Abstract
Nurturing behavior may be critically influenced by the interplay of different hormones. The neuropeptide oxytocin is known to promote maternal behavior and its reduction has been associated with postpartum depression risk and child neglect. Contrariwise, the observed decrease in testosterone level during early parenthood may benefit caretaking behavior, whereas increased testosterone may reduce attention to infants. Here we used functional magnetic resonance imaging to investigate the interactive influence of testosterone and oxytocin on selective attention to and neural processing of the baby schema (BS). 57 nulliparous women performed a target detection task with human faces with varying degree of BS following double-blinded placebo-controlled oxytocin administration in a between-subjects design. Our results support the idea that oxytocin enhances attention to the BS. Oxytocin had a positive effect on activation of the inferior frontal junction during identification of infant targets with a high degree of BS that were presented among adult distractors. Further, activation of the putamen was positively correlated with selective attention to the BS, but only in women with high endogenous testosterone who received oxytocin. These findings provide initial evidence for the neural mechanism by which oxytocin may counteract the negative effects of testosterone in the modulation of nurturing behavior.
Introduction
The survival of the offspring is crucial for one’s reproductive success. Newborns may therefore carry specific features that act as key stimulus, which automatically captures attention and motivates actions such as caretaking. The baby schema (BS) is such a key stimulus. In the infant face it is defined as a combination of child characteristic features like big eyes, small nose, chubby cheeks, and higher forehead1. Humans bear high costs in raising their offspring, because pregnancy and production of maternal milk are energetically expensive2, which should make newborns highly relevant, at least for mothers. But since humans practice alloparental care3 and the BS is further known to be a universally relevant stimulus4 the BS should be highly significant for women in general. The increased cuteness of infant faces in comparison to adults may therefore prioritize selective attention5,6 and may enhance evaluation of these stimuli as socially relevant for actions like caretaking7.
There is already evidence for an adaptive neural mechanism through which the perception of cuteness as a feature of the BS is most likely promoted in humans. Accordingly, cute baby faces have repeatedly yielded activations of regions of the mesolimbic reward system8,9. For example, Glocker et al.10 observed an increase of activation in the nucleus accumbens in women while evaluating pictures of babies with increasing BS (i.e., low BS < unmanipulated BS < high BS). Interestingly, the mesolimbic reward system may also be highly responsive to the influence of two hormones that have opposing roles in human nurturing behaviors. Firstly, a high oxytocin (OT) receptor density can be found in the mesolimbic reward system of social mammals11 and increased activation of the ventral tegmental area to images of crying infants after OT treatment suggested that OT influences the reward value of stimuli carrying the BS12. Further, OT administration increased the preference for pictures of young children. This preference was determined through rs53576G homozygote participants (a polymorphism in the OT receptor gene)13. Finally, OT has been consistently associated with higher maternal attachment and increased caretaking in humans14,15. The androgen testosterone (T) may also have reinforcing effects in the mesolimbic reward system16. Yet, in contrast to OT it may compromise behaviors associated with maternal care (e.g. nurturing)17. Previous studies have noted a reduced T level during early parenthood and this decline appeared to be associated with the quality of childcare18–22 (but see23).
However, the interactive influence of T and OT on neural processing of the BS currently remains elusive. Only a few behavioral studies have so far assessed their combined influence on aspects of nurturing behaviors. In the context of paternal caretaking behavior Weisman and colleagues24 found that OT administration led to an unexpected short-term alteration of endogenous T, whereby nurturing behaviors like social gaze also increased. Given that women are the main caretakers during the first months after childbirth when infants depend on maternal milk, and yet sometimes show impairments like postpartum depression or child neglect25, it is of crucial importance to take a closer look on these interactive influences in women as well. In nulliparous women a higher endogenous T reduced selective attention to infant targets in the context of adult distractors6. Importantly, attention to infant stimuli increased after OT administration, but only in women with high endogenous T. This suggests that OT may oppose the negative effects of T on nurturing behavior in women6. There are already some indications on the behavioral level that T and OT may have gender-specific impacts on parental behavior. Gordon and colleagues26 found that high T levels in fathers negatively influenced the association between OT and paternal behavior, whereas in mothers high T levels evoked positive associations. Nevertheless, the neural mechanisms underlying the effect of the OT by T interaction on selective attention to infants in general and the highly relevant key stimulus BS in particular remains elusive.
In order to further our understanding of the nature of hormonal interactions in nurturing behavior, the present study used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and a double-blind placebo-controlled OT administration between-subjects protocol to examine the influence of exogenous OT and endogenous T on selective attention to the BS in nulliparous women. To assess selective attention to the BS we used an implicit association task. Implicit association tasks have the advantage that the degree of attentional capture by an infant as opposed to an adult face can be determined implicitly by the mean reaction time (RT) of the participant, with a shorter RT indicating faster attentional processing of the baby schema and increased action. Previous neuroimaging studies combined passive viewing and explicit evaluation tasks (e.g., cuteness perception on a Likert-Scale), which are more vulnerable to experimenter demand effect. Further, these studies do not allow the assessment of the mechanism that promotes faster reactions to infants. Building on previous findings6 we hypothesized that OT administration would compensate the negative effects of high endogenous T on attentional processing of the BS, probably through the modulation of activation in key nodes of the mesolimbic reward system.
Results
Behavioral data
The influence of the endogenous T concentration (measured out of one saliva sample that was collected directly before administration) on attention to babies was analyzed with a 3 (morph type: low BS, natural BS and high BS) × 2 (treatment: OT vs. placebo) × 2 (T: median split of the sample that was collected directly before administration) ANOVA. Of particular interest was the significant three-way interaction between the factors “morph type”, “treatment” and “T” (F2,104 = 4.359; p = 0.015; see Table S1 for details). Building on previous research6 we assumed that women with high endogenous T derive greater benefit from OT treatment than women with low endogenous T and hence will become more sensitive for the BS. We found that OT-treated women with high endogenous T exhibited an attentional preference for the natural BS (positive Delta-RT: MhighT = 6.4 ± SEM 24.2 ms) compared to OT-treated women with low endogenous T (positive Delta-RT: MlowT = −50.6 ± SEM 16 ms) but these results remained on statistical trend level [Bonferroni-corrected statistical threshold was p ≤ 0.0166 (t27 = 1.86, p = 0.03; one-tailed, a priori hypothesis based on6)]. In the placebo group women with low endogenous T directed less attention to the plus morphed babies (MlowT = −39.1 ± SEM 21.3 ms) than women with high T concentrations (MhighT = 23.4 ± SEM 14.6 ms; t25 = 2.29, p = 0.015, one-tailed, a priori hypothesis based on6). The placebo-treated participants with high or low T did not show any differences in the preference for the natural BS.
Drug treatment did not influence the participants RTs to geometric figures. Yet, the type of target (F2,110 = 39.485; p < 0.001; np2 = 0.418) and the interaction between target and distractor (F2,110 = 25.871; p < 0.001; np2 = 0.32) influenced the participants’ RTs in the control task (see also Table S2). These results are consistent with the assumption that simple geometric forms could have emotional meanings and may even trigger an attentional bias27.
Neuroimaging results
When contrasting “baby vs. adult target” for the different morph types, we found an effect of treatment in the direct comparison of the OT and placebo group in the left frontolateral cortex (MNI-coordinates [t-value; pFWEcorr] −33, 5, 49 [4.62; 0.023]), but only for the high BS condition (Fig. 1). The identified region was located at the intersection of the precentral sulcus and inferior frontal sulcus, which corresponds to the inferior frontal junction (IFJ)28. Additional t-tests on the parameter estimates extracted from the left IFJ, confirmed the difference in activation between the OT and the placebo group in the high BS condition (t55 = 4.43; p < 0.001) (Fig. 1). We could not find any difference in the direct comparison of OT > placebo in the unmanipulated or the low BS conditions.
The SPM multiple regression analysis was used to analyze the influence of the degree of selective attention to babies on brain activation. For this, we used the mean Delta RT of all baby conditions from each subject and correlated them with the individual activation in the contrast of “baby vs. adult target”, separately for OT- and for placebo-treated participants. We found increased activation in the right putamen of OT-treated women (MNI-coordinates [t-value; pFWEcorr] 30, −1, 13 [5.65; 0.052]; Fig. 2a) that was positively related to selective attention to babies, but barely missed the FWE-corrected threshold in the whole-brain analysis. Yet, when extracting the parameter estimates from the right putamen, a significant positive correlation with selective attention to babies emerged (Fig. 2a). We also found a positive correlation with activation in the left putamen at the statistical threshold of p < 0.001, uncorrected (MNI-coordinates [t-value] −30, 14, 10 [4.85]; Fig. 2a). In the placebo group, two homologous clusters emerged in the posterior temporal cortex of which one survived the FWE correction on cluster level and one barely missed the statistical threshold (MNI-coordinates [t-value; pFWEcorr] 48, −22, −2 [5.69; 0.067]; MNI-coordinates [t-value; pFWEcorr] −66, −49, −2 [4.99; < 0.001]).
Following the behavioral results, we subdivided the sample in women with either low or high endogenous T concentrations. OT-treated women with high endogenous T showed significantly enhanced activation in the left and right putamen in the contrast of “baby vs. adult target” in association with the individual Delta-RTs as an indicator of enhanced selective attention to babies (i.e., a positive correlation). The positive correlation with the left putamen also surpassed the statistical criterion (MNI-coordinates [t-value; pFWEcorr] −21, 14, −11 [5.97; 0.005]; Fig. 2b), while the one with the right putamen was significant at p < 0.001, uncorrected (MNI-coordinates [t-value] 30, −1, 13 [5.33]; Fig. 2b). OT-treated participants with low endogenous T and participants of the placebo group did not show this positive correlation between activation in the putamen and selective attention to babies. The brain-behavior correlations based on the parameter estimates extracted from the left and the right putamen supported these results (Fig. 2c; Table S3).
Discussion
The current study investigated the influence of exogenous OT and its interaction with endogenous T on brain activity during processing of infant faces, which varied in the intensity of BS. We found a positive effect of drug treatment in the direct comparison of OT versus placebo using the contrast of “baby versus adult target” in the IFJ, but only in the high BS condition. This finding supports the idea that OT may enhance attention to increased BS and may further promote one’s motivation to act. Additionally, we found a positive correlation between selective attention to babies, as indicated by increasing Delta-RTs, and increased activation in the left putamen following OT treatment. Interestingly, this correlation was specific for women with high endogenous T. Collectively, these findings may provide a hint to the neural mechanism by which OT may support the sensitivity for the BS and may counteract the negative effects of T in the modulation of nurturing behavior.
Based on our previous study6 we expected that OT administration would modulate attention to infant faces especially in women with high endogenous T. In line with this hypothesis, we found an interaction between the degree of BS, the treatment group and endogenous T level. OT-treated women with high endogenous T showed an attentional bias towards babies (positive Delta-RTs) compared to OT-treated women with low endogenous T (negative Delta-RTs), but only for the natural BS and this result remained on statistical trend level. We could not find further differences in the attention to babies between high and low endogenous T concentrations in the OT group, but the Delta RTs of the conditions with lower and higher BS were positive in both T groups. We suppose that adult pictures could also represent salient stimuli for nulliparous women in reproductive age, which may have rendered any behavioral effects rather small (see8 for review). Yet, the presence of a statistical trend in the OT-treated women with high endogenous T may conform to the idea that a system influenced by high levels of T may be more receptive to OT treatment, because in the female brain T could be directly converted into estradiol and thus modulate OT receptor density, which has been demonstrated in rats29.
We found a positive effect of treatment (OT > placebo) on processing of targets with higher BS in the left IFJ. The IFJ is located in the frontolateral cortex between the premotor and the prefrontal cortex and has been implicated in cognitive control30,31. Recent research further demonstrated its specific involvement in the control of selective visual attention32, action perception33 and the detection of behaviorally salient cues34. In addition, the IFJ has been associated with the understanding of action35, which may be considered as one basis of social cognition and empathy. Previous studies also detected increased activation during processing of infant faces in areas near our IFJ cluster. Caria and colleagues36 located activation in the precentral cortex (BA 6; MNI coordinates: 36–6 45) in the contrast of “unknown infant > adult faces”. A study on synchrony and specificity between the maternal and paternal brain found activation in the inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) for both sexes in response to own-infant-parent-interaction videos37. Finally, the IFG has also been related to OT- induced emotional empathy38. In our study, the activation of the IFJ was limited to babies with a high BS. Previous observations indicating that babies with stronger BS were treated preferentially39 and perceived as cuter40,41 provide some space for speculation, that the stronger BS could provoke an increased motivation to act. The findings of Glocker et al.10 who also found activation in the precentral gyrus (MNI coordinates: −42 0 29; −46 6 33; −51 8 36) across the three BS conditions, which was located near our cluster, could also provide indications for this speculation. Yet, it is difficult to distinguish between participants who searched for the target or avoided the distractors, which makes it difficult to interpret the difference between the OT group (positive parameter estimates) and the placebo group (negative parameter estimates). This would be of great interest and should be taken into account in a future study.
Additionally, we observed a positive correlation between activity in the left putamen and increased selective attention to babies in OT-treated women. The putamen belongs to the mesolimbic reward system and is part of the striatum. Equivalent to many other studies the detected activation appeared only in participants that were treated with OT15. We suppose that this finding supports the idea that OT increases the reward value of babies, probably as an adaptation to motivate caretaking behavior through mesolimbic-reward pathways37. This assessment is also supported by previous reports that have demonstrated OT-dependent activations in areas of the reward system as consequence of listening to crying babies (amygdala and insula42), to laughing babies (amygdala43) and processing of child pictures (globus pallidus44). Yet, the OT-related response in the putamen did not parametrically change with the amount of BS inherent to the presented pictures. This indicates a rather general mechanism that may ensure reflexive attention to babies regardless of their cuteness.
Even more importantly, the positive correlation between increased selective attention to babies and activation in the left putamen only remained significant in the high endogenous T group after OT administration. Only a few behavioral studies have so far examined the interactions between OT and T in women, and, to the best of our knowledge, our study is the first that used functional neuroimaging to assess the underlying neural mechanism. Here we used the T sample that was collected directly before administration to capture the current T state of the participant and not the habitual concentration, because we could not control for daytime fluctuations. This method also allows the comparability with previous research26,45,46. The single T sample collection before administration provided the opportunity to analyze the real-time T concentration at the measuring time. But, for the same reasons, we cannot exclude further T fluctuations that might have occurred during scanning, which may have weakened the stability of this single measurement and might therefore constitute a potential limitation of this study. However, the T concentration before administration and post-test concentration after measurement were highly correlated (r = 0.447; p < 0.001). This may suggest that T concentration may have been relatively stable during the actual scanning period.
The present study shows that the reward system may be involved in this attentional bias towards babies. Little is known about the precise interaction of OT and T in the brain. In the female brain the majority of T may be converted to estradiol via aromatase47, and estradiol can increase the number of OT receptors in the brain29. Yet, the observation of higher systemic T concentrations in women may indicate an impaired ability to convert it to estradiol, thus either leaving a higher amount of T in the circuitry that may counteract maternal behavior through an unknown mechanism or by leaving the number of OT receptors unaffected by reduced influence of estradiol. In a similar vein one may speculate that high OT concentrations may serve as a compensatory mechanism for high T concentrations, we presume that women with low T concentrations were less sensitive to OT administration, because low T concentrations should support nurturing behavior per se6. This would indeed explain why we could not find any activation in the brain reward system in women with low endogenous T after OT administration. But since babies nevertheless remain a strong social cue, this does not explain why neither in the low T nor in the placebo group the baby pictures were rewarding by themselves. We suppose that the young adult stimuli may also be socially salient for young participants, for instance as potential mates, and therefore could be equally rewarding and evoke similar activation in the brain reward system48. Our experimental design does not allow the isolation of the response to adult or infant faces, since pictures were always presented in combination, but only enables the identification of activational differences during processing of these stimuli. So we cannot answer this question. However, for reasons discussed before we presume that the brain of women with a high endogenous T concentration may be more sensitive to OT administration than women with a low endogenous T. One could therefore speculate that same age adult stimuli as well as infant pictures both constitute a strong social stimulus, but OT could function as an enhancer of reproductive behavior and direct attention towards infant stimuli in women with high T level, who may be more sensitive to OT. In this study, we focused on nulliparous women that reacted to unfamiliar infant or adult stimuli to investigate the neuroendocrine basics of the key stimulus in general. As oxytocin is especially known to influence attachment behavior15 it could be of great interest to use the same paradigm with familiar infants in further research. The results of this study are therefore limited to the general mechanism of the key stimulus BS and are not necessarily transferable to attachment behavior in mothers or other caretakers. Gordon et al.26 found a positive interaction between increased T levels and OT on maternal touch in mothers, while the effect in fathers was in the opposite direction. Their results may indicate that the interaction between T and OT may be sexually dimorphic. Therefore, our results on nulliparous women may resemble those in mothers but it is possible that they deviate to the results in fathers and men who have never sired a child. Although the findings of Gordon and colleagues26 were focused on parents, we assume that the results in mothers support our previous findings that OT seemed to promote adult-infant discrimination and increased attention to baby pictures, but only in women with high endogenous T6. OT is a peptide hormone that is critically involved in mother-infant attachment49. Especially lactating mothers show increased OT levels50. The artificial increase through intranasal administration could be closely related to the concentration found in new mothers (see Weisman et al.51 for the active drug effects reflected in saliva). Although there already exists excellent research about the active drug effects and reflection in saliva49–51, a direct comparison of mothers and non-mothers salivary OT concentration with and without OT administration would be of great interest. But yet, aside from oxytocin, postpartum hormonal changes have large transitions which makes the results of nulliparous women not necessarily comparable with mothers52,53.
The detected activations in the putamen correspond well with the literature and accentuate the previous research on oxytocin and maternal behavior8,9,54. Since we know very little about the interactions of T and OT in the female brain, we abstained from using an a priori region-of-interest approach and analyzed our data on whole brain level. However, findings in the IFJ are not common in the context of OT administration studies and in an a priori region-of-interest approach the IFJ would have probably not been included. This might limit the interpretation of this finding, although the activation of the IFJ survived the correction for multiple testing on a whole-brain basis. Future studies will therefore be necessary to show if this finding can be replicated in this context.
All of the participating women used hormonal contraceptives and were tested in the pill-phase. This was done to control for a potential pregnancy and to prevent cyclic changes in steroid hormone levels. But it’s important to note that previous research showed that the intake of oral contraceptives increased performance in affective responsiveness and that affective responsiveness was positively influenced by oral contraceptives (pill-intake phase versus pill-free week)55. Further, testosterone in females on oral contraceptives may be downregulated56. Therefore, using participants on hormonal contraceptives for an OT-intervention study may not represent the ideal model to detect OT sensitivity, that may be modulated by T. Yet, ethical concerns (increased pregnancy risk in women with a natural cycle), the fact that T level fluctuates across the natural cycle (e.g., rises during the first half), as well as our previous results in women on oral contraceptives6, led to our decision to test only women who received hormonal contraception in this between-subjects fMRI design. As a future direction, it would be of great interest to further examine normal cycling women and the influence of the intake of oral contraceptives on the recent results.
Collectively, the present findings support the idea of an adaptive hormonal mechanism that promotes selective attention to babies. Here, we demonstrate that an increased BS was associated with enhanced activation of the IFJ after OT treatment, a brain region implicated in cognitive control and the motivation to act. We also found that OT may modulate selective attention to babies through the recruitment of the reward system. We found a positive correlation between activity in the putamen and attention to baby faces. However, after accounting for endogenous T level, this correlation only remained significant in women with a high T concentration. These results are consistent with our previous findings6 and may indicate that OT possibly compensates high T concentrations and the reduced attention to infant stimuli through an enhancement of the reward value of babies.
Material and Methods
Participants
Sixty nulliparous female university students (mean age ± SD = 24.63 ± 3.08 years) participated in the study. All participants were right handed, healthy, and were not taking any medication except from hormonal contraceptives to prevent cyclic changes in T level. They also performed a pregnancy test on the test day and met the criteria to participate in an fMRI study.
Subjects provided written- informed consent and were paid for participation. The study was approved by the local ethics committee of the Ärztekammer Hamburg. All methods were performed in accordance with the relevant guidelines and regulations.
Experimental Procedure and Paradigm
Administration of OT was placebo-controlled and double-blind (see Fig. 3 for experimental schedule). Participants brought three saliva samples from home sampling and provided two further saliva samples at the test place (one before administration and one after measurement). In the scanner participants performed two versions of a target detection paradigm (see also Supplementary Material and Methods).
The target detection paradigm was based on the odd-one-out principle (Fig. 4 for schematic illustration). In the first task subjects were asked to identify the odd-one-out on a display of four human faces, which was either an adult face (target) out of three infant faces (distractors) or an infant target out of three adult distractors, as fast and accurate as possible. The BS of the infant faces was parametrically manipulated (see also6 for the procedure). The same odd-one-out procedure was repeated in the baseline task, in which task geometric shapes (triangles, squares, or circles) were used (Supplementary Material and Methods for detailed information). The first paradigm was 16.4 minutes long and the baseline task was 6.4 minutes long.
Hormone Samples
The salivary T samples were collected on the test day and analyzed in our in-house laboratory with a T luminescence immunoassay from IBL International (TECAN group global; Hamburg, Germany) (see Supplementary Material and Methods for detailed information).
Hormone administration
OT was administered double-blind and placebo controlled in a between-subjects design. The participants self-administered 3 puffs in each nostril alternately of the unlabeled nasal spray. The amount of OT corresponded to 24 IU. The placebo spray consisted of chlorobutanol-hemihydrat (0.5%) with no active treatment. To ensure treatment efficiency57, exposure time was 45 min until the fMRI measurement began.
Behavioral data analysis
57 participants were considered for analysis (29 were in the OT group and 28 were in the placebo group; for further details see Supplementary Material and Methods).
IBM SPSS statistics 19 was used to analyze the behavioral data. To represent selective attention to babies of different BS conditions, the relative reaction times (Deltas-RTs) were calculated by subtracting the low distracting condition (i.e., infant target and three adult distractors) from the high distracting condition (i.e., adult target and three baby faces as distractors) (see also6). For a subject with an increased sensitivity for the BS, we predicted higher Delta-RTs, resulting from the combined effect of increased distraction by infant faces in the adult target condition and more rapid selection of the infant target in the first condition.
Following the procedures used previously26,46,58,our analysis of T content focused on the one saliva sample that was collected directly before treatment. Based on the endogenous T concentration, we calculated the median to separate the participants in two groups of either high or low endogenous T concentration (see6 for a similar procedure).
We used a repeated measures ANOVA and post hoc t-tests to assess the effects of the factors ‘morph type’ (low BS, unmanipulated BS, high BS), ’treatment group’ (OT or placebo) and ‘endogenous T concentration’ (high or low T) on the Delta-RTs. Bonferroni correction yielded a corrected statistical threshold of p ≤ 0.0166 and a corresponding statistical trend level of p ≤ 0.033.
For analysis of the baseline task with geometrical shapes we performed a repeated measures ANOVA with the factors ‘target figure’ (triangles, squares, or circles), ‘distractor figures’ (triangles, squares, or circles) and ‘treatment group’ (OT or placebo) on the RTs.
Statistical effects are considered significant at p < 0.05 (two-tailed), if not otherwise indicated. If the sphericity assumption was not met in the ANOVA, we report the Greenhouse-Geisser corrected values. Since all data followed the assumed normal distribution, we used post-hoc t-tests.
fMRI data analysis
Participants performed the paradigm in a 3 Tesla Siemens fMRI Scanner at the University Medical Center Hamburg-Eppendorf (see Supplementary Material and Methods for details).
On the first level, we used a general linear model (GLM) for statistical analysis of event-related activity. A vector with the temporal onsets of the experimental conditions was convolved with a canonical hemodynamic response function (hrf) to produce the predicted hemodynamic response to each experimental condition. The conditions ‘target’ (adult or infant), ‘morph type’ of the baby (higher, unmanipulated or lower BS), ‘gender’ (female or male) and the baseline task with the geometric figures were modeled as regressors. Linear t-contrasts were defined for assessing the specific effects of the varying amount of BS.
On the second level, a random effects analysis was performed. For this we used a full factorial 3 by 2 repeated-measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) that included the within-subject factor ‘morph type’ (3 steps) and the between-subject factor ‘treatment group’ (2 steps). T-tests tested for specific differences between conditions.
We also assessed whole-brain correlations for analysis of the association between treatment-related activations and selective attention to babies (Delta RTs). For this purpose we used the multiple regression routine implemented in SPM8. For the display of these behavior-brain correlations individual parameter estimates were extracted with marsbar-0.44.
A whole-brain correction for multiple testing using the family-wise error (FWE) on the cluster level was applied to all analyses, if not indicated otherwise. For display purposes we used a threshold of p < 0.001, uncorrected.
Data availability
All data analyzed and generated during this study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.
Electronic supplementary material
Acknowledgements
The authors would like to thank C. Büchel for providing us with the opportunity to use the 3 Tesla Siemens fMRI Scanner at the University Medical Center Hamburg-Eppendorf. We would further like to thank K. Bergholz and K. Wendt for assisting with the MRI scans, the MR physicians of the Institute of Systems Neuroscience for the MR-instruction of the participants, M. Langbehn for support in programming of the test protocol, A. Kroll for the hormone analysis, and L. Fröling for her contribution in the data acquisition.
Author Contributions
S.C.K.H. and E.K.D. designed the study. S.C.K.H. performed the research. E.K.D. supervised the research. A.T.E.G. and E.B. supervised the administration of the treatment and helped revising the manuscript. R.P. programmed the analysis batches and helped in the analysis of the data and revising the manuscript. S.C.K.H. and E.K.D. analyzed the data and wrote the original manuscript. All authors have given approval to the final version of the manuscript.
Competing Interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Electronic supplementary material
Supplementary information accompanies this paper at 10.1038/s41598-018-26020-4.
Publisher's note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
References
- 1.Lorenz K. Die angeborenen Formen möglicher Erfahrung. Z. Tierpsychol. 1943;5:235–409. doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.1943.tb00655.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Gittleman JL, Thompson D. Energy Allocation in Mammalian. Am. Zool. 1988;28:863–875. doi: 10.1093/icb/28.3.863. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Cárdenas RA, Harris LJ, Becker MW. Sex differences in visual attention toward infant faces. Evol. Hum. Behav. 2013;34:280–287. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2013.04.001. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Golle J, Probst F, Mast FW, Lobmaier JS. Preference for cute infants does not depend on their ethnicity or species: evidence from hypothetical adoption and donation paradigms. PLoS One. 2015;10:e0121554. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0121554. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Brosch T, Sander D, Scherer KR. That baby caught my eye… Attention capture by infant faces. Emotion. 2007;7:685–689. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.7.3.685. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Holtfrerich SKC, Schwarz KA, Sprenger C, Reimers L, Diekhof EK. Endogenous testosterone and exogenous oxytocin modulate attentional processing of infant faces. PLoS One. 2016;11:1–19. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0166617. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Glocker ML, et al. Baby schema in infant faces induces cuteness perception and motivation for caretaking in adults. Ethology. 2009;115:257–263. doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.2008.01603.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Luo L, et al. Neural systems and hormones mediating attraction to infant and child faces. Front. Psychol. 2015;6:1–22. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00970. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Kringelbach ML, Stark EA, Alexander C, Bornstein MH, Stein A. On Cuteness: Unlocking the Parental Brain and Beyond. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2016;20:545–558. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2016.05.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Glocker ML, et al. Baby schema modulates the brain reward system in nulliparous women. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 2009;106:9115–9119. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0811620106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Insel TR, Shapiro LE. Oxytocin receptor distribution reflects social organization in monogamous and polygamous voles. Neurobiology. 1992;89:5981–5985. doi: 10.1073/pnas.89.13.5981. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Gregory R, Cheng H, Rupp HA, Sengelaub DR, Heiman JR. Oxytocin increases VTA activation to infant and sexual stimuli in nulliparous and postpartum women. Horm. Behav. 2015;69:82–88. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2014.12.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Marsh AA, et al. The Influence of Oxytocin Administration on Responses to Infant Faces and Potential Moderation by OXTR Genotype. Psychopharmacol. 2012;224:469–476. doi: 10.1007/s00213-012-2775-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Levine A, Zagoory-Sharon O, Feldman R, Weller A. Oxytocin during pregnancy and early postpartum: Individual patterns and maternal-fetal attachment. Peptides. 2007;28:1162–1169. doi: 10.1016/j.peptides.2007.04.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Wigton R, et al. Neurophysiological effects of acute oxytocin administration: Systematic review and meta-analysis of placebo-controlled imaging studies. J. Psychiatry Neurosci. 2015;40:1–22. doi: 10.1503/jpn.130289. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Wood RI, Johnson LR, Chu L, Schad C, Self DW. Testosterone reinforcement: intravenous and intracerebroventricular self - administration in male rats and hamsters. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2004;171:298–305. doi: 10.1007/s00213-003-1587-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Harris JA, Rushton JP, Hampson E, Jackson DN. Salivary testosterone and self report aggressive and pro social personality characteristics in men and women. Aggress.Behav. 1996;22:321–331. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-2337(1996)22:5<321::AID-AB1>3.0.CO;2-M. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Gettler LT, McDade TW, Feranil AB, Kuzawa CW. Longitudinal evidence that fatherhood decreases testosterone in human males. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 2011;108:16194–16199. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1105403108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Kuzawa CW, Gettler LT, Muller MN, McDade TW, Feranil AB. Fatherhood, pairbonding and testosterone in the Philippines. Horm. Behav. 2009;56:429–435. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2009.07.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Saxbe DE, et al. Fathers’ decline in testosterone and synchrony with partner testosterone during pregnancy predicts greater postpartum relationship investment. Horm. Behav. 2017;90:39–47. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2016.07.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Kuzawa CW, Gettler LT, Huang Yyen, McDade TW. Mothers have lower testosterone than non-mothers: Evidence from the Philippines. Horm. Behav. 2010;57:441–447. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2010.01.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Barrett ES, et al. Marriage and motherhood are associated with lower testosterone concentrations in women. Horm. Behav. 2013;63:72–79. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2012.10.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Mazur A. Testosterone of young husbands rises with children in the home. Andrology. 2014;2:125–129. doi: 10.1111/j.2047-2927.2013.00164.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Weisman O, Zagoory-Sharon O, Feldman R. Oxytocin administration, salivary testosterone, and father-infant social behavior. Prog. Neuro-Psychopharmacology Biol. Psychiatry. 2014;49:47–52. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2013.11.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Johnson JG, et al. Association of Maladaptive Parental Behavior With Psychiatric Disorder Among Parents and Their Offspring. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry. 2001;58:453. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.58.5.453. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Gordon I, Pratt M, Bergunde K, Zagoory-Sharon O, Feldman R. Testosterone, oxytocin, and the development of human parental care. Horm. Behav. 2017;93:184–192. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2017.05.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Larson CL, Aronoff J, Stearns JJ. The shape of threat: simple geometric forms evoke rapid and sustained capture of attention. Emotion. 2007;7:526–534. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.7.3.526. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Derrfuss J, Brass M, von Cramon DY, Lohmann G, Amunts K. Neural activations at the junction of the inferior frontal sulcus and the inferior precentral sulcus: Interindividual variability, reliability, and association with sulcal morphology. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2009;30:299–311. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20501. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.de Kloet R, Voorhuis DAM, Boschma Y, Elands J. Estradiol Modulates Density of Putative Oxytocin Receptors’ in Discrete Rat Brain Regions. Neuroendocrinology. 2008;44:415–421. doi: 10.1159/000124680. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Adolphs R. Cognitive neuroscience: Cognitive neuroscience of human social behaviour. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2003;4:165–178. doi: 10.1038/nrn1056. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Brass M, Derrfuss J, Forstmann B, Von Cramon DY. The role of the inferior frontal junction area in cognitive control. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2005;9:314–316. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.05.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Baldauf D, Desimone R. Neural mechanisms of object-based attention. Science (80-.). 2014;344:424–427. doi: 10.1126/science.1247003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Avenanti A, Urgesi C. Understanding ‘what’ others do: Mirror mechanisms play a crucial role in action perception. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2011;6:257–259. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsr004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Muhle-Karbe PS, Andres M, Brass M. Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation Dissociates Prefrontal and Parietal Contributions to Task Preparation. J. Neurosci. 2014;34:12481–12489. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4931-13.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Rizzolatti G, Fogassi L, Gallese V. Motor and cognitive functions of the ventral premotor cortex. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2002;12:149–154. doi: 10.1016/S0959-4388(02)00308-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Caria A, et al. Species-specific response to human infant faces in the premotor cortex. Neuroimage. 2012;60:884–893. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.12.068. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Atzil S, Hendler T, Zagoory-Sharon O, Winetraub Y, Feldman R. Synchrony and specificity in the maternal and the paternal brain: Relations to oxytocin and vasopressin. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry. 2012;51:798–811. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2012.06.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Shamay-Tsoory SG. The Neural Bases for Empathy. Neurosci. 2011;17:18–24. doi: 10.1177/1073858410379268. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Hildebrandt KA, Fitzgerald HE. The infant’s physical attractiveness: Its effect on bonding and attachment. Infant Ment. Health J. 1983;4:1–12. [Google Scholar]
- 40.Little AC. Manipulation of infant-like traits affects perceived cuteness of infant, adult and cat faces. Ethology. 2012;118:775–782. doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.2012.02068.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Borgi M, Cogliati-Dezza I, Brelsford V, Meints K, Cirulli F. Baby schema in human and animal faces induces cuteness perception and gaze allocation in children. Front. Psychol. 2014;5:1–12. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00411. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Riem MME, et al. Oxytocin modulates amygdala, insula, and inferior frontal gyrus responses to infant crying: A randomized controlled trial. Biol. Psychiatry. 2011;70:291–297. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.02.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Riem MME, et al. No laughing matter: Intranasal oxytocin administration changes functional brain connectivity during exposure to infant laughter. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2012;37:1257–1266. doi: 10.1038/npp.2011.313. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Wittfoth-Schardt D, et al. Oxytocin Modulates Neural Reactivity to Children’s Faces as a Function of Social Salience. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2012;37:1799–1807. doi: 10.1038/npp.2012.47. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Weisman O, et al. Oxytocin shapes parental motion during father-infant interaction. Biol. Lett. 2013;9:20130828–20130828. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2013.0828. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Doi H, et al. Neural correlates of babyish adult face processing in men. Neuropsychologia. 2017;97:9–17. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.01.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Simpson ER, et al. Aromatase—A Brief Overview. Annu. Rev. Physiol. 2002;64:93–127. doi: 10.1146/annurev.physiol.64.081601.142703. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Kampe KKW, Frith CD, Dolan RJ, Frith U. Psychology: Reward value of attractiveness and gaze. Nature. 2001;413:589–589. doi: 10.1038/35098149. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Feldman R, Gordon I, Zagoory-Sharon O. Maternal and paternal plasma, salivary, and urinary oxytocin and parent-infant synchrony: Considering stress and affiliation components of human bonding. Dev. Sci. 2011;14:752–761. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.01021.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.White-Traut R, et al. Detection of salivary oxytocin levels in lactating women. Dev. Psychobiol. 2009;51:367–373. doi: 10.1002/dev.20376. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Weisman O, Zagoory-Sharon O, Feldman R. Intranasal oxytocin administration is reflected in human saliva. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2012;37:1582–1586. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2012.02.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Gammie SC, Driessen TM, Zhao C, Saul MC, Eisinger BE. Genetic and neuroendocrine regulation of the postpartum brain. Front. Neuroendocrinol. 2016;42:1–17. doi: 10.1016/j.yfrne.2016.05.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Hendrick V, Altshuler LL, Suri R. Hormonal changes in the postpartum and implications for postpartum depression. Psychosomatics. 1998;39:93–101. doi: 10.1016/S0033-3182(98)71355-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Feldman R. The adaptive human parental brain: Implications for children’s social development. Trends Neurosci. 2015;38:387–399. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2015.04.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Radke S, Derntl B. Affective responsiveness is influenced by intake of oral contraceptives. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 2016;26:1014–1019. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2016.03.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Zimmerman Y, Eijkemans MJC, Coelingh Bennink HJT, Blankenstein MA, Fauser BCJM. The effect of combined oral contraception on testosterone levels in healthy women: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Hum. Reprod. Update. 2014;20:76–105. doi: 10.1093/humupd/dmt038. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Born J, et al. Sniffing neuropeptides: a transnasal approach to the human brain. Nat. Neurosci. 2002;5:514–516. doi: 10.1038/nn0602-849. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Weisman O, Zagoory-sharon O, Feldman R. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry Oxytocin administration, salivary testosterone, and father – infant social behavior. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry. 2014;49:47–52. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2013.11.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
All data analyzed and generated during this study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.