Skip to main content
International Journal of Epidemiology logoLink to International Journal of Epidemiology
. 2016 Jul 10;45(4):1226–1235. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyw146

Childhood socioeconomic disadvantage is associated with lower mortality in older Japanese men: the JAGES cohort study

Yukako Tani 1,2,3,4, Naoki Kondo 1, Yuiko Nagamine 5, Tomohiro Shinozaki 6, Katsunori Kondo 5,7, Ichiro Kawachi 8, Takeo Fujiwara 2,4,*
PMCID: PMC5965916  PMID: 27401729

Abstract

Background: Childhood socioeconomic disadvantage has been previously linked to increased mortality risk in adulthood. However, most previous studies have focused on middle-aged adults in Western contexts. Here, we sought to examine the association between childhood socioeconomic status (SES) and mortality among healthy older Japanese adults.

Methods: We conducted a 3-year follow-up of participants in the Japan Gerontological Evaluation Study (JAGES), a population-based cohort of 65- to 103-year-old Japanese adults. Childhood SES was assessed by survey at baseline. Mortality from 2010 to 2013 was analysed for 15 449 respondents (7143 men and 8306 women). Cox regression models were used to estimate hazard ratios (HR) for risk of death.

Results: A total of 754 deaths occurred during the 3-year follow-up. Lower childhood SES was significantly associated with lower mortality in men, but not in women. Compared with men growing up in more advantaged childhood socioeconomic circumstances, the age-adjusted HR for men from low childhood SES backgrounds was 0.75 [95% confidence interval (CI): 0.56–1.00]. The association remained significant after adjustment for height, education, adult SES, municipalities of residence, health behaviours, disease status and current social relationships (HR = 0.64; 95% CI 0.47–0.87). This association was stronger among men aged 75 years or older, HR = 0.67 (95% CI: 0.47–0.95), compared with men aged 65–74 years, HR = 0.90 (95% CI: 0.54–1.51).

Conclusions: Childhood socioeconomic disadvantage is associated with lower mortality among men aged 75 years or older, which may be due to selective survival, or alternatively to childhood physical training or postwar calorie restriction in this generation of Japanese males.

Keywords: Childhood socioeconomic status, mortality, older adults, life course epidemiology


Key messages

  • Childhood socioeconomic disadvantage was ‘paradoxically’ associated with lower mortality in Japanese men, but not in women.

  • The association between childhood socioeconomic disadvantage and lower mortality was stronger among the 75-years-old or older participants compared with those aged 65–74 years, which may be due to selective survival, childhood physical training or postwar calorie restriction in this generation of Japanese males.

Introduction

Childhood socioeconomic disadvantage is usually associated with increased risks of disease and mortality in adulthood, especially from cardiovascular disease.1 The mechanism of this association is attributed to a combination of latency effects–for example, exposure to infectious disease during critical periods of development–and accumulation of risks over the life course (for example, low SES (socioeconomic status) children are more likely to be born with low birthweight, which may in turn affect school performance and SES achieved in adulthood).2,3

Most of the evidence, however, derives from middle-aged populations in Western contexts.4–9 In Japan, improvements in life expectancy during the past half-century have been achieved in part through a swift decline in mortality rates from communicable diseases and stroke, beginning in the 1950s to early 1960s, followed by the introduction of universal health coverage from 1961.10,11 High levels of literacy and education, as well as comparative equality in the distribution of incomes (at least through the 1980s) also contributed to the expansion of Japanese longevity.10,12,13

In addition to the foregoing factors, exposure to poverty and undernutrition in the aftermath of the Second World War (WWII) may be contributing to Japanese longevity, especially for the generation exposed to these conditions during childhood, i.e. people who are aged 70 years and older today. That is, the estimated average calorie intake after WWII in Japan was around 1900–2100 kcal,14 which was lower than the estimated energy intake for Japanese in 2015 (2850 and 2300 kcal for men and women, respectively, aged 15–17 years, physical activity level: 2),15 and substantially lower than postwar UK calorie intake (2474 kcal in 1950).16 Although the study was initiated in adult monkeys, experimental evidence from animal studies suggests that calorie restriction reduces age-related morbidity and prolongs survival.17 When initiated in young monkeys, there was a trend for a delay in age-associated disease onset in calorie-restricted monkeys.18 We therefore hypothesized that Japanese individuals who survived deprived circumstances in childhood might paradoxically experience lower mortality in later adulthood.

The Japan Gerontological Evaluation Study (JAGES) is a large-scale cohort study of community-dwelling older Japanese people aged 65 or older at the time of cohort inception in 2010. The youngest cohort participants were born around 1945 and grew up in the postwar period. They survived food shortages during and after WWII,19 and the study thus affords a test of the hypothesis that deprived childhood socioeconomic circumstances might prolong longevity.

