Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2019 Jun 1.
Published in final edited form as: Drug Alcohol Depend. 2018 Mar 31;187:100–108. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2018.01.037

Associations between pain intensity and urge to smoke: testing the role of negative affect and pain catastrophizing

Jesse D Kosiba 1, Emily L Zale 2, Joseph W Ditre 1,3
PMCID: PMC5959791  NIHMSID: NIHMS959370  PMID: 29655030

1. Introduction

Cigarette smoking remains the leading cause of preventable death worldwide (WHO, 2008). In the United States, an estimated 42.1 million adults (~18%) are classified as current tobacco smokers (CDC, 2014; USDHHS, 2014). Although the majority of smokers report a desire to stop smoking, the most common result of a quit attempt is relapse (CDC, 2011; Hughes et al., 2004; Piasecki, 2006). Relapse is more likely to occur among individuals reporting higher (compared to lower) pre-quit urge to smoke (Allen et al., 2008; Nakamura et al., 2014). Urge to smoke has been defined as the acute desire for the negatively reinforcing effects and rewarding properties of tobacco and is positively associated with nicotine dependence (Cox et al., 2001). Given the importance of urge to smoke in the maintenance of tobacco dependence, researchers have recently turned their attention to pain as a motivator of tobacco smoking and proximal determinant of relapse (Ditre et al., 2017).

Pain and tobacco smoking has been linked to the clinical and empirical literature for decades (e.g., Nesbitt, 1973; Zale et al., 2016). Epidemiological and clinical data indicate that the prevalence of smoking among persons in pain may be greater than twice the rate observed in the general population (Zvolensky et al., 2009), and an evolving reciprocal model posits that pain and smoking interact in the manner of a positive feedback loop, resulting in greater pain and the maintenance of tobacco dependence (Ditre et al., 2011; Zale et al., 2016). Consistent with this perspective, there is evidence of covariation between pain and tobacco smoking. The frequency of pain has been positively correlated with cigarette consumption (Pisinger et al., 2011), and smokers who endorse past-week pain have been shown to smoke more cigarettes per day than smokers with no pain (Hahn et al., 2006). Furthermore, current pain intensity has been positively associated with current smoking status (Volkman et al., 2015) and daily cigarette consumption (Andersson et al., 1998; Bakhshaie et al., 2016; Pirouzi et al., 2011).

There is also evidence that pain can be a potent motivator for smoking urge and behavior. For example, smokers in pain readily endorse the use of tobacco to cope with pain (Hooten, Vickers et al., 2011), painful episodes have been shown to precede bouts of smoking (Dhingra et al., 2013), and smokers have reported a perceived need to smoke in the context of clinical pain (Jamison et al., 1991). The results of two laboratory studies indicate a causal effect of pain on the urge to smoke. Ditre and Brandon (2008) showed that cold-pressor pain induction (vs. no pain induction) increased the urge and decreased latency to smoking among a sample of daily tobacco smokers who were recruited from the local community. A recent follow-up study further demonstrated that thermal heat pain (vs. no pain) increased urge to smoke among tobacco users recruited from the local community (Parkerson and Asmundson, 2016). Given that pain is comprised of sensory-physiological (e.g., pain intensity), motivational-affective (e.g., state negative affect), and cognitive-evaluative (e.g., pain catastrophizing) components (IASP, 2008; Melzack and Wall, 1965; Turk and Melzack, 2001), additional research is needed to examine the role of these factors in relations between pain and urge to smoke tobacco.

Negative affect refers to a general form of subjective distress that encompasses a variety of unpleasant emotions (Kassel et al., 2003; Watson et al., 1988). Negative affect has been conceptualized in terms of both trait and state experiences (Watson and Clark, 1984), and measures of state negative affect have been more closely linked to current pain intensity than trait measures (Gaskin et al., 1992). Although negative affect is empirically-distinguishable from pain intensity (Dworkin et al., 2005), it frequently co-occurs with pain (Jensen and Karoly, 2011; Melzack, 1975), and has been positively associated with both pain intensity and tolerance (Janssen, 2002; Lacourt et al., 2015). Importantly, Ditre and Brandon (2008) found that state negative affect accounted for approximately 30% of the variance in the effects of experimental pain induction on self-reported urge to smoke. This observation is broadly consistent with the negative reinforcement model of tobacco dependence, which posits that escape from negative affect is a primary catalyst for smoking urge and behavior (Baker et al., 2004b).

Pain catastrophizing is a cognitive-affective construct that reflects the tendency to interpret actual or anticipated pain in an exaggerated manner (Sullivan et al., 1995). Pain catastrophizing exhibits a wide distribution and is positively associated with physical and emotional distress in response to acute pain, among non-chronic pain samples (Sullivan et al., 1995; Kristiansen et al., 2014). Furthermore, catastrophizing has been positively associated with current pain ratings among non-chronic pain samples (Bialosky et al., 2008; Spanos et al., 1981; Sullivan et al., 1995), and individuals who score higher on measures of catastrophizing tend to report greater fear and worry about their pain (Edwards et al., 2011; Quartana et al., 2009). Interestingly, smokers have been shown to endorse greater pain catastrophizing than nonsmokers (Hooten et al., 2009), and cigarette smoking has been conceptualized as a behavioral coping strategy that is used to manage aversive internal experiences (Baker et al., 2004b; Lazarus and Folkman, 1984). Thus, smokers who are prone to greater pain catastrophizing may experience greater urge to smoke in the context of pain.

Previous studies that directly tested associations between pain and urge to smoke tobacco relied almost exclusively on experimental pain paradigms that are subject to limitations in terms of experimenter demand and generalizability (Ditre and Brandon, 2008; Ditre et al., 2015; Zale et al., 2016). Furthermore, most of the published cross-sectional studies investigating the relationship between pain and tobacco use have been conducted among smokers with chronic pain. Evidence that such associations extend to individuals without chronic pain would have clinical implications for many tobacco smokers (Nahin, 2015; Zvolensky et al., 2009). Thus, the current study was conceptualized as a cross-sectional extension of this formative laboratory work, with the goal of examining the association between current pain intensity (i.e., pain that is not experienced in the context of clinical pain or experimental pain induction) and self-reported urge to smoke, among a sample of daily cigarette smokers who did not report chronic pain. Given contemporary negative reinforcement models of addiction motivation (Baker et al., 2004a) and empirical work implicating negative affect in bidirectional relations between pain and smoking (Ditre et al., 2011; Zale et al., 2016), we were especially interested in the association between current pain intensity and self-reported urge to smoke for the purpose of relieving negative affect.

