Skip to main content
. 2018 May 22;7:e33334. doi: 10.7554/eLife.33334

Figure 10. Model fits for experimental data by Zamboni et al., (2016).

(a) Experiment 1a: Exact replication of the original experiment (Jazayeri and Movshon, 2007). Exemplarily shown is the estimation data (combined subject) at one stimulus coherence level (0.13) together with our model fit. (b) Experiment 1b was identical except that the boundary was not shown during the estimation task. Estimate distributions are no longer bimodal yet the self-consistent observer, relying on a noisy memory of the boundary orientation, consistently better fit the data than the independent observer model (log-likelihood difference). (c) Experiment 2 introduced a shift in the boundary orientation right before the estimation task, which subjects were not aware of (±six degrees). Subjects’ estimates were shifted accordingly (combined subject). The self-consistent model can account for the shift if we assume that the conditional prior is applied to the shifted boundary orientation. See Figure 10—figure supplements 13 for distributions, fits, and goodness-of-fits for all conditions.

Figure 10.

Figure 10—figure supplement 1. Zamboni et al., (2016) data (Experiment 1, combined subject) and fit with the self-consistent observer model.

Figure 10—figure supplement 1.

Figure 10—figure supplement 2. Relative log-likelihoods of model fits for Zamboni et al., (2016) data.

Figure 10—figure supplement 2.

Relative log-likelihood values of the self-consistent observer model fit to the combined subject data for each experiment. Relative scale is defined as described for Figure 4—figure supplement 1.
Figure 10—figure supplement 3. Zamboni et al., (2016) data (Experiment 2, combined subject) and fit with the self-consistent observer model.

Figure 10—figure supplement 3.