Materials and Methods

Study design and subjects

The JAGES was established in 2010 to evaluate the social determinants of healthy ageing among non-disabled people aged 65 years or older, sampled from 23 municipalities in 8 (out of 47) prefectures throughout Japan. The baseline survey was conducted between August 2010 and January 2012, wherein self-reported questionnaires were distributed by mail to 106 468 people aged 65 years or older who were physically and cognitively independent and living independently in the community. A simple random sample was obtained from the official residence registers in 12 large municipalities, as well as a complete census of older residents residing in the remaining 11 smaller municipalities. A total of 70 538 participants returned the questionnaire (response rate: 66%). One-fifth of the sample (n = 16 465) were randomly selected to receive the survey module enquiring about childhood socioeconomic circumstances. The analytical sample for the present study comprised 15 449 participants (7143 men and 8306 women), after the following exclusions: participants missing responses to the questions related to childhood SES (N = 682); and participants who reported limitations in activities of daily living, defined as being unable to walk, take a bath or use the toilet without assistance, who were included in this study accidentally, to ensure that the sample is actually physically and cognitively independent (N = 334).20 The Human Subjects Committees of Nihon Fukushi University (No. 10–05) and Chiba University Faculty of Medicine (No. 1777) approved the parent JAGES protocol. The Ethics Committee of the University of Tokyo, Faculty of Medicine (No. 10555) approved the analysis of data in the present study.

Ascertainment of vital status

Ascertainment of vital status during the mean follow-up period of 3 years, from 2010 to 2013 (minimum: 2.1 years, maximum: 3.8 years), was conducted by linking the cohort participants to the mortality records of the national long-term care insurance database. Under the Japanese long-term care insurance system (introduced in 2001), physicians submit information from death records to their local municipal government. Among the respondents, 98% of participants were successfully linked to the death records, and a total of 754 deaths occurred during the 3-year follow-up (cumulative mortality = 754/15 449, 4.9%). Number of cumulative mortality (deaths) was 306 (11.7%) for the 75 years or older men, 192 (4.3%) for 65–74-years-old men, 170 (5.1%) for the 75 years or older women, 86 (1.7%) for 65–74-years-old women (Supplementary Table 2, available as Supplementary data at IJE online). Information on specific cause of death was not released due to the small number of deaths in each municipality, and thus our main outcome was all-cause mortality.

Childhood socioeconomic status

We asked participants to rate their childhood socioeconomic circumstances using the following question: ‘How would you rate your socioeconomic status at the age of 15 years according to standards at that time?’ For example, an 80-year old respondent in 2010 would be recalling circumstances in 1945 when they were 15 years old. Responses were arranged on a five-point Likert scale: ‘high’, ‘middle-high’, ‘middle’, ‘middle-low’ and ‘low’. We collapsed responses into three categories: high (including ‘high’ and ‘middle-high’), middle, and low, (including ‘middle-low’ and ‘low’) to maximize the sample size for each category.21

Covariates

Age at entry was categorized into four groups (65–69, 70–74, 75–79 and ≥ 80 years; corresponding birth years are 1941–45, 1936–40, 1931–35 and –1930, respectively). Potential mediators of the association between childhood SES and mortality, such as years of schooling, height, adult SES, health behaviours, disease status, depressive symptoms and current social relationships, were assessed by self-administered questionnaire. Height was used as a proxy of the childhood nutritional environment and disease history,22 and was categorized into five groups at 5-cm intervals for each sex: for men: < 155, 155–159.9, 160–164.9, 165–169.9 and ≥ 170 cm; and for women: < 145, 145–149.9, 150–154.9, 155–159.9 and ≥ 160cm.21 Years of schooling were categorized into three groups (≤ 9, 10–12 or ≥ 13 years). Indicators of adulthood SES included current annual household income (< 2.00, 2.00–3.99 or ≥  4.00 million yen), home ownership (own home, or renting) and longest-held occupation, categorized as: non-manual (professional, technical or managerial workers), manual (clerical, sales/service, skilled/labour or agricultural/forestry/fishery workers or other) or no occupation.23 Health behaviours included smoking status (non-smoker or smoker/ex-smoker), frequency of meat/fish intake (≥ 1/day or < 1/day), frequency of vegetable/fruit intake (≥ 1/day or < 1/day), walking time (< 30 min/day or ≥  30 min/day) and body mass index [obesity (≥ 30.0 kg/m2), overweight (25.0–29.9 kg/m2), normal (18.5–24.9 kg/m2) or underweight (< 18.5 kg/m2)]. History of disease was assessed whether currently under medical treatment for any of the following 21 diseases (multiple responses were allowed): cancer, heart disease, stroke, diabetes mellitus, respiratory disease or others (hypertension, obesity, hyperlipidaemia, osteoporosis, joint disease/neuralgia, external injury/fracture, gastrointestinal disease, liver disease, psychiatric disease, dysphagia, visual impairment, hearing impairment, impaired excretion, sleep disorder and other). Depressive symptoms were assessed by the 15-item short form of the Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS) (Japanese version).24 The assessment of current social relationships included marital status (married, widowed or divorced/unmarried/other), number of social interactions with friends/acquaintances during the past month (≤ 5 vs 6 or more times), providing and receiving emotional social support,25,26 and social participation in a variety of groups or activities such as volunteer groups, sports groups, leisure activity groups, senior citizens’ clubs, neighbourhood or residents’ associations, etc.

Statistical analysis

Data were analysed separately for men and women because the interaction term between sex and childhood SES showed marginal interaction effect (P = 0.19), and previous study supports sex difference in the impact of SES on mortality in Japanese.27 Cox proportional hazards models were estimated, yielding hazard ratios (HR) and 95% confidence intervals (CI) for all-cause mortality over the 3-year follow-up period. The following sequence of models was constructed: Model 1 was adjusted for age; Model 2 was additionally adjusted for height and education; Model 3 was further adjusted for adult SES (current annual household income, home ownership and longest-held occupation) as well as municipality of residence; Model 4 was additionally adjusted for health behaviours (smoking, frequency of meat or fish intake, frequency of vegetable or fruit intake, walking time and BMI), disease status and depressive symptoms; and Model 5 was additionally adjusted for current social relationships (marital status, number of social interactions, providing and receiving emotional social support, social participation). We further stratified our analyses by age group–65–74 years old vs 75 years or older–since the impact of childhood SES on mortality might differ for those who experienced WWII before adolescence (i.e. 65–74 years old) or after adolescence (75 years or older). We applied the multiple imputation methods for adequately addressing missing data on covariates. We generated 50 imputed datasets using the multiple imputation by chained equations by the ‘mi impute chained’ command in STATA. All analyses were conducted using Statistical Analysis Systems software version 9.4 (SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC, USA) and STATA v13.

Results

Overall, 53.1% of men and 37.3% of women reported low or middle-low SES in childhood and 8.7% of men and 16.2% of women reported high or middle-high SES (Table 1). As shown in Supplementary Table 1 (available as Supplementary data at IJE online), the validity of self-reported childhood socioeconomic circumstances is supported by the correlation between low childhood SES and other indicators of deprivation, such as height, years of schooling and adult socioeconomic attainment. Around half of participants completed less than 10 years of formal schooling. Interestingly, percentage of missing values on current SES and smoking status was higher among females than males, which may represent the current Japanese situation among older adults that females worked as homemakers and tend not to work outside the home;28 thus they fail to capture their current SES. Lower prevalence of smoking among older females may reflect their reluctance to admit to a socially proscribed habit.29

Table 1.

Characteristics of older Japanese male (n = 7143) and female (n = 8306) participants

Male (n = 7143) Female (n = 8306)
N % N %
Age (years)
    65–69 (birth year: 1941–45) 2496 34.9 2663 32.1
    70–74 (birth year: 1936–40) 2022 28.3 2305 27.8
    75–79 (birth year: 1931–35) 1489 20.9 1768 21.3
    80 ≤ (birth year: -1930) 1136 15.9 1570 18.9
Childhood SES
    High or middle-high 620 8.7 1346 16.2
    Middle 2733 38.3 3859 46.5
    Middle-low or low 3790 53.1 3101 37.3
Heighta
    Short 592 8.3 1142 13.8
    Middle-short 1220 17.1 2376 28.6
    Middle 2276 31.9 2637 31.8
    Middle-tall 1810 25.3 1301 15.7
    Tall 922 12.9 360 4.3
    Missing 323 4.5 490 5.9
Education (years)
    High (≥ 13) 1330 18.6 801 9.6
    Middle (10–12) 2194 30.7 2579 31.1
    Low (≤ 9) 3316 46.4 4545 54.7
    Other 43 0.6 41 0.5
    Missing 260 3.64 340 4.1
Normalized current household income (million yen)
    High (≥ 4.00) 726 10.2 685 8.3
    Middle (2.00–3.99) 2468 34.6 2276 27.4
    Low (< 2.00) 2914 40.8 3278 39.5
    Missing 1035 14.5 2067 24.9
Living situation
    Own home 6436 90.1 7384 88.9
    Rent home 330 4.6 457 5.5
    Other 47 0.7 90 1.1
    Missing 330 4.6 375 4.5
Longest occupation
    Non-manual 2285 32.0 732 8.8
    Manual 4230 59.2 5225 62.9
    No occupation 27 0.4 625 7.5
    Missing 601 8.4 1724 20.8
Smoking
    Non-smoker 1720 24.1 6664 80.2
    Smoker/ex-smoker 4892 68.5 557 6.7
    Missing 531 7.4 1085 13.1
Frequency of meat or fish intake over the past month
    ≥ 1/day 2503 35.0 3308 39.8
    < 1/day 4178 58.5 4506 54.3
    Missing 462 6.5 492 5.9
Frequency of vegetable or fruit intake over the past month
    ≥ 1/day 5026 70.4 6570 79.1
    < 1/day 1688 23.6 1313 15.8
    Missing 429 6.0 423 5.1
Walking time
    < 30 min/day 2082 29.2 2771 33.4
    ≥ 30 min/day 4629 64.8 4874 58.7
    Missing 432 6.1 661 8.0
Body weight status (BMI, kg/m2)
    Obesity (≥ 30.0) 138 1.9 246 3.0
    Overweight (25.0–29.9) 1478 20.7 1511 18.2
    Normal (18.5–24.9) 4804 67.3 5364 64.6
    Underweight (< 18.5) 374 5.2 638 7.7
    Missing 349 4.9 547 6.6
Disease
    Cancer (yes) 431 6.0 236 2.8
    Heart disease (yes) 1029 14.4 759 9.1
    Stroke (yes) 141 2.0 72 0.9
    Diabetes mellitus (yes) 1133 15.9 862 10.4
    Respiratory disease (yes) 329 4.6 212 2.6
    Other (yes) 4574 64.0 5950 71.6
Depressive symptoms
    No depression (GDS < 5) 4438 62.1 4797 57.8
    Moderate depression (5 ≤ GDS < 10) 1261 17.7 1397 16.8
    Depression (10 ≤GDS) 412 5.8 481 5.8
    Missing 1032 14.5 1631 19.6
Marital status
    Married 6071 85.0 4748 57.2
    Widowed 570 8.0 2741 33.0
    Divorced/unmarried/other 248 3.5 458 5.5
    Missing 254 3.6 359 4.3
Social participation
    Yes 4888 68.4 5375 64.7
    No 1569 22.0 1964 23.7
    Missing 686 9.6 967 11.6
Number of social interactions with friends/acquaintances
    ≤ 5/month 3376 47.3 3441 41.4
    ≥ 6/month 3232 45.3 4111 49.5
    Missing 535 7.5 754 9.1
Social support (providing)
    Present 6019 84.3 7144 86.0
    Absent 572 8.0 476 5.7
    Missing 552 7.7 686 8.3
Social support (receiving)
    Present 6114 85.6 7482 90.1
    Absent 541 7.6 277 3.3
    Missing 488 6.8 547 6.6

aHeight: < 155, 155–159.9, 160–164.9, 165–169.9 and ≥ 170 cm for men, and < 145, 145–149.9, 150–154.9, 155–159.9 and ≥ 160 cm for women.

Data were analysed separately men from middle-low or low SES backgrounds in childhood. These 0.75 times less likely to die (95% CI: 0.56–1.00) compared with those who grew up in high or middle-high socioeconomic circumstance in childhood (Table 2). The proportional hazards assumption was checked by log-log survival plots. We observed no serious violation of the proportional hazards assumption. Successive adjustment for height and education, adult SES, municipality of residence, health behaviours, disease status and depressive symptoms strengthened the protective association between childhood deprivation and reduced mortality risk (HR 0.65; 95% CI: 0.48–0.88) (Model 4, Table 2). In the final model, after adjustment for social relationships, men from middle-low or low SES backgrounds in childhood were 0.64 (95% CI: 0.47–0.87) times less likely to die compared with their more advantaged peers. In contrast to men, no significant association between childhood SES and mortality was found for women (Table 3).

Table 2.

Hazard ratios (HR) with 95% CI for association of mortality with childhood SES in older Japanese men (n = 7143)

Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4 Model 5
HR (95%CI) HR (95%CI) HR (95%CI) HR (95%CI) HR (95%CI)
Childhood SES
High or middle-high ref ref ref ref ref
Middle 0.80 (0.60–1.08) 0.77 (0.57–1.03) 0.78 (0.58–1.06) 0.75 (0.56–1.02) 0.75 (0.55–1.01)
Middle-low or low 0.75 (0.56–1.00)* 0.69 (0.51–0.93)* 0.71 (0.52–0.95)* 0.65 (0.48–0.88)** 0.64 (0.47–0.87)**
Childhood other environment
Height Short ref ref ref ref
Middle-short 0.66 (0.48–0.91)* 0.69 (0.50–0.95)* 0.67 (0.49–0.93)* 0.67 (0.48–0.93)*
Middle 0.71 (0.53–0.95)* 0.72 (0.54–0.97)* 0.67 (0.50–0.89)** 0.67 (0.50–0.90)**
Middle-tall 0.79 (0.58–1.07) 0.81 (0.60–1.10) 0.73 (0.53–0.99)* 0.74 (0.54–1.01)
Tall 0.79 (0.54–1.14) 0.79 (0.55–1.15) 0.68 (0.47–0.99)* 0.68 (0.47–0.99)*
Education High ref ref ref ref
Middle 1.05 (0.79–1.40) 1.03 (0.77–1.38) 1.01 (0.76–1.35) 0.99 (0.74–1.33)
Low 1.26 (0.97–1.64) 1.15 (0.86–1.52) 1.12 (0.84–1.48) 1.06 (0.80–1.40)
Other 0.65 (0.16–2.65) 0.64 (0.16–2.60) 0.60 (0.15–2.47) 0.56 (0.13–2.30)

Model 1: adjusted for age; Model 2: Model 1 + adjusted for childhood other environment (height and education; Model 3: Model 2 + adjusted for adult SES (normalized current household income, living situation and longest occupation) and municipalities of residence; Model 4: Model 3 + adjusted for health behaviours: smoking, frequency of meat or fish and vegetable or fruit intakes, walking time, BMI, disease status, and depressive symptoms; Model 5: Model 4 + adjusted for social relationship (marital status, number of social interactions with friends/acquaintances, providing and receiving emotional social support, and social participation).

*P < 0.05;

**P < 0.01.

Table 3.

Hazard ratios (HR) with 95% CI for association of mortality with childhood SES in older Japanese women (n = 8306)

Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4 Model 5
HR (95%CI) HR (95%CI) HR (95%CI) HR (95%CI) HR (95%CI)
Childhood SES
High or middle-high ref ref ref ref ref
Middle 0.98 (0.69–1.38) 0.97 (0.68–1.38) 0.96 (0.67–1.38) 0.97 (0.68–1.39) 0.95 (0.66–1.36)
Middle-low or low 1.04 (0.73–1.48) 0.98 (0.67–1.42) 0.98 (0.67–1.43) 0.94 (0.64–1.38) 0.91 (0.62–1.34)
Other characteristics
Height Short ref ref ref ref
Middle-short 0.76 (0.54–1.07) 0.79 (0.56–1.10) 0.77 (0.55–1.09) 0.78 (0.55–1.10)
Middle 0.68 (0.48–0.99)* 0.70 (0.49–1.01) 0.67 (0.47–0.97)* 0.68 (0.47–0.99)*
Middle-tall 0.78 (0.50–1.22) 0.81 (0.51–1.27) 0.76 (0.48–1.20) 0.76 (0.48–1.21)
Tall 0.84 (0.40–1.77) 0.84 (0.40–1.78) 0.77 (0.36–1.64) 0.77 (0.36–1.64)
Education High ref ref ref ref
Middle 0.70 (0.42–1.11) 0.65 (0.40–1.06) 0.69 (0.42–1.13) 0.70 (0.42–1.15)
Low 0.92 (0.58–1.44) 0.84 (0.53–1.34) 0.92 (0.57–1.47) 0.90 (0.56–1.44)
Other 1.23 (0.28–5.12) 1.07 (0.25–4.64) 1.14 (0.26–4.92) 1.06 (0.24–4.60)

Model 1: adjusted for age; Model 2: Model 1 + adjusted for childhood other environment (height and education); Model 3: Model 2 + adjusted for adult SES (current normalized household income, living situation and longest occupation) and municipalities of residence; Model 4: Model 3 + adjusted for health behaviours: smoking, frequency of meat or fish and vegetable or fruit intakes, walking time, BMI, disease status and depressive symptoms;

Model 5: Model 4 + adjusted for social relationship (marital status, number of social interactions with friends/acquaintances, providing and receiving emotional social support, and social participation).

*P < 0.05;

**P < 0.01.

When we age-stratified the analyses, the protective association of deprived childhood socioeconomic circumstances and mortality was found for the 75-years-old or older males, but not for the 65–74-years-old males (Table 4), although the interaction term between age group and childhood SES was not statistically significant (P = 0.15). Among women, childhood SES was not associated with mortality in either age group.

Table 4.

Hazard ratios (HR) with 95% CI for association of mortality with childhood SES in older Japanese men (n = 7143) and women (n = 8306) by age group

Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4 Model 5
HR (95%CI) HR (95%CI) HR (95%CI) HR (95%CI) HR (95%CI)
Male Younger (65–74 years)
Childhood SES High or middle-high ref ref ref ref ref
Middle 0.84 (0.49–1.43) 0.81 (0.47–1.38) 0.88 (0.51–1.51) 0.79 (0.46–1.36) 0.78 (0.45–1.35)
Middle-low or low 0.90 (0.54–1.51) 0.84 (0.50–1.41) 0.88 (0.52–1.49) 0.78 (0.46–1.33) 0.76 (0.45–1.31)
Older (75 ≤ years)
Childhood SES High or middle-high ref ref ref ref ref
Middle 0.79 (0.55–1.12) 0.75 (0.53–1.08) 0.76 (0.53–1.09) 0.75 (0.52–1.08) 0.74 (0.51–1.07)
Middle-low or low 0.67 (0.47–0.95)* 0.61 (0.42–0.88)** 0.63 (0.43–0.91)* 0.58 (0.40–0.85)** 0.57 (0.39–0.83)**
Female Younger (65–74 years)
Childhood SES High or middle-high ref ref ref ref ref
Middle 0.81 (0.43–1.51) 0.81 (0.43–1.52) 0.77 (0.40–1.46) 0.76 (0.40–1.46) 0.72 (0.38–1.39)
Middle-low or low 1.11 (0.60–2.04) 1.05 (0.55–1.98) 1.00 (0.52–1.92) 1.01 (0.52–1.96) 0.95 (0.49–1.85)
Older (75 ≤ years)
Childhood SES High or middle-high ref ref ref ref ref
Middle 1.10 (0.72–1.66) 1.06 (0.69–1.62) 1.05 (0.68–1.61) 1.05 (0.68–1.62) 1.04 (0.67–1.61)
Middle-low or low 1.01 (0.65–1.58) 0.93 (0.58–1.49) 0.92 (0.57–1.48) 0.89 (0.55–1.45) 0.87 (0.54–1.42)

Model 1: adjusted for age; Model 2: Model 1 + adjusted for other childhood environment (height and education); Model 3: Model 2 + adjusted for adult SES (current normalized household income, living situation and longest occupation) and municipalities of residence; Model 4: Model 3 + adjusted for health behaviours: smoking, frequency of meat or fish and vegetable or fruit intakes, walking time, BMI, disease status and depressive symptoms; Model 5: Model 4 + adjusted for social relationship (marital status, number of social interactions with friends/acquaintances, providing and receiving emotional social support, and social participation).

*P < 0.05;

**P < 0.01.

Discussion

In this cohort of older Japanese individuals, deprived childhood socioeconomic circumstances were ‘paradoxically’ associated with reduced mortality risk among the 75-years-old or older males. Among women, low childhood SES was unrelated to later mortality. Neither pattern is consistent with findings previously reported from middle-aged populations in Western countries.5,6 We found that the impact of low childhood SES on mortality was weak among the 65–74-years-old group compared with those among the 75-years-old or older group. This may result from a weaker statistical power due to fewer deaths among the 65–74-years-old group, although the point estimates of HR for childhood SES in the 75-years-old or older group and in the 65–74-years-old group differ (Table 4, with interaction P = 0.15 for childhood SES).

One explanation for our finding is selective survival at older ages, i.e. people who survive adverse circumstances earlier in life are a select group of hardy individuals.30 This type of mortality ‘cross-over’ has been observed for African-Americans who experience higher mortality rates than White Americans at younger ages, but higher survival rates after about 80 years.31,32 In the context of our study, the participants from lower SES backgrounds during childhood were more likely to be exposed to adversities such as wartime food shortages and infectious diseases, and hence more likely to have died during younger ages (i.e. earlier than the baseline of observation). Further, it is possible that low childhood SES during WWII in Japan may be a specific aspect, such as childhood physical training through manual work, which acts as protective effect on mortality in older age. For example, childhood physical training through manual work was more likely to occur among low childhood SES. Post-WWII childhood manual work such as farming may strengthen physical toughness.33 Kimura also showed that about 80% of men and about 60% of women aged 15–19 years had jobs before WWII, and most of men were sent out to an apprenticeship after graduating from higher elementary school at the age of 14 years.34 Further, men aged 10–14 years were mainly involved in agriculture with hard physical tasks, unlike young women involved in housework.35 The additional fact that men aged 17–40 years had to go off to war would further explain the protective effect of low childhood SES on mortality only among men.

In addition, ceiling effects from high background mortality rates could explain the lack of association between childhood SES and survival in women. That is, when background rates of events are very high, the associations with most risk factors become more difficult to detect. For example, the association between cigarette smoking and mortality becomes progressively weaker at older ages.36 Turrell et al. also showed that risk of low childhood SES for mortality tended to be highest in the youngest age groups and the weakest in the oldest group.37 However, a ceiling effect from high background mortality cannot explain the stronger association between low childhood SES and improved survival found in the 75-years-old or older men in our sample.

We hypothesized that the paradoxical protective effect of lower childhood SES on mortality derives from calorie restriction theory. Colman et al. proposed that caloric restriction during adult life, without malnutrition, reduced mortality in rhesus monkeys,38 and caloric restriction during childhood may be associated with less chronic disease.18 Indeed, food shortages were recorded in some parts of the country even before Pearl Harbor. Hunger in the aftermath of WWII was compounded by a disastrous harvest in 1945 (the worst on record since 1910–a shortfall of almost 40% from normal yield) and further exacerbated by inefficiency and corruption in the postwar food distribution system.39 Although we do not collect information about whether people with low childhood SES were more likely to be exposed to calorie restriction, it is possible that nutritional deprivation was more likely occur among those with low childhood SES, especially those growing up in urban areas after WWII.40 Thus, the calorie restriction hypothesis may partially explain the protective effect of low childhood SES on later mortality, so long as the individual survived the adverse childhood experiences (e.g. more infectious disease) associated with low SES. Lower height–used as a proxy for childhood malnutrition22–was also associated with increased mortality; however, height is determined not only by childhood nutrition but also infection in utero or during childhood, as well as other adversity (e.g. psychosocial stress). In other words, in this cohort, height may not be strongly associated with nutritional status.

The difference between the sexes in the association between childhood SES and mortality may be a reflection of the biological fragility of males in utero and during infancy.41 Male fetuses have a higher risk of death,42 and boys are also affected by a higher infant mortality. Therefore, males, especially from deprived socioeconomic backgrounds in childhood, may have been more likely to die earlier in life compared with females.

We observed that education was not associated with mortality in men and women. Again, this is possibly because childhood working, especially among male,35 functioned as educational opportunity.

Several limitations of this study should be mentioned. First, childhood SES was assessed via subjective recall. Moreover, differential recall bias may occur between different age groups (e.g. adults aged 75 years or older may be more prone to recall bias). Previous research, however, supports the reliability of retrospective assessments of childhood SES using siblings’ recall of childhood SES.43 Moreover, we confirmed that childhood subjective SES was correlated with other objective indicators of deprivation such as height and SES achieved in adulthood (Supplementary Table 1). Subjective SES has been found to be a better predictor of health status than objective SES;44–46 however, it is not easy to confirm the possible extent and direction of the resulting bias. Further research is needed to confirm the findings using objective childhood SES, such as parental occupation. Another limitation is that we did not directly enquire about wartime and postwar experiences such as experiences of hunger or the childhood hygiene environment. Specifically, we needed to enquire about occupational status and physical activity when participants were 15 years old or younger. Our analysis was limited to all-cause mortality, not cause-specific mortality. Future studies should examine cause-specific mortality, such as coronary heart disease, stroke or cancer, to clarify the effect of childhood SES. Lastly, the duration of follow-up (3 years) was comparatively short, which may result in underestimation of the effect of childhood SES on mortality among the 65–74-years-old group and among females.

In conclusion, our study provides new and contradictory findings on the association between adverse childhood socioeconomic circumstances and survival in a healthy older population. The evidence on the early life course impacts of socioeconomic deprivation–hitherto dominated by studies from Western contexts–may not be transferable to other contexts. Whether low childhood SES is a risk factor for increased mortality at advanced ages would seem to depend on the specific historical context, of which this cohort of Japanese who grew up during and after WWII provides an illustrative counter-example.

Supplementary Data

Supplementary data are available at IJE online.

Funding

This work was supported by a Strategic Research Foundation Grant-aided Project for Private Universities from the Ministry of Education, Culture, Sport, Science, and Technology, Japan (MEXT), 2009–2013, for the Center for Well-being and Society, Nihon Fukushi University; a Health Labour Sciences Research Grant, Comprehensive Research on Aging and Health (H22-Choju-Shitei-008, H25-Choju-Ippan-003, H25-Kenki-Wakate-015, H26-Choju-Ippan-006) and Comprehensive Research on Lifestyle Disease (H27-Jyunkankito-ippan-002) from the Japanese Ministry of Health, Labour and Welfare; and Grants-in-Aid for Scientific Research (22119506, 22330172, 22390400, 22592327, 23590786, 23700819, 25253052, 23243070, and 26882010) from the Japan Society for the Promotion of Science.

Supplementary Material

Supplementary Data

Acknowledgments

This study used data from the Japan Gerontological Evaluation Study (JAGES), a research project conducted by Nihon Fukushi University Center for Well-being and Society. We are particularly grateful to the staff members from each study area and the central office for conducting the survey. We would like to thank everyone who participated in the surveys.

Author contributions

T.F., Y.T. and K.K. conceived the design; K.K. collected data; Y.T. reviewed literature; Y.T. wrote the first draft of the paper; I.K. and T.F. revised the first draft; N.K., Y.N.T., T.S. and K.K. edited the manuscript and all authors approved the final version of the manuscript.

References

  • 1.Shonkoff JP, Boyce WT, McEwen BS. Neuroscience, molecular biology, and the childhood roots of health disparities: building a new framework for health promotion and disease prevention. JAMA 2009;301:2252–59. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Kuh D, Ben-Shlomo Y, Lynch J, Hallqvist J, Power C. Life course epidemiology. J Epidemiol Community Health 2003;57:778–83. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Lynch J, Davey Smith G. A life course approach to chronic disease epidemiology. Annu Rev Public Health 2005;26:1–35. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Galobardes B, Lynch JW, Davey Smith G. Is the association between childhood socioeconomic circumstances and cause-specific mortality established? Update of a systematic review. J Epidemiol Community Health 2008;62:387–90. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Davey Smith G, Hart C, Blane D, Hole D. Adverse socioeconomic conditions in childhood and cause specific adult mortality: prospective observational study. BMJ 1998;316:1631–35. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Claussen B, Davey Smith G, Thelle D. Impact of childhood and adulthood socioeconomic position on cause specific mortality: the Oslo Mortality Study. J Epidemiol Community Health 2003;57:40–45. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Naess O, Strand BH, Davey Smith G. Childhood and adulthood socioeconomic position across 20 causes of death: a prospective cohort study of 800,000 Norwegian men and women. J Epidemiol Community Health 2007;61:1004–09. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Stringhini S, Dugravot A, Kivimaki M. et al. Do different measures of early life socioeconomic circumstances predict adult mortality? Evidence from the British Whitehall II and French GAZEL studies. J Epidemiol Community Health 2011;65:1097–103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Mishra GD, Chiesa F, Goodman A, De Stavola B, Koupil I. Socio-economic position over the life course and all-cause and circulatory diseases mortality at age 50-87 years: results from a Swedish birth cohort. Eur J Epidemiol 2013;28:139–47. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Ikeda N, Saito E, Kondo N. et al. What has made the population of Japan healthy? Lancet 2011;378:1094–105. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Yoshinaga K, Une H. Contributions of mortality changes by age group and selected causes of death to the increase in Japanese life expectancy at birth from 1950 to 2000. Eur J Epidemiol 2005;20:49–57. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Marmot MG, Davey Smith G. Why are the Japanese living longer? BMJ 1989;299:1547–51. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Tamiya N, Noguchi H, Nishi A. et al. Population ageing and wellbeing: lessons from Japan's long-term care insurance policy. Lancet 2011;378 1183–92. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Date C. Perspectives from public health nutrition. Eiyogaku-zasshi 2005;63:190–94 [in Japanese]. [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Ministry of Health, Labour and Welfare. Dietary Reference Intakes for Japanese-2015. Tokyo: Ministry of Health, Labour and Welfare, 2014. [in Japanese]. [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Foster R, Lunn J. 40th Anniversary Briefing Paper: Food availability and our changing diet. Nutr Bull 2007;32:187–249. [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Colman RJ, Beasley TM, Kemnitz JW, Johnson SC, Weindruch R, Anderson RM. Caloric restriction reduces age-related and all-cause mortality in rhesus monkeys. Nat Commun 2014;1:3557. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Mattison JA, Roth GS, Beasley TM. et al. Impact of caloric restriction on health and survival in rhesus monkeys from the NIA study. Nature 2012;489:318–21. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Yoshimura T, Tohya T, Onoda C, Okamura H. Poor nutrition in prepubertal Japanese children at the end of World War II suppressed bone development. Maturitas 2005;52:32–34. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Katz S, Downs TD, Cash HR, Grotz RC. Progress in development of the index of ADL. Gerontologist 1970. 10 20–30. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Fujiwara T, Kondo K, Shirai K, Suzuki K, Kawachi I. Associations of childhood socioeconomic status and adulthood height with functional limitations among Japanese older people: results from the JAGES 2010 Project. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 2014;69:852–59. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Silventoinen K. Determinants of variation in adult body height. J Biosoc Sci 2003;35:263–85. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Yamamoto T, Kondo K, Aida J, Fuchida S, Hirata Y. Association between the longest job and oral health: Japan Gerontological Evaluation Study project cross-sectional study. BMC Oral Health 2014;14:130. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Wada T, Ishine M, Kita T, Fujisawa M, Matsubayashi K. Depression screening of elderly community-dwelling Japanese. J Am Geriatr Soc 2003;51:1328–29. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Yamamoto T, Kondo K, Aida J. et al. Social determinants of denture/bridge use: Japan gerontological evaluation study project cross-sectional study in older Japanese. BMC Oral Health 2014;14:63. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Aida J, Kondo K, Hirai H. et al. Assessing the association between all-cause mortality and multiple aspects of individual social capital among the older Japanese. BMC Public Health 2011;11:499. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Nishi A, Kawachi I, Shirai K, Hirai H, Jeong S, Kondo K. Sex/gender and socioeconomic differences in the predictive ability of self-rated health for mortality. PLoS One 2012;7:e30179. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Kachi Y, Inoue M, Nishikitani M, Yano E. Differences in self-rated health by employment contract and household structure among Japanese employees: a nationwide cross-sectional study. J Occup Health 2014;56:339–46. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Suzuki E, Fujiwara T, Takao S, Subramanian SV, Yamamoto E, Kawachi I. Multi-level, cross-sectional study of workplace social capital and smoking among Japanese employees. BMC Public Health 2010;10:489. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Nam CB, Weatherby NL, Ockay KA. Causes of death which contribute to the mortality crossover effect. Soc Biol 1978;25:306–14. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Corti MC, Guralnik JM, Ferrucci L. et al. Evidence for a black-white crossover in all-cause and coronary heart disease mortality in an older population: the North Carolina EPESE. Am J Public Health 1999;89:308–14. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Johnson NE. The racial crossover in comorbidity, disability, and mortality. Demography 2000;37:1226–1235.. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Otsuka T. Internal migration in Postwar Japan. Bull Econ Sci Inst 1981;5:69–101 [in Japanese]. [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Kimura H. Historical Educational Demography: Education and Society of the 1930s. Tokyo: Taga-shuppan, 2005. [in Japanese]. [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Utsumi Y. Agricultural child labor during busy farming season (No.1). The Journal of Science of Labour 1942;19:583–600 [in Japanese]. [Google Scholar]
  • 36.LaCroix AZ, Lang J, Scherr P. et al. Smoking and mortality among older men and women in 3 communities. N Engl J Med 1991;324:1619–25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Turrell G, Lynch JW, Leite C, Raghunathan T, Kaplan GA. Socioeconomic disadvantage in childhood and across the life course and all-cause mortality and physical function in adulthood: evidence from the Alameda County Study. J Epidemiol Community Health 2007;61:723–30. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Colman RJ, Anderson RM, Johnson SC. et al. Caloric restriction delays disease onset and mortality in rhesus monkeys. Science 2009;325:201–04. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Hando K. Shouwa-shi, post war 1945-1989. Tokyo: HEIBONSHA 2009. (in Japanese). [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Kondo S, Takahashi E, Kato K, Takahashi S, Ikeda M. Secular trends in height and weight of Japanese pupils. Tohoku J Exp Med 1978;126:203–13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Kraemer S. The fragile male. BMJ 2000;321:1609–12. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Mizuno R. The male/female ratio of fetal deaths and births in Japan. Lancet 2000;356:738–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Ward MM. Concordance of sibling's recall of measures of childhood socioeconomic position. BMC Med Res Methodol 2011;11:147. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Kan C, Kawakami N, Karasawa M. et al. Psychological resources as mediators of the association between social class and health: comparative findings from Japan and the USA. Int J Behav Med 2014;21:53–65. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Singh-Manoux A, Marmot MG, Adler NE. Does subjective social status predict health and change in health status better than objective status? Psychosom Med 2005;67:855–61. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Sakurai K, Kawakami N, Yamaoka K, Ishikawa H, Hashimoto H. The impact of subjective and objective social status on psychological distress among men and women in Japan. Soc Sci Med 2010;70:1832–39. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplementary Data

Articles from International Journal of Epidemiology are provided here courtesy of Oxford University Press

RESOURCES