Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2019 Apr 1.
Published in final edited form as: Curr Opin Psychol. 2017 Aug 5;20:11–16. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.07.037

Callous-Unemotional Behaviors in Early Childhood: The Development of Empathy and Prosociality Gone Awry

Rebecca Waller 1,2, Luke W Hyde 1,3,4
PMCID: PMC5965673  NIHMSID: NIHMS963249  PMID: 28822897

Abstract

Callous-unemotional (CU) behaviors are critical to understanding the development of severe forms of aggression and antisocial behavior. CU behaviors include deficits in empathy and prosocial behavior, as well as reduced interpersonal responsivity to others. We review recent research examining CU behaviors in early childhood and the role that parents play in the development of early CU behaviors. We integrate research on the development of empathy and prosociality with that of CU behaviors to propose a developmental model of early CU behaviors considering person-context interactions.

Introduction

Empathy involves the vicarious experience of another person’s distress, which can precipitate prosocial acts of help. Both empathy and prosociality (e.g., helping behaviors) are fundamental to moral and social behavior [1]. Developmental research has established that children’s empathic concern for others and displays of prosocial behavior emerge by the second year of life [24]. In parallel, developmental psychopathology research has focused on the ways in which deficits in these processes lead to childhood antisocial behavior. Specifically, research has examined callous-unemotional (CU) behaviors, defined by low empathy and limited prosociality [5]. In the current paper, we integrate these two literatures and describe emerging research examining CU behaviors in early childhood. We discuss the consequences of early CU behaviors and the risk factors implicated in the etiology of CU behaviors, including parenting and child temperament. Finally, we propose a model to explain how early CU behaviors arise and evaluate CU behaviors as a marker of disrupted development of empathy and prosociality.

Callous-Unemotional Behaviors in Early Childhood

Most research on CU behaviors has focused on adolescents and school-aged children. However, recent studies have begun assessing CU behaviors in early childhood (before age 5) either via standard measures used with older-aged children [68] or using items from parent-reported scales that assess low empathy and guilt, uncaring about others, and low emotional responsivity [9,10]. This research shows that CU behaviors assessed as young as age 3 predict concurrent and future behavior problems. For example, across multiple prospective, longitudinal studies, CU behaviors at age 3 predicted later school-aged aggression (up to age 10) assessed via teacher report [911], and one study found this longitudinal link even when taking into account the stability of aggressive and oppositional behaviors and potential informant rater effects/bias [12]. Beyond predicting an escalation in behavior problems, CU behaviors show a unique set of correlates relative to those of oppositional and attention deficit behaviors, including lower empathy and guilt, which suggests that the CU construct does not simply index greater severity in behavior problems [9]. Finally, CU behaviors at age 3 have been shown to predict a measure of CU behaviors at age 9.5, emphasizing the homotypic continuity of the CU construct across childhood [12]. Together, these studies suggest that by 3 years old, measures of CU behaviors correlate with deficits in empathy and conscience and identify children at risk for chronic behavior problems across childhood.

Heritable and Non-Heritable Pathways to Early CU Behaviors

Given the lasting negative consequences of early CU behaviors, it is important to understand how they develop. A large developmental literature has linked parenting to the development of empathy and prosocial behavior [3,13,14]. For example, attachment security at age 3 was shown to predict increases in children’s empathy at age 4 [15], and in another study, parental warmth and sympathy predicted increases in prosocial behavior in late-childhood [16]. Warm parenting is thought to encourage and scaffold emotional expression and sensitivity, and increase the likelihood that children internalize parental messages about empathy and prosociality [14]. For example, “Mutually Responsive Orientation”, defined as a close, warm, and mutually cooperative parent-child relationship, appears particularly important for promoting the development of empathy and conscience [17]. In contrast, harsh or rejecting forms of parenting likely interfere with children’s ability to internalize rules and develop conscience [18]. Moreover, negative parent-child relationships appear to amplify the risk that the development of empathy and conscience could fail, particularly among children who are difficult or challenging to manage [18,19].

