E., Stapleton J., McGinnity T. M., Newell F. N. (2014). The sound-induced flash illusion reveals dissociable age-related effects in multisensory integration. 6:250. 10.3389/fnagi.2014.00250 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McGurk H., Macdonald J. (1976). Hearing lips and seeing voices. 264 746–748 10.1038/264746a0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meredith M., Stein B. (1983). Interactions among converging sensory inputs in the superior colliculus. 221 389–391. 10.1126/science.6867718 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meyerhoff H. S., Scholl B. J. (2018). Auditory-induced bouncing is a perceptual (rather than a cognitive) phenomenon: evidence from illusory crescents. 170 88–94. 10.1016/j.cognition.2017.08.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mishra J., Martinez A., Hillyard S. A. (2008). Cortical processes underlying sound-induced flash fusion. 1242 102–115. 10.1016/j.brainres.2008.05.023 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mishra J., Martinez A., Sejnowski T. J., Hillyard S. A. (2007). Early cross-modal interactions in auditory and visual cortex underlie a sound-induced visual illusion. 27 4120–4131. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4912-06.2007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Misiak H. (1947). Age and sex differences in critical flicker frequency. 37 318–332. 10.1037/h0061531 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Misiak H. (1951). The decrease of critical flicker frequency with age. 113 551–552. 10.1126/science.113.2941.551 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moore B. C. J., Vickers D. A., Mehta A. (2012). The effects of age on temporal fine structure sensitivity in monaural and binaural conditions. 51 715–721. 10.3109/14992027.2012.690079 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morein-Zamir S., Soto-Faraco S., Kingstone A. (2003). Auditory capture of vision: examining temporal ventriloquism. 17 154–163. 10.1016/S0926-6410(03)00089-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mozolic J. L., Hugenschmidt C. E., Peiffer A. M., Laurienti P. J. (2012). “Integration in aging,” in , eds Murray M. M., Wallace M. T. (Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press; ), 381–394. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Noel J.-P., De Niear M., Van der Burg E., Wallace M. T. (2016). Audiovisual simultaneity judgment and rapid recalibration throughout the lifespan. 11:e0161698. 10.1371/journal.pone.0161698 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Odegaard B., Shams L. (2016). The brain’s tendency to bind audiovisual signals is stable but not general. 27 583–591. 10.1177/0956797616628860 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Owsley C. (2011). Aging and vision. 51 1610–1622. 10.1016/j.visres.2010.10.020 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Parise C. V., Spence C., Ernst M. O. (2012). When correlation implies causation in multisensory integration. 22 46–49. 10.1016/j.cub.2011.11.039 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Phipps J. A., Dang T. M., Vingrys A. J., Guymer R. H. (2004). Flicker perimetry losses in age-related macular degeneration. 45 3355–3360. 10.1167/iovs.04-0253 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Plack C. J., Barker D., Prendergast G. (2014). Perceptual consequences of “hidden” hearing loss. 18:2331216514550621. 10.1177/2331216514550621 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Puschmann S., Thiel C. M. (2017). Changed crossmodal functional connectivity in older adults with hearing loss. 86 109–122. 10.1016/j.cortex.2016.10.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Quigley H. A., Broman A. T. (2006). The number of people with glaucoma worldwide in 2010 and 2020. 90 262–267. 10.1136/bjo.2005.081224 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ramkhalawansingh R., Keshavarz B., Haycock B., Shahab S., Campos J. L. (2016). Age differences in visual-auditory self-motion perception during a simulated driving task. 7:595. 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00595 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roach N. W., Heron J., McGraw P. V. (2006). Resolving multisensory conflict: a strategy for balancing the costs and benefits of audio-visual integration. 273 2159–2168. 10.1098/rspb.2006.3578 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ross L. A., Saint-Amour D., Leavitt V. M., Javitt D. C., Foxe J. J. (2007). Do you see what I am saying? Exploring visual enhancement of speech comprehension in noisy environments. 17 1147–1153. 10.1093/cercor/bhl024 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roudaia E., Sekuler A. B., Bennett P. J., Sekuler R. (2013). Aging and audio-visual and multi-cue integration in motion. 4:267. 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00267 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sanabria D., Correa Á., Lupiáñez J., Spence C. (2004). Bouncing or streaming? Exploring the influence of auditory cues on the interpretation of ambiguous visual motion. 157 537–541. 10.1007/s00221-004-1993-z [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sekiyama K., Soshi T., Sakamoto S. (2014). Enhanced audiovisual integration with aging in speech perception: a heightened McGurk effect in older adults. 5:323. 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00323 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sekiyama K., Tohkura Y. I. (1991). McGurk effect in non-English listeners: few visual effects for Japanese subjects hearing Japanese syllables of high auditory intelligibility. 90 1797–1805. 10.1121/1.401660 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sekuler A. B., Sekuler R. (1999). Collisions between moving visual targets: what controls alternative ways of seeing an ambiguous display? 28 415–432. 10.1068/p2909 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sekuler R., Sekuler A. B., Lau R. (1997). Sound alters visual motion perception. 385:308. 10.1038/385308a0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Setti A., Burke K. E., Kenny R., Newell F. N. (2013). Susceptibility to a multisensory speech illusion in older persons is driven by perceptual processes. 4:575. 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00575 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Setti A., Burke K. E., Kenny R. A., Newell F. N. (2011a). Is inefficient multisensory processing associated with falls in older people? 209 375–384. 10.1007/s00221-011-2560-z [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Setti A., Finnigan S., Sobolewski R., McLaren L., Robertson I. H., Reilly R. B., et al. (2011b). Audiovisual temporal discrimination is less efficient with aging: an event-related potential study. 22 554–558. 10.1097/WNR.0b013e328348c731 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shams L., Kamitani Y., Shimojo S. (2000). Illusions: what you see is what you hear. 408 788–788. 10.1038/35048669 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smayda K. E., Van Engen K. J., Maddox W. T., Chandrasekaran B. (2016). Audio-visual and meaningful semantic context enhancements in older and younger adults. 11:e0152773. 10.1371/journal.pone.0152773 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Snell K. B. (1997). Age-related changes in temporal gap detection. 101 2214–2220. 10.1121/1.418205 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Snowden R. J., Kavanagh E. (2006). Motion perception in the ageing visual system: minimum motion, motion coherence, and speed discrimination thresholds. 35 9–24. 10.1068/p5399 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sommers M. S., Phelps D. (2016). Listening effort in younger and older adults: a comparison of auditory-only and auditory-visual presentations. 37 62S–68S. 10.1097/aud.0000000000000322 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sommers M. S., Tye-Murray N., Spehar B. (2005). Auditory-visual speech perception and auditory-visual enhancement in normal-hearing younger and older adults. 26 263–275. 10.1097/00003446-200506000-00003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Soto-Faraco S., Alsius A. (2009). Deconstructing the McGurk-MacDonald illusion. 35 580–587. 10.1037/a0013483 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spehar B. P., Tye-Murray N., Sommers M. S. (2008). Intra-versus intermodal integration in young and older adults. 123 2858–2866. 10.1121/1.2890748 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spry P. G. D., Johnson C. A., Mansberger S. L., Cioffi G. A. (2005). Psychophysical investigation of ganglion cell loss in early glaucoma. 14 11–19. 10.1097/01.ijg.0000145813.46848.b8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stein B., Stanford T., Ramachandran R., Perrault T., Jr., Rowland B. (2009). Challenges in quantifying multisensory integration: alternative criteria, models, and inverse effectiveness. 198 113–126. 10.1007/s00221-009-1880-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stein B. E., Scott Huneycutt W., Alex Meredith M. (1988). Neurons and behavior: the same rules of multisensory integration apply. 448 355–358. 10.1016/0006-8993(88)91276-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stekelenburg J. J., Vroomen J. (2012). Electrophysiological evidence for a multisensory speech-specific mode of perception. 50 1425–1431. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.02.027 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stevenson R. A., Nelms C. E., Baum S. H., Zurkovsky L., Barense M. D., Newhouse P. A., et al. (2015). Deficits in audiovisual speech perception in normal aging emerge at the level of whole-word recognition. 36 283–291. 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2014.08.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stevenson R. A., VanDerKlok R. M., Pisoni D. B., James T. W. (2011). Discrete neural substrates underlie complementary audiovisual speech integration processes. 55 1339–1345. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.12.063 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stevenson R. A., Wallace M. T. (2013). Multisensory temporal integration: task and stimulus dependencies. 227 249–261. 10.1007/s00221-013-3507-3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stevenson R. A., Wallace M. T., Altieri N. (2014). The interaction between stimulus factors and cognitive factors during multisensory integration of audiovisual speech. 5:352. 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00352 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stevenson R. A., Zemtsov R. K., Wallace M. T. (2012). Individual differences in the multisensory temporal binding window predict susceptibility to audiovisual illusions. 38 1517–1529. 10.1037/a0027339 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stothart G., Kazanina N. (2016). Auditory perception in the aging brain: the role of inhibition and facilitation in early processing. 47 23–34. 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2016.06.022 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Strand J., Cooperman A., Rowe J., Simenstad A. (2014). Individual differences in susceptibility to the McGurk effect: links with lipreading and detecting audiovisual incongruity. 57 2322–2331. 10.1044/2014_JSLHR-H-14-0059 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Strouse A., Ashmead D. H., Ohde R. N., Grantham D. W. (1998). Temporal processing in the aging auditory system. 104 2385–2399. 10.1121/1.423748 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Takahashi G. A., Bacon S. P. (1992). Modulation detection, modulation masking, and speech understanding in noise in the elderly. 35 1410–1421. 10.1044/jshr.3506.1410 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- ten Oever S., Sack A. T., Wheat K. L., Bien N., van Atteveldt N. (2013). Audio-visual onset differences are used to determine syllable identity for ambiguous audio-visual stimulus pairs. 4:331. 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00331 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tiippana K. (2014). What is the McGurk effect? 5:725. 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00725 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Trainor L. J., Trehub S. E. (1989). Aging and auditory temporal sequencing: ordering the elements of repeating tone patterns. 45 417–426. 10.3758/bf03210715 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tran D. B., Silverman S. E., Zimmerman K., Feldon S. E. (1998). Age-related deterioration of motion perception and detection. 236 269–273. 10.1007/s004170050076 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tremblay C., Champoux F., Bacon B. A., Théoret H. (2007). Evidence for a generic process underlying multisensory integration. 1 1–4. 10.2174/1874230000701010001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Trick G. L., Silverman S. E. (1991). Visual sensitivity to motion: age-related changes and deficits in senile dementia of the Alzheimer type. 41 1437–1440. 10.1212/WNL.41.9.1437 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tye-Murray N., Sommers M., Spehar B., Myerson J., Hale S. (2010). Aging, audiovisual integration, and the principle of inverse effectiveness. 31 636–644. 10.1097/AUD.0b013e3181ddf7ff [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tye-Murray N., Sommers M., Spehar B., Myerson J., Hale S., Rose N. S. (2008). Auditory-visual discourse comprehension by older and young adults in favorable and unfavorable conditions. 47 S31–S37. 10.1080/14992020802301662 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tye-Murray N., Spehar B., Myerson J., Hale S., Sommers M. (2016). Lipreading and audiovisual speech recognition across the adult lifespan: implications for audiovisual integration. 31 380–389. 10.1037/pag0000094 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tye-Murray N., Spehar B., Myerson J., Sommers M. S., Hale S. (2011). Cross-modal enhancement of speech detection in young and older adults: does signal content matter? 32 650–655. 10.1097/AUD.0b013e31821a4578 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tyler C. W. (1989). Two processes control variations in flicker sensitivity over the life span. 6 481–490. 10.1364/JOSAA.6.000481 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ulbrich P., Churan J., Fink M., Wittmann M. (2009). Perception of temporal order: the effects of age, sex, and cognitive factors. 16 183–202. 10.1080/13825580802411758 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van Atteveldt N., Murray M. M., Thut G., Schroeder C. E. (2014). Multisensory integration: flexible use of general operations. 81 1240–1253. 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.02.044 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Virsu V., Lahti-Nuuttila P., Laasonen M. (2003). Crossmodal temporal processing acuity impairment aggravates with age in developmental dyslexia. 336 151–154. 10.1016/s0304-3940(02)01253-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wallaert N., Moore B. C. J., Lorenzi C. (2016). Comparing the effects of age on amplitude modulation and frequency modulation detection. 139 3088–3096. 10.1121/1.4953019 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Watanabe K., Shimojo S. (2001). Postcoincidence trajectory duration affects motion event perception. 63 16–28. 10.3758/bf03200498 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Welch R. B., Warren D. H. (1980). Immediate perceptual response to intersensory discrepancy. 88 638–667. 10.1037/0033-2909.88.3.638 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Winneke A. H., Phillips N. A. (2011). Does audiovisual speech offer a fountain of youth for old ears? An event-related brain potential study of age differences in audiovisual speech perception. 26 427–438. 10.1037/a0021683 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Witten I. B., Knudsen E. I. (2005). Why seeing is believing: merging auditory and visual worlds. 48 489–496. 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.10.020 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wood J. M. (2002). Age and visual impairment decrease driving performance as measured on a closed-road circuit. 44 482–494. 10.1518/0018720024497664 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zeljko M., Grove P. M. (2016). Sensitivity and bias in the resolution of stream-bounce stimuli. 46 178–204. 10.1177/0301006616672548 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]