Skip to main content
Heliyon logoLink to Heliyon
. 2018 Mar 15;4(3):e00569. doi: 10.1016/j.heliyon.2018.e00569

IRT analyses of the Swedish Dark Triad Dirty Dozen

Danilo Garcia a,b,c,, Björn N Persson c,d,e, Ali Al Nima a,c, Joel Gruneau Brulin f, Max Rapp-Ricciardi a,b,c, Petri J Kajonius b,c,e,g
PMCID: PMC5968130  PMID: 29862337

Abstract

Background

The Dark Triad (i.e., Machiavellianism, narcissism, and psychopathy) can be captured quickly with 12 items using the Dark Triad Dirty Dozen (Jonason and Webster, 2010). Previous Item Response Theory (IRT) analyses of the original English Dark Triad Dirty Dozen have shown that all three subscales adequately tap into the dark domains of personality. The aim of the present study was to analyze the Swedish version of the Dark Triad Dirty Dozen using IRT.

Method

570 individuals (nmales = 326, nfemales = 242, and 2 unreported), including university students and white-collar workers with an age range between 19 and 65 years, responded to the Swedish version of the Dark Triad Dirty Dozen (Garcia et al., 2017a,b).

Results

Contrary to previous research, we found that the narcissism scale provided most information, followed by psychopathy, and finally Machiavellianism. Moreover, the psychopathy scale required a higher level of the latent trait for endorsement of its items than the narcissism and Machiavellianism scales. Overall, all items provided reasonable amounts of information and are thus effective for discriminating between individuals. The mean item discriminations (alphas) were 1.92 for Machiavellianism, 2.31 for narcissism, and 1.99 for psychopathy.

Conclusion

This is the first study to provide IRT analyses of the Swedish version of the Dark Triad Dirty Dozen. Our findings add to a growing literature on the Dark Triad Dirty Dozen scale in different cultures and highlight psychometric characteristics, which can be used for comparative studies. Items tapping into psychopathy showed higher thresholds for endorsement than the other two scales. Importantly, the narcissism scale seems to provide more information about a lack of narcissism, perhaps mirroring cultural conditions.

Keywords: Psychology, Psychiatry, Clinical psychology

1. Introduction

The Dark Triad (i.e., Machiavellianism, narcissism, and psychopathy; Paulhus and Williams, 2002) can be captured quickly using the Dark Triad Dirty Dozen, which is a 12-item scale designed to measure malevolent character (Jonason and Webster, 2010; Jonason and Luévano, 2013; Haddad et al., 2016; González et al., 2017). Previous studies have established a reliable three factor structure for the original Dark Triad Dirty Dozen English version (Jonason and Webster, 2010) and, among other versions, also for the Swedish one (Garcia et al., 2017a,b). Moreover, previous Item Response Theory (IRT) analyses of the original English Dark Triad Dirty Dozen have shown that all three subscales adequately tap into these three dark domains of personality (Webster and Jonason, 2013; see also Kajonius et al., 2016). Nevertheless, validation studies of Dark Triad measures have mostly been conducted using Classic Test Theory methods and in very few cases using IRT methods. Indeed, to the best of our knowledge, no one has studied the Swedish version using IRT. Importantly, IRT enables researchers to study the performance of individual scale items against the underlying trait.

In this context, validation studies using IRT methods might shed some light on what the Swedish version of the Dark Triad Dirty Dozen actually measure. Item quality in survey-instruments can be quantified within the framework of IRT, which helps in clarifying how items contribute information across the trait distribution (referred to as theta or θ). In this particular IRT analysis, two parameters are calculated for each item. The first parameter, the slope coefficient or alpha (a), shows the extent to which an item relates to the underlying construct, theta (θ). Items with larger alpha values provide more information than items with lower values (normally ranging from 0.5 to 2 in personality research; Morizot et al., 2007). The second parameter is the threshold or difficulty coefficient, called beta (b). The beta-parameters indicate the level of the underlying construct (θ) at which the item scale categories have more than 50% probability of being endorsed (e.g., b1 denotes the probability of answering option 1 vs. 2, 3, 4, 5, 6, and 7). Beta-parameters are often reported in standard deviation units, but extreme values (i.e., >3 and <−3), are not uncommon, especially in clinical settings. Given that previous research has shown deviations from unidimensionality (e.g., Garcia et al., 2017a,b; Jonason and Webster, 2010), we chose to separate each subscale into its own IRT model. Our aim was to present the functioning of the Swedish version of the Dark Triad Dirty Dozen at the item-level.

2. Method

2.1. Ethical statement

After consulting with the University of Gothenburg's Network for Empowerment and Well-Being's Review Board and according to law (2003: 460, section 2) concerning the ethical research involving humans we arrived at the conclusion that the design of the present study (e.g., all participants' data were anonymous and will not be used for commercial or other non-scientific purposes) required only informed verbal consent from participants and that no ethical approval was necessary for the study. Of course, ethical guidelines for universities in Sweden were followed.

2.2. Participants

The study comprised 570 individuals (nmales = 326, nfemales = 242, and 2 unreported). The data were collected in a diverse sample, including university students and white-collar workers with an age range between 19 and 65 years (agemean = 27.45 ± 10.52). The data was collected within two different projects:

  • 1.

    Dark Character Theory: The Dark Cube and other Person-Centered Methods in the Study of Malevolent Character. In this project we use person-centered methods to investigate malevolent character as a concept and how to measure it.

  • 2.

    Narcissism(s) and Self-esteem(s) in Cross-cultural Context. This is an international project aimed to examine the strength of relationships between communal-agentic narcissism (both individual-level and collective-level) and self-esteem (individual, relational and collective) in different cultures (individualistic vs collectivistic).

2.3. Measures

The Swedish version (see Table 1) of the Dark Triad Dirty Dozen (Garcia et al., 2017a,b) is a 12-item self-report questionnaire of the three Dark Triad traits. Participants are asked to rate how much they agree (1 = Strongly disagree; 7 = Strongly agree) with statements such as: “I tend to manipulate others to get my way” (Machiavellianism), “I tend to lack remorse” (psychopathy), and “I tend to want others to admire me” (narcissism). Items were averaged to create each dimension. Reliability estimates in the form of coefficient alpha were computed using the R package psych version 1.7.5. Alphas were .77 for Machiavellianism, 71 for psychopathy, and .81 for narcissism.

Table 1.

The Swedish version of the Dark Triad Dirty Dozen.

Trait Item No. English Swedish Håller inte alls med [Strongly disagree] Håller inte med [Disagree] Håller delvis inte med [Slightly disagree] Varken eller [Neither agree nor disagree] Håller med delvis [Slightly agree] Håller med [Agree] Håller med helt [Strongly agree]
MACHIAVELLIANISM 1 I tend to manipulate others to get my way. Jag tenderar att manipulera andra för att få det jag vill. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
2 I have used deceit or lied to get my way. Jag har bedragit eller ljugit för att få det jag vill. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
3 I have use flattery to get my way. Jag har använt mig av smicker för att få min vilja igenom. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
4 I tend to exploit others towards my own end. Jag tenderar att utnyttja andra för att nå mina egna mål. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
PSYCHOPATHY 5 I tend to lack remorse. Jag tenderar att sakna ånger. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
6 I tend to be unconcerned with the morality of my actions. Jag tenderar att inte bry mig om moralen i mina handlingar. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
7 I tend to be callous or insensitive. Jag tenderar att vara kall eller okänslig. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
8 I tend to be cynical. Jag tenderar att vara cynisk. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
NARCISSISM 9 I tend to want others to admire me. Jag tenderar att vilja att andra beundrar mig. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
10 I tend to want others to pay attention to me. Jag tenderar att vilja att andra uppmärksammar mig. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
11 I tend to seek prestige or status. Jag tenderar att söka prestige och status. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
12 I tend to expect special favors from others. Jag tenderar att förvänta mig speciella tjänster av andra. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

Note. From Garcia, D., Rosenberg, P., MacDonald, S., Räisänen, C., & Rapp Ricciardi, M (2017). Measuring Malevolent Character: Data Using the Swedish Version of Jonason's Dark Triad Dirty Dozen. Data in Brief, 14, 648–652. doi:10.1016/j.dib. 2017.08.020.

3. Results

Descriptive statistics for the items and scales are available in Table 2. Contrary to previous research (Webster and Jonason, 2013), we found that the narcissism scale provided most information, followed by psychopathy, and finally Machiavellianism (see Fig. 1). Additionally, in Fig. 2 we present the item trace lines for each item (based on the three distinct models) and in Fig. 3 an information function for the entire Dark Triad Dirty Dozen (i.e., based on the unidimensional model with all 12 items). However, the respective scales did behave as expected in terms of their locations. The psychopathy scale requires a higher level of the latent trait for endorsement of its items than does narcissism and Machiavellianism, which is consistent with previous Dark Triad Dirty Dozen research (Webster and Jonason, 2013). In Table 3, we show the IRT parameters for three unidimensional IRT models are presented. In Table 4, the same parameters are presented, but for one unidimensional model (i.e., all items were included in the same model). Model fit statistics could not be calculated as there were too few degrees of freedom, but based on both logic (simplicity) and previous empirical findings on the English Dark Triad Dirty Dozen, the three separate models ought to yield more precise parameter estimates than the unidimensional model. Overall, all items provided reasonable amounts of information and are thus effective for discriminating between individuals. The mean item discriminations (alphas) were 1.92 for Machiavellianism, 2.31 for narcissism, and 1.99 psychopathy. An important observation was that the Narcissism item 1 (“Admire”) had a particularly large alpha of 3.23.

Table 2.

Descriptive Statistics for each item of the Dark Triad Dirty Dozen and each subscale.

Items M SD Skewness Kurtosis
Machiavellianism 1: Manipulate 2.65 1.54 0.72 −0.49
Machiavellianism 2: Deceit 2.91 1.78 0.69 −0.65
Machiavellianism 3: Flattery 3.89 1.79 −0.11 −1.08
Machiavellianism 4: Exploit 2.25 1.34 0.97 0.05
Narcissism 1: Admire 3.74 1.72 −0.07 −0.99
Narcissism 2: Attend 4.23 1.59 −0.39 −0.58
Narcissism 3: Prestige 3.34 1.69 0.25 −0.91
Narcissism 4: Favors 2.30 1.38 0.99 0.27
Psychopathy 1: Remorse 1.88 1.36 1.77 2.64
Psychopathy 2: Unconcerned 1.63 1.18 2.40 5.83
Psychopathy 3: Callous 1.98 1.38 1.52 1.74
Psychopathy 4: Cynical 2.56 1.68 0.81 −0.48
Subscales
Machiavellianism 11.70 4.98 0.36 −0.65
Narcissism 13.61 5.11 0.07 −0.44
Psychopathy 8.04 4.13 1.32 1.92

Note. Means are reported for all items and sum scores are reported for composite subscales (final three rows).

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

Scale Information Curves for each of the subscales of the Swedish version of the Dark Triad Dirty Dozen.

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2

Item Trace Lines for each item in the Swedish version of the Dark Triad Dirty Dozen.

Fig. 3.

Fig. 3

Scale Information Curve for the unidimensional model of the Swedish version of the Dark Triad Dirty Dozen.

Table 3.

Item Response Analysis of the Swedish version of the Dark Triad Dirty Dozen.

Item a b1 b2 b3 b4 b5 b6
Machiavellianism 1: Manipulate 2.33 −0.72 0.25 0.72 1.23 2.11 2.89
Machiavellianism 2: Deceit 1.46 −0.93 0.11 0.74 1.12 1.95 2.71
Machiavellianism 3: Flattery 1.61 −1.71 −0.85 −0.28 0.21 1.33 2.24
Machiavellianism 4: Exploit 2.29 −0.38 0.57 1.08 1.67 2.82 3.43
Narcissism 1: Admire 3.23 −1.21 −0.63 −0.21 0.34 1.13 1.93
Narcissism 2: Attend 2.62 −1.72 −1.13 −0.55 0.05 0.91 1.96
Narcissism 3: Prestige 2.05 −1.19 −0.44 0.13 0.71 1.56 2.45
Narcissism 4: Favors 1.32 −0.50 0.58 1.35 2.21 3.22 4.60
Psychopathy 1: Remorse 1.42 0.34 1.19 1.81 2.40 2.84 3.87
Psychopathy 2: Unconcerned 2.16 0.53 1.39 1.86 2.08 2.54 3.24
Psychopathy 3: Callous 2.81 0.11 0.80 1.25 1.74 2.21 2.71
Psychopathy 4: Cynical 1.58 −0.35 0.39 0.92 1.44 2.29 3.24

Note. b1 to b6 reports the item locations, reflecting the threshold level of the latent trait necessary to have at least a 50% probability of endorsing the next scale-step.

Table 4.

Further Item Response Analysis for the whole Swedish version of the Dark Triad Dirty Dozen.

Item a b1 b2 b3 b4 b5 b6
Machiavellianism 1: Manipulate 2,00 −0,72 0,29 0,76 1,28 2,24 3,12
Machiavellianism 2: Deceit 1,14 −1,06 0,13 0,86 1,30 2,28 3,17
Machiavellianism 3: Flattery 1,30 −1,92 −0,92 −0,30 0,23 1,48 2,52
Machiavellianism 4: Exploit 2,33 −0,34 0,58 1,07 1,63 2,79 3,53
Narcissism 1: Admire 1,55 −1,62 −0,81 −0,23 0,48 1,50 2,53
Narcissism 2: Attend 1,22 −2,50 −1,67 −0,81 0,06 1,30 2,81
Narcissism 3: Prestige 1,61 −1,34 −0,49 0,16 0,81 1,74 2,71
Narcissism 4: Favors 1,57 −0,43 0,54 1,23 1,98 2,86 4,10
Psychopathy 1: Remorse 0,87 0,48 1,67 2,57 3,44 4,08 5,66
Psychopathy 2: Unconcerned 1,31 0,67 1,77 2,39 2,67 3,32 4,37
Psychopathy 3: Callous 1,10 0,14 1,17 1,89 2,70 3,60 4,63
Psychopathy 4: Cynical 1,08 −0,45 0,44 1,10 1,77 2,91 4,21

Note. b1 to b6 reports the item locations, reflecting the threshold level of the latent trait necessary to have at least a 50% probability of endorsing the next scale-step.

Items assessing Machiavellianism and in particular psychopathy, followed a pattern whereby high level of thetas were required for item endorsement. This means that most of the scales' information was only efficiently retrieved at the higher end of the underlying trait distribution (i.e., θ). For instance, for all four psychopathy items, the b2 coefficients are positive, which means that an above average level of the latent trait is required for endorsement of Likert scale categories 3–7. A similar pattern, although to a lesser extent, is true for Machiavellianism. This means that the Machiavellianism and psychopathy scales are appropriate for assessing only average to high levels of those traits. The narcissism items deviated from this pattern (with the exception of narcissism 4, which behaved much like the psychopathy items) insofar as they reliably assessed the latent trait between approximately -2SD and +2SD (see Fig. 2). These findings have implications for interpretation of test scores, where the narcissism scale may provide more information about a lack of narcissism, which is unlikely to be the same for the psychopathy and Machiavellianism scales.

4. Discussion

The purpose of the present study was to present the functioning of the Swedish version of the Dark Triad Dirty Dozen at the item-level. Given that previous research has shown deviations from unidimensionality (e.g., Garcia et al., 2017a, Garcia et al., 2017b; Jonason and Webster, 2010), we chose to separate each subscale into its own IRT model. The results showed that the items in the Swedish version of the Dark Triad Dirty Dozen inventory performed adequately. Many items were “difficult”, in the sense that they required a high level of the latent trait for endorsement of even the middle categories. Based on previous research, this was expected (Webster and Jonason, 2013), as the Dark Triad Dirty Dozen inventory is known to be inefficient in discriminating between lower levels of the underlying dark personality construct (Carter et al., 2015; Kajonius et al., 2016; Lee et al., 2013; Miller and Lynam, 2012; Paulhus and Jones, 2014). The items performed similarly to a recent French-Canadian translation of the Dark Triad Dirty Dozen (Savard et al., 2017). All of the 12 items contributed with substantial information. Items tapping into psychopathy showed higher thresholds for endorsement than items from Machiavellianism, and in particular the narcissism scale. The present study was able to show adequate psychometric properties and thus support the future use of this Swedish version of the Dirty Dozen.

This is the first study to provide IRT analyses of the Swedish version of the Dark Triad Dirty Dozen. Our findings add to a growing literature on the Dark Triad in different cultures and highlight the psychometric characteristics of the Dark Triad Dirty Dozen, which can be used for comparative studies (for contrasting studies pointing to similarities, rather than differences, between Classic Test Theory and IRT see: Fan, 1998; MacDonald and Paunonen, 2002). That being said, one limitation of the present study was that we have no record of whether Swedish was the participants' first language. Moreover, future studies should test the Swedish versions of other measures of the Dark Triad, such as, the Short Dark Triad (Jones and Paulhus, 2014; Persson et al., 2017a,b) and also single measures of each malevolent character trait, such as, the Mach-IV and the Narcissistic Personality Inventory (see Garcia and Sikström, 2014). Additionally, for the Swedish version of the Dark Triad Dirty Dozen we opted to use a 7-point Likert scale, as in Jonason and Luévano's study (2013), but other studies have used a 5-point Likert scale (e.g., Jonason et al., 2012, 2013), yet others a 9-point Likert scale (e.g., Jonason and Webster, 2010). Hence, we recommend that future research on the Swedish version of the Dark Triad Dirty Dozen scrutinizes the most accurate number of points to use for the scale. This in order to avoid the incongruences found when researchers use the original English version. For instance, as shown here, for all four psychopathy items, an above average level of the latent trait is required for endorsement of Likert scale categories 3–7. In this context, it would be interesting to see how these parameters differ between a version with 4 or 5 points from the 7-points version used here. Another venue of research is the use of, besides Classic Test Theory and IRT, Quantitaive Semantic Test Theory, which uses computerized methods to addresses how concepts are represented in natural language (see Garcia and Sikström, 2013a,b, 2014; Garcia et al., 2015a, 2016; Rosenberg et al., 2013), and person-oriented methods, which often are data-driven or bottom-up methods that focus on differences between individuals by taking into consideration the existence of sub-populations and the properties of complex adaptive systems, such as in this case, malevolent character (Lundh, 2015; Garcia et al., 2015b; Garcia, 2018a,b; Garcia and Rosenberg, 2016; Garcia and González, 2017; Garcia et al., 2018).

Finally, we found that the narcissism scale may provide more information about a lack of narcissism, which is unlikely to be the same for the psychopathy and Machiavellianism scales. This specific finding for the narcissism scale stands in contrast to research on the original English version. Despite the fact that Sweden is an individualistic culture (Kjell et al., 2013), it is widely known for “Jantelagen” (Law of Jante). Jantelagen was first made popular by Aksel Sandemose (1933) as 10 rules that all are variations of one single social rule, that is, “You are not to think you're anyone special or that you're better than us.”. Jantelagen, is used generally in colloquial speech in the Nordic countries as a sociological term to describe a condescending attitude towards individuality and success, the term refers to a mentality that de-emphasizes individual effort and places all emphasis on the collective, while simultaneously denigrating those who try to stand out as individual achievers (Fayolle et al., 2005). In other words, suggesting that Swedes have a tendency to not show off by presenting themselves as better than someone else (Fayolle et al., 2005), thus, probably enhancing cultural differences found in the narcissism scale of the Swedish version of the Dark Triad Dirty Dozen.

  • 1.

    You're not to think you are anything special.

  • 2.

    You're not to think you are as good as we are.

  • 3.

    You're not to think you are smarter than we are.

  • 4.

    You're not to imagine yourself better than we are.

  • 5.

    You're not to think you know more than we do.

  • 6.

    You're not to think you are more important than we are.

  • 7.

    You're not to think you are good at anything.

  • 8.

    You're not to laugh at us.

  • 9.

    You're not to think anyone cares about you.

  • 10.

    You're not to think you can teach us anything.

From En flyktning krysser sitt spor [A Fugitive Crosses His Tracks] by Aksel Sandemose from 1933.

Declarations

Author contribution statement

Danilo Garcia, Björn Persson: Conceived and designed the experiments; Performed the experiments; Analyzed and interpreted the data; Contributed materials, analysis tools and data; Wrote the paper.

Ali Al Nima: Analyzed and interpreted the data; Contributed data; Wrote the paper.

Joel Gruneau Brulin, Max Rapp-Ricciardi, Petri Kajonius: Contributed data; Wrote the paper.

Funding statement

This work was supported by a grant from the Swedish Research Council (Dnr. 2015-01229). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

Competing interest statement

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Additional information

The raw data is available upon request to the Network for Empowerment and Well-Being, lead researcher Danilo Garcia: http://ltblekinge.se/Forskning-och-utveckling/Blekinge-kompetenscentrum/Summary-in-English/.

References

  1. Carter G.L., Campbell A.C., Muncer S., Carter K.A. A Mokken analysis of the Dark Triad ‘Dirty Dozen’: sex and age differences in scale structures, and issues with individual items. Pers. Indiv. Differ. 2015;83:185–191. [Google Scholar]
  2. Fan X. Item response theory and classical test theory: an empirical comparison of their item/person statistics. Educ. Psychol. Meas. 1998;58:357–381. [Google Scholar]
  3. Fayolle A., Kyrö P., Ulijin J. The entrepreneurship debate in Europe: a matter of history and culture? In: Fayolle A., Kyrö P., Ulijin J., editors. Entrepreneurship Research in Europe: Outcomes and Perspectives. Edward Elgar; Cheltenham, UK: 2005. [Google Scholar]
  4. Garcia D. Affective profiles model. In: Zeigler-Hill V., Shackelford T., editors. Encyclopedia of Personality and Individual Differences. Springer; Cham, Switzerland: 2018. [Google Scholar]
  5. Garcia D. Dark cube. In: Zeigler-Hill V., Shackelford T., editors. Encyclopedia of Personality and Individual Differences. Springer; Cham, Switzerland: 2018. [Google Scholar]
  6. Garcia D., Anckarsäter H., Kjell O.N.E., Archer T., Rosenberg P., Cloninger C.R., Sikström S. Agentic, communal, and spiritual traits are related to the semantic representation of written narratives of positive and negative life events. Psychol. Well-Being: Theor. Res. Pract. 2015;5:1–20. [Google Scholar]
  7. Garcia D., González F.R. The Dark Cube: dark profiles character profiles and OCEAN. PeerJ. 2017;5:e3845. doi: 10.7717/peerj.3845. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Garcia D., Kjell O.N.E., Sikström S., Archer T. Using language and affective profiles to investigate differences between individuals. Clin. Exp. Psychol. 2016;2 [Google Scholar]
  9. Garcia D., MacDonald S., Archer T. Two different approaches to the affective profiles model: median splits (Variable-Oriented) and cluster analysis (Person-Oriented) PeerJ. 2015;3:e1380. doi: 10.7717/peerj.1380. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Garcia D., MacDonald S., Rapp-Ricciardi M. Factor analysis of the Swedish version of the dark triad dirty dozen. PsyCh J. 2017;2:166–167. doi: 10.1002/pchj.168. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Garcia D., Rosenberg P. The dark cube: dark and light character profiles. PeerJ. 2016;4:e1675. doi: 10.7717/peerj.1675. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Garcia D., Rosenberg P., González F.R., Rapp Ricciardi M. Dark time matter: dark character profiles and time perspective. Psychology. 2018;9:63–79. [Google Scholar]
  13. Garcia D., Rosenberg P., MacDonald S., Räisänen C., Rapp Ricciardi M. Measuring malevolent character: data using the Swedish version of Jonason’s dark triad dirty dozen. Data Brief. 2017;14:648–652. doi: 10.1016/j.dib.2017.08.020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Garcia D., Sikström S. Quantifying the semantic representations in adolescents' memories of positive and negative life events. J. Happiness Stud. 2013;14:1309–1323. [Google Scholar]
  15. Garcia D., Sikström S. A collective theory of happiness: words related to the word happiness in Swedish online newspapers. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Netw. 2013;16:469–472. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2012.0535. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Garcia D., Sikström S. The dark side of facebook – dark triad of personality predicts semantic representation of status updates. Pers. Indiv. Differ. 2014;67:92–94. [Google Scholar]
  17. González F.R., Nima A.A., Garcia D. Sex and dark times' strategy: dark triad and time perspective. PsyCh J. 2017;6:98–99. doi: 10.1002/pchj.153. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Haddad B., Ångman M., Archer T., Garcia D. Dark triad, sociosexual orientation and religious affiliation: an association and moderation study. Clin. Exp. Psychol. 2016;2 [Google Scholar]
  19. Jonason P.K., Luévano V.X. Walking the thin line between efficiency and accuracy: validity and structural properties of the Dirty Dozen. Pers. Indiv. Differ. 2013;55:76–81. [Google Scholar]
  20. Jonason P.K., Li N.P., Czarna A.Z. Quick and Dirty: some psychosocial costs associated with the Dark Triad in three countries. Evol. Psychol. 2013;11:172–185. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Jonason P.K., Slomski S., Partyka J. The Dark Triad at work: how toxic employees get their way. Pers. Indiv. Differ. 2012;52:449–453. [Google Scholar]
  22. Jonason P.K., Webster G.D. The dirty dozen: a concise measure of the dark triad. Psychol. Assess. 2010;22:420–432. doi: 10.1037/a0019265. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Jones D.J., Paulhus D.L. Introducing the short Dark Triad (SD3): a brief measure of dark personality traits. Assessment. 2014;21(1):28–41. doi: 10.1177/1073191113514105. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Kajonius P.J., Persson B.N., Rosenberg P., Garcia D. The (mis)measurement of the Dark Triad Dirty Dozen: exploitation at the core of the scale. PeerJ. 2016;4:e1748. doi: 10.7717/peerj.1748. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Kjell O.N.E., Nima A.A., Sikström S., Archer T., Garcia D. Iranian and Swedish adolescents: differences in personality traits and well-being. PeerJ. 2013;1:e197. doi: 10.7717/peerj.197. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Lee K., Ashton M.C., Wiltshire J., Bourdage J.S., Visser B.A., Gallucci A. Sex, power, and money: prediction from the dark triad and honesty–humility. Eur. J. Pers. 2013;27:169–184. [Google Scholar]
  27. Lundh L.-G. The person as a focus for research – the contributions of windelband, stern, allport, lamiell, and magnusson. J. Person-Oriented Res. 2015;1:15–33. [Google Scholar]
  28. MacDonald P., Paunonen S.V. A Monte Carlo comparison of item and person statistics based on item response theory versus classical test theory. Educ. Psychol. Meas. 2002;62:921–943. [Google Scholar]
  29. Miller J.D., Lynam D.R. An examination of the Psychopathic Personality Inventory's nomological network: a meta-analytic review. Pers. Disord.: Theor. Res. Treat. 2012;3:305–326. doi: 10.1037/a0024567. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Morizot J.M., Ainsworth A.T., Reise S.P. Towards modern psychometrics: application of item response theory models in personality research. In: Robins R.W., Fraley R.C., Krueger R.F., editors. Handbook of Research Methods in Personality Psychology. Guilford Press; New York, NY: 2007. pp. 407–423. [Google Scholar]
  31. Paulhus D.L., Jones D.N. Measuring dark personalities. In: Boyle G.J., Saklofske D.H., Matthews G., editors. Measures of Personality and Social Psychological Constructs. Academic Press; San Diego, CA: 2014. [Google Scholar]
  32. Paulhus D.L., Williams K.M. The dark triad of personality: narcissism, Machiavellianism, and psychopathy. J. Res. Pers. 2002;36:556–563. [Google Scholar]
  33. Persson B.N., Kajonius P.J., Garcia D. Revisiting the structure of the Short dark triad. Assessment. 2017 doi: 10.1177/1073191117701192. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Persson B.N., Kajonius P.J., Garcia D. Testing construct independence in the Short dark triad using item response theory. Pers. Indiv. Differ. 2017;117:74–80. [Google Scholar]
  35. Rosenberg P., Sikström S., Garcia D. The difference between living biblically and just imagining it: a study on experiential-based learning among Swedish adolescents. Sch. Psychol. Int. 2013;34:565–571. [Google Scholar]
  36. Sandemose A. Aschehoug (Repr. 2005); Oslo: 1933. En Flyktning Krysser Sitt Spor [A Fugitive Crossess His Tracks] [Google Scholar]
  37. Savard C., Simard C., Jonason P.K. Psychometric properties of the French-Canadian version of the dark triad dirty dozen. Pers. Indiv. Differ. 2017;119:122–128. [Google Scholar]
  38. Webster G.D., Jonason P.K. Putting the “IRT” in “dirty”: item response theory analyses of the dark triad dirty dozen—an efficient measure of narcissism, psychopathy, and machiavellianism. Pers. Indiv. Differ. 2013;54:302–306. [Google Scholar]

Articles from Heliyon are provided here courtesy of Elsevier

RESOURCES