ABSTRACT
Stories are a powerful means to change people’s attitudes and beliefs. The aim of the current work was to shed light on the role of argument strength (argument quality) in narrative persuasion. The present study examined the influence of strong versus weak arguments on attitudes in a low or high narrative context. Moreover, baseline attitudes, interindividual differences in working memory capacity, and recipients’ transportation were examined. Stories with strong arguments were more persuasive than stories with weak arguments. This main effect was qualified by a two-way interaction with baseline attitude, revealing that argument strength had a greater impact on individuals who initially were particularly doubtful toward the story claim. Furthermore, we identified a three-way interaction showing that argument strength mattered most for recipients who were deeply transported into the story world in stories that followed a typical narrative structure. These findings provide an important specification of narrative persuasion theory.
Introduction
Since ancient times, religious leaders, politicians, and marketers have relied on stories to change people’s attitudes, beliefs, and behavior (Gottschall, 2012). Empirical research has repeatedly demonstrated the power of narratives to persuade recipients, even if the stories were introduced as fictional (e.g., Appel & Mara, 2013; Green & Brock, 2000; Prentice, Gerrig, & Bailis, 1997). Narratives often engage the recipient and transport him or her into the world of the story (transportation; Gerrig, 1993; Green & Brock, 2000). This state of transportation, rather than the elaboration of arguments (cf., Petty & Cacioppo, 1986), is considered to play a key role in narrative persuasion. In contrast to theory and research on non-narrative communication (Chaiken, 1980; Petty & Cacioppo, 1986), narrative transportation theory suggests that argument strength plays a minor role in narrative persuasion (e.g., Green & Brock, 2002; Slater, 2002). One of the possible causes put forward to explain the minor role of argument strength is that when transported into a story, recipients lack sufficient working memory resources to scrutinize arguments. To test this assumption, the aim of the present study was to examine the role of argument strength in narrative persuasion. Extending prior studies in the field, we manipulated the strength of the arguments included in the story as well as the narrativity of the story. We also measured baseline attitudes and the levels of self-reported narrative transportation. Moreover, we examined whether individual differences in recipients’ working memory capacity affected the role of argument strength in narrative persuasion.
Transportation into story worlds, narrativity, and argument strength
Stories or narratives (both terms are used interchangeably in this work) are defined as “the representation of an event or a series of events” (Abbott, 2002, p. 12). Stories entail the actions and experiences of one or more protagonists and a plot line with certain schematic elements (e.g., setting, event, attempt, reaction, and consequence; Rumelhart, 1975). In recent years, empirical research has demonstrated that fictional as well as nonfictional narratives can have a pervasive impact on attitudes and beliefs about real-world issues (narrative persuasion; e.g., Green & Brock, 2000; Prentice et al., 1997), on knowledge and memory (Fazio & Marsh, 2008; Marsh, Meade, & Roediger, 2003), and on social abilities and personality (Fong, Mullin, & Mar, 2013; Mar & Oatley, 2008). In some of these studies the stories as a whole or their main narrative arc suggested a particular stance toward a topic (e.g., a story about a psychiatric patient who murdered a child led recipients to have more negative beliefs about the group of psychiatric patients; Green & Brock, 2000). In other studies, the stories included assertions that were not a key element of the plot (e.g., Appel & Richter, 2007; Dahlstrom, 2010; 2012; Prentice et al., 1997). Prior research indicates that the persuasive influence of narratives can be quite durable, being strong even after two weeks (Appel & Richter, 2007).
The potency of stories to change a recipient’s worldview and his or her attitudes has been attributed to the situational state of being transported into the story world (transportation; Gerrig, 1993; Green & Brock, 2000; or narrative engagement; Busselle & Bilandzic, 2008, 2009). The term transportation is based on the metaphor that recipients undertake a mental journey when reading a book or watching a movie. After this journey, they return to real life somewhat changed by the story events. When highly transported, “all mental systems and capacities become focused on the events occurring in the narrative” (Green & Brock, 2000, p. 701). The resulting mental state has been conceptualized as a co-activation of attention, imagery, and emotion (e.g., Green, 2004; Green & Brock, 2000).
Whether or not and how deeply recipients are transported into a story world is a function of the story itself, the situation in which recipients encounter the story, and rather stable recipient dispositions (Dal Cin, Zanna, & Fong, 2004; Green, 2004; Green & Brock, 2002). Not all texts that qualify as narratives are equally likely to elicit transportation. Stories that are well written and well structured are more transporting (Green & Donahue, 2009). Transportation is encouraged by stories that are made with high craftsmanship and adhere to the narrative format (Green & Brock, 2002). Differences in these and related qualities and characteristics within the field of narrative have been subsumed under the concept of narrativity. Narrativity is a scalable feature, meaning that a text (defined broadly, including oral discourse and audiovisual media) can have a greater or lesser degree of narrativity (e.g., Fludernick, 2002). Well-written literary texts often include stylistic techniques such as metaphor or foregounding (cf. Miall & Kuiken, 1994), and literary texts were found to be more transporting than prose developed for the purpose of an experiment (Green & Brock, 2000). Likewise, disrupting the text structure and thereby reordering the events (while keeping the content intact) leads to lower transportation scores (e.g., Gnambs, Appel, Schreiner, Richter, & Isberner, 2014; Wang & Calder, 2006). Little is known, however, about the influence of argument strength on transportation. Although it is conceivable that poor arguments embedded in a story might disrupt transportation, a study by Gnambs et al. (2014) that compared stories with weak versus strong arguments did not find any evidence for such an effect of argument strength on transportation.
Several studies related rather stable individual difference measures to transportation, such as the need for affect (e.g., Appel & Richter, 2010; Appel, Gnambs, & Maio, 2012) or trait transportability (e.g., Dal Cin et al., 2004; Mazzocco, Green, Sasota, & Jones, 2010). Moreover, studies examined whether baseline attitudes closely related to the beliefs advocated in the story predicted transportation. The results are somewhat inconclusive. Whereas Dal Cin et al. (2004) observed no relationship between baseline attitudes (measured weeks before encountering the story) and transportation, Green (2004) showed that having gay friends in real life predicted greater transportation into a story with a gay protagonist.
In sum, the structure and the craftsmanship of a story as well as personality variables are well-known predictors of transportation. Evidence is mixed regarding the initial attitudes about the topics dealt with in a story, and little is known about whether argument strength affects transportation.