Consistent with this normative developmental literature, a systematic review of 30 studies found that during late-childhood and adolescence, parenting is correlated with CU behaviors [20]. Recent studies have also begun to investigate the relationship between parenting and CU behaviors during the preschool period, when individual differences in empathy and prosocial behavior emerge [24]. These studies show that greater parental warmth predicts decreases in CU behaviors across the preschool period [2123], whereas parental harshness at age 2 predicts increases in CU behaviors at ages 4 [24] and 6 [22]. However, it is important to consider that parenting does not occur in a vacuum and is likely influenced by the broader context. For example, both low socioeconomic status [25] and neighborhood deprivation [26] predicted CU behaviors in late-childhood. Similarly, neighborhood impoverishment and maternal traits (aggressiveness and low empathy) predicted CU behaviors in late-childhood via their influence on observed maternal warmth at age 2 [27]. This latter study suggests that broader negative contexts can undermine child socioemotional development via effects on parenting [27]. In sum, growing evidence suggests that both harsh and warm early parenting practices, as well as the broader context of the parent-child relationship, are important for understanding why CU behaviors develop [28].

At the same time, genetically-informed studies suggest at least moderate heritability of CU behaviors in early childhood [29], similar to empathy [30] and prosociality [31]. Thus, the links between parenting and CU behavior could reflect gene-environment correlations (i.e., parents low on warmth pass on genes that increase risk for CU behaviors), implying that parenting may simply be a correlated but “non-causal” variable. To address this confound, a recent study used an adoption design to separate the contributions of genetic and environmental influences. Biological mothers’ antisocial behavior predicted child CU behaviors at 27 months, suggesting that CU behaviors arise, to some extent, via heritable pathways [32]. However, low positive parenting of adoptive mothers at 18 months also predicted CU behaviors when children were 27 months old, indicating non-heritable effects and supporting the idea that parenting is an environmentally-mediated predictor of CU behaviors. Crucially, this same study found a gene-by-environment interaction in which the antisocial behavior of biological mothers only predicted adopted child CU behaviors when adoptive mothers showed low positive parenting. Thus, this study supports the assertion that parenting is critical to the development of early CU behaviors [20], and highlights the notion that CU behaviors develop through a complex interplay between genes and environment.

Temperament Precursors of Early Childhood CU behaviors

Given evidence for heritable pathways to CU behaviors, it is important to identify specific temperamental factors that may be inherited, emerge early, and signal risk for CU behaviors. Across several recent studies, early temperamental markers of CU behaviors have been identified. For example, reduced face preference at 5 weeks old [33] and low baseline sinus arrythima (thought to influence social behaviors) measured across 3-24 months [34] predicted increases in CU behaviors in early childhood. In addition, among clinic-referred 4-year-olds, children with CU behaviors showed lower affection and eye contact with parents relative to both healthy control children and those with behavior problems only [35]. Finally, CU behaviors in early childhood were related to poorer recognition of interpersonal emotions in two recent studies [8,36].

Together these findings suggest that impairments in attending to, recognizing, and responding to interpersonal emotions as early as infancy may increase risk for CU behaviors. These impairments could contribute to deficits in the development of affective empathy. Empathy is commonly divided between emotionally resonating with the feelings of another (i.e., affective empathy) versus understanding the perspective of another (i.e., cognitive empathy). Evidence suggests that children high on CU behaviors have intact cognitive but impaired affective empathy during the preschool period [9] and late-childhood [37]. Thus, the failure of children with CU behaviors to act prosocially could result from a specific deficit in affective empathy arising from low interpersonal emotional sensitivity. This premise is supported by research in typically-developing children where greater affective empathy at 10 months of age predicted higher prosocial behavior at 2 years old [38]. Moreover, the notion that interpersonal emotional sensitivity precedes affective empathy and promotes prosociality is consistent with developmental theory on early empathy development [1]. Specifically, children are thought to be born with the capacity to “feel” others’ distress [1], manifested from birth via numerous mechanisms that grow in complexity across development from reactive crying in infancy [39] to facial expressions and vocalizations indicating concern by age 2 [2]. Thus, CU behaviors may emerge, in part, from low affective empathy resulting from early deficits in these basic emotional sensitivity processes [30,40].