We hypothesized that current pain intensity would be positively associated with scores on self-report measures of overall urge to smoke and urge to smoke for the relief of negative affect. We also hypothesized an indirect association via state negative affect, such that greater pain would be associated with the greater state negative affect, which in turn would be associated with greater urge. Finally, we hypothesized that pain catastrophizing would moderate the association between pain intensity and urge to smoke, such that the relationship between pain intensity and urge scores would be greater among smokers who endorse high levels of catastrophizing.

2. Method

2.1. Participants

Participants (N = 229) were recruited from the greater Syracuse, New York area via local newspapers and internet advertisements (i.e., online versions of local newspapers, Craigslist advertisements) for a larger experimental study requiring two visits. Current analyses were conducted using data obtained from the baseline survey at the first visit. Potential participants were screened by phone for the following inclusion criteria: (1) current age between 18–65; (2) ability to speak and read English; and (3) currently smoking a mean of ≥ 15 cigarette per day. Participants were excluded if they endorsed: (1) current chronic pain, (2) current use of prescription pain medications, (3) pepper allergy (contraindicated for the larger experimental study), or (4) actively engaging a current attempt to reduce or quit smoking. Eligible participants were scheduled for a baseline visit, at which time they provided informed consent and completed a baseline assessment that included measures of current pain, state negative affect, state urge to smoke, and trait catastrophizing. Both negative affect and urge to smoke have been shown to fluctuate over short periods of time (Morgan et al., 1999) and state negative affect has been positively associated the with urge to smoke in laboratory settings (Ditre and Brandon, 2008). Thus, we selected measures that reflect current experience. Smoking status was biochemically-verified via expired breath carbon monoxide (CO ≥ 8ppm; Benowitz et al., 2002). Exclusion based on smoking status was based solely on biochemical verification, regardless of self-reported smoking status.

2.2. Measures

2.2.1. Pain Intensity

Current pain intensity was rated on a Numerical Rating Scale (NRS; McCaffery and Beebe, 1989) that ranged from 0 (no pain) to 10 (pain as bad as you can imagine). The instructions asked participants to rate their pain at the time of assessment (i.e., “Please rate your pain by selecting the one number that tells how much pain you have right now.”). The NRS has been used extensively to index pain in both research and clinical settings and has demonstrated excellent construct and discriminant validity (Hjermstad et al., 2011; Williamson and Hoggart, 2005).

2.2.2. Nicotine Dependence

The Heaviness of Smoking Index (HSI; Heatherton et al., 1989) is comprised of two items: cigarettes smoked per day, and time to the first cigarette after waking. The HSI has demonstrated excellent sensitivity and specificity (Huang et al., 2008), and good reliability over time (Borland et al., 2010). The HSI is highly correlated with the widely used Fagerstrom Test for Cigarette Dependence (FTCD) and avoids potential confounds associated with the additional items included in the FTCD (Chabrol et al., 2005; Etter et al., 1999; Fagerstrom et al., 2012).

2.2.3. State Negative Affect

The Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS; Watson et al., 1988) is a measure of positive and negative emotions, that asks participants to rate the extent to which they are experiencing each emotion at the time of the assessment (i.e., “right now”).The negative affect subscale (PANAS-NA), which consists of 10 adjectives that describe a range of current negative emotions (e.g., “distressed”, “nervous”, and “irritable”). Each adjective is rated on a 5-point Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (very slightly or not at all) to 5 (extremely). Items are summed to generate a total score (range = 10 – 50), with higher scores indicating greater state negative affect. The PANAS-NA has demonstrated good internal reliability and validity (Watson et al., 1988), and exhibited good internal consistency in the current sample (α = .89).

2.2.4. Pain Catastrophizing

The Pain Catastrophizing Scale (PCS; Sullivan et al., 1995) measures the tendency to interpret pain in an exaggerated or threatening manner and yields a total score that reflects a sense of helplessness in managing pain, exaggeration of the threat value of pain, and heightened attention toward pain (Chaves and Brown, 1987; Rosenstiel and Keefe, 1983; Spanos et al., 1981). Each of the 13 items is rated on a 5-point Likert-type scale ranging from 0 (not at all) to 4 (all the time). Items are summed to yield a total score that ranges from 0 – 52, with higher scores indicating more threatening interpretations of pain. High levels of pain catastrophizing can be indexed by scores of 30 or more on the PCS (Sullivan et al., 1995). The PCS has exhibited good reliability and internal consistency among community samples without chronic pain (Osman et al., 2000; Sullivan et al., 1995). Participants were asked to report on the degree to which they have each thought or feeling when experiencing pain. The PCS demonstrated excellent internal consistency in the current sample (α = .95).

2.2.5. Urge to Smoke

The Questionnaire of Smoking Urges-Brief (QSU-B; Cox et al., 2001) includes 10 items (e.g., “I have an urge for a cigarette.”) that are rated on a 7-point Likert type scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree). Items are summed to yield a total score (QSU-B total) and two subscale scores; urge to smoke for pleasure/reward (QSUB-PR) and urge to smoke for negative affect relief (QSU-B-NA). Higher scores indicate greater urge to smoke and the QSU-B total scores range from 10 to 70. The subscale scores range from 5 to 35. Participants were instructed to report on their urge to smoke “right now” for all items. The QSU-B has demonstrated good reliability and internal consistency (Cox et al., 2001; West and Ussher, 2010), and we observed evidence of good internal consistency in the current sample (QSU-B total score α = .93; QSU-B-NA α = .88).

2.3. Data Analytic Strategy

All analyses were conducted using SPSS Statistics 21 (IBM Corp, 2012). First, descriptive statistics were generated to characterize sample demographics and smoking history. The distributions of all continuous variables were examined for normality, and positive skewness was observed for current pain intensity scores (skew = 1.28). A log transformation was applied by adding one to all scores (given that the range of values included zero) and applying a logarithmic transformation (log10+1) Tabachnick and Fidell, 2004), which resulted in a normalized distribution (skew = 0.24). Sociodemographic and smoking variables were selected as covariates based on observed relations with urge to smoke scores, as evidenced by a statistically significant t-test (for categorical variables) or correlation (for continuous variables).

Separate hierarchical regressions were conducted to test linear relations between the independent variable of pain intensity and the criterion variables of the urge to smoke (QSU-B total) and urge to smoke to relieve negative affect (QSU-B-NA) and urge to smoke for pleasure/reward (QSU-B-PR). Covariates were entered in the first step, and pain intensity was entered in the second step of each model. The relative contribution of pain intensity in explaining observed variance in each criterion variable was assessed by examining change in the R-squared statistic (ΔR2) at the second step of each model.

Indirect effects and moderation were tested using a series of conditional process models via the PROCESS macro for SPSS (Preacher and Hayes, 2008), which employs an ordinary least-squares-based approach. See Figure 1 and Figure 2 for a graphical depiction of these models. Individual paths (for indirect effects) or slope interactions (for moderation) are considered statistically significant if the upper and lower limits of the confidence intervals do not cross zero (Hayes, 2013). For indirect effects, recommended 95% confidence intervals and path coefficients were generated via bootstrapping set at 10,000 resamples with replacement (Hayes, 2013; Preacher and Hayes, 2008). A statistically significant indirect effect is observed when the 95% confidence intervals surrounding test of the indirect path do not overlap with zero. Bootstrapping is a non-parametric approach to path analysis, which reduces limitations associated with statistical power and Type 1 error inherent to stepwise regression (Preacher and Hayes, 2004). We also tested our theoretically-driven ordering of variables by re-running the indirect models in reverse by swapping the ordering of the predictor and indirect variables (Hayes, 2013).

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Conceptual models depicting negative affect as a mediator of the association between pain intensity and urge to smoke for pleasure/reward and urge to smoke for negative affect relief. *p < .05, **p < .01

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Conceptual models depicting pain catastrophizing as a moderator of the association between pain intensity and urge to smoke scores. *p < .05

Tests of moderation were considered significant if slopes are significantly different between two specified values of the moderator, which is tested by deriving an interaction term from the product of the independent variable and moderator variable. A statistically significant moderation effect is observed when the 95% confidence interval around this product does not overlap with zero. One question of theoretical and empirical interest is whether clinical levels of pain catastrophizing interact with pain intensity to predict smoking urge. Scores of 30 or more on the PCS are consistently observed among clinical pain samples across multiple countries (e.g., Hayashi et al., 2015; Hooten et al., 2011), are predictive of pain-related unemployment and disability status following injury and correspond to the upper 75th percentile of the clinical pain validation sample (Sullivan et al., 1995). Thus, moderation was tested by dummy-coding values of the moderator using a clinical cutoff of greater than or equal to 30.

3. Results

3.1. Sample Characteristics

Participants were 229 current daily tobacco smokers (42% female; Mage = 41.60, SD = 12.41). More than a third of the sample (38.9%) identified as black or African American. The majority of the sample (58.9%) reported their highest level of education as a high school graduate or less, and nearly 40% reported incomes of less than $10,000.00 per year. Participants smoked an average of 22 cigarettes per day (M = 21.95; SD = 12.94) for approximately 25 years (M = 24.50; SD = 12.31). The mean HSI score was 3.78 (SD = 1.34), indicating a relatively high level of nicotine dependence (Chaiton et al., 2007). The mean QSU-B total score was 39.29 (SD = 16.25), which is similar to that observed among other samples of daily tobacco smokers (West and Ussher, 2010). The mean QSU-B-NA (urge to smoke to relieve negative affect) score was 16.23 (SD = 8.52). Nearly half of the sample (49.6%) endorsed current pain intensity greater than zero, with an average pain intensity score of approximately 3.5/10 among those reporting any current pain (M = 3.57; SD = 2.29). Approximately 20% of the sample (n = 44) scored in the clinical range of pain catastrophizing (Sullivan et al., 1995). Measures of pain catastrophizing, negative affect, and urge to smoke all exhibited good-to-excellent internal consistency in the current sample (α = .88 to .95). See Table 1 for full sample characteristics and Table 2 for bivariate associations.

Table 1.

Sample Characteristics

N (%)
Gender
 Female 97 (42.4)
 Male 132 (57.6)
Ethnicity
 Hispanic/Latino 10 (4.4)
Race
 White 132 (57.6)
 Black/African American 89 (38.9)
 American Indian/Alaskan Native 8 (3.5)
Education
 Did not graduate high school 53 (23.1)
 High school graduate 82 (35.8)
 Some college 53 (23.1)
 Technical school or Associates degree 27 (11.8)
 Four-year college degree 7 (3.1)
 Some school beyond 4-year college 6 (2.6)
 Professional degree 1 (0.4)
Income
 < 10,000/year 91 (39.7)
 10,000–19,999/year 41 (17.9)
 20,000–29,999/year 38 (16.6)
 30,000–39,999/year 17 (7.4)
 40,000–49,999/year 10 (4.4)
 50,000–59,999/year 15 (6.6)
 ≥ 60,000/year 17 (7.4)
M (SD)
Age 41.60 (12.41)
Cigarettes per day 21.95 (12.94)
Smoking Dependence Severity1 3.78 (1.34)
Time Since Last Cigarette in Minutes 23.90 (26.33)
Pain Intensity2 1.77 (2.41)
Negative Affect 3 16.88 (7.37)
Pain Catastrophizing4 17.90 (12.62)
Urge to Smoke Total Score5 39.29 (16.25)
 Negative Affect Relief Factor 16.23 (8.52)

Note. 0 = Female, 1 = Male;

1

Heaviness of Smoking Index;

2

Numerical Rating Scale;

3

Positive and Negative Affect Schedule – Negative Affect subscale;

4

Pain Catastrophizing Scale;

5

Brief Questionnaire of Smoking Urges.

Table 2.

Bivariate Associations

Variable 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9
1 Age −.04 −.07 .02 .07 −.03 −.03 −.13 −.03
2 Gender .05 .06 .11 −.01 −.16 −.14 −.09
3 HSI1 −.18** .13* .10 .05 .34** .30**
4 TSLC2 .06 .08 .04 .09 .07
5 NRS3 .34** .28** .19** .22**
6 PANAS-NA4 .37** .31** .36**
7 PCS5 .23** .25**
8 QSU-B – Total6 .92**
9 QSU-B – NA7

Note. N = 229. Gender: 0 = Male, 1 = Female;

1

Heaviness of Smoking Index;

2

Time Since Last Cigarette in Minutes;

3

Numerical Rating Scale Pain Intensity;

4

Positive and Negative Affect Schedule – Negative Affect,

5

Pain Catastrophizing Scale;

6

Brief Questionnaire of Smoking Urges- total score; Brief Questionnaire of Smoking Urges – Negative Affect Relief Scale;

8

Brief Questionnaire of Smoking Urges – Pleasure/Reward Scale.