Persuasion through narratives and argument strength
Theory suggests that the more strongly recipients are transported, the more they are persuaded by the story (Green & Brock, 2002). Indeed, a number of experiments demonstrated that higher scores on the postexposure transportation scale (Green & Brock, 2000) were associated with a stronger persuasive impact of stories (e.g., Appel & Richter, 2010; Green, 2004; Green & Brock, 2000). In recent years researchers have become particularly interested in the mechanisms and boundary conditions that are responsible for the persuasiveness of stories and the increased attitude change that seems to be caused by deeper transportation into the story world. One family of explanations revolves around emotional processes and the empathy or identification with a character (e.g., De Graaf, Hoeken, Sanders, & Beentjes, 2012; Hoeken & Sinkeldam, 2014).
A second family of explanations revolves around the cognitive processes underlying narrative persuasion that are particularly relevant with respect to the influence of arguments within a story context: The allocation of attention to a text is a key component of transportation and a predictor of persuasive effects (Bezdek & Gerrig, 2016; Gerrig, 1993; Green & Brock, 2000, 2002). The more recipients are transported into the story world, the more their cognitive and emotional processing is focused on the story world and the less their processing is focused on the immediate surroundings (such as the room in which a story is read) or other information that is unconnected to the story (such as an upcoming sports event). As recently pointed out by Bezdek and Gerrig (2016), the self-report scales used to measure transportation (such as the Transportation Scale; Green & Brock, 2000) include items on attentional focus (e.g., “While I was reading the narrative, activity going on in the room was on my mind,” reverse coded; see also the narrative engagement scale by Busselle & Bilandzic [2009], which includes an attentional focus subscale).
Moreover, when transported into the story world, recipients build a representation that is rich in imagery. These vivid images of the story world are considered to be a key ingredient of story impact (Gerrig, 1993; Green & Brock, 2002; van Laer et al., 2014). With respect to the influence of strong versus weak arguments, attention and imagery likely facilitate the processing of arguments. Thus, for highly transported recipients, argument strength should matter more than for less transported recipients. In line with this reasoning, Quintero Johnson, Harrison, and Quick (2013) identified a positive relationship between transportation and self-reported systematic processing of a story that included health-related information.
Building intense imagery, however, could have a reverse effect on the influence of argument strength on narrative persuasion. Rich imagination likely consumes cognitive resources or working memory resources that are not available for competing simultaneous cognitive tasks (Green & Brock, 2002). Thus, being transported into the story world is supposed to be incompatible with demanding cognitive-elaborative activities such as counterarguing (Busselle & Bilandzic, 2009; Dal Cin et al., 2004; Green & Brock, 2000; Slater & Rouner, 2002), which is known as a key obstacle to persuasive efforts (e.g., Brock, 1967; Petty & Cacioppo, 1986). Drawn into the story world, recipients might lack sufficient working memory resources to engage in a thorough analysis of the communication. Moreover, if a tale is gripping and the experience is pleasant, the motivation for elaborative activities may be low (Green & Brock, 2002). From this perspective, the influence of argument strength on attitude change should be limited, given that recipients of stories lack the cognitive resources to scrutinize information included in a story. Tentative evidence in support of this hypothesis can be found in advertising research. Recipients who saw a print ad and imagined using the advertised product were less influenced by variations in argument strength than recipients who saw the print ad but did not imagine product use (Escalas, 2004; 2007; Lien & Chen, 2013; Praxmarer, 2011). Transferred to the effects of stories, these findings suggest that argument strength matters less when stories follow a typical story-structure or recipients are highly transported into the story world or both.
In sum, two competing assumptions regarding the influence of argument strength can be identified from the literature. However, these assumptions have yet to be tested directly.
Argument strength and research on non-narrative texts
Theory and research on narrative persuasion is based on the assumption that the processing and the effects of narratives differ in key regards from the processing of non-narrative texts (e.g., Appel & Richter, 2007; Gerrig, 1993; Green & Brock, 2000; Green & Brock 2002; Slater, 2002). Nonetheless, classical persuasion research, which often relied on dual-process models like the elaboration likelihood model (Petty & Cacioppo, 1986) or the heuristic systematic model (Chaiken, 1980), constitutes an important background for explorations on argument strength. Both models propose that the extent to which arguments are processed varies from not really elaborating on the arguments’ validity (in favor of peripheral cues such as characteristics of the message source) to entirely elaborating on it.
If the recipient has both the motivation and the ability to process the information thoroughly (high elaboration likelihood), strong arguments are more persuasive than weak arguments. Occasionally, countermessage or boomerang effects have been observed when weak arguments were presented (e.g., Park et al., 2007), in the sense that weak arguments led to persuasive effects contrary to the claims when elaboration likelihood was high. Factors that influence an individual’s ability (such as the speed at which assertions are presented; Smith & Shaffer, 1995) or the motivation to engage in elaboration (such as the personal relevance of a topic; cf., Johnson & Eagly, 1989) in turn affect the likelihood that strong arguments yield higher persuasion than weak arguments (cf. Carpenter, 2015; Petty & Wegener, 1998). Moreover, the influence of argument strength was found to vary with the disparity between recipients’ pre-exposure attitudes and the stance implied by the message. The more negative the pre-exposure attitude with respect to an advocated position, the more influential were manipulations of strong versus weak arguments (Johnson, Smith-McLallen, Killeya, & Levin, 2004).
Regarding general interindividual differences, the need for cognition was identified as a trait indicating high motivation to elaborate (Cacioppo & Petty, 1982). Supporting the assumption of different processes underlying the persuasion through narrative versus non-narrative texts, no consistent influence of the need for cognition was found for recipients’ transportation into the story world (Appel & Maleckar, 2012; Green & Brock, 2000) or the persuasive impact of narratives (Appel & Richter, 2007; Green & Brock, 2000; Wheeler, Green, & Brock, 1999). Regarding the ability to elaborate, it has been suggested that working memory capacity might moderate the influence of argument strength within a persuasion framework (Barrett, Tugade, & Engle, 2004). Elaboration requires cognitive resources and controlled processing. Thus, individuals lower in working memory capacity may not be able to effectively suppress nonrelevant information while focusing on and deliberately elaborating the content of a presented persuasive message. Individuals low in working memory capacity are more vulnerable to interference effects, which results in weaker performance in secondary tasks compared with individuals high in working memory capacity (e.g., Conway, Cowan, & Bunting, 2001; Kane & Engle, 2000; Rosen & Engle, 1998). However, an extensive literature research did not yield any studies in which working memory capacity was empirically related to argument strength in the context of persuasion, neither for non-narrative nor for narrative texts.
Study overview
Much of the available theoretical work on narrative persuasion suggests that the role that argument strength plays in narrative persuasion is smaller than the role argument strength plays in non-narrative persuasion (e.g., Green & Brock, 2002; Slater, 2002; see also Appel & Richter, 2007). To date, empirical research on the exact influence of strong versus weak arguments embedded in stories is missing. This work was meant to address this lacuna. The present study examined the influence of strong versus weak arguments in a low or high narrative context. In addition to postexposure attitudes, baseline attitudes, working memory capacity, and recipients’ transportation were examined.
Our first aim was to examine the role of argument strength in stimulus texts that vary in narrativity. In our experiment we manipulated argument strength by including claims in stories which were backed by strong or weak arguments. To examine whether the narrativity of the text influenced the processing and effects of arguments of varying strength, identical arguments were embedded in stories with low or high narrativity. To guarantee that the narrativity manipulation did not affect the content of the texts and thereby influence argument strength, identical content was presented with intact versus disrupted narrative structure (Wang & Calder, 2006). We expected that transportation would be influenced by story narrativity. We were more reluctant to assume an effect of argument strength on transportation, given the paucity of prior research and even one study that did not find this effect (Gnambs et al., 2014). Thus, we addressed this potential influence as a research question.
Working memory capacity could be a key factor that determines to what extent argument strength affects persuasion. It is assumed that transported recipients lack working memory resources to engage in cognitive operations that yield a resistance to attitude change (e.g., counterarguing). Given that individuals differ in working memory capacity as a trait (Engle, 2002), our aim was to examine the influence of individual differences in working memory capacity on narrative persuasion under different conditions of narrativity and argument strength. We tested the assumption that the influence of argument strength would increase with higher working memory capacity, particularly if a story’s narrativity is high. The independent manipulation of argument strength and narrativity allowed us to investigate the relationship between transportation and attitude change given high or low narrativity and weak or strong arguments.
Finally, most previous studies in the field examined attitudes after exposure to a story without pre-exposure assessment. Attitude change is inferred if participants’ postexposure attitudes in the experimental conditions differ. This procedure does not allow examining how story features might interact with pre-existing attitudes. Argument strength, narrativity, or both might be particularly influential if a story message is in contrast to the recipients’ attitudes toward the topic. To test this prediction, attitudes were measured 1 week before and immediately after story exposure.
Including several predictors into an experimental design and examining interactions provides intriguing insights on the boundary conditions of narrative persuasion. However, this endeavor can only be accomplished if statistical power is sufficient. To this end, a repeated-measures design was implemented. Using several stories, arguments, and attitudes further increased the generalizability of the expected findings (each participant received four texts on four different topics; see Methods below). The assignment of texts to experimental conditions as well as the order in which the conditions were presented was completely counterbalanced to control for topic and order effects.
Methods
Argument strength pilot study
Our arguments consisted of a claim or statement and three reasons that supported the claim (Shaw, 1996; Toulmin, 1958). The aim of our pilot study was to identify three weak and three strong reasons for each of four claims that were selected because they were not widely accepted by our participants (see below). The claims were (1) that cloning of plants is beneficial, (2) that eating salad is less healthy than people think, (3) that tuition fees yield positive consequences for students, and (4) that thick fruit beverages (smoothies) are unhealthy. For each claim we created six reasons that we expected to be judged as rather weak and six reasons expected to be judged as rather strong. We instructed the participants of the pilot study to indicate how suitable each reason was to convince a friend to agree to the claim and to provide their judgments on a seven-point scale with higher scores indicating stronger arguments. The participants were undergraduates; 127 students rated arguments for three of the topics, and 156 students rated arguments for the fourth topic. 1 Argument strength ratings ranged from 1.66 to 3.87 (M = 2.97, SD = 1.64) for the weak reasons and from 3.93 to 6.34 (M = 4.89, SD = 1.77) for the strong reasons. Because we wanted to include three reasons in each story, we chose the three reasons rated as least convincing and the three reasons rated as most convincing for each topic (see Appendix). The differences between the average ratings of the three weak and the three strong arguments were large and statistically significant. Paired t-tests revealed a significant effect for all four topics: tuition fees: t(126) = 9.46, p < .001, d = 1.29; cloning: t(126) = 6.13, p < .001, d = 1.03; smoothies: t(155) = 15.01, p < .001, d = 3.05; salad t(126) = 15.79, p < .001, d = 2.37 (pooled means M = 2.20, SD = 1.49 for the weak reasons and M = 5.23, SD = 1.67 for the strong reasons).
Participants
In our main study 82 undergraduates (65 women) from the University of Koblenz-Landau (Germany) participated for partial course credit. The experiment was run in a lab and consisted of two sessions. The participants’ age ranged from 19 to 43 years (M = 23.66 years; SD = 3.56).
Material
Stories and story manipulation
Each participant read four short stories (610–732 words), and each story included one belief-relevant topic. English translations of the original German stories are made available at https://osf.io/n2qsh/. The first story was about a young man lying in a hospital who fell in love with a woman over the Internet. The mysterious woman turned out to be his doctor. In this story, the protagonists exchanged arguments about the cloning of plants. The second story featured a young female journalist interviewing a famous opera singer who might have had an affair with the journalist’s mother several months before the journalist was born. This story included a discussion of the (non-)benefits of eating salad. The third story dealt with a young woman and a young man who meet in a public park for a blind date and discuss tuition fees. The fourth story described a young couple on a road trip in which the woman seems to hide something that turns out to be a new tattoo. At a gas station the protagonists overhear a discussion about the (non-)benefits of thick fruit beverages (smoothies).
Four versions of each story were developed. The versions differed with regard to the narrativity of the text (narrativity high vs. low) and the strength of the arguments (argument strength high vs. low) included in the story, following a two-factorial design. Each story consisted of five paragraphs and incorporated one paragraph in which a character made a claim supported by three reasons. In the high narrativity condition, the stories had a smooth, linear, chronological flow, whereas in the low narrativity condition, the elements of the stories were scrambled to disrupt the plot line without making it incomprehensible. The paragraph that contained the belief-relevant topic always remained at the same position, which was the fourth of five paragraphs. This method was successfully used in previous studies to manipulate transportation (e.g., Appel, Gnambs, Richter, & Green, 2015; Voss, Wiley, & Sandak, 1999; Wang & Calder, 2006). In addition, the texts varied with respect to the strength of the three reasons that supported the statement. Thus, the arguments, pretested in the pilot study reported above, were either high in argument strength or low in argument strength. All text versions within and across topics were comparable in writing style, length (number of sentences: story 1, 65–67; story 2, 59; story 3, 57–58; story 4, 50–51; words: story 1, 703–737; story 2, 773–787; story 3, 613–622; story 4, 701–717), and difficulty as operationalized by Flesch’s Reading Ease Index (55–59; cf. Amstad, 1978; Flesh, 1948).
Assessment of working memory capacity
The reading span task (Daneman & Carpenter, 1980; Oberauer, Süß, Schulze, Wilhelm, & Wittmann, 2000) served as the measure of working memory capacity. The reading span task required the participants to read 84 unconnected sentences presented in blocks. For each sentence, the participants had 5 seconds to decide if it was true or false. Additionally, they had to memorize the last word of each sentence. The number of sentences presented per block was incrementally increased from three to seven. After each block, the participants had to recall the end-of-the-sentence words of the block. Items were only counted as correct if the right word was remembered at the right position within the specific block. The true or false judgment was not analyzed in the end, but the participants were made to believe that it was an important part of the task. This prevented the participants from adopting a strategy that focused on the final words without devoting much attention to the reading of the sentence. It has been shown (Daneman & Carpenter, 1980) that the reading span task is especially suitable to measure working memory as related to reading abilities because it requires text processing and storing. The reading span task was scored as recommended by Friedman and Miyake (2005) by averaging the proportion of correct words per block across all blocks.
Transportation
The state of being transported into a narrative world was measured with the six items of the Transportation Scale–Short Form (Appel et al., 2015), which exhibits levels of reliability comparable with the original Transportation Scale and has been validated in a series of studies (cf. Appel et al., 2015). The six self-report items (with seven-point response scales, ranging from 1 to 7) describe the cognitive, emotional, and imagery involvement in a narrative (e.g., “I could picture myself in the scene of the events described in the narrative” or “The narrative affected me emotionally”). Cronbach’s α ranged between .74 and .92, indicating high internal consistency of the measure regarding all story versions.
Attitude measures
For each topic addressed by the statements, participants answered five attitude items (e.g., “The cloning of plants has significant positive effects,” “There should be more information about the negative consequences of smoothies”), yielding 20 attitude items altogether. A seven-point rating scale was provided (1 = disagree completely and 7 = agree completely). The responses were recoded so that high scores represent attitudes in line with the arguments included in the story.
Additional measures
Our experiment further included a thought listing task and a Pinocchio circling task (cf. Green & Brock, 2000). The thought listing took place after the transportation and attitude items regarding a text were administered, shortly before the participants read the next story. The Pinocchio circling task was introduced at the very end of the experiment after participants had read all four stories and after all dependent variables were assessed. To ensure participants only focused on the part of the story that contained the arguments in their responses, we changed the traditional instructions of the thought listing and the Pinocchio circling tasks (Cacioppo & Petty, 1981; Green & Brock, 2000). The results suggest that these instructions were misspecified, because most participants did not follow the instructions as expected. We included one additional item that asked about future behaviors as a potential additional outcome. We had doubts about the reliability and validity of this measure and did not investigate further. Results regarding the three measures are not presented.
Procedure
All participants came to our lab twice with at least 7 days between Session 1 and Session 2 (M = 8.71 days; SD = 3.50). In Session 1 we assessed participants’ working memory capacity, operationalized by the reading span task. We further administered the attitude items to assess participants’ pre-exposure baseline attitudes toward the topics. To disguise our research interest, these focal attitude items were mixed with 20 additional items about topics unrelated to our experimental treatment.
In Session 2 participants were seated in front of a computer where the study material was presented. They read four stories, each one representing a different combination of narrativity (high vs. low) and argument strength (high vs. low). The story-factor combinations for each participant were counterbalanced so that each participant read only one version of each story. Additionally, the order of the stories within all possible combinations was completely balanced, which resulted in 16 different versions to control for position effects. Each of the 16 combinations was administered to at least five participants by random assignment.
After each story, the short form of the Transportation Scale was presented, followed by the attitude items about the topic involved in the story. Then participants continued reading the next story. At the end of the experiment, participants provided demographic information and were thanked and debriefed.
Results
Means and standard deviations of baseline and postexposure attitudes for all four experimental conditions are displayed in Table 1. In all four experimental conditions, recipients’ attitudes shifted toward the message included in the story.
Table 1.
Baseline Attitudes |
Postexposure Attitudes |
Difference Between Baseline and Postexposure Attitudes |
Effect Size |
Significance |
||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
M | SD | M | SD | t | Cohen’s d | p | ||
Argument strength low - narrativity low | 3.02 | 1.31 | 3.45 | 1.21 | 3.23 | .36 | <.001 | |
Argument strength low - narrativity high | 2.92 | 1.15 | 3.45 | 1.17 | 4.42 | .42 | <.001 | |
Argument strength high - narrativity low | 3.15 | 1.30 | 3.68 | .95 | 3.75 | .49 | <.001 | |
Argument strength high - narrativity high | 3.26 | 1.38 | 3.86 | 1.05 | 3.90 | .43 | <.001 |
To identify factors that influence the magnitude of this persuasive effect, our main statistical approach were multilevel analyses (linear mixed models), which allowed us to account for the repeated-measures design and for the fact that participants, topics, and attitude items were sampled from larger populations. Thus, our model included participants, topics, and items as random factors (random intercepts). Moreover, multilevel analyses allow examining main effects as well as interaction effects of continuous and categorical predictors located on different levels (participants and topics). The independent variables argument strength and narrativity were incorporated as contrast-coded predictors with fixed effects. The variables reading span, transportation, and baseline attitude were entered in the model as z-standardized predictors (fixed effects). To control for sequence effects, we also ran models that additionally included the position of the text (experimental condition) within the experiment (1, 2, 3, or 4) as grand-mean centered predictor and the interactions of this predictor with all other predictors in the model.
In the model with attitude as dependent variable, including text position as a predictor did not result in a significant increase in explanatory power (χ2 (8) = 13.91, p = .08). In the model with transportation as dependent variable, text position did contribute significantly to the explanatory power of the model (χ2 (4) = 164.73 p < .001). This was due to a significant negative main effect of text position (t = –13.02, p < .001), which indicated that participants were less transported into texts they read later. Importantly, however, these analyses suggest that the pattern of hypothesis-relevant effects was not significantly affected by text position. In the interest of parsimony of the estimated models and conciseness of presentation, we only refer to the models without text position here. The analyses were conducted with the packages lme4 (Bates et al., 2014) and lmerTest (Kutznetsova, Brockhoff, & Christensen, 2014). All packages are part of the R environment for statistical computing and graphics (R Development Core Team, 2015). All significance tests were based on a Type I error probability of .05.
Transportation as dependent variable
Our first multilevel analysis focused on recipients’ experience of transportation as the criterion with participants and topics as random factors. The results of this analysis are displayed in Table 2.
Table 2.
Predictor | Estimate | SE | df | t | p |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Main effects | |||||
Narrativitya | .29 | .02 | 1,545 | 15.82 | <.001 |
Argument strengtha | .00 | .02 | 1,545 | –.11 | .92 |
Baseline attitudeb | .02 | .02 | 1,561 | 1.05 | .30 |
Reading spanb | .20 | .10 | 80 | 1.94 | .06 |
Two-way interactions | |||||
Narrativity × argument strength | –.12 | .02 | 1,545 | –6.51 | <.001 |
Narrativity × baseline attitude | .00 | .02 | 1,556 | –.15 | .88 |
Argument strength × baseline attitude | –.01 | .02 | 1,554 | –.41 | .68 |
Narrativity × reading span | .02 | .02 | 1,545 | 1.33 | .18 |
Argument strength × reading span | –.01 | .02 | 1,545 | –.73 | .46 |
Three-way interactions | |||||
Narrativity × argument strength × baseline attitude | .02 | .02 | 1,559 | .84 | .40 |
Narrativity × argument strength × reading span | .03 | .02 | 1,545 | 1.87 | .06 |
Contrast-coded (Narrativity: low = –1, high = 1; Argument strength: low = –1, high = 1).
z-standardized.
It was expected that transportation scores should be higher in high-narrative stories. There was indeed a significant main effect of narrativity (Cohen’s d = .44): Transportation scores were higher in high-narrative stories (M = 4.71, SEM = .25) compared with low-narrative stories (M = 4.14, SEM = .25). Neither the main effects for argument strength nor baseline attitude reached significance. However, the interaction between narrativity and argument strength was significant. In stories with low narrativity, transportation scores were higher if the story contained strong arguments (M = 4.26, SEM = .25) compared with weak arguments (M = 4.02, SEM = .25), t(1,545) = 4.53, p < .001, d = .19. In the high narrativity condition, transportation scores were higher if the story contained weak arguments (M = 4.83, SEM = .25) compared with strong arguments (M = 4.59, SEM = .25), t(1545) = –4.67, p < .001, d = .19. All other interactions failed to reach significance. The main effect of reading span approached significance, suggesting that the higher the trait reading span scores, the higher the transportation ratings. Reading span did not moderate the main effect of narrativity or the interaction between narrativity and argument strength.
Attitudes as dependent variable
Our second multilevel analysis focused on recipients’ postexposure attitudes as the criterion with participants, topics, and items as random factors. The results of this analysis are displayed in Table 3.
Table 3.
Predictor | Estimate | SE | df | t | p |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Main effects | |||||
Narrativitya | .04 | .04 | 1,601 | .96 | .34 |
Argument strengtha | .12 | .04 | 1,537 | 3.24 | .001 |
Transportationb | .03 | .05 | 346 | .62 | .53 |
Baseline attitudeb | .26 | .04 | 1,578 | 6.03 | <.001 |
Reading spanb | .02 | .05 | 79 | .30 | .76 |
Two-way interactions | |||||
Narrativity × argument strength | .02 | .04 | 1,553 | .64 | .52 |
Narrativity × transportation | .02 | .04 | 1,545 | .04 | .97 |
Argument strength × transportation | .07 | .04 | 1,592 | 1.81 | .07 |
Narrativity × baseline attitude | –.01 | .04 | 1,600 | –.16 | .87 |
Argument strength × baseline attitude | –.09 | .04 | 1,605 | –2.59 | .01 |
Narrativity × reading span | –.00 | .04 | 1,534 | –.08 | .93 |
Argument strength × reading span | .04 | .04 | 1,530 | 1.16 | .25 |
Three-way interactions | |||||
Narrativity × argument strength × transportation | .08 | .04 | 1,591 | 2.10 | .04 |
Narrativity × argument strength × baseline attitude | .06 | .04 | 1,588 | 1.70 | .09 |
Narrativity × argument strength × reading span | –.01 | .04 | 1,530 | –.16 | .87 |
Contrast-coded (Narrativity: low = –1, high = 1; Argument strength: low = –1, high = 1).
z-standardized.
There were two significant main effects. First, the main effect of baseline attitude was significant. Not surprisingly, pre- and postexposure attitudes were positively related. Second, we found a small but significant main effect of argument strength (d = .12): Strong arguments led to attitude scores more in line with the story’s message (M = 3.74, SEM = .37) compared with weak arguments (M = 3.51, SEM = .37).
The main effects of argument strength and baseline attitude were qualified by a two-way interaction between both variables (Fig. 1): Baseline attitudes were a stronger predictor of postexposure attitudes when stories of low rather than high argument strength were presented (simple slope for low argument strength: B = .35, SEB = .06; t(1,609) = 6.21, p < .001; simple slope for high argument strength: B = .16, SEB = .06; t(1,593) = 2.95, p = .003). From an argument strength perspective, the interaction shows that argument strength matters most for individuals who were initially more skeptical toward the story message (simple main effect for a baseline attitude 2 standard deviations below the mean: B = .30, SEB = .08; t(1,600) = 3.79, p < .001; simple main effect for a baseline attitude 2 standard deviations above the mean: B = –.07, SEB = .08; t(1,602) = –.87, p = .39). The effect of argument strength was further qualified by a three-way interaction with narrativity and transportation (Fig. 2). Argument strength mattered most in the high narrativity condition, provided that individuals were transported into the story world. Simple slope analyses showed that if the story was high in argument strength and high in narrativity, the extent to which participants were transported into the story world had a positive influence on postexposure attitudes (B = .18, SEB = .09; t(1,072) = 2.10, p = .04), whereas there was no significant relationship between transportation and attitude in the other groups (for all effects: |t|< 1.30, p > .19).
All other effects on attitude change were nonsignificant. This includes the effects of working memory capacity, which appeared to be unrelated to the narrative effects. Thus, we found no support for a more pronounced effect of argument strength with increasing working memory capacity under any of the story narrativity conditions.
Discussion
Individuals often read, watch, and listen to stories purely for pleasure and entertainment (cf. Nell, 1988), but these stories can have substantial consequences on recipients’ attitudes and beliefs (narrative persuasion; Green & Brock, 2000; van Laer et al., 2014). The persuasive power of stories has been harnessed to change attitudes and behavior in various applied contexts, including health communication (e.g., Hinyard & Kreuter, 2007; Murphy, Frank, Chatterjee, & Baezconde-Garbanati, 2013), political communication (e.g., LaMarree & Landreville, 2009; Paluck, 2012), and product advertising (Petrova & Cialdini, 2008; Phillips & McQuarrie, 2010).
In this work we took a closer look at the role of argument strength in narrative persuasion. Whereas argument strength is a key factor in classic persuasion theories (Chaiken, 1980; Petty & Cacioppo, 1986), little is known about its influence in narrative persuasion. To address this research lacuna, a repeated-measures experiment was conducted in which argument strength and narrativity were manipulated. Postexposure attitudes served as the dependent variable, and the design further included an assessment of working memory capacity, baseline attitudes, and transportation.
We found that in all four experimental conditions, attitudes shifted in the direction of the story’s message. Stories with strong arguments, however, were more persuasive than stories with weak arguments. This main effect was qualified by a two-way interaction with baseline attitudes, indicating that argument strength mattered most among individuals who were particularly skeptical toward the story claim before they read the story. We further identified a three-way interaction of argument strength, narrativity, and transportation: Provided that a story had an intact story structure (high narrativity), argument strength mattered most for recipients who were deeply transported into the story world—only when transportation was high, strong arguments were more persuasive than weak arguments. When the story structure was disrupted (as in our operationalization of low narrativity), transportation was unrelated to persuasive effects. On the one hand, this finding is in line with transportation theory which posits that transportation can explain narrative effects in stories but not in nonstory formats. It is also in line with the notion that attention is an integral part of transportation (Bezdek & Gerrig, 2016; Green & Brock, 2000) and a study that revealed a positive link between transportation and self-reported systematic message processing (Quintero-Johnson et al., 2013). On the other hand, this finding appears to be at odds with basic theoretical work on narrative persuasion. Van Laer et al. (2014), for example, summarize prior theory stating that “narrative transportation is a mental state that produces enduring persuasive effects without careful evaluation of arguments” (p. 800).
In contrast to some theoretical notions, our findings show that argument strength can be a factor in narrative persuasion. Our research, however, also demonstrates that even weak arguments included in a story can change recipients’ attitudes. Thus, stories can persuade in the absence of strong arguments (cf. Gerrig & Prentice, 1991; Green & Brock, 2002), but argument strength contributes to larger attitude change. These findings were not qualified by recipients’ working memory capacity, suggesting that story effects are not restricted to those who lack mental resources.
Limitations and future research
The stories examined in this study included persuasive content that was not a key element of the plot. Similar stories were used in a number of studies in the field (e.g., Appel & Richter, 2007; Dahlstrom, 2010; 2012; Fazio, Dolan, & Marsh, 2015; Gerrig & Prentice, 1991; Prentice et al., 1997). We deliberately followed this practice as our goal was an independent manipulation of argument strength and narrativity. An alternative method for manipulating argument strength in stories might be to manipulate the severity of the consequences (e.g., minor illness vs. death) of a given target behavior (e.g., getting vaccinated) that is closely connected to an attitude (e.g., attitude toward getting vaccinated). Future studies are encouraged to explore this alternative operationalization.
Second, we opted for a repeated measures design and we conducted multilevel analyses as our main statistical tool. The design allowed us to examine predictors on the person, the text, and the item level while preserving sufficient power. To reduce unwanted influences of carry-over effects, the stories were presented in counterbalanced order and the pre-exposure attitudes were assessed along with filler items at least 7 days before the main experimental session. Essentially, the counterbalancing of the order of experimental conditions rules out a systematic influence of story position (e.g., reading the first story vs. a later story) on our results. Nonetheless, we conducted additional analyses controlling for potential effects of text position. These analyses showed that transportation decreased with text position, but the pattern of our key results remained unaffected. We believe that future replications based on a complementary between-subjects design (including a reduced set of predictors) could provide an important corroboration of the present results.
Third, we investigated crucial factors and boundary conditions of narrative persuasion but did not directly measure the psychological processes underlying these effects. It is arguably one of the greatest challenges in the research on narrative influence to delineate processes during reception. In future studies, psychophysiological measures might be used to examine online emotional reactions (Sukalla, Bilandzic, Bolls, & Busselle, 2015), and researchers can profit from methods used in text comprehension research to examine cognitive processes. Future studies are encouraged to assess reading times on information that might contradict participants’ pre-exposure attitudes (individuals need more time to process information that is inconsistent with prior information or beliefs; e.g., Singer, 2006) or the re-reading of words and sentences (which is also more frequent for information perceived as implausible or inconsistent; e.g., Hyönä, Lorch, & Rinck, 2003). Ideally, studies could obtain several measures simultaneously (e.g., psychophysiological measures and eye-tracking measures such as re-reading) to tap into different components of recipients’ transportation.
Conclusion
In contrast to the pivotal role of argument strength in non-narrative persuasion research, surprisingly little is known about the role that weak versus strong arguments play in narrative persuasion. The current study shows that even weak arguments embedded in stories can persuade but that argument strength increases the persuasive impact, particularly if recipients are initially rather skeptical toward an issue, and that argument strength matters most when individuals are deeply transported into a story of high narrativity. Thus, researchers and practitioners can expect the highest narrative impact if a good story contains strong arguments and recipients are deeply immersed into the story world.
Appendix A.
Argument Strength |
||
---|---|---|
Story Claim | Low | High |
Cloning of plants is beneficial | Cloning is part of an unstoppable development. | Unlike in genetically modified food, no genetic material is changed in cloning. This means that no new life form is created. |
Agricultural companies can make good money from the cloning of plants. | A major advantage of cloning is that the characteristics of the plant type that are fixed in the genome can be kept intact. | |
Cloning is supported by some Nobel Prize winners. | A “plant clone” is nothing but an offshoot, which sometimes also occurs naturally without human intervention. | |
Eating salad is less healthy than people think | The look of salad reminds many people of animal food. | The salad available in shops often contains many germs. |
A society’s orientation toward a diet rich in salad and other vegetables endangers the meat producing and processing industry. | Salad is often contaminated with heavy metals, such as lead, cadmium, and quicksilver, and with residues of pesticides. | |
The preparation of salad is far too time-consuming. | Lettuce leaves, especially when cultivated in a greenhouse, are enriched with nitrate, which is transformed into noxious nitrite by the human body. | |
Tuition fees yield positive consequences for students | Tuition fees lead to a useful competition between universities. | It is only fair that students who benefit longer from the university’s services also have to pay more. |
If some students cannot afford the fees, there will be more resources for the students who can. | With the students’ financial contributions, considerably more money can be invested in teaching, which enhances the educational quality enormously. | |
If students have to pay tuition fees, then less money is available for irrational spending. | With tuition fees, more teaching staff can be employed. As a consequence, professors have more time for each student, which, for example, has a positive impact on the supervision of theses. | |
Smoothies are unhealthy | The product design is unnecessarily modern. | Smoothies often contain up to 40 grams of sugar, which is 30% more sugar compared with a coke. |
Smoothies are not essential, because hardly anyone has a vitamin deficiency. | Doctors state that smoothies contain less vitamins than promised by the manufacturer. | |
The list of ingredients is often needlessly printed in several languages. | The high amount of sugar and acidity in smoothies can cause serious tooth decay. |
Funding Statement
This work was supported in part by grants from the Austrian Science Fund (FWF, I 996-G22) and the German Science Foundation (DFG, RI 1100/8-1) awarded to Markus Appel and Tobias Richter.
Footnotes
In the pilot sample of 127 undergraduates (78 women), we also assessed the agreement to the four claims included in the experiment proper. On a seven-point scale (1 = completely disagree with the claim, 7 = fully agree with the claim), the mean endorsement ranged from 2.38 to 4.51. This indicated that the claims were not extremely popular, leaving a potential for belief change in our main study.
Funding
This work was supported in part by grants from the Austrian Science Fund (FWF, I 996-G22) and the German Science Foundation (DFG, RI 1100/8-1) awarded to Markus Appel and Tobias Richter.
References
- Abbott H. P. (2002). . Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Appel M., Gnambs T., & Maio G. (2012). A short measure of the need for affect. , 94, 418–426. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Appel M., Gnambs T., Richter T., & Green M. (2015). The Transportation Scale-Short Form (TS-SF). , 18, 243–266. [Google Scholar]
- Appel M., & Malečkar B. (2012). The influence of paratext on narrative persuasion: Fact, fiction, or fake? , 38, 459–484. [Google Scholar]
- Appel M., & Mara M. (2013). The persuasive influence of a fictional character’s trustworthiness. , 63, 912–932. [Google Scholar]
- Appel M., & Richter T. (2007). Persuasive effects of fictional narratives increase over time. , 10, 113–134. [Google Scholar]
- Appel M., & Richter T. (2010). Transportation and need for affect in narrative persuasion. A mediated moderation model. , 13, 101–135. [Google Scholar]
- Barrett L. F., Tugade M. M., & Engle R. W. (2004). Individual differences in working memory capacity and dual-process theories of the mind. , 130, 553–573. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bates D., Maechler M., Bolker B., Walker S., Christensen R. H. B., & Sigmann H. (2014). Lme4: linear mixed-effects models using Eigen and S4 [Software]. R-package version 1.1-6. Retrieved May 1, 2014 from: http://cran.r-project.org/package=lme4
- Bezdek M. A., & Gerrig R. J. (2016). When narrative transportation narrows attention: Changes in attentional focus during suspenseful film viewing. Media Psychology, advanced online publication. doi: 10.1080/15213269.2015.1121830 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Brock T. C. (1967). Communication discrepancy and intent to persuade as determinants of counterargument production. , 3, 296–309. [Google Scholar]
- Busselle R., & Bilandzic H. (2008). Fictionality and perceived realism in experiencing stories: A model of narrative comprehension and engagement. , 18, 255–280. [Google Scholar]
- Busselle R., & Bilandzic H. (2009). Measuring narrative engagement. , 12, 321–347. [Google Scholar]
- Cacioppo J. T, & Petty R. E. (1981). Social psychological procedures for cognitive response assessment: The thought-listing technique In Merluzzi T. V., Glass C. R., & Genest M. (Eds.), (pp. 309–342). New York, NY: Guilford Press. [Google Scholar]
- Cacioppo J. T., & Petty R. E. (1982). The need for cognition. , 42, 116–131. [Google Scholar]
- Carpenter C. J. (2015). A meta-analysis of the ELM’s argument quality X processing type predictions. , 41, 501–534. [Google Scholar]
- Chaiken S. (1980). Heuristic versus systematic information processing and the use of source versus message cues in persuasion. , 39, 752–766. [Google Scholar]
- Conway A.R.A., Cowan N., & Bunting M.F. (2001). The cocktail party phenomenon revisited: The importance of working memory capacity. , 8, 331–335. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dahlstrom M. F. (2010). The role of causality in information acceptance in narratives: An example from science communication. , 37, 857–875. [Google Scholar]
- Dahlstrom M. F. (2012). The persuasive influence of narrative causality: Psychological mechanism, strength in overcoming resistance, and persistence over time. , 15, 303–326. [Google Scholar]
- Dal Cin S., Zanna M. P., & Fong G. T. (2004). Narrative persuasion and overcoming resistance In Knowles E. S. & Linn J. A. (Eds.), (pp. 175–191). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. [Google Scholar]
- Daneman M., & Carpenter P. A. (1980). Individual differences in working memory and reading. , 19, 450–466. [Google Scholar]
- de Graaf A., Hoeken H., Sanders J., & Beentjes J. W. (2012). Identification as a mechanism of narrative persuasion. , 39, 802–823. [Google Scholar]
- Engle R. W. (2002). Working memory capacity as executive attention. , 11, 19–23. [Google Scholar]
- Escalas J. E. (2004). Imagine yourself in the product: Mental simulation, narrative transportation, and persuasion. , 33, 37–48. [Google Scholar]
- Escalas J. E. (2007). Self-referencing and persuasion: Narrative transportation versus analytical elaboration. , 33, 421–429. [Google Scholar]
- Fazio L. K., Dolan P. O., & Marsh E. J. (2015). Learning misinformation from fictional sources: Understanding the contributions of transportation and item-specific processing. , 23, 166–167. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fludernik M. (2002). . London, UK: Routledge. [Google Scholar]
- Fong K., Mullin J. B., & Mar R. A. (2013). What you read matters: The role of fiction genres in predicting interpersonal sensitivity. , 7, 370–376. [Google Scholar]
- Friedman N. P., & Miyake A. (2005). Comparison of four scoring methods for the reading span test. , 37, 581–590. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gerrig R. J. (1993). . New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Gerrig R. J., & Prentice D. A. (1991). The representation of fictional information. , 2, 336–340. [Google Scholar]
- Gnambs T., Appel M., Schreiner C., Richter T., & Isberner M.-B. (2014). Experiencing narrative worlds: A latent state-trait analysis. , 69, 187–192. [Google Scholar]
- Gottschall J. (2012). . New York, NY: Houghton Mifflin Harcourt. [Google Scholar]
- Green M. C. (2004). Transportation into narrative worlds: The role of prior knowledge and perceived realism. , 38, 247–266. [Google Scholar]
- Green M. C., & Brock T. C. (2000). The role of transportation in the persuasiveness of public narratives. , 79, 701–721. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Green M. C., & Brock T. C (2002). In the mind’s eye. Transportation-imagery model of narrative persuasion In Green M.C., Strange J.J., & Brock T.C. (Eds.), (pp. 315–342). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. [Google Scholar]
- Green M. C., Brock T. C., & Kaufman G. F. (2004). Understanding media enjoyment: The role of transportation into narrative worlds. , 14, 311–327. [Google Scholar]
- Green M. C., & Donahue J. K. (2009). Simulated worlds: Transportation into narratives In Markman K., Klein W.M., & Suhr J.A. (Eds.), (pp. 241–256). New York, NY: Psychology Press. [Google Scholar]
- Hinyard L. J., & Kreuter M. W. (2007). Using narrative communication as a tool for health behavior change: A conceptual, theoretical, and empirical overview. , 34, 777–792 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hoeken H., & Fikkers K. M. (2014). Issue-relevant thinking and identification as mechanisms of narrative persuasion. , 44, 84–99. [Google Scholar]
- Hoeken H., & Sinkeldam J. (2014). The role of identification and perception of just outcome in evoking emotions in narrative persuasion. , 64, 935–955. [Google Scholar]
- Hyönä J., Lorch R.F. Jr., & Rinck M. (2003). Eye movement measures to study global text processing In Hyönä J., Radach R., & Deubel H. (Eds.), (pp. 313–334). Amsterdam, Netherlands: Elsevier. [Google Scholar]
- Johnson B. T., & Eagly A. H. (1989). Effects of involvement on persuasion: A meta-analysis. , 106, 290–314. [Google Scholar]
- Johnson B. T., Smith-McLallen A., Killeya L. A., & Levin K. D. (2004). Truth or consequences: Overcoming resistance to persuasion with positive thinking In Knowles E. S. & Linn J. A. (Eds.), (pp. 215–233). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. [Google Scholar]
- Kane M. J., & Engle R. W. (2000). Working-memory capacity, proactive interference, and divided attention: Limits on long-term memory retrieval. , 26, 336–358. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kuznetsova A., Brockhoff P. B., & Christensen R. H. B. (2014). lmerTest: tests for random and fixed effects for linear mixed effect models (lmer objects of lme4 package ). R-package version 2.06. Retrieved in June 2014 from http://cran.r- project.org/web/packages/lmerTest/index.html
- LaMarre H. L., & Landreville K. D. (2009). When is fiction as good as fact? Comparing the influence of documentary and historical reenactment films on engagement, affect, issue interest, and learning. , 12, 537–555. [Google Scholar]
- Lien N.-H., & Chen Y.-L. (2013). Narrative ads: The effect of argument strength and story format. , 66, 516–522. [Google Scholar]
- Mar R. A., & Oatley K. (2008). The function of fiction is the abstraction and simulation of social experience. , 13, 173–192. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mazzocco P. J., Green M. C., Sasota J. A., & Jones N. W. (2010). This story is not for everyone: Transportability and narrative persuasion. , 1, 361–368. [Google Scholar]
- Miall D. S., & Kuiken D. (1994). Foregrounding, defamiliarization, and affect: Response to literary stories. , 22, 389–407. [Google Scholar]
- Murphy S.T., Frank L.B., Chatterjee J.S., & Baezconde-Garbanati L. (2013). Narrative versus non-narrative: The role of identification, transportation, and emotion in reducing health disparities. , 63, 116–137. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nell V. (1988). . New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Neuman Y., Glassner A., & Weinstock M. (2004). The effect of a reason’s truth-value on the judgment of a fallacious argument. , 116, 173–184. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oberauer K., Süß H. M., Schulze R., Wilhelm O., & Wittmann W. W. (2000). Working memory capacity—facets of a cognitive ability construct. , 29, 1017–1045. [Google Scholar]
- Paluck E. L. (2012). Media as an instrument for reconstructing communities following conflict In Jonas K. & Morton T. (Eds.), (pp. 284–298). West Sussex: Wiley-Blackwell. [Google Scholar]
- Park H. S., Levine T. R., Kingsley Westerman C. Y., Orfgen T., & Foregger S. (2007). The effects of argument quality and involvement type on attitude formation and attitude change: A test of dual‐process and social judgment predictions. , 33, 81–102. [Google Scholar]
- Petrova P. K., & Cialdini R. B. (2008). Evoking the imagination as a strategy of influence In Haugtvedt C. P., Herr P. M. & Kardes F. R. (Eds.), (pp. 505–523). New York, NY: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. [Google Scholar]
- Petty R. E., & Cacioppo J. T. (1986). . New York, NY: Springer. [Google Scholar]
- Phillips B. J., & McQuarrie E. F. (2010). Narrative and persuasion in fashion advertising. , 37, 368–392. [Google Scholar]
- Praxmarer S. (2011). Message strength and persuasion when consumers imagine product usage. , 10, 225–231. [Google Scholar]
- Prentice D. A., Gerrig R. J., & Bailis D. S. (1997). What readers bring to the processing of fictional texts. , 4, 416–420. [Google Scholar]
- Quintero Johnson J. M., Harrison K., & Quick B. (2013). Understanding the effectiveness of the entertainment-education strategy: An investigation of how audience involvement, message processing, and message design influence health information recall. , 18, 160–78. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Core Team R. (2014). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. Retrieved from http://www.R-project.org/.
- Rosen V. M., & Engle R. W. (1998). Working memory capacity and suppression. , 39, 418–436. [Google Scholar]
- Rumelhart D. E. (1975). Notes on a schema for stories In Bobrow D. G. & Collins A. (Eds.), (pp. 211–236). New York, NY: Academic Press. [Google Scholar]
- Shaw V. F. (1996). The cognitive processes in informal reasoning. , 2, 51–80. [Google Scholar]
- Singer M. (2006). Verification of text ideas during reading. , 54, 574–591. [Google Scholar]
- Slater M. D. (2002). Involvement as goal-directed strategic processing: Extending the elaboration likelihood model In Dillard J. P. & Pfau M. (Eds.), (pp. 175–194). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. [Google Scholar]
- Slater M. D., & Rouner D. (2002). Entertainment-education and elaboration-likelihood: Understanding the processing of narrative persuasion. , 12, 173–191. [Google Scholar]
- Smith S. M., & Shaffer D. R. (1995). Speed of speech and persuasion: Evidence for multiple effects. , 21, 257–268. [Google Scholar]
- Sukalla F., Bilandzic H., Bolls P. D., & Busselle R. W. (2016). Embodiment of narrative engagement: Connecting self-reported narrative engagement to psychophysiological measures. Journal of Media Psychology,28, 175–186. [Google Scholar]
- Toulmin S. (1958). . Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Van Laer T., de Ruyter K., Visconti L. M., & Wetzels M. (2014). The extended transportation-imagery model: A meta-analysis of the antecedents and consequences of consumers’ narrative transportation. , 40, 797–817. [Google Scholar]
- Voss, J. F., Wiley, J., & Sandak, R (1999). On the use of narrative as argument In S.R. Goldman, A.C. Graesser, & P. van den Broek (Eds.), Narrative comprehension, causality, and coherence: Essays in honor of Tom Trabasso (pp. 235–252). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. [Google Scholar]
- Wang J., & Calder B. J. (2006). Media transportation and advertising. , 33, 151–162. [Google Scholar]
- Wheeler S. C., Green M. C., & Brock T.C. (1999). Fictional narratives change beliefs: Replications of Prentice, Gerrig & Bailis (1997) with mixed corroboration. , 6, 136–141. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]