In addition to emotional responsivity, a second temperament relevant for understanding CU behaviors is low fear. Fear is thought to readily capture attention and inhibit behavioral approach to threat [41,42]. CU behaviors are hypothesized to emerge from a “fearless” temperament [43], a hypothesis supported by longitudinal evidence linking child fearlessness at ages 2-3 to CU behaviors in adolescence [25] and psychopathy in adults [44]. These findings have been taken to suggest that early fearlessness confers low arousal to threat, which undermines learning about the consequences of behavior, thus increasing risk for CU behaviors [43]. Moreover, low fear to threat could lead to high approach, reward dominance, and low sensitivity to punishment, which typically characterize children high on both aggression and CU behaviors [5,43]. As evidence that fearlessness is an early emerging and heritable temperament that leads to CU behaviors, a recent study using an adoption sample found that biological parent fearlessness predicted observations of adopted child fearlessness at 18 months, which in turn predicted higher CU behaviors at 27 months [45].

Person-by-Context Interactions in the Development of Early CU behaviors

Drawing research on parenting and child temperament together, studies suggest that interactions between child temperament and parenting are critical in the development of moral emotions [17,18]. For example, non-assertive parental discipline in toddlerhood predicted conscience development at age 10 among fearful, but not fearless, children [18]. Moreover, among children rated as fearful at age 4 based on high electrodermal reactivity, gentle maternal discipline predicted conscience development, whereas attachment security predicted conscience development children rated as fearless who showed low electrodermal reactivity [46]. These studies suggest that inherited child characteristics interact with caregiving to shape the development of empathy and prosocial behavior and that person-context fit may be particularly important in the development of prosocial behaviors.

Similar person-by-context interactions are now being identified in the etiology of CU behaviors. For example, in two independent samples, observed fearlessness at 18 months predicted later CU behaviors, but only among children who experienced low positive parenting [45,47]. Thus, highly positive parenting buffered the risk that fearlessness posed to the development of CU behaviors. In addition, harsh caregiving was more strongly associated with CU behaviors at age 3 among children with a genetic allele thought to impair fear conditioning (met allele in BDNF) [48]. In this example, parental harshness exacerbated genetic risk for fearlessness, which in turned increased risk for CU behaviors. Together, this emerging literature highlights that child temperament interacts with parental caregiving to increase or buffer risk for CU behaviors.

Developmental Model of Early CU behaviors

Based on this literature, we propose that early CU behaviors arise in the context of inherited temperament risk for both low interpersonal emotional sensitivity [33,34,45] and fearlessness [25,45,48] (see Graphical Abstract). In turn, these temperament precursors interact with parenting that is harsh and low in warmth to further increase risk for CU behaviors [18,45,47,48]. Specifically, we hypothesize that CU behaviors arise from the interaction of two heritable pathways: (1) Inherited low interpersonal emotional sensitivity sets the foundation for failure to develop affective empathy, operationalized via low emotional contagion in infancy, and fewer facial or verbal expressions of concern for others’ distress, low positive affect, and eye contact deficits from age 2 onwards; (2) Inherited fearlessness sets the foundation for a failure to develop behavioral inhibition to threat, including non-social threat (e.g., developmentally-appropriate “scary” stimuli) and social threat (e.g., parental punishment), which lead to high approach, reward dominance, and difficulty learning from punishment. Accordingly, children are less likely, via contingency processes, to learn to associate fear/other aversive emotions with actions that involve risk, cause harm, or invoke punishment, meaning that such actions will be repeated [43]. This combination of low interpersonal emotional sensitivity and fearlessness is most risky in the context of parental harshness or low warmth (i.e., person-by-context interactions). In particular, reciprocal parent-child interactions occur over time that exacerbate risk for CU behaviors (i.e., temperamentally fearless children evoke harsher parenting; children low on interpersonal emotional sensitivity evoke less warm parenting). Of course, beyond these two hypothesized pathways, other temperament factors are likely important for understanding how CU behaviors give rise to more severe forms of antisocial behavior and psychopathy across the lifespan, including poor executive function and disinhibition/impulsivity [42,49]

Conclusions

CU behaviors in early childhood predict behavior problems across childhood, tapping a construct involving low empathy and limited prosociality. Early CU behaviors appear to be both heritable and non-heritable in origin, with parenting practices serving a critical component of the person-by-context interactions implicated in the development of CU behaviors. Recent research suggests that CU behaviors emerge from the combination of low interpersonal emotional sensitivity and fearlessness, temperaments that appear to be inherited and subsequently interact with parenting. This work is consistent with basic developmental studies that report similar child-parent interactions as fundamental to the development of affective empathy and prosociality, suggesting shared mechanisms in the development of these attributes (i.e., that CU behaviors are, at least partially, the “other side of the coin” from affective empathy). Ultimately, the study of CU behaviors in early childhood can inform our understanding of the development of severe antisocial behavior and psychopathy across development, and can be guided by our understanding of the normative and atypical development of moral behaviors, such as empathy and prosociality.

Highlights.

  • Callous-unemotional (CU) behaviors predict more severe childhood behavior problems

  • Parenting practices predict the development of CU behaviors in early childhood

  • Inherited traits contribute to CU behaviors via child-context interactions

  • We offer a developmental model for CU behaviors beginning in infancy

Acknowledgments

We thank all of our collaborators who have supported our work on early CU behaviors, particularly Daniel S. Shaw and investigators from the Early Steps Study and Early Growth and Development Study. R. Waller was supported by a NIAAA T32 Fellowship in the Addiction Center, Department of Psychiatry, University of Michigan (2T32AA007477-24A1). L.W. Hyde was supported by Grant L40MH108392.

References

  • 1.Hoffman ML. Empathy and prosocial behavior. In: Lewis M, Haviland-Jones JM, Feldman Barrett L, editors. Handbook of Emotions. Guildford Press; 2010. pp. 440–455. [Google Scholar]
  • 2**.Zahn-Waxler C, Radke-Yarrow M, Wagner E, Chapman M. Development of concern for others. Developmental Psychology. 1992;28:126–136. Using observational paradigms, this study showed that children’s propensity to help, share, and provide comfort to mothers and unfamiliar persons emerges by age 2. [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Brownell CA. Early development of prosocial behavior: Current perspectives. Infancy. 2013;18:1–9. doi: 10.1111/infa.12004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4**.Warneken F, Tomasello M. Altruistic helping in human infants and young chimpanzees. Science. 2006;311:1301–1303. doi: 10.1126/science.1121448. This experimental study established that 18-month children are capable of enacting instrumental helping behaviors towards an unfamiliar adult, with some comparable behaviors shown in 3-year old chimpanzees. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5*.Frick PJ, Ray JV, Thornton LC, Kahn RE. Annual Research Review: A developmental psychopathology approach to understanding callous-unemotional traits in children and adolescents with serious conduct problems. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2014;55:532–548. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12152. This paper provides a comprehensive review of the literature examining callous-unemotional traits in older children and adolescents, including measurement, meaning, predictive validity, and risk factors. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Frick PJ. 2004, Unpublished rating scale. The Inventory of Callous-Unemotional Traits. [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Ezpeleta L, de la Osa N, Granero R, Penelo E, Domenech JM. Inventory of callous-unemotional traits in a community sample of preschoolers. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology. 2013;42:91–105. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2012.734221. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8**.Kimonis ER, Fanti KA, Anastassiou-Hadjicharalambous X, Mertan B, Goulter N, Katsimicha E. Can callous-unemotional traits be reliably measured in preschoolers? Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2016;44:625–638. doi: 10.1007/s10802-015-0075-y. This empirical paper established that children under 6 years old with high callous-unemotional behaviors and behavior problems show poorer recognition of facial expressions and less attentional orientating to distress cues. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9**.Waller R, Hyde LW, Grabell AS, Alves ML, Olson SL. Differential associations of early callous unemotional, oppositional, and ADHD behaviors: multiple domains within early starting conduct problems? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2015;56:657–666. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12326. This paper found separable correlates of callous-unemotional, oppositional, and attention-deficit behaviors in 3-year old children, with callous-unemotional behaviors being uniquely correlated with lower empathy, guilt, and moral regulation, and more teacher-reported proactive aggression. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Willoughby MT, Mills-Koonce RW, Gottfredson NC, Wagner NJ. Measuring callous unemotional behaviors in early childhood: factor structure and the prediction of stable aggression in middle childhood. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment. 2014;36:30–42. doi: 10.1007/s10862-013-9379-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Waller R, Hyde LW, Baskin-Sommers AR, Olson SL. Interactions between callous unemotional behaviors and executive function in early childhood predict later aggression and lower peer-liking in late-childhood. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2016 doi: 10.1007/s10802-016-0184-2. epub ahead of print. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Waller R, Dishion TJ, Shaw DS, Gardner F, Wilson M, Hyde LW. Does early childhood callous-unemotional behavior uniquely predict behavior problems or callous-unemotional behavior in late childhood? Developmental Psychology. 2016 doi: 10.1037/dev0000165. in press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Dunn J. Moral development in early childhood and social interaction in the family. In: Killen M, Smetana JG, editors. Handbook of Moral Development. Lawrence Erlbaum; 2006. pp. 331–350. [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Eisenberg N, Spinrad TL, Knafo-Noam A. Prosocial development. In: Lamb ME, Garcia Coll C, Lerner RM, editors. Handbook of Child Psychology, Vol 3, Social, emotional, and personality development. 7th. Wiley; 2015. pp. 610–656. [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Murphy TP, Laible DJ. The influence of attachment security on preschool children’s empathic concern. International Journal of Behavioral Development. 2013;37:426–440. [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Carlo G, Mestre MV, Samper P, Tur A, Armenta BE. The longitudinal relations among dimensions of parenting styles, sympathy, prosocial moral reasoning, and prosocial behaviors. International Journal of Behavioral Development. 2011;35:116–124. [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Kochanska G. Mutually responsive orientation between mothers and their young children: A context for the early development of conscience. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2002;11:191–195. [Google Scholar]
  • 18**.Kochanska G. Multiple pathways to conscience for children with different temperaments: from toddlerhood to age 5. Developmental Psychology. 1997;33:228. doi: 10.1037//0012-1649.33.2.228. This study established that individual differences conscience development depend on interactions between children’s temperament and early maternal socialization efforts. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Ellis BJ, Boyce WT, Belsky J, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Van IJzendoorn MH. Differential susceptibility to the environment: An evolutionary–neurodevelopmental theory. Development and Psychopathology. 2011;23:7–28. doi: 10.1017/S0954579410000611. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20*.Waller R, Gardner F, Hyde LW. What are the associations between parenting, callous–unemotional traits, and antisocial behavior in youth? A systematic review of evidence. Clinical Psychology Review. 2013;33:593–608. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2013.03.001. This systematic review established that parenting practices influence callous-unemotional traits across multiple stages of development, and that parenting interventions can be effective in reducing callous-unemotional traits and behavior problems. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Waller R, Gardner F, Viding E, Shaw DS, Dishion TJ, Wilson MN, Hyde LW. Bidirectional associations between parental warmth, callous unemotional behavior, and behavior problems in high-risk preschoolers. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2014;42:1275–1285. doi: 10.1007/s10802-014-9871-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Mills-Koonce WR, Willoughby MT, Garrett-Peters P, Wagner N, Vernon-Feagans L. The interplay among socioeconomic status, household chaos, and parenting in the prediction of child conduct problems and callous–unemotional behaviors. Development and Psychopathology. 2016;28:757–771. doi: 10.1017/S0954579416000298. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Pasalich DS, Dadds MR, Hawes DJ, Brennan J. Do callous unemotional traits moderate the relative importance of parental coercion versus warmth in child conduct problems? An observational study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2011;52:1308–1315. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2011.02435.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Waller R, Gardner F, Hyde LW, Shaw DS, Dishion TJ, Wilson MN. Do harsh and positive parenting predict parent reports of deceitful callous behavior in early childhood? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2012;53:946–953. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2012.02550.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Barker ED, Oliver BR, Viding E, Salekin RT, Maughan B. The impact of prenatal maternal risk, fearless temperament and early parenting on adolescent callous‐unemotional traits: a 14 year longitudinal investigation. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2011;52:878–888. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2011.02397.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Markowitz AJ, Ryan RM, Marsh AA. Neighborhood income and the expression of callous–unemotional traits. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 2015;24:1103–1118. doi: 10.1007/s00787-014-0663-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Waller R, Shaw DS, Forbes EE, Hyde LW. Understanding Early Contextual and Parental Risk Factors for the Development of Limited Prosocial Emotions. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2015;43:1025–1039. doi: 10.1007/s10802-014-9965-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28*.Waller R, Shaw DS, Neiderhiser JM, Ganiban JM, Natsuaki MN, Reiss D, Trentacosta CJ, Leve LD, Hyde LW. Towards an understanding of the role of the environment in the development of early callous behavior. Journal of Personality. 2017;85:90–103. doi: 10.1111/jopy.12221. This review paper summarizes the literature linking environmental risk factors, including parenting practices, income inequality, and neighborhood impoverishment, to callous-unemotional behaviors in early childhood. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Flom M, Saudino KJ. Callous–unemotional behaviors in early childhood: Genetic and environmental contributions to stability and change. Development and Psychopathology. 2016 doi: 10.1017/S0954579416001267. epub ahead of print. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Knafo A, Zahn-Waxler C, Van Hulle C, Robinson JL, Rhee SH. The developmental origins of a disposition toward empathy: Genetic and environmental contributions. Emotion. 2008;8:737–752. doi: 10.1037/a0014179. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Knafo A, Israel S, Ebstein RP. Heritability of children’s prosocial behavior and differential susceptibility to parenting by variation in the dopamine receptor D4 gene. Development and Psychopathology. 2011;23:53–67. doi: 10.1017/S0954579410000647. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32**.Hyde LW, Waller R, Trentacosta CJ, Shaw DS, Neiderhiser JM, Ganiban J, Reiss D, Leve LD. Heritable and non-heritable pathways to early callous unemotional behavior. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2016;173:903–910. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2016.15111381. This is the first study to identify a heritable pathway from biological mother severe antisocial behavior to children’s early callous-unemotional behaviors, which was buffered by highly positive parenting of adoptive parents. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33**.Bedford R, Pickles A, Sharp H, Wright N, Hill J. Reduced Face Preference in Infancy: A Developmental Precursor to Callous-Unemotional Traits? Biological Psychiatry. 2015;78:83–84. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.09.022. This empirical study established a link between reduced attention to the caregiver’s face at 5 weeks old and subsequent callous-unemotional behaviors at 2.5 years old. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Wagner N, Mills-Koonce R, Willoughby M, Propper C, Rehder P, Gueron-Sela N. Respiratory sinus arrhythmia and heart period in infancy as correlates of later oppositional defiant and callous-unemotional behaviors. International Journal of Behavioral Development. 2017;4:127–135. doi: 10.1177/0165025415605391. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Dadds MR, Allen JL, Oliver BR, Faulkner N, Legge K, Moul C, Woolgar M, Scott S. Love, eye contact and the developmental origins of empathy v. psychopathy. The British Journal of Psychiatry. 2012;200:191–196. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.110.085720. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.White SF, Briggs-Gowan MJ, Voss JL, Petitclerc A, McCarthy KR, Blair RJ, Wakschlag LS. Can the fear recognition deficits associated with callous-unemotional traits be identified in early childhood? Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology. 2016;38:672–684. doi: 10.1080/13803395.2016.1149154. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37**.Jones AP, Happé FG, Gilbert F, Burnett S, Viding E. Feeling, caring, knowing: different types of empathy deficit in boys with psychopathic tendencies and autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2010;51:1188–1197. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02280.x. This study established a dissociation whereby children high on callous-unemotional traits showed impaired affective but intact cognitive empathy, whereas children high on autistic traits showed impaired cognitive but intact affective empathy. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Roth-Hanania R, Davidov M, Zahn-Waxler C. Empathy development from 8 to 16 months: Early signs of concern for others. Infant Behavior and Development. 2011;34:447–458. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2011.04.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Geangu E, Benga O, Stahl D, Striano T. Contagious crying beyond the first days of life. Infant Behavior and Development. 2010;33:279–288. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2010.03.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Zahn-Waxler C, Robinson JL, Emde RN. The development of empathy in twins. Developmental Psychology. 1992;28:1038–1047. [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Rothbart MK. Temperament, development, and personality. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2007;16:207–212. [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Gray JA. A model for Personality. Springer; 1981. A critique of Eysenck’s theory of personality; pp. 246–276. Edited by. [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Blair RJR. The neurobiology of psychopathic traits in youths. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2013;14:786–799. doi: 10.1038/nrn3577. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44*.Glenn AL, Raine A, Venables PH, Mednick SA. Early temperamental and psychophysiological precursors of adult psychopathic personality. Personality Disorders: Theory, Research, and Treatment. 2009;S(1):46–60. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.116.3.508. This study was the first to link fearlessness in early childhood to psychopathy in adulthood. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45**.Waller R, Trentacosta C, Shaw DS, Neiderhiser JM, Ganiban J, Reiss D, Leve LD, Hyde LW. Heritable temperament pathways to early callous-unemotional behavior. British Journal of Psychiatry. 2016;209:475–482. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.116.181503. This is the first genetically-informed study to establish a link between inherited tempermental fearlessness and early childhood callous-unemotional behaviors, which was buffered by highly positive parenting of adoptive mothers. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Fowles DC, Kochanska G. Temperament as a moderator of pathways to conscience in children: The contribution of electrodermal activity. Psychophysiology. 2000;37:788–795. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Waller R, Shaw DS, Hyde LW. Observed fearlessness and positive parenting interact to predict childhood callous-unemotional behaviors among low-income boys. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2017;58:282–291. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12666. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48*.Willoughby MT, Mills-Koonce R, Propper CB, Waschbusch DA. Observed parenting behaviors interact with a polymorphism of the brain-derived neurotrophic factor gene to predict the emergence of oppositional defiant and callous–unemotional behaviors at age 3 years. Development and Psychopathology. 2013;25:903–917. doi: 10.1017/S0954579413000266. This paper examined person-by-context interactions in the early etiology of callous-unemotional behaviors focusing on specifically on risky child genotype and interactions with parenting practices. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Patrick CJ, Fowles DC, Krueger RF. Triarchic conceptualization of psychopathy: Developmental origins of disinhibition, boldness, and meanness. Development and Psychopathology. 2009;21:913–938. doi: 10.1017/S0954579409000492. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES