Skip to main content
Scientific Reports logoLink to Scientific Reports
. 2018 May 23;8:8062. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-26382-9

Neuromolecular basis of faded perception associated with unreality experience

Keita Yokokawa 1,2, Takehito Ito 1, Keisuke Takahata 1, Harumasa Takano 1,3, Yasuyuki Kimura 1, Masanori Ichise 1, Yoko Ikoma 4, Ayako Isato 1, Ming-Rong Zhang 5, Kazunori Kawamura 5, Hiroshi Ito 6, Hidehiko Takahashi 7, Tetsuya Suhara 1, Makiko Yamada 1,8,
PMCID: PMC5966381  PMID: 29795167

Abstract

Perceptual changes in shape, size, or color are observed in patients with derealization symptoms; however, the underlying neural and molecular mechanisms are not well understood. The current study explored the relationship between neural activity associated with altered colorfulness perception assessed by fMRI and striatal dopamine D2 receptor availability measured by [11C]raclopride PET in healthy participants. Inside an fMRI scanner, participants performed the saturation adaptation task, where they rated how much vivid/faded visual objects looked like real/unreal ones using a visual analog scale. We found that participants experienced greater unreality when they perceived fadedness than vividness despite physically identical saturation. The combined fMRI and PET analyses revealed that the faded perception-related activities of the dorsolateral prefrontal and parietal cortex were positively correlated with striatal D2 receptor availability. This finding may help to understand the neuromolecular mechanisms of faded perception associated with feeling unreal in derealization symptoms.

Introduction

Derealization is an abnormal subjective experience in which the external world appears strange or unreal. DSM-V defines the perceptual alteration in derealization as “experiences of unreality or detachment with respect to surroundings (e.g., individuals or objects are experienced as unreal, dreamlike, foggy, lifeless, or visually distorted)1”. Derealization is observed not only in a wide range of psychiatric disorders, such as depersonalization disorder, schizophrenia, depression and anxiety disorder24, but also in healthy people. Population surveys revealed that derealization as well as depersonalization that is the experience of unreality in sense of the self5 were experienced in healthy subjects with the prevalence of about half in a sample population of college students6,7.

Perceptual alterations of shape, size, or color in derealization have been less studied, and thus little is known about the neural mechanisms of altered visual perception associated with feeling unreal. In the field of vision studies, however, there is ample experimental evidence of perceptual changes of visual objects induced by adaptation techniques in healthy individuals8. In particular, the perceptual alteration of colorfulness is produced by saturation adaptation, in which, after being exposed with highly saturated stimuli, the following object looks faded, while the same object looks vivid after perceiving low saturation stimuli9. Thus, application of saturation adaptation in fMRI may reveal the neural activities associated with subjective faded/vivid perception toward physically identical saturation objects.

Dopamine is a key modulator for learning and cognitive control, but also for visual experience. It has been shown that dopaminergic activation facilitates visual perceptual performance1013, improves the cortical neuronal signal-to-noise ratio (SNR)1416, and enhances the aftereffects of visual adaptation17. A recent study using positron emission tomography (PET) further revealed that the dopamine D2 receptor availability in striatum was positively correlated with subjective visual experience18. Thus, it is speculated that striatal dopaminergic transmission may affect how visual information, including the aftereffects of saturation adaptation, is processed in cerebral cortex.

Here, we set out to assess the subjective colorfulness and unreality experience of visual objects, and to examine the relationships between the neural representation of subjective colorfulness and unreality, measured by fMRI, and striatal D2 receptor availability, measured by [11C]raclopride PET, in healthy subjects.

Results

Behavioral results

The reality ratings of low saturation stimuli (LS, objective faded condition) (35.4 ± 15.9) were significantly lower than those of high saturation stimuli (HS, objective vivid condition) (70.6 ± 15.9) (n = 14, paired t-test, t(13) = −9.1, p < 0.001, Fig. 1a), showing that participants perceived stronger unreality for the physically low saturation stimuli. In case of physically identical middle saturation stimuli (MS), the reality ratings of middle saturation stimuli preceded by high saturation stimuli (HSMS, subjective faded condition) and middle saturation stimuli preceded by low saturation stimuli (LSMS, subjective vivid condition) were 55.0 ± 11.0 and 65.2 ± 8.7, respectively. The ratings of the subjective faded condition (HSMS) were significantly lower than those of the subjective vivid condition (LSMS) (n = 14, paired t-test, t(13) = −4.8, p < 0.001, Fig. 1b), indicating that participants perceived stronger unreality for the subjective faded condition (HSMS) than the subjective vivid condition (LSMS) despite their saturation levels being physically identical.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Behavioral data. (a) There were statistical differences in reality ratings between objective faded condition (LS) and objective vivid condition (HS), and (b) between subjective faded condition (HSMS) and subjective vivid condition (LSMS). **p < 0.001. Error bars indicate s.d.

fMRI results

The contrast of the objective faded condition (LS) versus the objective vivid condition (HS) recruited the activity of parietal cortex (voxel level of p < 0.001; cluster level of p < 0.05, FDR corrected, Fig. 2a, Table 1), while the contrast of the objective vivid condition (HS) versus the objective faded condition (LS) revealed significant activations in visual areas including bilateral V4, known as the color processing regions (voxel level of p < 0.001; cluster level of p < 0.05, FDR corrected, Fig. 2b, Table 1). We further compared the ß-values extracted from V4 (MNI = −30/−84/−8 and 34/−80/−8 at 4-mm sphere, based on Rottschy et al.19) in the contrast of color pictures (HS, LS, HSMS, LSMS) versus scramble images. This revealed that the average activity of right and left V4 was significantly higher for the objective vivid condition (HS) than any other conditions (HS versus LS: p = 0.018; HS versus HSMS: p = 0.028; HS versus LSMS: p = 0.048, all p-values after Bonferroni corrected for multiple comparison, Fig. 2c). Thus, the effect of physically different saturation was observed in parietal cortex for the objective faded condition, whereas the objective vivid condition recruited more activation in visual processing areas. The comparison of physically identical MS did not yield any significant activations, but only at the liberal threshold (voxel level of p < 0.001; k > 20)20, the contrast of the subjective faded condition (HSMS) versus the subjective vivid condition (LSMS) showed increased activations in the right thalamus, left amygdala and left inferior frontal cortex (Supplementary Fig. S1, Supplementary Table S1). The contrast of the subjective vivid condition (LSMS) versus the subjective faded condition (HSMS) did not show any activated regions at this liberal threshold.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Regions associated with objective faded and vivid conditions. (a) The contrast of objective faded condition (LS) versus objective vivid condition (HS) revealed increased activities in the left parietal cortex (voxel level of p < 0.001; cluster level of p < 0.05, FDR corrected). (b) The contrast of objective vivid condition (HS) versus objective faded condition (LS) yielded activities in the bilateral occipital cortex (voxel level of p < 0.001; cluster level of p < 0.05, FDR corrected). (c) The bilateral V4 activity of objective vivid condition (HS) was higher than any other conditions. *p < 0.05, Bonferroni corrected. Error bars indicate s.d.

Table 1.

Brain regions associated with the contrast of objective conditions.

Brain region L/R BA MNI coordinate T values (peak level) Cluster size p-values (voxel level) p-values (cluster level)
x y z
LS versus HS
Supramarginal gyrus L 40 −50 −48 28 7.83 620 <0.001 <0.001
Middle temporal gyrus L 39 −52 −56 8 5.79 <0.001
HS versus LS
Fusiform gyrus R 28 −66 −14 6.28 341 <0.001 =0.001
Lingual gyrus (V3) R 18 22 −88 −12 6.12 <0.001
Fusiform gyrus (V4) R 18 30 −74 −14 5.51 <0.001
Calcarine gyrus (V1) R 17 20 −96 −2 5.06 <0.001
Calcarine gyrus (V2) L 17 −8 −96 −8 6.83 214 <0.001 =0.004
Superior occipital gyrus (V1) L 17 −12 −96 4 6.08 <0.001
Calcarine gyrus (V1) L 18 −6 −96 4 5.98 <0.001
Superior occipital gyrus L 18 −10 −96 10 5.81 <0.001
Calcarine gyrus (V1) L 17 −14 −96 −4 5.78 <0.001
Lingual gyrus (V4) L 18 −22 −86 −14 5.06 <0.001
Lingual gyrus (V3) L 18 −16 −88 −12 4.69 <0.001

R: right, L: left, BA: Brodmann’s area. Significant clusters obtained from the contrast of “LS vs. HS” and “HS vs. LS” (voxel level of p < 0.001; cluster level of p < 0.05, FDR corrected).

PET and fMRI results

The mean D2 BPND values of three subdivisions of left and right striatum are shown in Supplementary Table S2. Multiple regression analyses were conducted to examine the association between the neural activities of the subjective faded condition in the contrast of HSMS versus LSMS and D2 BPND in three sub-regions of left and right striatum. The D2 BPND value of left executive striatum was positively correlated with the activity of the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (middle frontal gyrus, BA 46, voxel level of p < 0.001; cluster level of p < 0.05 (FDR corrected)) and the left parietal cortex (including left angular gyrus (BA 39), left inferior parietal lobe (BA 40), and left superior parietal lobule (BA 7); voxel level of p < 0.001; cluster level of p < 0.05 (FDR corrected)) (Fig. 3, Table 2), but there were no regions associated with D2 BPND values of other subdivisions of striatum or in the contrast of the subjective vivid condition (LSMS) versus the subjective faded condition (HSMS). Analyses of the contrasts of physically different saturation levels (LS versus HS) did not yield any significant regions associated with D2 BPND values of any subdivisions of striatum, but the D2 BPND value of left executive striatum in the objective faded condition (LS) versus the objective vivid condition (HS) contrast correlated with the left parietal cortex (left inferior parietal lobule (BA 40)) at a marginally significant level (cluster level of p = 0.08, FDR corrected), which overlapped with the left parietal cortex found in subjective faded condition (overlapped region was presented in Supplementary Fig. S2).

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Subjective fadedness regions associated with dopamine D2 receptor availability. The D2 BPND values of executive striatum were positively correlated with (a) activity of middle frontal gyrus (voxel level of p < 0.05, FWE corrected; cluster level of p < 0.05, FDR corrected), and (b) activity of inferior parietal lobule (voxel level of p < 0.001; cluster level of p < 0.05, FDR corrected) for the contrast of subjective faded condition (HSMS) versus subjective vivid condition (LSMS).

Table 2.

Brain regions of unreal perception correlated with striatal D2 BPND of left executive striatum.

Brain region L/R BA MNI coordinate T values (peak level) Cluster size p-values (voxel level) p-values (cluster level)
x y z
Frontal lobe
 Middle frontal gyrus R 46 46 40 28 10.44 265 <0.001 =0.033
 Middle frontal gyrus R 9 32 36 38 4.49 <0.001
 Middle frontal gyrus R 9 44 30 38 4.44 <0.001
Parietal lobe
 Angular gyrus L 32 −30 −56 32 9.05 689 <0.001 <0.001
 Angular gyrus L 39 −34 −62 36 7.40 <0.001
 Inferior parietal lobule L 40 −42 −44 36 5.78 <0.001
 Supramarginal gyrus L 40 −50 −40 34 5.78 <0.001
 Inferior parietal lobule L 40 −42 −58 54 5.76 <0.001
 Inferior parietal lobule L 40 −50 −54 50 5.14 <0.001
 Angular gyrus L 40 −42 −56 44 4.57 <0.001
 Inferior parietal lobule L 40 −56 −46 44 4.55 <0.001
 Inferior parietal lobule L 40 −54 −48 48 4.54 <0.001

R: right, L: left, BA: Brodmann’s area. Significant clusters obtained from the contrast of HSMS vs. LSMS with left striatal D2BPND as a covariate (voxel level of p < 0.001; cluster level of p < 0.05, FDR corrected).

Discussion

The current study examined the dopaminergic neural mechanisms of perceptual changes in colorfulness associated with the unreality experience in healthy subjects. We revealed that subjective fadedness was accompanied with unreality experience, and that the individual differences of dopamine D2 receptor availability in executive striatum, as measured by PET, was positively associated with dorsolateral prefrontal and parietal activities related to subjective faded perception, as measured by fMRI.

Previous vision study observed that the subjective experience of colorfulness changed following the adaptation to the different levels of saturation9. By applying this adaptation methodology in the current study, we further revealed that the unreality experience was increased toward the visual object adapted to a high level of saturation (subjective faded condition), in contrast to the identical object adapted to a low level of saturation (subjective vivid condition). This behavioral result suggests that subjective fadedness accompanies the subjective experience of unreality.

We discovered that the individual differences of dopamine D2 receptor availability in executive striatum were positively associated with activities of the right middle frontal gyrus (BA46), a part of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), and the left parietal cortex for the subjective faded condition. Dopamine has been proposed as playing a key role in prediction and the specification of own expectation21,22, which helps to build adaptive internal models of the body and the world. This scheme is proposed particularly in motor control23 but may also be employed for the formation of visual world, based on the current findings as well as several previous studies that demonstrated the influences of dopamine on the enhancement of visual perceptual performance and acuity11,13. In addition, impaired color perception has frequently been reported in patients with Parkinson’s disease that is the case of dopaminergic dysfunction24, and their abnormal color vision can be reversed by treatment with levodopa that is an amino acid precursor of dopamine25. Our previous study using [11C]raclopride and L-[β-11C]DOPA revealed that postsynaptic D2 receptor binding potentials were inversely related with presynaptic dopamine synthesis26. Therefore, the current finding indicates that individuals with lower endogeneous dopamine synthesis show higher activity in the frontoparietal network for subjective faded perception, and the lower dopamine may invade to build internal models of the predicted world, observed as the unreality feeling in the current study. Enhancement of dopamine is supposed to treat depersonalization theoretically27, and the current study may provide the possible role of dopamine on unreality experience associated with faded perception.

Frontal and parietal cortex, where the former is anatomically connected with executive striatum28 and both are functionally connected with striatum29, compose the top-down attentional control system30,31. Previous neuroimaging studies of depersonalization disorder patients observed increased activities in the frontal and parietal cortex3235, and proposed that the emotional numbing or detachment in these patients was associated with the hyperactive top-down control system that inhibits emotional responses. DLPFC is also associated with the experience of presence in a virtual reality environment36, where the higher activity in DLPFC was associated with the lower experience of presence and reality37. Moreover, derealazation is observed as ictal phenomena in patients with temporal and parietal lobe epilepsy38. Taken together with our findings, these results indicate that the increased neural activity in frontal and parietal cortex plays a role for unreality feeling associated with subjective fadedness.

Furthermore, over the last two decades, a strong association of frontal and parietal activity and visual awareness has been established39. A number of studies have shown that frontoparietal activity is associated with changes in the contents of visual consciousness40. Taking together both the observations of the striatal dopaminergic transmission and the proposed frontoparietal system, it could be speculated that the unreality experience based on subjective faded perception is influenced by the interaction between striatal dopaminergic transmission that plays a role in broadcasting the expectation-based sensory information to the cortex and the frontoparietal network, which in turn are necessary for conscious perception of fadedness and for the top-down allocations based on the subjective faded perception induced by the experimental settings of the current study.

Lastly, it should also be noted that while the objective vivid condition recruited more activation in visual areas including the color processing region, these regions were not modified by subjective faded and vivid conditions or by individual differences of dopamine D2 receptor availability in striatum.

One of the major limitations of the current study is that the findings of aftereffects may be explained not at neural levels but rather at retinal levels. Although studies indicate that saturation adaptation is a product of the brain41, we still cannot deny the possibility of retinal adaptation. In particular, it is known that dopamine receptors in retina have impacts on visual adaptation42 and that Parkinson’s disease has also shown some evidence of retinal dysfunction43. Thus, future study will hopefully clarify the association and dissociation between retinal and neural visual information processing, which are relevant to the emergence of unreality experience, together with the role of dopaminergic transmission. Second, the current data strongly suggest the involvement of striatal dopamine transmission in the neural activity of subjective faded and unreality experience, but do not determine the precise functional role of dopamine nor clarify the nature of the underlying neural processes. One line of future experimentation will be to apply the pharmacological approaches that have been used to study the impact of dopamine transmission on visual perceptual performance. At last, [11C]raclopride has equal affinity for D2 and D3 receptors. However, the previous study showed putamen and caudate are almost filled with all D2 receptors44. Executive striatum is composed of mostly putamen and caudate; thus, the current finding is regarded to reflect D2 receptor availability rather than D3 receptors.

In conclusion, the current findings highlight the neural underpinnings of the unreality experience induced by subjective faded perception, and the individual variabilities of striatal dopamine D2 receptor availability were associated with the function of the frontoparietal network in a healthy population. Although these results are only correlational and a causal relation will still need to be established, we demonstrated that striatal D2 receptor availability is positively related to the neural networks of faded perception and unreality experience. The findings may suggest the underlying mechanisms of the relatively neglected phenomena of derealization, and help in the consideration of possible treatment targets for this population.

Methods

Participants

Seventeen right-handed healthy male subjects (mean age = 23.9 ± 5.4 (mean and standard deviation) years) participated in the study. All participants had no history of neurologic or psychiatric disorder, and they were not taking any medications that could interfere with task performance or PET/fMRI data. Three participants were excluded from the analysis owing to insufficient quality of data recording: two were excluded due to excessive head movements in fMRI scan (more than 1 voxel), and one was excluded due to inadequate understanding of the task procedure. Therefore, the final sample comprised fourteen participants (mean age = 23.4 ± 4.2). Each subject underwent fMRI and PET scans. All participants provided written informed consent before participating in the study, which was approved by the Ethics and Radiation Safety Committee of the National Institute of Radiological Sciences in accordance with the ethical standards laid down in the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki and its later amendments.

PET Procedures

Data acquisition

After intravenous injection of [11C]raclopride (228.4 ± 8.1 MBq with specific activity of 158.4 ± 53.5 GBq/µmol), three-dimensional dynamic images were acquired on a PET camera (Eminence SET-3000GCT/X, Shimadzu, Kyoto, Japan) for 60 min. All PET images were reconstructed by filtered back-projection method (Gaussian filter, kernel 5 mm; the reconstructed in-plane resolution was 7.5 mm in full-width at half-maximum; voxel size: 2 × 2 × 2.6 mm) corrected for attenuation, randoms, scatter and head motion.

Data analysis

The PET image analysis was performed using PMOD software 3.5 (PMOD Technologies Ltd. Zurich, Switzerland). Quantitative analysis was performed using the multilinear reference tissue model (MRTM2)45, which provides parametric images of the binding potentials (BPND). All parametric images were spatially normalized to the MNI152 standard space based on the transformation parameters from the MR images estimated by DARTEL toolkit of SPM8. A connectivity-based probabilistic atlas of the striatum28 was applied to the spatially normalized parametric images to extract weighted-means of BPND values among corresponding voxels for the three sub-regions of left and right striatum (limbic, executive, and sensorimotor). Contribution of the bilateral sub-regions to the total striatal volume was 20, 49, and 24%, respectively28.

fMRI Procedures

Stimuli

We used 20 pictures of flowers from the image library (Datacraft Inc. Hokkaido, Japan). The level of saturation was adjusted to yield high saturation, middle saturation and low saturation levels, using Adobe Photoshop software (Adobe Systems Inc. California, USA). The level of saturation for high saturation stimuli (HS) was increased by 79.0% (±7.7%) from middle saturation stimuli (MS), and that for low saturation stimuli (LS) was decreased by 73.3% (±11.5%) from MS based on our pilot experiment.

Task

The target MS images were presented after both HS and LS to induce different saturation perception and a sense of reality for the targets. “MS presented after HS” (HSMS, subjective faded condition) was supposed to induce low saturation perception, and “MS presented after LS” (LSMS, subjective vivid condition) was supposed to induce high saturation perception. Our pilot examination showed that saturation perception was significantly lower for the subjective faded condition (HSMS) than the subjective vivid condition (LSMS) (n = 8, paired t-test, t(7) = −3.40, p = 0.012, Supplementary Fig. S3a), and saturation perception was positively correlated with reality perception (all p-values ≤ 0.001, Supplementary Fig. S3b). Thus, perception of higher (or lower) saturation in the subjective faded condition (HSMS) was associated with higher (or lower) reality in the subjective vivid condition (LSMS) for the current fMRI experiment.

The task instruction was given as follows: “The purpose of the present study is to evaluate the quality of 4 cameras. A series of flowers taken by 4 different cameras will be presented on the screen, and your task is to evaluate how real each picture looks.” In each trial, after a fixation (mean = 3.5 seconds, jittered between 2 and 6 seconds), a picture was presented for 8 seconds, and participants rated how much they felt reality using a visual analog scale (VAS), ranging from 0 (feeling no reality) to 100 (feeling strong reality).

The order of the trials was as follows: objective vivid condition (HS), subjective faded condition (HSMS), objective faded condition (LS), and subjective vivid condition (LSMS) (Fig. 4). The objective vivid condition (HS) and the objective faded condition (LS) were counterbalanced across trials. A trial with the gray-scaled scrambled image was always presented before the 4 saturation trials, and the participants were asked to move a curser to any place using VAS. Thus, five trials (one scrambled and four pictures) were grouped as a set, and one run consisted of 5 sets (25 trials). Participants performed 4 runs (100 trials) in total.

Figure 4.

Figure 4

Task design. Each block contains five trials. At the beginning of a block, the scramble image was presented, and then a series of flower pictures was presented in the following order: high saturation (HS), middle saturation (HSMS), low saturation (LS) and middle saturation (LSMS). MS trials were always preceded by HS and LS trials. The orders of HS and LS were counterbalanced. These images were created only for the purpose of presentation.

Behavioral data analysis

We examined the differences of reality ratings between the objective vivid condition (HS) and the objective faded condition (LS) and between the subjective faded condition (HSMS) and the subjective vivid condition (LSMS) to test the effects of physically different saturation and of saturation adaptation on reality experience, respectively. Two-tailed paired t-tests were performed using SPSS (IBM Corp. Released 2011. IBM SPSS Statistics, Version 20.0. Armonk, NY: IBM Corp.). p < 0.05 was considered statically significant.

fMRI data acquisition

Functional imaging was performed with a GE 3.0-T Excite system to acquire gradient echo T2*-weighted echo planar images with blood oxygenation level-dependent contrast. Each volume comprised 35 transaxial contiguous slices with a slice thickness of 3.8 mm to cover almost the whole brain (flip angle, 90°; echo time, 25 ms; repetition time, 2,000 ms; matrix, 64 × 64; interleaved acquisition). A high-resolution T1-weighted magnetization-prepared gradient echo sequence (124 contiguous axial slices; 3D spoiled-GRASS sequence; slice thickness, 1.5 mm; flip angle, 30°; echo time, 9 ms; repetition time, 22 ms; matrix, 256 × 192) was also collected for spatial normalization and localization.

fMRI preprocessing

Functional image analysis was performed using Statistical Parametric Mapping (SPM8; www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm). EPI images were corrected for slice timing and rigid head motion. Head motion parameters were examined, and we confirmed that all trials had less than one voxel of translation and 2.0° of rotation in each participant. T1 images were segmented and spatially normalized to the same stereotaxic space by using the diffeomorphic anatomical registration through exponentiated Lie algebra (DARTEL) algorithm46. The voxel values of segmented and normalized gray matter images were modulated by the Jacobian determinants obtained from nonlinear normalization steps. After normalization, images were spatially smoothed using a Gaussian kernel with a full-width at half-maximum of 6 mm. High-pass temporal filtering (filter width of 128 seconds) was also applied to the data.

fMRI data analysis

The first-level analysis contained five regressors of conditions (HS, LS, HSMS, LSMS, and a scrambled image) using a general linear model (GLM). For each condition, the whole period from the onset of the stimuli (8 seconds) was modeled and was convolved with SPM8’s standard canonical hemodynamic response function. Six realignment parameters and two derivatives were used as covariates. All artifacts in fMRI time-series data were detected and corrected with Robust WLS Toolbox in SPM847.

For the group analyses, the individual contrast images of LS versus HS and of HSMS versus LSMS were analyzed in a random-effects model. Each model included the mean-centered D2 BPND values of each striatal sub-region as a covariate of interest to examine how the individual differences of D2 BPND were associated with the neural responses of unreality experience. Resulting p values were corrected for multiple comparisons. We report here a voxel level of p < 0.001 (uncorrected), a cluster level of p < 0.05 (corrected by false discovery rate (FDR)), as significant results48. Brain figures were created using MRIcron (http://people.cas.sc.edu/rorden/mricron/index.html)49.

ROI analysis

To compare the activity of bilateral V4, we constructed the bilateral V4 mask image (MNI = −30/−84/−8 and 34/−80/−8 at 4-mm sphere, based on Rottschy et al.19, and extracted the contrast coefficient (ß-value) of each condition. These regions corresponded to V4 areas defined by the SPM Anatomy Toolbox (v2.2). One-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) with Bonferroni’s multiple comparison post-test was used to determine statistical significance (p < 0.05) using SPSS.

Electronic supplementary material

Supplementary Information (397.8KB, docx)

Acknowledgements

We thank K. Suzuki, S. Kawakami, and I. Izumida for assistance as clinical research coordinators, H. Sano for assistance as an MRI technician, Y. Eguchi for assistance as a neuropsychologist, and members of the Clinical Imaging Team for support with PET scans. This study was supported in part by PRESTO (Precursory Research for Embryonic Science and Technology), one of the strategic basic research programs under Japan Science and Technology Agency; the Naito Foundation; by AMED under Grant Number JP17dm0107094; by AMED under Grant Number JP17dm0207007; and by JSPS KAKENHI Grant Number 17H02173.

Author Contributions

M.Y. designed the study; M.Y., K.T., Ha.T., M.Z., K.K. and H.I. conducted the experiment; K.Y., T.I., Y.K., M.I., Y.I., A.I. and M.Y. analyzed data; All authors wrote the paper.

Competing Interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Footnotes

Electronic supplementary material

Supplementary information accompanies this paper at 10.1038/s41598-018-26382-9.

Publisher's note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

References

  • 1.American Psychological Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5®). (American Psychiatric Pub, 2013).
  • 2.Trueman D. Anxiety and depersonalization and derealization experiences. Psychol. Rep. 1984;54:91–96. doi: 10.2466/pr0.1984.54.1.91. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Cassano GB, et al. Derealization and panic attacks: a clinical evaluation on 150 patients with panic disorder/agoraphobia. Compr. Psychiatry. 1989;30:5–12. doi: 10.1016/0010-440X(89)90112-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Sass LA, Parnas J. Schizophrenia, consciousness, and the self. Schizophr. Bull. 2003;29:427–444. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a007017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Radovic F, Radovic S. Feelings of Unreality: A Conceptual and Phenomenological Analysis of the Language of Depersonalization. Philos. Psychiatr. Psychol. 2002;9:271–279. doi: 10.1353/ppp.2003.0048. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Roberts WW. Normal and abnormal depersonalization. J. Ment. Sci. 1960;106:478–493. doi: 10.1192/bjp.106.443.478. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Dixon JC. Depersonalization phenomena in a sample population of college students. Br. J. Psychiatry. 1963;109:371–375. doi: 10.1192/bjp.109.460.371. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Kohn A. Visual adaptation: physiology, mechanisms, and functional benefits. J. Neurophysiol. 2007;97:3155–3164. doi: 10.1152/jn.00086.2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Mizokami Y, Kamesaki C, Ito N, Sakaibara S, Yaguchi H. Effect of spatial structure on colorfulness adaptation for natural images. J. Opt. Soc. Am. A Opt. Image. Sci. Vis. 2012;29:A118–27. doi: 10.1364/JOSAA.29.00A118. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Müller U, Cramon Von DY, Pollmann S. D1- Versus D2-Receptor Modulation of Visuospatial Working Memory in Humans. J. Neurosci. 1998;18:2720–2728. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.18-07-02720.1998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Lou HC, et al. Dopaminergic stimulation enhances confidence and accuracy in seeing rapidly presented words. J. of Vis. 2011;11:1–6. doi: 10.1167/11.2.15. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Noudoost B, Moore T. Control of visual cortical signals by prefrontal dopamine. Nature. 2011;474:372–375. doi: 10.1038/nature09995. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Fillmore MT, Rush CR, Abroms BD. d-amphetamine-induced enhancement of inhibitory mechanisms involved in visual search. Exp. Clin. Psychopharmacol. 2005;13:200–208. doi: 10.1037/1064-1297.13.3.200. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Winterer G, Weinberger DR. Genes, dopamine and cortical signal-to-noise ratio in schizophrenia. Trends Neurosci. 2004;27:683–690. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2004.08.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Kroener S, Chandler LJ, Phillips PEM, Seamans JK. Dopamine modulates persistent synaptic activity and enhances the signal-to-noise ratio in the prefrontal cortex. PLoS ONE. 2009;4:e6507. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0006507. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Yousif N, et al. Dopamine Activation Preserves Visual Motion Perception Despite Noise Interference of Human V5/MT. J. Neurosci. 2016;36:9303–9312. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4452-15.2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Calvert JE, Harris JP, Phillipson OT. Probing the visual system of Parkinson’s disease and chronic schizophrenic patients on depot neuroleptic using the tilt after effect. Clin. Vis. Sci. 1992;7:119–127. [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Van Opstal F, et al. Correlation between individual differences in striatal dopamine and in visual consciousness. Curr. Biol. 2014;24:R265–R266. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2014.02.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Rottschy C, et al. Ventral visual cortex in humans: Cytoarchitectonic mapping of two extrastriate areas. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2007;28:1045–1059. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20348. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Woo C-W, Krishnan A, Wager TD. Cluster-extent based thresholding in fMRI analyses: Pitfalls and recommendations. NeuroImage. 2014;91:412–419. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.12.058. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Scott DJ, et al. Individual Differences in Reward Responding Explain Placebo-Induced Expectations and Effects. Neuron. 2007;55:325–336. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.06.028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Corlett PR, Frith CD, Fletcher PC. From drugs to deprivation: a Bayesian framework for understanding models of psychosis. Psychopharmacology. 2009;206:515–530. doi: 10.1007/s00213-009-1561-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Shadmehr R, Smith MA, Krakauer JW. Error Correction, Sensory Prediction, and Adaptation in Motor Control. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2010;33:89–108. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-060909-153135. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Price MJ, Feldman RG, Adelberg D, Kayne H. Abnormalities in color vision and contrast sensitivity in Parkinson’s disease. Neurology. 1992;42:887–890. doi: 10.1212/WNL.42.4.887. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Büttner T, Kuhn W, Patzold T, Przuntek H. L-Dopa improves colour vision in Parkinson’s disease. J. Neural Transm. Park. Dis. Dement. Sect. 1994;7:13–19. doi: 10.1007/BF02252659. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Ito H, et al. Relation between presynaptic and postsynaptic dopaminergic functions measured by positron emission tomography: implication of dopaminergic tone. J. Neurosci. 2011;31:7886–7890. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6024-10.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Simeon, D. & Abugel, J. Feeling Unreal: Depersonalization Disorder and the Loss of the Self. Oxford: Oxford University Press (2006).
  • 28.Tziortzi AC, et al. Connectivity-Based Functional Analysis of Dopamine Release in the Striatum Using Diffusion-Weighted MRI and Positron Emission Tomography. Cereb. Cortex. 2013;24:1165–1177. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs397. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Di Martino A, et al. Functional Connectivity of Human Striatum: A Resting State fMRI Study. Cereb. Cortex. 2008;18:2735–2747. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Corbetta M, Shulman GL. Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2002;3:201–215. doi: 10.1038/nrn755. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Vossel S, Geng JJ, Fink GR. Dorsal and ventral attention systems: distinct neural circuits but collaborative roles. Neuroscientist. 2014;20:150–159. doi: 10.1177/1073858413494269. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Lemche E, et al. Limbic and prefrontal responses to facial emotion expressions in depersonalization. Neuroreport. 2007;18:473–477. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0b013e328057deb3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Lemche E, et al. Cerebral and autonomic responses to emotional facial expressions in depersonalisation disorder. Br. J. Psychiatry. 2008;193:222–228. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.107.044263. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Medford N, et al. Emotional Experience and Awareness of Self: Functional MRI Studies of Depersonalization Disorder. Front. Psychol. 2016;7:432. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00432. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Simeon D, et al. Feeling Unreal: A PET Study of Depersonalization Disorder. Am. J. Psychiatry. 2000;157:1782–1788. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.157.11.1782. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Jäncke L, Cheetham M. Virtual reality and the role of the prefrontal cortex in adults and children. Front. Neurosci. 2009;3:52–59. doi: 10.3389/neuro.01.006.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Baumgartner T, et al. Feeling present in arousing virtual reality worlds: prefrontal brain regions differentially orchestrate presence experience in adults and children. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2008;2:8. doi: 10.3389/neuro.09.008.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Lambert MV, Sierra M, Phillips ML, David AS. The spectrum of organic depersonalization: a review plus four new cases. J. Neuropsychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 2002;14:141–154. doi: 10.1176/jnp.14.2.141. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Rees G. Neural correlates of consciousness. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 2013;1296:4–10. doi: 10.1111/nyas.12257. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Rees G. Neural correlates of the contents of visual awareness in humans. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond., B, Biol. Sci. 2007;362:877–886. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2007.2094. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Webster MA. Visual Adaptation. Annu. Rev. Vis. Sci. 2015;1:547–567. doi: 10.1146/annurev-vision-082114-035509. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Jackson CR, et al. Retinal dopamine mediates multiple dimensions of light-adapted vision. J. Neurosci. 2012;32:9359–9368. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0711-12.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Archibald NK, Clarke MP, Mosimann UP, Burn DJ. The retina in Parkinson’s disease. Brain. 2009;132:1128–1145. doi: 10.1093/brain/awp068. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Tziortzi AC, et al. Imaging dopamine receptors in humans with [11C]-(+)-PHNO: Dissection of D3 signal and anatomy. Neuroimage. 2011;54:264–277. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.06.044. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Ichise M, et al. Linearized Reference Tissue Parametric Imaging Methods: Application to [11C]DASB Positron Emission Tomography Studies of the Serotonin Transporter in Human Brain. J. Cereb. Blood Flow Metab. 2003;486:1096–1112. doi: 10.1097/01.WCB.0000085441.37552.CA. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Ashburner J. A fast diffeomorphic image registration algorithm. NeuroImage. 2007;38:95–113. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.07.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Diedrichsen J, Shadmehr R. Detecting and adjusting for artifacts in fMRI time series data. NeuroImage. 2005;27:624–634. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.04.039. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Eklund A, Nichols TE, Knutsson H. Cluster failure: Why fMRI inferences for spatial extent have inflated false-positive rates. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2016;113:7900–7905. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1602413113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Rorden C, Brett M. Stereotaxic display of brain lesions. Behav. Neurol. 2000;12:191–200. doi: 10.1155/2000/421719. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplementary Information (397.8KB, docx)

Articles from Scientific Reports are provided here courtesy of Nature Publishing Group

RESOURCES