*

p < .05;

**

p < .01.

3.2. Identification of Covariates

No group differences by sex were observed for pain intensity (t = 1.44; p = .15) or urge to smoke scores (ps >05). Smoking dependence scores were positively associated with QSUB total scores (r = .34; p < .01), QSUB-NA scores (r = .30; p < .01), and QSUB-P/R scores (r = .33; p < .01). Participants identifying as white reported significantly lower QSUB total scores (M = 35.11), relative to participants who identified as black or African American (M = 44.62; t = −4.48; p < .001; 95% CI = −13.68, −5.32). There were no group differences by race in terms of HSI scores or average cigarettes per day over the past week (ps < .05), suggesting that observed differences in QSUB scores cannot be explained by differences in nicotine dependence or current cigarette consumption. Thus, HSI scores and Race were selected as covariates for subsequent analyses. No other sociodemographic factors were associated with criterion variables, and both race and smoking dependence were included as covariates in subsequent models.1

3.3. Current Pain Intensity and Urge to Smoke

As hypothesized, hierarchical regression indicated that greater pain intensity was positively associated higher QSU-B total scores, ΔR2 = .02; β = .12; t = 2.03; p < .04; 95% CI [0.18, 11.49]. Also, as hypothesized, hierarchical regression indicated that pain intensity was positively associated with QSU-B-NA scores, ΔR2= .02; β = .16; t = 2.61; p = .01; 95% CI [0.97, 6.93]. Thus, individuals who endorsed greater current pain intensity also endorsed greater current overall urge to smoke and greater urge to smoke for the relief of negative affect, after accounting for smoking dependence and race. No association between pain intensity and QSU-B-PR scores was observed ΔR2 = .01; β = .07; t = 1.54; p = .25; 95% CI [−1.35, 5.12], and this criterion variable was therefore not included in further analyses. See Table 3 for full data from regression analyses.

Table 3.

Hierarchical Models: Urge to Smoke Regressed on Pain Intensity

R2 R2 β t p
Criterion Variables
QSU-B1 - Total .205 .015 .122 2.032 .043*
QSU-B1 - Negative Affect Relief .196 .024 .158 2.608 .010**
QSU-B1 - Positive Reinforcement .158 .005 .071 1.149 .252

Note: N =229. These data reflect step two of the respective hierarchical models of smoking urge scores regressed on pain intensity scores. All analyses presented adjusted for participant race and heaviness of smoking index (HSI) scores.

1

Brief Questionnaire of Smoking Urges.

*

p < .05,

**

p < .01

3.4. Indirect Association between Pain Intensity and Urge to Smoke via State Negative Affect

As hypothesized, analysis of indirect associations revealed that state negative affect statistically mediated the positive association between pain intensity and QSU-B total scores, b = 3.53; SE = 1.33; 95% CI [1.35, 6.65], such that current pain intensity was positively associated with negative affect, which in turn was associated with greater overall urge to smoke. Examination of QSU-B-NA subscale score revealed a similar pattern of findings, such that state negative affect statistically mediated the positive association between pain intensity and QSU-B-NA scores b = 2.23; SE = 0.71; 95% CI [1.08, 3.89]. Models were no longer significant when ran in reverse (all 95% confidence intervals crossed zero), thus providing statistical support for our theoretically-driven ordering of the variables. See Table 4 for full data from the indirect models.

Table 4.

Pain Intensity and Urge to Smoke: Indirect Role of Negative Affect

Path Path Description b SE t p CI (l) CI (u)
Model 1 (Y1) Criterion Variable: Urge to Smoke Total Score
a NRS1 → NA2 7.045 1.360 5.182 <.001 4.366 9.724
b NA → QSU-B3 0.500 0.137 3.651 <.001 0.230 0.770
c NRS → QSU-B 5.833 2.871 2.032 .043 0.176 11.490
c NRS → QSU-B 2.308 2.957 .780 .436 −3.520 8.135
Model 2 (Y2) Criterion Variable: Urge to Smoke for the Relief of Negative Affect
a NRS → NA 7.045 1.360 5.182 <.001 4.366 9.724
b NA → QSU-B-NA4 0.317 0.071 4.441 <.001 0.176 0.457
c NRS → QSU-B-NA 3.946 1.513 2.608 .010 0.965 6.928
c’ NRS → QSU-B-NA 1.716 1.538 1.116 .266 −1.315 4.747

Note. N = 229.

1

Numerical Rating Scale- pain intensity;

2

Positive and Negative Affect Schedule – Negative Affect;

3

Questionnaire of Smoking Urges-Brief (QSU-B) - total score;

4

QSU-B – Negative Affect Relief.

In all models, path a represents the association between the independent variable and the mediator, path b represents the associations between the mediator and the dependent variable, controlling for the independent variable, path c represents the total effect of the model (direct effect + indirect effect), and path c’ represents the direct effect of the independent variable on the dependent variable

3.5. Catastrophizing as a Moderator of the Relation between Pain Intensity and Urge to Smoke

Pain catastrophizing was found to partially moderate the association between current pain intensity and QSU-B total scores (p =.01), with a corresponding significant change in R-squared, F(1, 222) = 7.72; ΔR2 = .03. However, examination of the path coefficients and confidence intervals revealed that the direction of moderation was opposite of expectation, such that the association between pain intensity and QSU-B total scores was only observed among individuals who scored below the clinical cutoff on our measure of pain catastrophizing, b = 9.22, SE = 3.26, t = 2.82, p < .01, 95% CI [2.78, 15.65]. A similar moderating effect was observed for QSU-B-NA scores, F(1, 222) = 6.58; ΔR2 = .02; p =.01, such that the positive association between pain intensity and urge to smoke for the relief of negative affect was only evident among individuals who scored below the clinical cutoff on our measure of pain catastrophizing, b = 5.56, SE = 1.72, t = 3.22, p < .01, 95% CI [2.16, 8.96]. See Table 5 for full data from the indirect models.

Table 5.

Pain Intensity on Urge to Smoke: Moderating Role of Pain Catastrophizing

Path Coeff. SE t p CI (l) CI (u)
Pain Catastrophizing as a Moderator of Pain on Urge to Smoke Total Scores
Constant c 36.915 5.021 7.352 < .001 27.019 46.810
Pain Intensity (X) b1 9.22 3.26 2.82 .005 2.78 15.65
Pain Catastrophizing (M) b2 12.438 3.439 3.616 < .001 5.659 19.216
NRS x PCS-M (X*M) b3 −17.944 6.456 −2.779 .006 −30.668 −5.221
Pain Catastrophizing as a Moderator of Pain on Urge to Smoke for Negative Reinforcement
Constant c 16.214 2.655 6.107 < .001 10.982 21.446
Pain Intensity (X) b1 5.558 1.725 3.221 .001 2.158 8.959
Pain Catastrophizing (M) b2 6.434 1.818 2.538 < .001 2.850 10.018
NRS x PCS-M (X*M) b3 −8.757 3.413 −2.565 .011 −15.484 −2.030

Notes. N = 229. All models covaried for nicotine dependence (Heaviness of Smoking Index) scores and Race. Pain Intensity (NRS) scores were log transformed after adding 1 to the raw value. Pain catastrophizing was dichotomized at ± 30 and dummy coded at “0” and “1”. NRS scores were entered as a continuous variable

4. Discussion

The goal of this study was to test cross-sectional relations between current pain intensity and self-reported urge to smoke, among a sample of daily tobacco users who did not endorse chronic pain. Consistent with prediction, current pain intensity was found to be positively associated with the total urge to smoke and the urge to smoke for the relief of negative affect. Thus, the observed cross-sectional relationship between pain intensity and overall urge to smoke was driven by the associations with urge to smoke for negative reinforcement. Also consistent with prediction, state negative affect was shown to statistically-mediate associations between current pain and urge, such that pain intensity was positively associated with negative affect, which in turn was associated with the greater urge, in general, as well as urge to smoke for the relief of negative affect, specifically. These findings are consistent with the negative reinforcement model of tobacco dependence (Baker et al., 2004b), which posits that tobacco smoking is maintained primarily by the motivation to alleviate aversive internal experiences. These findings are also consistent with previous experimental work demonstrating that pain can motivate smoking urge, partly via indirect effects of increased negative affect (Ditre and Brandon, 2008; Parkerson and Asmundson, 2016).

Although pain catastrophizing was found to moderate relations between pain intensity and urge to smoke, the direction of effects was in contrast to expectation. Specifically, the positive associations between pain intensity and urge to smoke were only evident among smokers who endorsed low (vs. high) levels of catastrophizing. This was unexpected given that evidence indicates that smoking has been shown to motivate smoking behavior (Ditre and Brandon, 2008), and that chronic pain patients endorse smoking to cope with pain (Hooten et al., 2011). In examining the broader substance use literature, there is some evidence that lower or emerging levels of pain catastrophizing may be linked to greater use of addictive substances (i.e., a prescription pain medication; Sharifzadeh et al., 2017). A second possibility is that fear and concern about the pain among high catastrophizers may be associated with avoidance of activities perceived to increase the risk for pain, such as smoking (Zale et al., 2013). A third possible explanation for this finding is that rumination about the pain that is characteristic of high pain catastrophizing may shift attentional resources away from urges to smoke. Additional research is needed to clarify the role and clinical importance of catastrophizing in pain-smoking relations.

Several limitations should be noted. First, the use of cross-sectional data prohibits causal inferences and any conclusions regarding temporal precedence in the models tested. Future research would benefit from using prospective and naturalistic research methods, such as momentary ecological assessment (e.g., Dhingra et al., 2013), to examine temporal and potentially bidirectional relations between the experience of pain and smoking urge/behavior over time. Second, although these findings may not generalize to smokers who have chronic pain, individuals who live with persistent pain would likely encounter a greater number of occasions to experience pain as a proximal antecedent of smoking urge and behavior (Ditre et al., 2017; Zale et al., 2016). Third, although pain intensity and urge to smoke may vary as a function of pain etiology (e.g., musculoskeletal, migraine), the current data did not allow for such comparisons. Fourth, our sample was comprised of nicotine-dependent tobacco users who reported smoking greater than 20 cigarettes per day. Future research should examine associations between pain intensity and motivation to smoke among lighter and intermittent smokers (e.g., Ditre et al., 2016b).

The current population of "recalcitrant" smokers may harbor characteristics that make tobacco cessation especially difficult (Hughes and Brandon, 2003; Irvin and Brandon, 2000), and current pain intensity may represent an important barrier to quitting (Zale et al., 2014). Consistent with this notion, the current findings contribute to an emerging literature indicating that pain and pain-related constructs may be relevant to nicotine/tobacco consumption among smokers with and without co-occurring chronic pain (Ditre and Brandon, 2008; Ditre et al., 2015; Zale et al., 2014; Zale et al., 2016). Indeed, urge to smoke is a reliable predictor of lapse and relapse following a cessation attempt (Allen et al., 2008; Nakamura et al., 2014), and these findings suggest that pain may constitute a high-risk situation for smokers who are attempting to quit. Future work is needed to determine whether smokers would benefit from tailored interventions that address the role of current pain in the context of smoking cessation. For example, given that nicotine has been shown to aid in the management of smoking urges (Hansson et al., 2012; Hartwell et al., 2013) and reduce pain in the short term (Ditre et al., 2016a), smokers who endorse current pain may benefit from high-dose or combination nicotine replacement therapy (Hatsukami et al., 2007; Mills et al., 2012).

Highlights.

  • Research indicates substantial covariation between pain and tobacco smoking.

  • This is the first cross-sectional examination of current pain and urge to smoke.

  • Pain was positively associated with urge to smoke for negative affect relief.

  • Pain was associated with greater smoking urge via greater negative affect.

Acknowledgments

Role of Funding Source

This research was supported by NIH Grant No. R21DA034285 awarded to Joseph W. Ditre

The authors would like to thank all participants who volunteered to participate in this research.

Footnotes

1

Race did not moderate the relationship between pain intensity and overall urge to smoke, F(1, 216) = 0.04; ΔR2 < .0001; p = .84, or urge to smoke for the relief of negative affect, F(1, 216) = 0.04; ΔR2 < .0001; p = .94. HSI scores did not moderate the relationship between pain intensity and total urge to smoke, F(1, 216) = 0.66; ΔR2 < .002; p = .42, or urge to smoke for the relief of negative affect, F(1, 216) = 2.97; ΔR2 < .001; p = .59.

Contributors

All authors made substantial contributions to this project, and have read and approved the manuscript.

Conflicts of Interest

No conflict declared.

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

References

  1. Ahmed J, Israel TA, O’Connor E, King BA, Kenemer JB, Neff L. Current Cigarette Smoking Among Adults—United States, 2005–2013. MMWR Morb Mortal Wkly Rep. 2014;63:1108–1112. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Allen SS, Bade T, Hatsukami D, Center B. Craving, withdrawal, and smoking urges on days immediately prior to smoking relapse. Nicotine Tob Res. 2008;10:35–45. doi: 10.1080/14622200701705076. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Andersson H, Ejlertsson G, Leden I. Widespread musculoskeletal chronic pain associated with smoking. An epidemiological study in a general rural population. Scand J Rehabil Med. 1998;30:185–191. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Baker TB, Brandon TH, Chassin L. Motivational influences on cigarette smoking. Annu Rev Psychol. 2004a;55:463–491. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.142054. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Baker TB, Piper ME, McCarthy DE, Majeskie MR, Fiore MC. Addiction motivation reformulated: An affective processing model of negative reinforcement. Psychol Rev. 2004b;111:33–51. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.111.1.33. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bakhshaie J, Ditre JW, Langdon KJ, Asmundson GJ, Paulus DJ, Zvolensky MJ. Pain intensity and smoking behavior among treatment seeking smokers. Psychiatry Res. 2016;237:67–71. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2016.01.073. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Benowitz NL, Jacob P, Ahijevych K, Jarvis MJ, Hall S, LeHouezec J, Hansson A, Lichtenstein E, Henninggield J, Tsoh J, Hurt RD, Velicer W. Biochemical verification of tobacco use and cessation. Nicotine Tob Res. 2002;4:149–159. [Google Scholar]
  8. Bialosky JE, Hirsh AT, Robinson ME, George SZ. Manipulation of pain catastrophizing: An experimental study of healthy participants. J Pain Res. 2008;1:35–41. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Borland R, Yong HH, O'Connor RJ, Hyland A, Thompson ME. The reliability and predictive validity of the Heaviness of Smoking Index and its two components: Findings from the International Tobacco Control Four Country study. Nicotine Tob Res. 2010;12(Suppl):S45–50. doi: 10.1093/ntr/ntq038. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. CDC. Quitting smoking among adults--United States, 2001–2010. MMWR Morb Mortal Wkly Rep. 2011;60:1513–1519. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Chabrol H, Niezborala M, Chastan E, de Leon J. Comparison of the heavy smoking index and of the Fagerstrom Test for Nicotine Dependence in a sample of 749 cigarette smokers. Addict Behav. 2005;30:1474–1477. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2005.02.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Chaiton MO, Cohen JE, McDonald PW, Bondy SJ. The Heaviness of Smoking Index as a predictor of smoking cessation in Canada. Addict Behav. 2007;32:1031–1042. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2006.07.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Chaves JF, Brown JM. Spontaneous cognitive strategies for the control of clinical pain and stress. J Behav Med. 1987;10:263–276. doi: 10.1007/BF00846540. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Cox LS, Tiffany ST, Christen AG. Evaluation of the brief questionnaire of smoking urges (QSU-brief) in laboratory and clinical settings. Nicotine Tob Res. 2001;3:7–16. doi: 10.1080/14622200020032051. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Dhingra LK, Homel P, Grossman B, Chen J, Scharaga E, Calamita S, Shin J, Portenoy R. Ecological momentary assessment of smoking behavior in persistent pain patients. Clin J Pain. 2013;30:205–13. doi: 10.1097/AJP.0b013e31829821c7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Ditre JW, Brandon TH. Pain as a motivator of smoking: Effects of pain induction on smoking urge and behavior. J Abnorm Psychol. 2008;117:467–472. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.117.2.467. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Ditre JW, Brandon TH, Zale EL, Meagher MM. Pain, nicotine, and smoking: Research findings and mechanistic considerations. Psychol Bull. 2011;137:1065–1093. doi: 10.1037/a0025544. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Ditre JW, Heckman BW, Zale EL, Kosiba JD, Maisto SA. Acute analgesic effects of nicotine and tobacco in humans: A meta-analysis. Pain. 2016a;157:1373–1381. doi: 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000000572. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Ditre JW, Kosiba JD, Zale EL, Zvolensky MJ, Maisto SA. Chronic pain status, nicotine withdrawal, and expectancies for smoking cessation among lighter smokers. Ann Behav Med. 2016b;50:427–435. doi: 10.1007/s12160-016-9769-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Ditre JW, Langdon KJ, Kosiba JD, Zale EL, Zvolensky MJ. Relations between pain-related anxiety, tobacco dependence, and barriers to quitting among a community-based sample of daily smokers. Addict Behav. 2015;42C:130–135. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2014.11.032. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Ditre JW, Zale EL, Heckman BW, Hendricks PS. A measure of perceived pain and tobacco smoking interrelations: Pilot validation of the pain and smoking inventory. Cogn Behav Ther. 2017;46:339–351. doi: 10.1080/16506073.2016.1256347. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Dworkin RH, Turk DC, Farrar JT, Haythornthwaite JA, Jensen MP, Katz NP, Kerns RD, Stucki G, Allen RR, Bellamy N, Carr DB, Chandler J, Cowan P, Dionne P, Galer BS, Hertz S, Jadad AR, Kramer LD, Manning DC, Martin S, McCormick CG, McDermott MP, McGrath P, Quessy S, Rappaprot BA, Robbins W, Robinson JP, Rothman M, Royal MA, Simon L, Stauffer JW, Stein W, Tollett J, Wenicke J, Witter J IMMPACT. Core outcome measures for chronic pain clinical trials: IMMPACT recommendations. Pain. 2005;113:9–19. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2004.09.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Edwards RR, Calahan C, Mensing G, Smith M, Haythornthwaite JA. Pain, catastrophizing, and depression in the rheumatic diseases. Nat Rev Rheumatol. 2011;7:216–224. doi: 10.1038/nrrheum.2011.2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Etter JF, Duc TV, Perneger TV. Validity of the Fagerstrom Test for Nicotine Dependence and of the Heaviness of Smoking Index among relatively light smokers. Addiction. 1999;94:269–281. doi: 10.1046/j.1360-0443.1999.94226910.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Fagerstrom K, Russ C, Yu CR, Yunis C, Foulds J. The Fagerstrom Test for Nicotine Dependence as a predictor of smoking abstinence: A pooled analysis of varenicline clinical trial data. Nicotine Tob Res. 2012;14:1467–1473. doi: 10.1093/ntr/nts018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Gaskin ME, Greene AF, Robinson ME, Geisser ME. Negative affect and the experience of chronic pain. J Psychosom Res. 1992;36:707–713. doi: 10.1016/0022-3999(92)90128-o. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Hahn EJ, Rayens MK, Kirsh KL, Passik SD. Brief report: Pain and readiness to quit smoking cigarettes. Nicotine Tob Res. 2006;8:473–480. doi: 10.1080/14622200600670355. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Hansson A, Hajek P, Perfekt R, Kraiczi H. Effects of nicotine mouth spray on urges to smoke, a randomised clinical trial. BMJ Open. 2012:2. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2012-001618. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Hartwell KJ, Lematty T, McRae-Clark AL, Gray KM, George MS, Brady KT. Resisting the urge to smoke and craving during a smoking quit attempt on varenicline: Results from a pilot fMRI study. Am J Drug Alcohol Abuse. 2013;39:92–98. doi: 10.3109/00952990.2012.750665. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Hatsukami D, Mooney M, Murphy S, LeSage M, Babb D, Hecht S. Effects of high dose transdermal nicotine replacement in cigarette smokers. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2007;86:132–139. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2006.12.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Hayashi K, Arai YC, Ikemoto T, Nishihara M, Suzuki S, Hirakawa T, Matsuo S, Kobayashi M, Haruta M, Kawabata Y, Togo H, Noguchi T, Hase T, Hatano G, Ushida T. Predictive factors for the outcome of multidisciplinary treatments in chronic low back pain at the first multidisciplinary pain center of Japan. J Phys Ther Sci. 2015;27:2901–2905. doi: 10.1589/jpts.27.2901. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Hayes AF. Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach. Guilford Press; New York, NY: 2013. Methodology in the social sciences. [Google Scholar]
  33. Heatherton TF, Kozlowski LT, Frecker RC, Rickert W, Robinson J. Measuring the heaviness of smoking: Using self-reported time to the first cigarette of the day and number of cigarettes smoked per day. Br J Addict. 1989;84:791–799. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.1989.tb03059.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Hjermstad MJ, Fayers PM, Haugen DF, Caraceni A, Hanks GW, Loge JH, Fainsinger R, Aass N, Kaasa S European Palliative Care Research Collaborative. Studies comparing Numerical Rating Scales, Verbal Rating Scales, and Visual Analogue Scales for assessment of pain intensity in adults: A systematic literature review. J Pain Symptom Manage. 2011;41:1073–1093. doi: 10.1016/j.jpainsymman.2010.08.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Hooten WM, Shi Y, Gazelka HM, Warner DO. The effects of depression and smoking on pain severity and opioid use in patients with chronic pain. Pain. 2011;152:223–229. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2010.10.045. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Hooten WM, Townsend CO, Bruce BK, Schmidt JE, Kerkvliet JL, Patten CA, Warner DO. Effects of smoking status on immediate treatment outcomes of multidisciplinary pain rehabilitation. Pain Med. 2009;10:347–355. doi: 10.1111/j.1526-4637.2008.00494.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Hooten WM, Vickers KS, Shi Y, Ebnet KL, Townsend CO, Patten CA, Warner DO. Smoking cessation and chronic pain: patient and pain medicine physician attitudes. Pain Practice. 2011;11:552–563. doi: 10.1111/j.1533-2500.2011.00462.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Huang CL, Lin HH, Wang HH. Evaluating screening performances of the Fagerstrom tolerance questionnaire, the Fagerstrom Test for Nicotine Dependence and the Heavy Smoking Index among Taiwanese male smokers. J Clin Nurs. 2008;17:884–890. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2702.2007.02054.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Hughes JR, Keely J, Naud S. Shape of the relapse curve and long-term abstinence among untreated smokers. Addiction. 2004;99:29–38. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2004.00540.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. IASP. International Association for the Study of Pain: Pain definitions. 2008 from http://www.iasp-pain.org Retrieved December 1, 2008.
  41. IBM Corp. IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 21.0. Armonk, NY: IBM Corp; Released 2012. [Google Scholar]
  42. Jamison RN, Stetson BA, Parris WC. The relationship between cigarette smoking and chronic low back pain. Addict Behav. 1991;16:103–110. doi: 10.1016/0306-4603(91)90002-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Janssen SA. Negative affect and sensitization to pain. Scand J Psychol. 2002;43:131–137. doi: 10.1111/1467-9450.00278. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Jensen MP, Karoly P. Self-report scales and procedures for assessing pain in adults. In: Turk DC, Melzack R, editors. Handbook of pain assessment. The Guilford Press; New York, NY: 2011. pp. 19–44. [Google Scholar]
  45. Kassel JD, Stroud LR, Paronis CA. Smoking, stress, and negative affect: Correlation, causation, and context across stages of smoking. Psychol Bull. 2003;129:270–304. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.129.2.270. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Lacourt TE, Houtveen JH, Veldhuijzen van Zanten JJ, Bosch JA, Drayson MT, Van Doornen LJ. Negative affectivity predicts decreased pain tolerance during low-grade inflammation in healthy women. Brain Behav Immun. 2015;44:32–36. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2014.10.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Lazarus RS, Folkman S. Stress, appraisal, and coping. New York: Springer; 1984. [Google Scholar]
  48. McCaffery M, Beebe A. Pain: Clinical manual for nursing practice. C.V. Mosby Company; St. Loius, MS: 1989. [Google Scholar]
  49. Melzack R. The McGill Pain Questionnaire: Major properties and scoring methods. Pain. 1975;1:277–299. doi: 10.1016/0304-3959(75)90044-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Melzack R, Wall PD. Pain mechanisms: A new theory. Science. 1965;150:971–979. doi: 10.1126/science.150.3699.971. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Mills EJ, Wu P, Lockhart I, Thorlund K, Puhan M, Ebbert JO. Comparisons of high-dose and combination nicotine replacement therapy, varenicline, and bupropion for smoking cessation: A systematic review and multiple treatment meta-analysis. Ann Med. 2012;44:588–597. doi: 10.3109/07853890.2012.705016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Morgan MJ, Davies GM, Willner P. The Questionnaire of Smoking Urges is sensitive to abstinence and exposure to smoking-related cues. Behav Pharmacol. 1999;10:619–626. doi: 10.1097/00008877-199911000-00008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Nahin RL. Estimates of pain prevalence and severity in adults: United States, 2012. J Pain. 2015;16:769–780. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2015.05.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Nakamura M, Oshima A, Ohkura M, Arteaga C, Suwa K. Predictors of lapse and relapse to smoking in successful quitters in a varenicline post hoc analysis in Japanese smokers. Clin Ther. 2014;36:918–927. doi: 10.1016/j.clinthera.2014.03.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Nesbitt PD. Smoking, physiological arousal, and emotional response. J Pers Soc Psych. 1973;25:137–144. doi: 10.1037/h0034256. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Osman A, Barrios FX, Gutierrez PM, Kopper BA, Merrifield T, Grittmann L. The Pain Catastrophizing Scale: Further psychometric evaluation with adult samples. J Behav Med. 2000;23:351–365. doi: 10.1023/a:1005548801037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Parkerson HA, Asmundson GJ. The role of pain intensity and smoking expectancies on smoking urge and behavior following experimental pain induction. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2016;164:166–171. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2016.05.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Piasecki TM. Relapse to smoking. Clin Psychol Rev. 2006;26:196–215. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2005.11.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Pirouzi S, Ghanbari A, Moslemi Haghighi F, Ghafarinejad F, Pouya F, Motiallah T. The prevalence of musculoskeletal pain in male cigarette smoking students at Shiraz University of Medical Sciences, Iran. Addict Health. 2011;3:125–129. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Pisinger C, Aadahl M, Toft U, Birke H, Zutphen-Adler J, Jorgensen T. The association between active and passive smoking and frequent pain in a general population. Eur J Pain. 2011;15:77–83. doi: 10.1016/j.ejpain.2010.05.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Preacher KJ, Hayes AF. SPSS and SAS procedures for estimating indirect effects in simple mediation models. Behav Res Methods Instrum Comput. 2004;36:717–731. doi: 10.3758/bf03206553. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Preacher KJ, Hayes AF. Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behav Res Methods. 2008;40:879–891. doi: 10.3758/brm.40.3.879. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Quartana PJ, Campbell CM, Edwards RR. Pain catastrophizing: A critical review. Expert Rev Neurother. 2009;9:745–758. doi: 10.1586/ERN.09.34. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Rosenstiel AK, Keefe FJ. The use of coping strategies in chronic low back pain patients: Relationship to patient characteristics and current adjustment. Pain. 1983;17:33–44. doi: 10.1016/0304-3959(83)90125-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Sharifzadeh Y, Kao MC, Sturgeon JA, Rico TJ, Mackey S, Darnall BD. Pain catastrophizing moderates relationships between pain intensity and opioid prescription: Nonlinear sex differences revealed using a learning health system. Anesthesiology. 2017;127:136–146. doi: 10.1097/ALN.0000000000001656. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Spanos NP, Brown JM, Jones B, Horner D. Cognitive activity and suggestions for analgesia in the reduction of reported pain. J Abnorm Psychol. 1981;90:554–561. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.90.6.554. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Sullivan MJL, Bishop SR, Pivik J. The Pain Catastrophizing Scale: Development and validation. Psychol Assess. 1995;7:524–532. [Google Scholar]
  68. Tabachnick BG, Fidell LS. Using multivariate statistics. New York: Harper Collins; 2004. [Google Scholar]
  69. Turk DC, Melzack R. The measurement of pain and the assessment of people experiencing pain. In: Turk DC, Melzack R, editors. Handbook of pain assessment. The Guilford Press; New York: 2001. pp. 1–11. [Google Scholar]
  70. United States Department of Health and Human Services. The health consequences of smoking—50 years of progress: A report of the Surgeon General. Atlanta: Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, National Center for Chronic Disease Prevention and Health Promotion, Office on Smoking and Health; 2014. [Accessed March 23, 2018]. https://www.surgeongeneral.gov/library/reports/50-years-of-progress/index.html. [Google Scholar]
  71. Volkman JE, DeRycke EC, Driscoll MA, Becker WC, Brandt CA, Mattocks KM, Haskell SG, Bathulapalli H, Goulet JL, Bastian LA. Smoking status and pain intensity among OEF/OIF/OND Veterans. Pain Med. 2015;16:1690–1696. doi: 10.1111/pme.12753. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Watson D, Clark LA. Negative affectivity: The disposition to experience aversive emotional states. Psychol Bull. 1984;96:465–490. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Watson D, Clark LA, Tellegen A. Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. J Pers Soc Psych. 1988;54:1063–1070. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.54.6.1063. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. West R, Ussher M. Is the ten-item Questionnaire of Smoking Urges (QSU-brief) more sensitive to abstinence than shorter craving measures? Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2010;208:427–432. doi: 10.1007/s00213-009-1742-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Williamson A, Hoggart B. Pain: A review of three commonly used pain rating scales. J Clin Nurs. 2005;14:798–804. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2702.2005.01121.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. World Health Organization. [Accessed March 26, 2018];WHO report on the global tobacco epidemic, 2008: The MPOWER package. 2008 http://www.who.int/tobacco/mpower/2008/en/
  77. Zale EL, Ditre JW, Dorfman ML, Heckman BW, Brandon TH. Smokers in pain report lower confidence and greater difficulty quitting. Nicotine Tob Res. 2014;16:1272–6. doi: 10.1093/ntr/ntu077. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Zale EL, Lange KL, Fields SA, Ditre JW. The relation between pain-related fear and disability: A meta-analysis. J Pain. 2013;14:1019–1030. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2013.05.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Zale EL, Maisto SA, Ditre JW. Anxiety and depression in bidirectional relations between pain and smoking: Implications for smoking cessation. Behav Modif. 2016;40:7–28. doi: 10.1177/0145445515610744. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Zautra A, Smith B, Affleck G, Tennen H. Examinations of chronic pain and affect relationships: Applications of a dynamic model of affect. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2001;69:786–795. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.69.5.786. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Zvolensky MJ, McMillan K, Gonzalez A, Asmundson GJ. Chronic pain and cigarette smoking and nicotine dependence among a representative sample of adults. Nicotine Tob Res. 2009;11:1407–1414. doi: 10.1093/ntr/ntp153. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES