Skip to main content
eLife logoLink to eLife
. 2018 May 29;7:e31185. doi: 10.7554/eLife.31185

Cognitive regulation alters social and dietary choice by changing attribute representations in domain-general and domain-specific brain circuits

Anita Tusche 1,, Cendri A Hutcherson 2,3
Editor: Geoffrey Schoenbaum4
PMCID: PMC5973829  PMID: 29813018

Abstract

Are some people generally more successful using cognitive regulation or does it depend on the choice domain? Why? We combined behavioral computational modeling and multivariate decoding of fMRI responses to identify neural loci of regulation-related shifts in value representations across goals and domains (dietary or altruistic choice). Surprisingly, regulatory goals did not alter integrative value representations in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex, which represented all choice-relevant attributes across goals and domains. Instead, the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) flexibly encoded goal-consistent values and predicted regulatory success for the majority of choice-relevant attributes, using attribute-specific neural codes. We also identified domain-specific exceptions: goal-dependent encoding of prosocial attributes localized to precuneus and temporo-parietal junction (not DLPFC). Our results suggest that cognitive regulation operated by changing specific attribute representations (not integrated values). Evidence of domain-general and domain-specific neural loci reveals important divisions of labor, explaining when and why regulatory success generalizes (or doesn’t) across contexts and domains.

Research organism: Human

Introduction

Choices often require us to weigh competing considerations. Does a decadent piece of cake merit the pounds we’ll put on afterwards? Should the pleas of a homeless person trump our own selfish needs? Empirical evidence suggests that the answer to these questions depends in part on a decision maker’s goals (Bettman et al., 1998) and can be affected by intentional control (Hare et al., 2011a; Hutcherson et al., 2012; Sokol-Hessner et al., 2013). Cognitive regulation of decision making thus serves an important function in goal-directed behavior (Magar et al., 2008), relying on attention, working memory, and executive control to promote particular, goal-congruent choices (e.g., eat healthier, be kinder). Cognitive regulation of decision making is an important technique in therapeutic interventions for problematic behaviors, including obesity (Shaw et al., 2005), addiction (Carroll and Onken, 2005), and other decision making disorders (Sylvain et al., 1997). Previous findings have significantly advanced our understanding of the psychological and neural bases of cognitive regulation of decision making (Hare et al., 2011a; Hutcherson et al., 2012; Hare et al., 2009; Kober et al., 2010), yet important questions about its computational underpinnings remain. At what level of the processing stream does goal-dependent cognitive regulation change the typical trajectory of choice? Does it operate in the same manner in different contexts, or does it depend on the domain? Answering these questions has important ramifications for understanding when people succeed or fail to implement their regulatory goals during decision making, why some people seem to succeed more often than others, and whether there are neural targets for treatment or biomarkers to identify at-risk individuals.

In studies of basic choice, weighted additive utility models have been used successfully to capture patterns in human behavior across a variety of domains (Charness and Rabin, 2002; Schoemaker and Waid, 1982). In these models, decision makers compute the decision value (DV) of each option as the weighted sum of its choice-relevant attributes (DV=iwiAttributei) (Keeney and Raiffa, 1993; Anderson et al., 1971) and compare them to make a choice. Recent neuroscience work provides evidence in favor of this model, observing signals related to the value of specific attributes in distinct cortical and subcortical areas, for both social (Hutcherson et al., 2015a; Hutcherson et al., 2015b) and non-social choices (Lim et al., 2011; Basten et al., 2010). In turn, signals correlated with the overall, integrated decision value of an option have been observed in multiple regions, such as the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (VMPFC) and ventral striatum (Hutcherson et al., 2012; Plassmann et al., 2007; Kable and Glimcher, 2007; Knutson et al., 2007; Grueschow et al., 2015; Bartra et al., 2013; Clithero and Rangel, 2014). A key goal of neuroeconomics is to describe how these attribute and decision value computations change as a function of regulatory goals and contexts, and to link such changes to regulatory success. Here, we sought to address three important questions about this process.

First, at what level does cognitive regulation operate to change value representations? Based on the neuroeconomic model outlined above, we hypothesized two possibilities. The attribute-level hypothesis suggests that cognitive regulation of decision making could alter value representations at a relatively low level, by amplifying or diminishing attribute representations directly in a distributed set of specific, dedicated attribute-coding areas, similar to attentional effects on visual object encoding (Egner and Hirsch, 2005). Alternatively, the integration-level hypothesis suggests that cognitive regulation of decision making might operate at comparatively higher levels in centralized, domain-general value integration areas such as the VMPFC (Hare et al., 2009; Hare et al., 2010).

Second, we aimed to explicitly test whether cognitive regulation alters value representations at the same level regardless of domain, or whether it differs as a function of attributes, goals or choice domain. For example, some attributes (such as taste) may be innate and prepotent, while other attributes (such as health or social considerations) may be more abstract or effortful to construct (Sullivan et al., 2015; Metcalfe and Mischel, 1999; Loewenstein and Small, 2007). We sought to test whether these distinctions might affect where and how cognitive regulation operates to alter value representations during decision making. We also sought to determine whether this translates into distinct regulatory capacities as a function of regulatory goal or choice domain.

Finally, we sought to shed light on whether information represented in VMPFC and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) supports either attribute-level or integration-level changes in value during cognitive regulation of decision making. For example, some experimental evidence supports the idea that the DLPFC might represent more abstract attributes like health (Hare et al., 2011b2011; Bhanji and Beer, 2012), and that regulatory control could modulate interactions between the DLPFC and VMPFC to change attribute weights in integrative decision value computations (Hare et al., 2011a; Sokol-Hessner et al., 2013; Kober et al., 2010; Bhanji and Beer, 2012). However, several failures to observe changes in the VMPFC during cognitive regulation of decision making (Hutcherson et al., 2012; Hollmann et al., 2012; Yokum and Stice, 2013) suggest the need to either measure value computation in a more sensitive way, or to identify alternate routes to behavioral change.

Addressing these issues requires investigating regulatory control across multiple attributes and domains, using a sophisticated array of approaches for identifying changes in the representations of both specific attributes and integrated value signals. We used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to measure brain responses while subjects completed two choice tasks, separated in time by up to 24 months (Figure 1A,B). Choices involved foods varying in healthiness and tastiness (food task) or monetary proposals varying in payoffs for subjects and an anonymous partner (altruism task). To mimic the kinds of cognitive reframing approaches that are often used in therapy for decision making disorders (Shaw et al., 2005; Carroll and Onken, 2005), both tasks asked subjects to adopt distinct regulatory goals designed to highlight different choice attributes (e.g., ‘focus on the food’s healthiness’, ‘focus on your partner’s feelings’). To pinpoint whether and how regulation altered specific attribute representations or integrative value computations at the behavioral and neural level, we combined a multi-attribute extension of the drift diffusion model (DDM) (Ratcliff and McKoon, 2008; Smith and Ratcliff, 2004) with multivariate pattern analyses (MVPA) of neural responses (Kriegeskorte et al., 2006; Haynes and Rees, 2006). MVPA approaches to fMRI data exploit information encoded across multiple voxels and have been suggested to detect information that would be missed by conventional univariate analyses (Kriegeskorte and Bandettini, 2007). Past research on cognitive regulation has relied primarily on mass univariate approaches, which could account for some of the inconsistencies observed in the literature. Our study used MVPA to examine whether and how directed attention to specific goals affects the neural information content (i.e., decoding accuracies) for attribute values in different social and non-social decision contexts. We hypothesized that goal-dependent changes in neural decoding accuracies would match predictions on altered attribute weights from the behavioral computational model. We investigated where such changes occurred, whether they operate in generic or domain-specific manner, and whether they predicted specific aspects of regulatory success across individuals.

Figure 1. fMRI Paradigms and Choices.

(A) Food Task. Subjects chose between on-screen food items that varied in tastiness and healthiness and a neutral default food. Choices were made in ‘Natural’ [NC], ‘Focus on Health’ [HC], and ‘Focus on Taste’ Conditions [TC]. (B) Altruism Task. Subjects chose between on-screen proposals that affected the payoff of themselves ($Self) and an anonymous partner ($Other) and a default option ($20 for both). Choices were made in ‘Natural’ [NC], ‘Focus on Ethics’ [EC], and ‘Focus on Partner’ Conditions [PC]. (C) (D). Bar plots illustrate condition-wise percentages of healthy (C) and generous (D) choices (M ± SD), and subject-specific scores (circles). *p < 0.05, corrected, p < 0.05, uncorrected. (E) Computational behavioral model (DDM). Choices (yes/no) are made when the sequential accumulation of noisy value information that unfolds over time crosses the predefined upper or lower threshold for choice. The relative decision value (RDV) at a point in time (t) is computed as the weighted sum of choice relevant attributes plus noise (ε) (i.e., RDVt = RDVt-1 + wTastiness * Tastiness + wHealthiness * Healthiness + εt). In the example displayed here, the value of a candy bar will tend to accumulate in a positive direction if the weight on Tastiness is high (blue line), yielding a choice in favor of a tasty but unhealthy item. However, the value of the food item is more likely to accumulate in a negative direction if the weight on Healthiness is high (brown line). Note that saying Yes can sometimes indicate a healthy choice, and sometimes an unhealthy choice. (RT = reaction times [sec]; figure adapted from [Hutcherson et al., 2015b; Adolphs and Tusche, 2017]).

Figure 1.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1. Drift diffusion model (DDM) fits to behavior in both choice tasks.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1.

(A) Correspondence in the altruism task between observed acceptance rates (top) and response times (bottom) for different proposal types (bars) and model predictions (blue circles, determined using best-fitting parameters for each subject). On average, subject-level correlation between observed and predicted acceptance rates across trial types was generally quite high. (B) Correspondence in the food task between observed and model-predicted acceptance rates (top) and response times (bottom) for foods of varying taste and healthiness (subject-specific ratings outside the scanner). For illustration purposes, for both tasks model fit to behavior is shown for eight bins created based on the displayed color scheme (right) for variations in choice-relevant attributes (increased attribute values from left to right). Thus, bar colors correspond to trials with specified combination of attributes.

Results

Behavior

To identify how value computations change to accommodate regulatory goals, our analysis strategy proceeded in the several steps. First, on the behavioral level, we confirmed that regulatory goals resulted in altered choice behavior. We also used our computational behavioral models (multi-attribute drift diffusion models, DDMs) to link these alterations to amplification or suppression of the influence of specific choice-relevant attributes on choices.

Choice behavior

Choices in both tasks varied considerably by regulatory goal (Figure 1C,D). In the food task, subjects made choices in three conditions: Respond Naturally [NC] (‘respond as you naturally would’), Focus on Health [HC] (‘focus on the healthiness of the food when making the choice’), and Focus on Taste [TC] (‘focus on the tastiness of the food when making the choice’), implemented in interleaved blocks (see Appendix 1 – Instructions for regulatory conditions in both choice tasks for instructions). We defined a healthy choice as accepting the on-screen food if it was healthier than the default food (based on subject-specific healthiness ratings obtained outside of the scanner, see Materials and methods), and rejecting it otherwise. As expected, subjects made significantly healthier choices during HC (M ± SD: 78.83% ± 18.46) compared to both NC (44.31% ± 10.71) and TC (41.99% ± 11.46; paired t-tests: p’s < 0.001, Bonferroni corrected unless stated otherwise). They also made marginally less healthy choices during TC than NC (p = 0.043, uncorrected; repeated measures ANOVA across all conditions: F(2,35) = 97.01, p < 0.001).

In the altruism task, subjects were instructed either to Respond Naturally [NC] (‘respond as you naturally would’), Focus on Ethics [EC] (‘focus on doing the right thing and consider the ethical or moral implications of your choice’), or Focus on Partner [PC] (‘focus on your partner’s feelings and how the other person is affected by your choice’) (see Appendix 1 – Instructions for regulatory conditions in both choice tasks for instructions). We defined an altruistic choice as accepting an on-screen proposal whose outcome (relative to the default) benefitted the other at a cost to the self, or rejecting one in which the subject stood to benefit but their partner did not. As expected, subjects made altruistic choices significantly less often under NC (28.71% ± 15.48) compared to EC (49.94% ± 16.22) or PC (66.97% ± 24.35; p’s < 0.001; F(2,35) = 65.96, p < 0.001) (Figure 1D). Altruistic choices were also significantly higher in PC than EC (p < 0.001), suggesting that directing attention to another persons’ feelings generally increased altruism more effectively than considering social and moral norms. Overall, these findings confirmed that regulatory goals resulted in altered choice behavior in the food task and the altruism task.

Regulatory success

Given the considerable individual heterogeneity in the extent of these changes, we also sought to understand whether this heterogeneity might be consistent across tasks and regulatory instructions. Regulatory success – defined as goal-consistent changes in percent healthy or altruistic choices (Hare et al., 2011a) (e.g., the increase in healthy choices during HC compared to NC) – covaried across tasks (Table 1). People who chose healthy foods more often when attending to a food’s healthiness also behaved more altruistically when focusing on pro-social attributes. These results did not depend on the delay between tasks (partial correlations controlling for delay of up to 24 months, M ± SD: 16.42 ± 8.66, range: 1 to 24) or differences in baseline responding within a particular condition: the percentage of healthy and altruistic choices during NC blocks of both tasks did not correlate (all p’s > 0.05, uncorrected). Instead, they were driven by choice behavior during regulation: healthy choice during HC correlated with altruistic choice in both EC (r = 0.45, p < 0.05) and PC (r = 0.66, p < 0.001). Overall, these findings indicate that an individuals’ regulatory success generalized across choice domains. We found no significant correlation of self-reported motivation to comply with instructions with regulation success in the food task (all p’s > 0.14, uncorrected) or the altruism task (all p’s > 0.16, uncorrected) (Appendix 1 – Self-reported motivation to comply with instructions and observed regulation-success).

Table 1. Correlation of regulatory success (RS) in both choice tasks.
Regulatory Success (RS) in Food Task
RS [HC - NC] RS [HC - TC] RS [NC - TC]
RS in
Altruism Task
ΔRS [PC - NC] 0.52 * 0.56 * 0.33
ΔRS [EC - NC] 0.37 0.37 0.14
ΔRS [PC - EC] 0.48 * 0.53 * 0.38

*p < 0.05 Bonferroni corrected, p < 0.05 uncorrected; HC = Health Condition, NC = Natural Condition, TC = Taste Condition, PC = Partner Condition, EC = Ethics Condition.

Computational parameter estimates (DDMs)

We hypothesized that changes in choice behavior could result either from increased weighting of goal-consistent attributes (e.g. healthiness in HC), decreased weighting of goal-inconsistent attributes (e.g. tastiness in HC), or both. We tested these possibilities by fitting multi-attribute DDMs to behavior, separately for each subject in each condition and task (see Appendix 1 – Drift diffusion model for details). Model fits to behavior indicated that we were able to capture both choices and RTs with high accuracy (Figure 1—figure supplement 1). Supplemental analyses also confirmed that the DDM did not perform worse in capturing behavior during regulation conditions compared to natural choices (Appendix 1 – Drift diffusion model). To determine if regulatory goals altered weights assigned to distinct attributes, we computed repeated measures ANOVAs with regulatory goal as a within-subject factor, separately for each attribute.

As predicted, regulatory goals in the food task changed the weights assigned to tastiness and healthiness (all F(2,35) ≥ 103.36, p’s < 0.001; see Table 2 for attribute-specific estimates; for complete list of model-estimates and RTs see Supplementary file 1A). Healthiness influenced food choices more in HC, and less in TC, compared to NC (Figure 2A, all p’s ≤ 0.001). By contrast, tastiness influenced food choices less in HC, compared to both NC (p < 0.001) and TC (p < 0.001) (Figure 2C). No differences emerged between NC and TC (p = 0.47, uncorrected, 2-tailed), suggesting that decision processes in TC likely resemble natural choice contexts.

Table 2. Model-estimated weights (w) assigned to choice-relevant attributes in the food task and altruism task (DDMs).
Attributes Sample size (N) Regulation Conditions in Food Task
Mean (±SD) Mean (±SD) Mean (±SD)
Natural [NC] Focus on Health [HC] Focus on Taste [TC]
w Healthiness 36 −0.0003 (±0.0040) 0.0121 (±0.0074) −0.0019 (±0.0037)
w Tastiness 36 0.0163 (±0.0054) 0.0044 (±0.0064) 0.0167 (±0.0051)
Regulation Conditions in Altruism Task
Natural [NC] Focus on Partner [PC] Focus on Ethics [EC]
w $Self 49 0.0082 (±0.0038) 0.0037 (±0.0057) 0.0070 (±0.0050)
w $Self 36 0.0082 (±0.0040) 0.0037 (±0.0059) 0.0068 (±0.0049)
w $Other 49 0.0010 (±0.0039) 0.0059 (±0.0040) 0.0047 (±0.0049)
w $Other 36 0.0009 (±0.0039) 0.0057 (±0.0040) 0.0048 (±0.0049)
w Fairness 49 0.0018 (±0.0034) 0.0029 (±0.0035) 0.0062 (±0.0056)
w Fairness 36 0.0019 (±0.0035) 0.0026 (±0.0036) 0.0062 (±0.0058)
Figure 2. Goal-dependent modulation of attribute value encoding.

Behavioral weights (left column) assigned to attributes in food choices (A. Healthiness, C. Tastiness) or altruistic choices (E. $Self, G. $Other, I. Fairness) varied by regulatory goal (estimates of drift diffusion models, DDMs). Neural decoding accuracies of attribute values (right column) also varied across conditions in specific brain regions (B. Healthiness, D. Tastiness, F. $Self, H. $Other, J. Fairness) (p < 0.05, FWE corrected at cluster-level) (estimates of Support Vector Regression models, SVRs). Bars represent median estimates (blue = behavioral DDMs, red = neural SVRs; black boxes signify 25–75 percentile, lines illustrate the overall distribution), HC = Health Condition, NC = Natural Condition, TC = Taste Condition, PC = Partner Condition, EC = Ethics Condition, L = left hemisphere, R = right hemisphere, LPFC = Lateral Prefrontal Cortex, SFG = Superior Frontal Gyrus, MFG = Mid Frontal Gyrus, TPJ = Temporoparietal Junction, SFS = Superior Frontal Gyrus.

Figure 2.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1. Goal-dependent modulation of neural value encoding in DMPFC ($Self) and Precuneus ($Other) in the altruism task.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1.

Panel displays average decoding accuracies for clusters where neural representations of attributes varied across regulation conditions for neural Support Vector Regressions (SVRs) (p<0.001, FWE corrected at cluster-level). Bars represent median estimates; black boxes signify 25–75 percentile, lines illustrate the overall distribution. HC = Health Condition, NC = Natural Condition, TC = Taste Condition, PC = Partner Condition, EC = Ethics Condition, L = left hemisphere, R = right hemisphere.

Regulatory goals had a similarly dramatic influence on attribute weights in the altruism task (all F(2,48) ≥ 21.48, p’s < 0.001; Table 2). Subjects’ choices were swayed more strongly by their own monetary outcome ($Self) in NC compared to PC (p < 0.001) and marginally compared to EC (p = 0.059, uncorrected, 2-tailed) (Figure 2E). Moreover, the influence of their own payoffs on choices decreased more dramatically in PC than EC (p < 0.001). In contrast, estimated weights on the partner’s monetary outcome ($Other) increased for both pro-social regulatory conditions compared to NC (p’s < 0.001), with marginally higher weights in PC than EC (p = 0.013, uncorrected, 2-tailed) (Figure 2G). Fairness of proposed payouts (−1*|$Self - $Other|) influenced choices significantly less in NC compared to EC (p < 0.001), and marginally less compared to PC (p = 0.021, uncorrected, 2-tailed). Weight on fairness was also significantly higher in EC than PC (p < 0.001) (Figure 2I). Note that within-task results for the altruism task are reported for the slightly larger sample size of 49 subjects. Considering only the subset of subjects that also participated in the food task (N = 36) yielded comparable weights for attributes in altruistic choices (Table 2). Overall, the results suggest that regulatory goals changed choice behavior by both increasing weighting of goal-consistent attributes (e.g. healthiness in HC) and decreasing weighting of goal-inconsistent attributes (e.g. tastiness in HC).

Neural encoding of choice attributes and effects of regulation

Next, we examined neural underpinnings of goal-consistent increases/decreases in the influence of attributes on altered choices in both tasks. This analysis step was designed to provide evidence for the effects of regulation at the attribute-level or integration-level. Both hypotheses suggest that changes in the influence of distinct attributes on choice should correspond to changes in neural encoding of those attributes. However, they make different predictions about where these changes should be observed. The attribute-level hypothesis predicts that attributes are encoded in attribute-specific brain areas and that regulation should result in changes to these local representations. By contrast, the integration-level hypothesis suggests that attribute-specific areas should encode attributes similarly regardless of the regulatory goal. Instead, altered representations should appear only within centralized brain regions associated with value-integration, such as the VMPFC, and should be detectable in a common signal associated with integrated values. We tested these distinct predictions by examining where attribute values were represented in the brain, and how these representations varied as a function of regulatory focus. We also explicitly tested whether the locus of effect differed across attributes (e.g. tastiness/healthiness, $Self/$Other/Fairness) or choice domain (e.g. social, non-social).

Neural encoding of choice attributes and decision values across conditions

Our behavioral results suggest that a weighted combination of different choice-relevant attributes captures behavior in both choice tasks (Figure 1—figure supplement 1), implying that attribute information should be represented in the brain. However, the generality and specificity of this encoding has important implications both for theories about how different attributes are constructed, and how regulation operates to modulate their influence. We first sought to determine which brain regions reliably encoded trial-by-trial variation in a given attribute across experimental conditions and goals. Thus, this first set of decoding analyses tested if neural activation patterns encode attribute values, irrespective of whether one or several conditions drive this predictive information. To this end, we averaged the condition-specific decoding maps of an attribute for each subject and tested for brain regions that reliably predict values of the attribute at the group level. Consistent with predictions, information about each attribute could be decoded significantly above chance in multiple brain regions (Table 3), including the VMPFC, and, for some attributes, the DLPFC. This was also true for trial-by trial encoding of decision values (DVs, corresponding to observable choices in the altruism and food task). See Supplementary file 1B (main effects) for a complete list of results and details on the clusters in the (V)MPFC and DLPFC for the neural decoding of DVs.

Table 3. Neural prediction of trial-wise attribute values in food choices and altruistic choices.
Brain region Side T k MNI
x y z
Main Effect of Healthiness
  Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex (DLPFC) L 5.83 24 −57 23 34
  Lateral PFC (LPFC) L 6.29 45 −42 35 4
  LPFC R 5.83 17 54 41 19
  Ventromedial PFC (VMPFC) R/L 5.69 6 -3 47 −20
Main Effect of Tastiness
  VMPFC, extends to Mid (MFG) and Superior Frontal Gyrus (SFG) L/R 8.29 1097 -9 50 -2
  Inferior Parietal Lobe (IPL)/Supramarginal Gyrus (SMG) R 6.01 39 48 −46 46
  Pre-Supplemental Motor Area (pre-SMA) L 7.06 82 -3 23 46
  SMA L/R 6.51 100 6 5 70
  Motor Cortex L 8.85 410 −42 −28 58
  Visual Cortex L 7.68 288 −30 −91 25
  Visual Cortex/IPL/Precuneus L/R 7.23 1814 6 −61 34
  Cerebellum L 6.23 9 −27 −70 −35
Main Effect of $Self *
  Prefrontal Cortex (VLPFC, DLPFC, VMPFC, DMPFC) L/R 5.39 1306 −27 50 19
  SMA L/R 4.42 111 3 -1 55
  Visual Cortex L/R 6.94 2901 -3 −82 4
Main Effect of $Other
  Dorsomedial PFC (DMPFC) L/R 7.16 485 -3 44 25
  VMPFC R 5.92 108 18 50 -2
  LPFC L 5.58 12 −39 32 19
  Inferior Frontal Gyrus (IFG) L 5.51 10 −48 26 -5
  SMA R 5.54 15 6 23 46
  Visual cortex L/R 7.87 661 -3 −79 4
  Cuneus L 5.71 74 −24 −76 40
Main Effect of Fairness
  Prefrontal Cortex (includes MPFC, MFG, IFG, right anterior insula) L/R 7.54 1866 45 23 34
  VMPFC R 5.84 67 24 59 7
  Precuneus L/R 6.25 60 0 −73 46
  SMG R 6.22 72 60 −37 46
  IPL L 5.52 7 −39 −55 43
  Visual cortex R 6.07 95 12 −88 10

Results are reported at a statistical threshold of p < 0.05, FWE corrected at voxel-level (cluster threshold of 5 voxels); * main effect for $Self reported at a statistical threshold of p < 0.05, FWE corrected at cluster-level (height threshold of p < 0.001); only peak activations of clusters are reported; L = left hemisphere, R = right hemisphere, MNI = Montreal Neurological Institute, k = cluster size in voxel.

Conjunction of neural representations of choice attributes

Given the robust coding of individual attributes, we asked whether any brain regions encoded all attribute values across all contexts, as might be expected of domain-general areas contributing to value integration processes. A formal conjunction of all attribute-specific decoding maps (Healthiness, Tastiness, $Self, $Other, Fairness; thresholded at p < 0.05, FWE cluster-level correction, height threshold of p < 0.001) identified VMPFC ([MNI −6, 49, 1], Figure 3) as well as a handful of other regions (Figure 3—figure supplement 1). This suggests that the VMPFC contains information on trial-wise values of all choice-relevant attributes, consistent with its hypothesized importance for valuation and choice.

Figure 3. Conjunction of neural representations of attribute values.

Multivariate response patterns in the VMPFC encoded trial-wise values of all choice-relevant food attributes (Tastiness, Healthiness) and altruistic attributes ($Self, $Other, Fairness) across regulation conditions, as indicated by a conjunction of attribute-specific decoding maps thresholded at p < 0.05, FWE corrected at cluster-level.

Figure 3.

Figure 3—figure supplement 1. Conjunction of brain areas that encoded trial-by-trial values of all attributes.

Figure 3—figure supplement 1.

Attribute values for Healthiness, Tastiness, $Self, $Other, Fairness. Each attribute was thresholded at p < 0.001, cluster-level corrected, k = 10 voxels. Not displayed are clusters in the supplemental motor area ([MNI −3 26, 46], 43 voxels) and visual cortex ([MNI −28,–85, 32], 15 voxels, [MNI −21,–67, 40], 10 voxels). Coordinates refer to center of mass for the identified clusters in MNI space (Montreal Neurological Institute), R = right hemisphere.
Figure 3—figure supplement 2. Exploratory functional connectivity analyses.

Figure 3—figure supplement 2.

(A) Region of the VMPFC (red) where increased connectivity with the DLPFC during Health vs. Natural and Taste focus conditions correlates with Δw Healthiness in Health vs. Natural and Taste Focus. The yellow region shows the VMPFC ROI defined by the conjunction of all attributes. Orange indicates overlap. Inset: DLPFC seed region. (B) Region of the VMPFC (red) where decreased connectivity with the DLPFC during Partner and Ethics conditions compared to Natural predicted decreases in Δw $Self in Partner and Ethics vs. Natural condition. The yellow region shows the VMPFC ROI defined by the conjunction of all attributes. Inset: DLPFC seed region. (C) Region of the Precuneus where increased connectivity with the VMPFC during Partner vs. Ethics conditions Δw $Other weight in Partner vs. Ethics trials. Yellow region shows the VMPFC ROI defined by the conjunction of all attributes. Inset: Precuneus seed region. All results are shown thresholded at p < 0.005 uncorrected.

Goal-dependent representations of choice attributes and decision values

Having confirmed that attribute values (and decision values) could be decoded from neural response patterns, we next asked whether, how and where neural information content changed as a function of regulatory goals. We hypothesized that altered behavioral weights of an attribute should be mirrored by changes in the neural encoding of that attribute as expressed in varying predictive accuracies. Crucially, these analyses allowed us to test whether goal-dependent change in neural encoding of attribute values occurs in attribute-specific regions or at a common neural locus regardless of attribute or domain. For each attribute, we used a repeated measures ANOVA implemented in SPM together with condition-specific decoding accuracy maps to test for changes in neural information on attribute values across conditions (see Figure 4). This allowed us to identify brain regions where neural information content about an attribute, or decision values (Supplementary file 1B), was enhanced or diminished in a way that matched behaviorally-estimated changes in attribute weighting (thresholded at p < 0.05, cluster-level corrected, height threshold of p < 0.001; see Table 4).

Figure 4. Goal-dependent coding of attribute values (left to right).

Figure 4.

For each participant, we created a spherical searchlight (left panel, black sphere) and extracted multi-voxel response patterns for every trial of a choice task (middle panel). Next, we trained a support vector machine (SVM) regression model with data of 8 runs (80 trials), using neural response patterns as features and trial-wise attribute values as labels (e.g. a food’s perceived tastiness). Test data consisted of data of the ninth run (10 trials) for which we predicted the trial-wise attribute values solely based on neural response patterns of these trials. The decoding accuracy (average of 9-fold cross-validation) was assigned to the central voxel of the sphere from which we extracted the neural data (right upper panel). This procedure was repeated for every measured voxel (left panel, dotted red line), yielding a whole brain accuracy map for an attribute, separately for each task condition and participant. Finally, at the group level (lower right panel), we used these whole-brain accuracy maps to test for brain regions where predictive information on an attribute was increased/decreased depending on the task condition, based on predictions of the behavioral computational model (DDM). (Note that condition-specific accuracy maps also allowed testing for main effects of neural encoding of an attribute (i.e. encodes attribute values), irrespective of whether one or several conditions drive the effect.).

Table 4. Goal-dependent change of neural information content on attribute values.
Attribute Brain region Side T k MNI
x y z
Healthiness
  [HC > (NC, TC)] (D)LPFC R 4.40 402 51 23 25
Visual Cortex L/R 6.38 593 0 −79 7
  [HC > NC] (D)LPFC R 4.54 241 48 44 19
Visual Cortex L/R 5.52 210 −3 −79 10
  [HC > TC] (D)LPFC R 4.28 212 51 23 25
Visual Cortex L/R 6.51 910 3 −82 1
Tastiness
  [(NC, TC) > HC] SFG R 4.58 362 24 35 37
Motor Cortex L 4.46 265 −36 −16 37
Visual Cortex L/R 5.19 230 −3 −70 1
  [NC > HC] SFG R 4.08 227 24 35 40
Visual Cortex L/R 4.39 159 −3 −70 1
R 4.83 252 45 −88 14
  [TC > HC] SFG R 3.91 102 24 35 37 *
Motor Cortex L 4.48 319 −48 −22 64
Visual Cortex L/R 4.73 123 −3 −73 1
$Self
  [NC > (EC, PC)] DMPFC L/R 4.18 127 −12 53 46
  [NC > EC] DMPFC L/R 4.14 98 −3 44 43
MFG R 3.88 52 39 50 34 *
$Other
  [PC > EC] Precuneus L 4.45 648 −15 −67 46
Temporoparietal
junction (TPJ)
R 3.85 170 51 −61 16
Visual cortex L/R 4.26 276 −3 −64 4
  [(PC, NC) > EC] Precuneus/TPJ L/R 4.70 1142 12 −61 49
SMA L 4.50 189 −18 5 67
Fairness
  [(EC, PC) > NC] Mid Cingulate Cortex/MFG L 5.19 118 −15 23 31
  [PC > NC] Mid Cingulate Cortex/MFG L 5.18 183 −15 23 31

Results are reported at a statistical threshold of p < 0.05, FWE corrected at cluster-level (height threshold of p < 0.001), * indicates clusters that were FDR-corrected at the cluster level; only peak activations of clusters are reported; L = left hemisphere, R = right hemisphere, MNI = Montreal Neurological Institute, k = cluster size in voxels.

Healthiness

Behavioral model-fitting suggests that healthiness was weighted more heavily in HC compared to both NC and TC (Figure 2A). Consistent with model-based predictions, decoding accuracies in the right lateral prefrontal cortex (LPFC) were higher when focusing on health [HC] compared to both other task conditions ([HC >NC], and [HC >TC]) and combined [HC > (NC, TC)]; Figure 2B; Table 4).

Tastiness

Behaviorally, tastiness was represented less strongly in HC compared to NC and TC, with no significant differences between the latter (Figure 2C). Decoding accuracies in the right superior frontal gyrus (SFG), extending to the mid frontal gyrus (MFG), closely matched these predictions [(NC, TC) > HC] (Figure 2D). Neural representations of trial-wise tastiness were also significantly higher for separate comparisons of [NC > HC] and [TC > HC], but did not differ between NC and TC. Only two other regions (visual cortex and left motor cortex) followed this pattern (Table 4).

$Self

Estimates of the best-fitting behavioral parameters for $Self suggest that neural information representing subjects’ own benefits should decrease in both pro-social regulation conditions (PC and EC) compared to NC (Figure 2E). Formal tests of this pattern ([NC > (EC, PC)]) identified neural responses in both DMPFC (Figure 2—figure supplement 1) and the MFG (p < 0.001, uncorrected; Figure 2F; for [NC] > [EC] significant at p < 0.05, cluster-corrected).

$Other

Based on the behavioral model we predicted that, compared to NC, the partner’s benefits should be represented more strongly when attending to either ethical implications or the other’s thoughts and feelings (Figure 2G). Surprisingly, no brain regions matched this precise pattern (for [(PC, EC) > NC], or [PC > NC], or [EC > NC], at p < 0.05, cluster-corrected). However, a comparison of [PC > EC] revealed that decoding accuracies in the bilateral precuneus and right temporoparietal junction (TPJ) (Figure 2H) (Figure 2—figure supplement 1) were significantly more predictive of the others’ payoffs when goals focused on the partner compared to ethical implications. Supplemental ROI analyses within these two areas indicated that average predictive accuracies were significantly higher in PC than NC, partially confirming the prediction of amplified information for $Other [PC > NC] from the behavioral model (Figure 2H).

Fairness

Behaviorally, fairness of payoffs for self and partner influenced choices more strongly when attending to ethics [EC] and, to a lesser extent, the partner’s feelings [PC] (Figure 2I). Consistent with model-based predictions, decoding accuracies in the left superior frontal sulcus (SFS) predicted the degree of fairness more strongly in the two regulatory conditions compared to natural choice contexts (Figure 2J). Contrary to the model prediction, comparisons of [EC > PC] (and [PC > EC]) did not yield any significant results, suggesting that both regulation conditions increased neural representations of fairness considerations to a comparable level.

Notably, repeated measures ANOVAs also allowed testing for changes in neural attribute representations or decision values that were not predicted by changes in the behavioral DDM estimates. These supplemental tests did not yield any further significant results (p < 0.05, FWE cluster-corrected).

Decision values

See Supplementary file 1B for details on goal-dependent coding of decision values in both tasks. Only two regions (motor cortex in food task [TC > HC], cerebellum in altruism task [EC >PC]) were found to be significant (p < 0.05, FWE cluster-corrected). We thus focused on goal-dependent changes in information content on attribute values.

A common hub for cognitive regulation of attribute values in the DLPFC

To determine whether any areas might serve as a common pathway for goal-dependent changes in encoding of choice attributes, we computed 2-, 3- and 4-way conjunctions of all clusters that showed modulations of predictive information across conditions (Table 4). A cluster in the MFG (Figure 5A), hereafter referred to as DLPFC, emerged in the 3-way conjunction of voxels that flexibly encoded attribute values for Healthiness, Tastiness, and $Self. We found no other areas showing such a convergence of attributes.

Figure 5. Domain-general locus of goal-dependent attribute coding.

Figure 5.

(A) Conjunction of voxels in DLPFC that flexibly encoded attribute values of Healthiness, Tastiness, and $Self across conditions within the respective task (p < 0.05, FWE corrected at cluster-level). (B) Cross-condition decoding analyses tested for shared neural code in the DLPFC conjunction area across attributes and regulatory goals. Multivariate SVR models were trained on data in one condition (e.g. Taste NC) and tested on another (e.g. Taste TC), and vice versa (2-fold cross-validation; within-cell sanity checks used split-half approach). Red illustrates significant cross-condition decoding, blue illustrates non-significant results (permutation tests, cutoff-values of 95th percentile of empirical null-distribution). Within-attribute decoding (yellow frames): similar neural codes in DLPFC encode values of an attribute across contexts/regulatory conditions (with the exception of 2 of 18 tests). Cross-attribute decoding: neural response patterns that encode values of one attribute don’t allow predicting values of another attribute (neither within-task [tastiness-healthiness] nor across tasks [tastiness-$Self, healthiness-$Self]), independent of contexts. This pattern of results indicates that goal-sensitive representations of attribute values in DLPFC rely on attribute-specific neural codes.

This finding suggests that the DLPFC acts as a domain-general circuit for goal-sensitive value representations. But what does this convergence in the DLPFC signify? On the one hand, the DLPFC might encode a unitary decision value signal that is sensitive to current goals. While limited to a specific set of attributes, this would support the integration-level hypothesis. If this was the case, the same code that represents a food’s tastiness in the food task (e.g. when focusing on taste) should also permit decoding of other attribute values used in other contexts (i.e., healthiness when focused on health, $Self in natural settings of altruistic choice). On the other hand, the DLPFC might compute attribute-specific representations in a goal-sensitive manner. This hypothesis is more consistent with attribute-level modulation. In this case, encoding of attribute values in this region should be unique to each specific attribute (i.e. codes for one attribute should not permit decoding of other attributes). We tested these competing predictions in a post-hoc ROI-based analysis examining the extent to which neural codes for one attribute in one context (e.g. tastiness in TC) generalize across attributes and contexts (e.g. healthiness in HC). These post-hoc decoding analyses differ from the previous set of analyses: more specifically, to probe for shared neural code in the DLPFC, we trained the SVM regression model on data of one attribute in one condition and see if it allows predicting trial-wise values of another attribute in the same or different regulatory condition (and vice versa, 2-fold cross-validation). We also tested for common neural codes for the same attribute across regulatory contexts.

Results most clearly supported the attribute-level hypothesis. While codes for each attribute (tastiness, healthiness, and $Self) in the DLPFC generally allowed for decoding of the same attribute in other conditions at significant or marginally significant levels, no attribute allowed for coding of a different attribute, regardless of condition (Figure 5B). This supports the idea that the DLPFC acts as a domain-general mechanism for representing different attributes in a goal-sensitive manner, using unique codes for each attribute.

No evidence for goal-dependent coding of attribute values and decision values in the VMPFC

The vmPFC has previously been suggested to encode attribute values as a function of their current relevance to choice control (Hare et al., 2011a). Notably, our analyses on the whole brain level did not reveal any significant variation of attribute value encoding in this area as a function of the regulatory goal. However, in light of previous evidence, we conducted a number of post-hoc ROI-analyses to probe in a more sensitive manner for goal-dependent value coding in the VMPFC (see Appendix 1 – ROI-based post-hoc tests to identify goal-consistent value coding in the VMPFC). While activation patterns in the VMPFC (as well as several other regions) reliably predicted overall decision values in both tasks, regulation failed to modulate decoding accuracies for decision value (Supplementary file 1C) or for any specific attribute (Appendix 1 – ROI-based post-hoc tests to identify goal-consistent value coding in the VMPFC), and did not predict individual differences in regulatory success (Appendix 1 – ROI-based post-hoc tests to identify goal-consistent value coding in the VMPFC).

Individual differences in regulatory success

Are some people generally more successful using cognitive regulation of decision making or does it depend on the choice domain? Why? To address these questions, we examined the generality and specificity of value representations and their role in regulatory success. In particular, we predicted that if regulatory success operates through common domain-general mechanisms, individual success in regulating the effects of one attribute should be correlated with regulatory success in modifying different attributes in completely different contexts. Consequently, neural responses within such a domain-general neural locus should predict individual differences in people’s regulatory success across domains. By contrast, to the extent that cognitive regulation of decision making operates at the attribute-level in a domain-specific manner, success regulating one attribute in one domain should be uncorrelated with regulatory success for other attributes in other domains. It should also be predicted by neural activation in distinct, non-overlapping brain regions.

Regulatory success in goal-dependent attribute weighting

Although our previous analyses suggested that regulatory success as measured by frequency of healthy and generous choices was correlated across participants, this analysis did not examine how such success relates to changes in specific attributes. Thus, to determine whether regulatory success operates through common channels across attributes and domains, we first tested using behavior whether subjects’ ability to modulate specific attribute weights (estimated in separate DDMs) was correlated across the two tasks. Consistent with the notion of a common neural mechanism (in DLPFC), successful reduction in the weight on selfish considerations (Δw $Self) in altruistic choices was correlated with successfully amplifying the weight on health considerations in food choices (e.g., r = 0.50, for Δw $Self [NC - PC] and Δw Healthiness [HC - NC], p < 0.05, corrected) and suppressing the weight of taste considerations in food choices (e.g., r = 0.45, Δw $Self [NC - PC] and Δw Tastiness [NC - TC], p < 0.05, corrected). Notably, however, enhancement of the weight on another person’s outcomes did not correlate with changes in other attributes (all p’s > 0.05, uncorrected). See Supplementary file 1C for detailed list of results. Overall, this pattern suggests that regulation may operate through both common and distinct channels as a function of specific attributes, a point we return to in the neural results below.

Domain-general predictions of individual differences in regulatory success in DLPFC

Our preceding neural decoding results support a model in which regulation alters specific attribute representations within domain-general brain areas for some attributes (e.g., tastiness, healthiness, $Self) and within domain-specific areas for other attributes (e.g., $Other, fairness). This idea may explain the specific pattern of correlations we observed in behavioral measures of regulatory success and makes a further prediction: if the integrity and flexibility of the DLPFC is only necessary for representing certain attributes in a goal-consistent manner, then responses in this region should predict regulatory success only for those attributes that converge in this area, while regulatory success for other attributes (e.g., $Other) should be predicted by other regions (e.g., TPJ or precuneus). We tested this hypothesis using a cross-subject decoding approach: in a nutshell, this decoding analysis tested whether multi-voxel activation patterns in an ROI (e.g. DLPFC) allowed predicting an individuals regulatory success in a choice task, solely based on the participants regulation-related neural activation patterns (see Materials and methods and Appendix 1 – Multivariate regression of individual differences in regulatory success for details). The analyses focused on an ROI in DLPFC (with supplemental tests for TPJ, precuneus, and VMPFC) and regulatory success scores defined both by changes in attribute weights and by percentage of goal-consistent choices.

As hypothesized, regulation-related neural activation patterns in the right DLPFC conjunction area (Figure 5A) during the food task reliably predicted how well a subject decreased taste weights and increased health weights in food choices (Δw Tastiness [(NC, TC) - HC]: r = 0.51, p < 0.014, permutation test; Δw Healthiness [HC - (NC, TC)]: r = 0.42, p < 0.041). Predictions further improved when we focused on altered attribute weights for HC versus TC (Δw Tastiness [TC - HC]: r = 0.68, p = 0.002; Δw Healthiness [HC - TC]: r = 0.47, p = 0.014). Similar results were found when we predicted subject-specific changes in regulation success based on improved dietary choices (ΔHealthy Choices [HC - (NC, TC)]: r = 0.50, p = 0.016; ΔHealthy Choices [HC - TC]: r = 0.46, p = 0.027), demonstrating that regulation-related neural predictions extend to actual behavior with real consequences.

Next, we asked whether neural activation patterns in the right DLPFC also predict individual differences in regulation success in the altruism task. Remarkably, neural patterns in DLPFC during food choices predicted subjects’ ability to reduce the weighting of their own monetary payoffs during altruistic choices separated in time by an average of 16 months from the food task (Δw $Self [NC - (EC, PC)]: r = 0.50, p = 0.015; Δw Self [NC - PC]: r = 0.55, p = 0.005; permutation tests). They also predicted increases in generous behavior when attending to pro-social attributes (ΔGenerous Choices [(PC, EC) - NC]: r = 0.63, p < 0.001; ΔGenerous Choices [EC - NC]: r = 0.44, p = 0.028; ΔGenerous Choices [PC - NC]: r = 0.63, p = 0.002). Supplemental analyses suggest that predictive information on altered generosity was driven by neural information on changes in the attribute encoded in the DLPFC ($Self) and not by other attributes of the altruistic choice task (e.g., $Other, fairness) (see Appendix 1 – DLPFC-based prediction of goal-consistent changes of generosity is driven by goal-consistent changes in attribute representations of $Self (but not $Other or Fairness)). We also confirmed that decoding accuracies were not correlated with the delay between both choice tasks (all p’s > 0.05, uncorrected), indicating that predictions of individual difference scores of regulatory success were unrelated to temporal delays between tasks. Complementary decoding analyses based on brain data obtained during altruistic choices revealed similar patterns, further supporting our findings (Supplementary file 1D).

Precuneus encodes individual differences in regulatory success in altruistic choice

Strikingly, patterns in the DLPFC did not decode regulatory success for social attributes that were flexibly encoded in other regions of the brain (i.e., $Other, Fairness). A post-hoc analyses tested whether neural activation patterns that encoded values of $Other in a goal-consistent manner would allow predicting individual differences in regulatory success in the altruism task. We found that response patterns in the precuneus reliably predicted individuals’ altered generosity in the altruism task (ΔGenerous Choices [PC - EC]: r = 0.57, p = 0.002 [CI: −0.41, 0.38]; ΔGenerous Choices [(NC, PC) - EC]: r = 0.61, p = 0.004 [CI: −0.41, 0.41]), suggesting that domain-specific attribute coding contributes to individual differences in regulatory control.

VMPFC does not encode individual differences in regulatory success

Because of its hypothesized role in valuation, a post-hoc analyses also examined whether the VMPFC region that encoded all attributes predicted regulatory success in either choice task. However, local activation patterns in VMPFC were not predictive of regulatory success for any attribute (all p’s > 0.31). This result suggests that while this region may encode all choice-relevant attributes, it was not the locus for changes in value representation in this task. However, exploratory functional connectivity analyses provided subtle hints that the VMPFC could be indirectly related to regulatory success through its modulation of both DLPFC and precuneus (see Figure 3—figure supplement 2 and Appendix 1 – Changes in functional connectivity with the VMPFC correlate with regulatory success for details).

Discussion

Cognitive regulation of decision making represents a crucial tool for altering behavior to fit momentary goals (e.g. eat healthy, be kinder). Capitalizing on the strengths of behavioral model-fitting (Crockett, 2016) and the greater sensitivity of neural multivariate pattern analysis (Kriegeskorte et al., 2006), we demonstrate how regulatory goals modulate value representations at the level of choice-relevant attributes, supporting goal-consistent behavior. Unexpectedly, cognitive regulation of decision making did not reliably modulate value signals within the VMPFC. Instead, regulatory effects converged to modulate a subset of distinct attribute representations in both the social and non-social domain within a region of the DLPFC that has previously been implicated in value-based choice (Hutcherson et al., 2015a; Plassmann et al., 2007; Plassmann et al., 2010). Cognitive regulation of decision making also altered attribute representations for specific social attributes in distinct areas, including TPJ and precuneus. This pattern of neural convergence and divergence was reflected by behavioral patterns of covariation in regulatory success across tasks, made more remarkable by the fact that they were measured anywhere from weeks to more than a year apart. Our results provide important and novel insights into the domain generality and specificity of cognitive regulation of decision making, explain when and why regulatory success generalizes across contexts and domains, and raise exciting new questions for exploration.

Attribute-level vs. integration-level effects of cognitive regulation of decision making

Do goals (e.g. eat healthier, be kinder) influence construction of value by operating on distinct attribute representations, or by changing integration of these values in centralized, common-value regions of the brain? Our results provide three key pieces of evidence in favor of attribute-level value modulation by cognitive regulatory control. First, although the VMPFC contained reliable information on the values of all attributes and encoded overall decision values across social and non-social contexts, these signals showed no modulation by regulatory goal for any attribute or decision value and did not predict individual differences in regulatory success. Moreover, no other area showed a complete correspondence between behavioral and neural effects of regulation, arguing against a single, centralized locus for effects of cognitive regulation on decision making. Second, we observed goal-dependent representations of some attributes (i.e., others’ benefits) in distinct, specialized brain regions like the TPJ and precuneus. Third, although we observed converging effects of regulation for a subset of attributes in the DLPFC (including tastiness, healthiness, and self-related benefits), representations of these attributes utilized distinct, differentiated codes. Taken together, although our results do not preclude the possibility that in other contexts cognitive regulation of decision making might operate on a single, centralized value integration mechanism, they suggest that it may often operate by changing distinct attribute representations.

Domain-general vs. domain-specific effects of cognitive regulation

If cognitive regulation of decision making is mediated by changes in distinct attribute representations, when might we expect regulatory success – or failure – to generalize across contexts and domains? Our results indicate that although the DLPFC used distinct codes to represent different attributes, it may nevertheless be a common denominator in regulatory success across domains. Behaviorally, goal-consistent shifts toward ‘virtuous’ behavior in one domain (i.e. healthier food choice) correlated with shifts in the other (i.e. more generosity). This covariation was driven by correlated changes in the behavioral weighting of precisely those attributes represented in the DLPFC (i.e., tastiness, healthiness, and self-related benefits), but not in attributes encoded elsewhere (i.e. other-related benefits, fairness). These findings are even more remarkable given delays of up to 24 months separating the two choice tasks (average 16 months), ruling out alternative explanations like memory, mood, or priming effects. Thus, the DLPFC may represent a stable individual resource permitting flexible representation of specific attributes according to current goals.

At the same time, goal-consistent changes in pro-social attributes (e.g. others benefits) appeared in areas like the TPJ and precuneus, especially when focused on the partner’s thoughts and feelings. This accords with growing evidence linking these regions to domain-specific computations related to Theory of Mind (ToM) (Van Overwalle, 2009; Bzdok et al., 2012; Schurz et al., 2014) and representing others’ mental states and needs during social choice: for instance, activation patterns in the rTPJ were recently shown to encode individual differences in the level of ToM during altruistic choice (Tusche et al., 2016). Notably, activity in these regions did not encode other social attributes (e.g., fairness) or their goal-consistent changes. Moreover, focusing on ethical and normative reasons for giving (which may require less focus on others’ specific thoughts and feelings) increased altruistic choice, but actually decreased representations of the other’s payoffs in these regions. Thus, the TPJ and precuneus appear to encode features specifically related to representing others’ outcomes in a goal-sensitive manner, pointing to specialized loci of cognitive regulation in social choice domains.

The role of VMPFC and DLPFC in valuation and cognitive regulation

Our study adds to a growing body of experimental work finding that behavioral effects of regulation can occur in the absence of corresponding changes to either overall levels of VMPFC response (Hutcherson et al., 2012; Hollmann et al., 2012; Yokum and Stice, 2013), or VMPFC representation of specific attributes like taste (Hare et al., 2011a). They also raise the intriguing possibility that the flexibility of DLPFC attribute representations may be particularly important for compensating when regulation of the VMPFC fails, a finding also observed in other studies of cognitive regulation of decision making (Hutcherson et al., 2012). This raises an important question: what determines the capacity of the DLPFC to properly represent these different attributes? Intriguingly, exploratory connectivity results suggested that this may actually derive, at least in part, from functional interactions with the VMPFC area that represented all choice-relevant attributes, with the strength of connectivity between DLPFC and VMPFC correlating with regulatory success. Although speculative, this finding is consistent with research in both animals and humans suggesting that the VMPFC may modulate affective attribute representations in other areas (Quirk and Beer, 2006; Etkin et al., 2006). These results could also suggest that VMPFC represents an earlier stage in the value construction process, with DLPFC representations emerging more closely to response. Future work including the use of measures with higher temporal precision may help to elucidate when and how interactions between the VMPFC and DLPFC determine regulatory success in different contexts.

Explaining individual differences in regulatory success and failure

Our study is the first to document goal-consistent changes for all choice-relevant attributes, across diverse choice domains, both within and across individuals, shedding light on when and why regulatory efforts may succeed or fail. Our findings point to important divisions in regulatory success as a function of choice attributes and domain: an individual who struggles both to resist cheesecake and ignore their own self-interest may nevertheless have little difficulty in harnessing regulation to represent others’ needs and use this as input into social choices. This has important implications in treatment for decision making disorders: if therapeutic interventions fail when focused on one attribute (e.g., be less selfish), a switch to strategies focused on other attributes (e.g., think more about others) might be more effective. Future work will need to explore the full range of domains and attributes in which regulation could play an important role (e.g., risk, intertemporal choice, etc.) in order to determine the extent to which regulatory effects vary or converge across attributes and domains.

It is also worth noting that goal-consistent changes in attribute representations were generally exceptions rather than the rule. Most regions permitting attribute decoding showed no discernable change in representation of attributes as a function of goal. This may explain why regulatory success often feels so difficult: unregulated attribute representations in some areas (including the VMPFC) may continue to leak into choices, complicating regulatory success. It also argues against a trivial interpretation of our results that the changes we observed are simply uninteresting reflections of behavior: we observed highly specific and localized success-related changes in regions like DLPFC, TPJ, and precuneus, but not in other areas. This suggests that these regions may perform a special role in mediating the impact of regulatory goals on behavior.

Limitations and future directions

We cannot completely rule out that regulatory affects on behavior and attribute representations might partly reflect differences in motivation to satisfy expectations of the experimenter. However, we note that the specific patterns of convergence and divergence in regulatory success argue against this interpretation of our results: we suspect that if this were the case, we would not have observed either the distinct profile of within-subject correlations in regulatory success for different attributes, or differences in their neural correlates. Nevertheless, further research will be needed to fully resolve the extent to which individual differences in regulatory success result from limits in motivation or limits on capacity. Work examining whether gray matter volume in either the DLPFC and VMPFC predicts regulatory success across individuals might help to resolve such issues (Schmidt et al., 2018). Tying laboratory measures of regulation to real-world consequences also remains a necessary future step in understanding the significance of these findings.

Our results also point to a number of other open questions and future directions. The implementation of a strictly data driven approach confirmed that several a priori hypothesized regions of interest such as the VMPFC or the DLPFC are crucial for implementing cognitive control of goal-directed choice. However, we cannot rule out that other brain regions not identified by the current analyses (e.g. the ventral striatum) also contribute to decision making during regulation. Indeed, we observed changes in attribute decoding in restricted, non-overlapping areas of visual and motor cortex for some but not all attributes, which might reflect non-causal changes in visual attention or motor preparation, but could also be important precursors to downstream changes in areas like the DLPFC, TPJ and precuneus.

The close correspondence between neural patterns and model-estimated changes in behavioral weighting suggests that our information-based neural measure captured a critical aspect of changes in neural computations during goal-dependent behavior. However, further investigation is necessary to understand what separates attributes whose representations converged in DLPFC from those that did not. One exciting avenue for future research will be to identify the precise factors that determine whether and when the DLPFC acts as the site for cognitive regulation of value. Understanding this distinction may help to predict when an individual will show more global deficits in regulatory success and when those deficits will tend to stand apart from success or failure in other domains or contexts.

Materials and methods

Participants

Fifty-five healthy volunteers (25 female, M ± SD: 28 years ± 5.02) participated in the altruism task. A subset (N = 37, 17 female, 29 years ± 5.24) also completed the food task. Sample size for both established fMRI tasks were selected based on previous successful implementations of the food task (Hare et al., 2011a) and the altruism task (Hutcherson et al., 2015b). All subjects had normal or corrected-to-normal vision and were free of psychiatric or neurological history. Subjects received $20/hour for their participation, plus the money from a trial selected randomly at the end of the altruism task. They also received a randomly selected food item at the end of the food experiment that had to be consumed in the lab. The altruism data of five subjects and the food data of one subject were excluded from further analyses due to excessive movement (>3 mm/3degree). The altruism data of another subject was excluded from the analysis due to invariant choice behavior. All subjects gave written informed consent and Caltech’s Internal Review Board approved the study.

Tasks

Subjects performed two separate fMRI tasks as part of a large-scale cross-sectional research project. Task order was fixed, with the food task completed on average 16 months (SD: ±8.66; range: 1–24) after the altruism task to specifically probe for common and distinct computations in non-social and social goal-dependent choices.

Food task

 The non-social fMRI task was a modified version of an established food task (Hare et al., 2011a). On every trial, subjects chose between one of 90 food items presented on-screen (4 s) and a default food chosen prior to scanning (Figure 1A). Subjects responded by pressing one of four buttons corresponding to ‘strong yes’, ‘yes’, ‘no’, ‘strong no’ (displayed at the bottom of the screen), using a button box placed in their right hand. The assignment of choice preferences to buttons was fixed throughout the task and the right-left orientation of the scale was counterbalanced across subjects. Inter-trial intervals varied from 1 to 4 s (average of 2 s), during which a white fixation cross was presented against a black background. After scanning, one trial was randomly drawn to determine what the subject would eat before leaving the lab. If subjects failed to respond within the 4 s of the selected trial either the on-screen or the default option was randomly chosen.

Subjects made food choices under three conditions: Respond Naturally (‘respond as you naturally would’, [NC]), Focus on Health (‘focus on the healthiness of the food when making the choice’, [HC]), or Focus on Taste (‘focus on the tastiness of the food when making the choice’, [TC]) (see Appendix 1 – Instructions for regulatory conditions in both choice tasks for instructions). Importantly, subjects were explicitly instructed to always make the decision based on their preference, regardless of the condition. Every condition comprised nine blocks (with 10 trials per block), resulting in a total of 90 trials per condition. Prior to every block, detailed instructions appeared for 4 s. In addition, during food display, a short description (‘Respond Naturally’, ‘Focus on Health’, ‘Focus on Taste’) appeared at the top of the screen to remind participants of the current instruction. Each of the nine functional scanning runs contained one block of every condition (i.e., three task blocks per run), with the order of conditions randomized across runs and subjects. The only exception was the first task block, which was pre-assigned to ‘natural’ for every subject. Practice trials as well as a short quiz prior to scanning ensured that subjects understood the instructions for each condition and were comfortable with the timing of the task.

Food items varied in their perceived tastiness and healthiness and included healthy snacks (e.g., apples, broccoli) and junk foods (e.g., candy bars, chips). Items were selected based on subjects ratings in a self-paced computerized task prior to scanning that assessed perceived tastiness (5-point Likert scale, ‘very untasty’ to ‘very tasty’) and healthiness (5-point Likert scale, ‘very unhealthy’ to ‘very healthy’) of 200 food items (Hare et al., 2011a; Hutcherson et al., 2012). Ninety food items were selected from this larger set to cover the range of health and taste ratings in a roughly uniform manner. In addition, for each subject we chose one default food that was perceived as neutral for taste and health. Each food item was presented once in each of three choice conditions, with presentation order randomized across blocks, functional runs, and subjects. To ensure the motivational saliency of the food items, subjects were asked to refrain from eating 4 hr prior to testing. Stimulus presentation was implemented using high-resolution color pictures (72 dpi) and Psychophysics Toolbox Version 3 (Brainard, 1997) together with Matlab (2014a).

Altruism task

The altruism task was an fMRI compatible version of the dictator game modified from (Hutcherson et al., 2015b). On every trial, subjects were presented with a monetary proposal that affected their own ($Self) and another persons’ ($Other) monetary payoff (Figure 1B). Subjects had 4 s to chose between the on-screen proposal and a constant default allocation ($20 to both) by pressing one of the four response buttons (‘strong yes’, ‘yes’, ‘no’, ‘strong no’; direction counter-balanced across subjects). Payouts for self and other ranged from $0 to $40 and always involved a tradeoff between self and other (i.e. prizes for one individual were equal or less than the default, while prizes for the other individual exceeded the default). Thus, subjects always had to choose between acting altruistically (benefitting the other at a cost to oneself) or selfishly (benefitting oneself at a cost to the other) on every trial. At the end of the experiment, one trial was randomly selected and implemented according to the subjects’ choice. If subjects failed to respond within 4 s for this trial, both individuals received $0.

Similar to the food task, subjects performed the task under three different conditions: Respond Naturally (‘respond as you naturally would’, [NC]), Focus on Ethics (‘focus on doing the right thing and consider the ethical or moral implications of your choice’, [EC]), or Focus on Partner (‘focus on your partner’s feelings and how the other person is affected by your choice’, [PC]). Subjects were reminded to always make their choice based on their preference, regardless of the condition. Conditions were implemented in separate blocks of 10 trials each, with the beginning of a new block signaled by a short reminder instruction (4 s). Matching the food task, subjects performed 9 blocks of each condition (i.e., 90 trials per condition and a total of 270 trials), with the block order counter-balanced across subjects and functional runs, with the exception that the first two blocks were always natural choice trials. Choices in these NC blocks were used to estimate a logistic regression [Choice = wSelf * $Self + wOther * $Other] and used for a subject-specific selection of 30% of proposals most likely to elicit generous behavior and 30% of proposals likely to elicit selfish behavior. The remaining 40% of trials were randomly chosen from the full proposal space. Practice trials and a quiz prior to scanning verified that subjects were capable and comfortable to make the choice within 4 s.

Probabilistic choices

To decrease experimental demand and to ensure anonymity in the altruism task, subjects were informed that implementation of their choices was probabilistic and that in 40% of trials their choices would be reversed (Hutcherson et al., 2015b). Subjects were informed that their partner would only know the proposal and the outcome of the randomly chosen trial, but not their decision (i.e., if the outcome was due to the subjects’ choice or a choice reversal). The implementation was as follows: After each choice (jittered delay of 2–4 s), an outcome screen (4 s) informed subjects of the implementation of choices (implemented/choice reversal), followed by a jittered inter-trial interval of 1–4 s (average of 2 s) before the next choice screen appeared. Computerized control questions during training confirmed that subjects understood the probabilistic nature of the task and that it was still in their best interest to choose according to their individual preferences. In the food task, we matched the probabilistic implantation in the altruism task, and informed participants prior to scanning that their choices would be implemented with 60% probability.

Data from an independent behavioral pilot study (N = 17, 11 female, M ± SD: 24.12 years ± 5.83) confirmed that choices under almost perfect implementation (90%) closely matched those observed under 60% implementation conditions (within-subject design, all p’s > 0.37, uncorrected, for paired t-tests of RTs, percentage of generous and healthy choices). These findings strongly suggest that the probabilistic nature of the task did not systematically alter preference-based choices in both tasks.

Behavioral computational model (DDM)

We used a multi-attribute extension of the standard drift diffusion model (DDM) (Ratcliff and McKoon, 2008; Smith and Ratcliff, 2004) to capture behavior in both the food and altruism task, using a maximum-likelihood procedure similar to that described in (Hutcherson et al., 2015b) to find the best-fitting parameters (see Appendix 1 – Drift diffusion model for details). For capturing behavior in the food task, we fit a model using five parameters: two parameters for the weights on tastiness and healthiness, a parameter for non-decision time (NDT) representing perceptual and motor processes, and two parameters specifying the initial height of the choice-determining threshold (b) as well as the exponential decay rate of this threshold toward zero (d) as the time limit for responding approached. For capturing behavior in the altruism task, we fit a model using six parameters: three parameters related to the weights on $Self, $Other, and fairness (−1*|$Self - $Other|), as well as parameters related to NDT, b, and d (see Supplementary file 1A for details).

Functional image acquisition

Functional imaging was performed on a 3T MRI scanner (Magnetom Trio, Tim System, Siemens Medical Systems, Erlangen) equipped with a 32-channel head coil. T2*-weighted functional images were obtained using an echoplanar imaging (EPI) sequence (TR = 2.5 s, TE = 30 ms, flip angle = 85°, 3 × 3 × 3 mm, matrix size 64 × 64, 47 axial slices, descending sequential acquisition order). For the altruism task, a maximum of 1521 volumes were acquired. For the food task we acquired 990 volumes. High-resolution T1-weighted structural images were acquired at the end of each scanning session using an MPRAGE sequence (TR = 1.5 s, TE = 2.91 ms, flip angle = 10°, TI = 800 ms, 1 × 1 × 1 mm, matrix size 256 × 256, 176 slices).

fMRI data analysis

Functional images were analyzed using the statistical parametric mapping software SPM12 (http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm) implemented in Matlab. Preprocessing consisted of slice-time correction (reference slice 47), spatial realignment (by first registering each subjects’ data to the first image of each run, then all functional runs were co-registered with each other), and normalization to the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) brain template (EPI template). For every subject, we estimated several general linear models (GLMs), using a canonical hemodynamic response function (hrf), and a 128 s high-pass cutoff filter to eliminate low-frequency drifts in the data.

Trial-wise estimates of choice phases: GLM1 (food task) and GLM2 (altruism task)

These GLMs aimed to identify brain responses that encode trial-by-trial variations in attributes (i.e., foods’ healthiness or tastiness in the food task; payoffs for subjects and confederate and the fairness of the offer in the altruism task) and decision-values (four-point response from ‘strong no’ to ‘strong yes’) during choice periods. To this end, these models obtained a trial-wise measure of BOLD responses during food (GLM1) and altruistic choices (GLM2) at the time of the choice. For each subject, GLM1 included a regressor for each choice period (R1-R270) in the food task, lasting from the onset of a food presentation to the button press that represented the choice for the trial. In addition, the model estimated a separate regressor for the outcome phases for each functional run, movement parameters, and run-wise session constants as regressors of no interest. GLM2 mirrored GLM1 and estimated regressors of interest for every altruistic choice (R1-R270), lasting from the onset of the monetary proposal to the button press that signified the choice in this trial. GLM2 also estimated regressors of no interest including outcome phases, movement parameters, and session constants. Estimated responses for the regressors of interest – the choice periods of each task (R1-R270 from GLM1 and 2, respectively) – were then used as inputs for the multivariate decoding analyses (support vector regressions, SVRs) described below.

Neural computational model: within-subject decoding of choice attributes

This multivariate pattern analysis (MVPA) aimed to identify brain regions that encode trial-by-trial fluctuations of choice-relevant attributes (e.g. foods healthiness, payoff to self) or decision values (four-point response from ‘strong no’ to ‘strong yes’), and to assess how current goals affect neural information on the attribute level. Thus, these decoding analyses allowed us to explicitly test if regulation-based changes in neural information on choice-relevant variables (e.g., healthiness of foods) matched predictions from the behavioral computational model.

For each choice attribute and each condition, we applied a separate support vector regression (SVR) analysis in combination with a whole-brain ‘searchlight’ approach (Figure 4). The key advantage of the searchlight decoding approach is that it does not depend on a priori assumptions about informative brain regions and ensures unbiased information mapping throughout the whole brain (Kriegeskorte et al., 2006; Haynes et al., 2007). For every subject, we defined a sphere with a radius of 4 voxels around a given voxel vi of the measured brain volume (Tusche et al., 2016; Wisniewski et al., 2015; Kahnt et al., 2011; Heinzle et al., 2012) For each of the N voxels within this sphere, we extracted trial-wise parameter estimates of a particular condition (i.e., 90 of the 270 trial-wise regressors of choice periods from GLM1 (food task) or GLM2 (altruism task)). N-dimensional pattern vectors were created separately for each of the 90 trials of the respective fMRI task. Neural pattern vectors for 8 of the 9 task blocks (‘training data’) served as input features, with trial-wise values of the attribute (e.g., healthiness rating) as labels of the prediction. The prediction was realized using a linear kernel support vector machine regression (http://www.csie.ntu.edu.tw/~cjlin/libsvm) (ν-SVR) with a fixed cost parameter c = 0.01 that was preselected based on previous implementations of this decoding approach (Tusche et al., 2016; Kahnt et al., 2011; Kahnt et al., 2014; Gross et al., 2014). The resulting model provided the basis for the prediction of the trial-wise values of an attribute (e.g. healthiness ratings) of the 10 trials of the remaining task block (‘test data’) based on their neural response patterns. This procedure was repeated nine times, always using pattern vectors of a different task block as test data, yielding a 9-fold cross-validation. Predictive information about the choice attribute was defined as the average Fisher’s z-transformed correlation coefficient between the value predicted by the SVR model and the actual values of an attribute in these trials (Tusche et al., 2016; Kahnt et al., 2011; Kahnt et al., 2014; Gross et al., 2014). This decoding accuracy value was assigned to the central voxel of the searchlight. The procedure was repeated for every voxel of the measured brain volume, yielding a three-dimensional decoding accuracy map for every subject, separately for each choice attribute and each condition. Decoding maps were smoothed (6 mm full width at half maximum, FWHM) and submitted to two different random-effects group analyses.

First, to establish that neural response patterns encode the current value of a choice-relevant attribute during choices, we averaged subjects’ decoding accuracy maps for a particular attribute obtained in the three conditions (e.g., separate SVRs for healthiness in NC, HC, and TC). Subject-specific average information maps were than used in a random effect second level analysis (single t-test as implemented in SPM) and tested against chance level at a statistical threshold of p < 0.05 (FWE cluster-corrected, height threshold of p < 0.001). Note that if resulting cluster sizes at this statistical threshold prevented effective functional localization (i.e., clusters that exceeded 6000 voxels), we report results at p < 0.05, FWE corrected at voxel-level. Second, to examine if predictive neural information on a choice attribute varied systematically across the three conditions of the respective task, we used a repeated measures ANOVA as implemented in SPM. Only regions that passed the statistical threshold of p < 0.05 (FWE corrected at cluster-level, height threshold of p < 0.001) are reported.

We also compared the results of the multivariate SVRs with those of a conventional univariate analysis (see Supplementary file 1E, and Appendix 1 – Univariate Analysis of fMRI Data). However, note that multivariate decoding approaches have been prosed to be more sensitive than traditional mass-univariate approaches: because multivariate pattern classifiers take advantage of information encoded across multiple voxels and exploit systematic differences in voxel selectivity within a specific brain region, they have been suggested to detect information that would be missed by conventional analyses (Kriegeskorte and Bandettini, 2007).

There is a potential concern that the some intervals of the response scales for choice attributes (e.g. tastiness) or decision values (‘strong no’, ‘no’, ‘yes’, ‘strong yes’) might be subjectively larger than other intervals. While the present data don’t allow ruling out this potential concern, previous implementations of the response scales in both established tasks suggest that the operationalization of attribute-specific judgments and decision values allows to reliably identify value signals in the brain (Hare et al., 2011a; Hutcherson et al., 2012; Hare et al., 2009; Hutcherson et al., 2015b).

Neural computational model: cross-subject decoding of individual differences in regulatory success

This multivariate decoding analysis investigated whether neural activation patterns predict individual differences in regulatory success. A cross-subject decoding approach was used to test for information on the degree to which cognitive regulation affected attribute weights and choices. Clusters identified in the repeated measures ANOVA (see above) were defined as regions of interest (ROIs). Importantly, a main goal of our study was to test for potential common neural substrates underlying context-sensitive weighting of choice-attributes across choice-domains. Hence, these decoding analyses focused on voxels identified in a formal conjunction of the significant clusters for flexible representations of Healthiness, Tastiness, and $Self (at p < 0.05, FWE corrected at cluster-level, height threshold of p < 0.001; see Figure 5A). First, we tested if neural activation in this ROI obtained during food choices encodes subject-specific regulation success in the food task, but also in an independent social altruism task. To this end, we extracted parameter estimates for all voxels in the ROI (Figure 5A) from subjects first-level GLM1 (food choices) using the contrast image [HC > (NC, TC)] (based on DDM results suggesting differential attribute representations for this comparison, Figure 2A,C). Resulting pattern vectors (one per subject) were used as input features for the prediction and individual difference scores in regulation success served as labels. Regulation success was defined using difference scores in DDM parameters (e.g., Δw Tastiness [HC – (NC, TC)]) and in observed choice behavior (e.g., ΔHealthy choices [HC – (NC, TC)]). Predictions used a linear ν-SVR (libSVM) with a fixed cost parameter c = 0.01 (similar to within-subject decoding) and a leave-one-subject out approach (yielding a 36-fold cross-validation). Decoding accuracies reflect correlations of the observed and predicted regulation score. Statistical significance was assessed by comparisons to empirical null-distribution (realized by randomly permuting the pairing of subjects’ neural pattern vectors and behavioral regulation scores 1000 times). Only decoding accuracies above the 95th percentile of null-distributions were considered statistically significant (Corradi-Dell'Acqua et al., 2016). As a sanity check, analyses were repeated training on data obtained during altruistic choices (see Supplementary file 1D).

Note that ROI-based cross-subject decoding analyses used permutation tests to assess the statistical significance of the predictions instead of simple t-tests (as implemented in SPM) applied to the whole-brain within-subject searchlight decoding maps. Regarding the latter, computational costs for estimating empirical null-distributions for several tens of thousands of searchlight analyses - implemented separately for every attribute, decision-value, condition, and subject - prevented us from using permutation tests for whole-brain decoding analyses. However, supplemental analyses that used t-tests to statistically assess ROI-based results yielded similar results as permutation tests, demonstrating that both statistical approaches generate comparable interpretations for the present data. Notably, empirical permutation-based null-distributions for ROIs also confirmed the theoretical chance level of the prediction that underlies statistical inferences for the whole-brain searchlight results (t-tests as implemented in SPM). Nevertheless, statistical tests based on empirical null-distributions can be viewed as superior insofar as they address the potential concern that means and distribution of predictions based on chance alone might vary across brain regions.

Note also that the ROI (Figure 5A) used for this analysis was defined based on a fully independent and orthogonal set of tests for altered decoding accuracies across task conditions at the group level. Thus, ROI-selection was not subject to double dipping (Kriegeskorte et al., 2009).

Acknowledgements

This research was supported by funding from NIMH Conte Center 2P50 MH094258. We also thank Ralph Adolphs and Antonio Rangel for their support for the project.

Appendix 1

Instructions for regulatory conditions in both choice tasks

Food task

  • Focus on Health [HC]. On some trials, when you make decisions, we would like you to focus on the HEALTHINESS OF THE FOOD. In other words, you should think about the how healthy the food is (i.e., is it nutritious, good for you, etc.?). Try to bring your actions in line with these considerations. We will indicate these trials with the instruction ‘FOCUS ON HEALTH.’

  • Focus on Taste [TC]. On other trials, when you make decisions, we would like you to focus on TASTINESS OF THE FOOD. In other words, you should think about the food tastes (i.e., is it delicious, savory, etc.?). Try to bring your actions in line with these considerations. We will indicate these trials with the instruction ‘FOCUS ON TASTE.’

  • Respond Naturally [NC]. Finally, on a third type of trial, when you make decisions, we would like you to JUST CHOOSE HOW YOU NATURALLY WOULD. In other words, you should allow whatever thoughts and feelings come most naturally to you. We will indicate these trials with the instruction ‘RESPOND NATURALLY.’

Altruism task

  • Focus on Ethics [EC]. On some trials, when you make a decision, we would like you to focus on DOING THE RIGHT THING. In other words, you should think about the justice of your choice and its ethical or moral implications. Is the choice you are making the moral thing to do? Try to bring your actions in line with these considerations. We will indicate these trials with the instruction ‘FOCUS ON ETHICS.’

  • Focus on Partner [PC]. On some trials, when you make a decision, we would like you to focus on YOUR PARTNER’S FFELINGS. In other words, you should think about the other person affected by your choices and how much they would like the money. Will they be happy with your choice? Try to bring your actions in line with these considerations. We will indicate these trials with the instruction ‘FOCUS ON PARTNER.’

  • Respond Naturally [NC]. On some trials, when you make a decision, we would like you to JUST CHOOSE HOW YOU NATURALLY WOULD. In other words, you should allow whatever thoughts and feelings come most naturally to you. We will indicate these trials with the instruction ‘RESPOND NATURALLY.’

Drift diffusion model

On every trial the model assumes that a choice results from a dynamically evolving stochastic relative decision value (RDV) signal that provides an estimate of the desirability of the proposed prize. In the case of foods, this consisted of a combination of two attributes (health and taste; as rated by the subject outside of the scanner). For altruistic choices this consisted of three attributes ($Self, $Other, and Fairness defined by [−1*|$Self - $Other|]; based on monetary values in the trials proposal). On each trial, the RDV signal starts at zero and, after an initial non-decision time (NDT) related to perception or memory processes, accumulates stochastically at time t according to Equation 1 (food choices) and Equation 2 (altruistic choices):

RDVt=RDVt1+wtaste(TasteDefault)+whealth(HealthDefault)+εt (1)
RDVt=RDVt1+wself($Self$20)+wother($Other$20)+wineq(Fairness)+εt (2)

Weights (w) are constant across time and trials, and εt denotes white Gaussian noise that is identically and independently distributed with standard deviation σ. Consistent with previous work, we fixed σ=0.1, and measure other parameters in proportion to σ (Ratcliff and McKoon, 2008). The model assumes that the on-screen option is accepted if the positive barrier is crossed first and rejected if the negative barrier is crossed first. Reaction time (RT) equals the sum of the NDT and crossing time t. As in our previous work (Hutcherson et al., 2015) we allowed for the possibility of collapsing barriers. The barriers are symmetric and described by the equation

±Bt=be-td

where b denotes the initial height of the barrier, d ≥ 0 denotes the rate of decay, and t is measured from the end of the non-decision period. The models for both choice tasks thus have free parameters related to the weights on relevant attributes as well as NDT, b and d.

Model fitting was performed similarly to our previous work (Hutcherson et al., 2015a), with some important modifications to reduce computational time. In brief, we began with a range of discrete possible values for each parameter, which defined an N-dimensional grid for the full combination of all N parameters. To measure attributes for food and altruistic choices on a common scale, food attribute ratings were converted into the same range as monetary proposals for self and other (i.e., −20 to +20), using the linear transformation

Rating=5*Rating15.

This allowed us to explore a common set of parameter values for the two tasks: whealth = wtaste = wself wother = wfairness = [−0.012, −0.01, … −0.002, −0.001, 0, 0.001, 0.002, 0.004, ….024], NDT = [0.25, 0.5, … 1.25], b = [0.1, 0.11, 0.12, …. 38, 0.39, 0.4] and d = [0, 0.00025, 0.0005, 0.001, 0.00125, 0.0015, 0.00175, 0.002]. We determined all unique RDVs resulting from the combination of trial-level attribute values (e.g., $Self, $Other, and Fairness; or Taste and Health) and distinct attribute weights, and then simulated distributions of choices and RTs for all unique combinations of values, barrier parameters, and non-decision times.

In addition, to speed up computation time while generating more complete simulations of choice and RT, we used an approximation of the DDM in which we divided the space between the two thresholds into 50 contiguous bins representing possible locations of the accumulated RDV signal. Next, we specified a transition matrix representing the probability of transitioning from bin i to bin j, given a specified mean drift rate. This was calculated by assuming that the transition probability could be described as a Normal distribution with mean = RDV, SD=σ.001. In other words, the likelihood of transitioning from bin i to bin j depends on the relative positions of their centers, with transitions of size and direction RDV being more likely. To compute the probability density function representing the possible locations of the DDM at time t, we multiplied a vector representing the current probability density of DDM locations over all 50 bins by the transition matrix. We assumed that at time t = 0, 100% of the DDMs started at the point halfway between the two thresholds. In addition, we assumed that the two barriers represent absorbing states, such that the probability of transitioning out of bins with centers beyond ±Bt is equal to 0. To compute the probability of a DDM terminating at time t, we subtracted the cumulative probabilities of evidence having accumulated past ±Bt at time t from that same quantity at time t+1. Although this method is an approximation to the DDM, it is consistent with recent approaches to fitting drift diffusion models to behavioral evidence (Brunton et al., 2013), and allows us to compute a smoother estimate of the probability of observing a particular choice at time t. Our previous work using individual simulations required us to compute the likelihood of an RT falling into 250ms bins. The current approach allows us to compute this probability for each millisecond from choice onset to the response time limit for a given set of parameters. Matlab code for this approximation is available upon request.

We used these simulated likelihoods to estimate the best-fitting combination of parameter values, separately for each subject in each condition, using a modified version of maximum likelihood estimation. Based on the simulated likelihood distributions, we calculated the likelihood of the observed combinations of choice and RT on every trial for every subject under a given combination of parameters. We then identified the set of parameters that minimized the negative log likelihood of the selected trials in a two-step process. To minimize exploding computational costs, we first performed a grid search to identify the combination of parameter values that maximized the data likelihood over a courser combination of parameter values. We then performed a finer grid search on a range of parameter values centered on these values, in order to estimate parameters at a finer resolution. Although this strategy has the disadvantage that it may miss the true minimum among all 1.29*10e2Shaw et al., 2005 combinations, it substantially reduces computational costs, provides satisfactory fits to the data, and is consistent with previous approaches (Krajbich et al., 2010).

One concern is that regulation may introduce additional cognitive complexity into a task that makes the DDM ill suited to capture behavior. To address this concern, and to determine whether the DDM models were able to accurately capture differences in behavior across conditions, we computed the mean squared error (MSE) between the predicted likelihood of accepting a proposal and the observed choice, separately for each subject in each condition of the altruism task. These analyses suggested that the DDM was slightly but significantly better at predicting choices in the two regulation conditions (mean MSEPC = 0.108 ± 0.052, mean MSEEC = 0.102 ± 0.052, mean MSENC = 0.120 ± 0.043, both p’s < 0.05), but that the two regulation conditions did not differ from each other (p = 0.32). We performed a similar analysis examining MSE for behavior in the food task. We observed no difference in predictive accuracy between the Natural and Health conditions (MSENC = 0.100 ± 0.050, MSEHC = 0.102 ± 0.060, p = 0.31), and observed a small but significant improvement in predictive accuracy for the Taste condition (MSETC = 0.088 ± 0.045). These results suggest that the DDM provided a reasonable description of behavior in all three conditions of tasks.

ROI-based post-hoc tests to identify goal-consistent value coding in the VMPFC

We explicitly tested whether the VMPFC encodes attribute values as a function of their current relevance to choice. Notably, our analyses on the whole brain level did not reveal any significant variation of attribute value encoding in this area as a function of the regulatory goal. However, in light of previous evidence, we conducted a number of post-hoc ROI-analyses to probe in a more sensitive manner for goal-dependent value coding in the VMPFC. The ROI consisted of voxels in the VMPFC identified in the conjunction analyses (see Figure 3 for illustration). We tested this question in three different sets of analyses:

  1. For the first approach, we examined average predictive information in the VMPFC-ROI identified by the whole brain searchlight decoding analyses described in the paper: For each participant and each condition, we extracted decoding accuracies for each voxel in the VMPFC-ROI and estimated an ROI-based average for each choice-attribute. Next, for each attribute, we ran a repeated-measures ANOVA to explicitly test for altered information content. Confirming results from whole-brain analyses, average neural information about the values of choice attributes (as measured by decoding accuracies) was not significantly modulated by the regulatory goal (all p’s > 0.13, uncorrected). This finding is in line with our whole-brain results (implemented in SPM), and suggests that the lack of results was not due to overly stringent statistical thresholds. This provides further support for our interpretation that value representations in the VMPFC on the attribute level are unaffected at a group level by our implemented regulatory goals.

  2. However, this analysis leaves open the possibility that local response patterns in the VMPFC (rather than average decoding accuracies) might differentiate attributes by regulatory goal. Thus, we ran supplemental within-subject decoding analyses to predict trial-wise values of an attribute based on local response patterns in the VMPFC. These analyses were similar to the searchlight decoding approach with the difference that ROI-wise response patterns in VMPFC served as neural features for the predictions (i.e., instead of extracting multi-voxel patterns from a sphere around individual voxels in the ROI, we use one response pattern consisting of all voxels of the ROI). For each attribute separately, we estimated how well a subject’s multi-voxel response patterns encoded attribute values in a particular task condition. Next, on a group level, we used repeated measures ANOVA to examine whether neural information on an attribute varied depending on the behavioral goal. Consistent with the searchlight decoding approach, predictive information encoded in the VMPFC did not significantly vary cross task conditions (all p’s > 0.29).

  3. Finally, we examined if response patterns in the VMPFC encoded goal-dependent changes across participants. Such a pattern might suggest that even though we did not observe main effects, VMPFC attribute representations still contribute in some way to regulatory success. Thus, in addition to the within-subject analyses described above (1-2), we also ran cross-subject decoding analyses with an approach that was identical to the decoding analyses of individual regulatory success from neural response patterns of the DLPFC, with the exception that response patterns were extracted from the VMPFC cluster identified in the conjunction analysis. Consistent with the lack of main effects for the supplemental analyses reported above, cross-subject decoding analyses did not yield significant predictions of participants’ regulatory success for any attribute or for choice behavior (i.e. healthy or generous choices) in the food task or altruism task (all p’s > 0.31).

To address potential concerns that the selected ROI might have been too small for the multi-voxel analyses approach, we also repeated these analyses (1-3) using a slightly larger ROI. Results of the analyses (1-3) matched those for the original VMPFC ROI, suggesting that null-findings in supplemental analyses are not merely driven by the comparatively small number of voxels identified in the conjunction analyses (Tastiness, Healthiness, $Self, $ Other, Fairness).

Taken together, these more sensitive and fine-grained ROI analyses confirm our conclusion that, in this task, our VMPFC ROI encodes the values of each of the individual attributes, but not in a manner that is sensitive to regulatory goals.

Multivariate regression of individual differences in regulatory success

We used a multivariate cross-subject decoding approach to examine whether neural activation patterns in the DLPFC (Figure 5A) predict individual differences in the degree to which regulatory manipulations affected attribute weights and choices. Results for this decoding analysis reported in the main text refer to an analysis based on neural responses in the DLPFC obtained during food choices (extracted from subject-specific contrast images for [HC > (NC, TC)]). We found that these neural activation patterns predict individual differences in regulatory success not only for the food task, but also for regulation success in a completely independent altruism task.

For the sake of completeness, we repeated this analysis using DLPFC response patterns obtained during altruistic choices. The cross-subject decoding approach mirrored the one for the analysis described in the main text: First, we tested if neural activation in the DLPFC obtained during altruistic choices encodes subject-specific regulation success in the altruism task. Next, we tested if predictive information would generalize to a completely independent (non-social) food task separated in time by an average of 16 months. To this end, we extracted parameter estimates for all voxels in the ROI (Figure 5A, similar to the ROI used for analysis based on food choice data) from participants first-level GLM2 (altruism task) using the contrast image [NC > (EC, PC)] (based on differential attribute representation for $Self for this comparison). Resulting pattern vectors (one per subject) were used as input features for the prediction, and individual difference scores in regulatory success served as labels. Regulation success was defined using difference scores in observed choice behavior (e.g., ΔGenerous choices [NC - (EC, PC)]) and in DDM parameters (e.g., Δw Self [NC - (EC, PC)]). Predictions used a linear ν-SVR (libSVM) with a fixed cost parameter c = 0.01 (similar to within-subject decoding) and a leave-one-subject out approach (yielding a 36-fold cross-validation). Decoding accuracies reflect correlations of the observed and predicted scores of individuals’ regulatory success. Statistical significance was assessed by comparisons of decoding accuracies to empirical null-distributions (realized by randomly permuting the pairing of participants’ neural pattern vectors and behavioral regulation scores 1000 times). Only decoding accuracies above the 95th percentile of null-distributions were considered statistically significant. See Supplemental Table 5 for results.

DLPFC-based prediction of goal-consistent changes of generosity is driven by goal-consistent changes in attribute representations of $Self (but not $Other or Fairness)

On the one hand, neural activation patterns in the DLPFC flexibly encoded values of tastiness, healthiness, and $Self, but not $Other or $Fairness. On the other hand, the DLPFC strongly predicted goal-consistent change in altruistic choices. Is the DLPFC prediction of altruistic choices then mediated especially by the change in $Self during altruistic choices, but not by the change to $Other or $Fairness?

We addressed this idea in the following way. First, at the behavioral level, we regressed changes in generosity (ΔGenerous Choices [(PC, EC) - NC]) on changes in model-based weights for subject’s own benefits (Δw $Self [NC - (EC, PC)]), and calculated the residuals. This gave us a measure of change in generosity after controlling for change in weight on $Self. Next, we repeated the cross-subject decoding analysis (SVR) using neural response patterns in the DLPFC as features and the estimated residuals as labels for the prediction. This tells us whether the DLPFC continues to predict altered generosity after controlling for change in the input of $Self on choices. Our results are consistent with the idea that the DLPFC predicts change in generosity because it encodes changes in weight on $Self: activation patterns in the DLPFC no longer predicted individuals’ goal-consistent changes in generosity when we controlled for changes in the weight of self-related benefits on choices (r = 0.11, p = 0.317, permutation test, [CI: −0.41, 0.41]). We next sought to show that this effect is specific to changes in self-related considerations. To do this, we repeated this supplemental analysis for each of the other choice attributes (i.e., regressing out change in weight on $Other or change in weight on Fairness, and then asking whether DLPFC predicts residual changes in generosity). We found that DLPFC prediction of altered generosity remained highly significant when we controlled for altered inputs of other’s benefits (r = 0.51, p = 0.016, [CI −0.41, 0.40]) or fairness considerations (r = 0.56, p = 0.007, [CI −0.39, 0.39]) on choices. Taken together, these findings indicate that predictions of goal-consistent changes in generosity in the DLPFC are mediated specifically by altered weighs of subjects’ own monetary benefits on choices (Δw $Self). This finding is also consistent with evidence of a specific functional role of the DLPFC for altered representations for self-related considerations, while changes in attributes of $Other and Fairness are encoded elsewhere.

Changes in functional connectivity with the VMPFC correlate with regulatory success

Results reported in the main text pose an important question: if attribute encoding within certain brain regions changes as a function of regulatory goals, how are such changes accomplished? We speculated one of two possibilities. First, we observed changes in attribute encoding not only within the DLPFC and social-cognitive brain areas (e.g. Precuneus, TPJ), but also within areas of visual cortex for some contrasts. This suggests that altered functional coupling between goal-sensitive areas and visual cortex could be producing some of the changes observed in attribute encoding. Alternatively, given that the VMPFC encoded all choice-relevant information, but showed no modulation as a function of goal, we speculated that the VMPFC might be differentially connected to these goal-sensitive areas as a function of regulatory success. To test these different hypotheses, we examined regulation-associated changes in connectivity patterns of the DLPFC conjunction area, as well as the TPJ and Precuneus, focusing specifically on evidence for changes in connectivity within functionally-defined masks of the occipital or motor cortices and the VMPFC. We also performed the reverse analysis, examining changes in functional connectivity of the VMPFC with other areas.

We used a beta series approach to functional connectivity (Rissman et al., 2004). In brief, this method uses a GLM to estimate the BOLD response in a region, separately for each trial in a condition of interest, and after controlling for other variables of non-interest (e.g., response on other trial, response in other conditions, motion, etc.). The resulting beta coefficients represent the estimated neural response in that region on each trial. These values are then included as a parametric modulator in a new GLM, to identify brain regions where response systematically covaries with the trial-by-trial fluctuations in the ROI. We can extract these ‘beta-series’ for each condition of interest (e.g., Health Focus, Taste Focus, Natural Response) and then compare whether neural correlates of trial-by-trial fluctuations in an ROI’s response vary as a function of condition. We used this approach rather than more traditional psychophysiological interaction (PPI) analyses, because recent work suggests that this method may be more sensitive to contextual differences in functional connectivity than more traditional PPI approaches (Rissman et al., 2004). We provide greater details of this analysis below.

Methods: Exploratory functional connectivity analyses

Beta series estimation

The first step in a beta series analysis is to extract estimates of trial-by-trial response in an ROI for conditions of interest, controlling for other experimental factors.

Thus, we estimated trial-by-trial responses in each regulation condition for four different regions-of-interest: 1) the VMPFC area demonstrating a conjunction in coding of all attributes regardless of regulatory goal; 2) the DLPFC area showing a conjunction in regulation effects for Tastiness, Healthiness, and $Self attributes; 3) and 4) the TPJ and Precuneus ROIs showing changes in response to $Other in Partner vs. Ethics trials. To do this, we conducted a GLM consisting of the following regressors: R1-R90: Indicator functions delineating the choice period on each separate trial for one of the regulation conditions (e.g. Natural trials); R91 and R92) Indicator functions indicating the choice period for the two non-target regulation conditions (e.g., Focus on Health, Focus on Taste); R93-R96) Parametric modulators of R91-92 consisting of health and taste ratings for the food shown on each trial; R97) An indicator function for missed response trials. This model allowed us to extract trial-specific estimates of neural response in an ROI for a specific regulatory condition, controlling for activation related to other trials and conditions. These estimates were created by averaging the beta-coefficient for each trial (R1-R90) across all voxels for each of the four target ROIs listed above, which served as our signal of interest. This process was repeated once each for the other two regulation conditions, yielding 270 beta estimates, one for each trial in each condition.

A similar beta series analysis was conducted for the Altruism Task, with the exception that the model also included regressors for the outcome period on each trial, separately for each regulation condition (R97-99), as well as parametric modulators of R97-99 representing the monetary outcome for Self (R100-102) and Other (R103-105).

We also extracted trial-by-trial responses from a mask of the whole brain, which allowed us to control for non-specific global hemodynamic changes in response, and to focus on connectivity specific to a given region of interest.

Beta series connectivity analysis

The next step of a beta series analysis involves using the estimated trial-by-trial betas in a new GLM to identify areas where response correlates with trial-by-trial fluctuation in a given ROI. Details of the GLMs we ran for each ROI are as follows:

GLM S3: Changes in connectivity of the DLPFC during cognitive regulation of food choices

To examine connectivity of the DLPFC, the extracted trial-by-trial beta estimates were entered into a model identical to GLM S1 (described in detail in Supplemental Material 3.7 below), but including six additional parametric modulators. (R10-12) consisted of the trial-by-trial estimates of whole-brain response, and served as a regressor of non-interest that controlled for non-specific hemodynamic changes. (R13-15) consisted of the critical variables of interest: the trial-by-trial beta estimates from the DLPFC ROI during NC trials (R13), TC trials (R14) and HC trials (R15). This allowed us to examine changes in functional connectivity with the DLPFC ROI after controlling for other features of the experimental design, including systematic variation in attribute values.

Contrast estimates were calculated at the individual subject level for the following contrasts: R14 – R13 (DLPFC connectivity in Taste vs. Natural trials), R15 – R13 (DLPFC connectivity in HC vs. NC trials), and 2*R15 – [R13 +R14] (DLPFC connectivity in HC vs. NC +TC trials). Group-level t-tests were performed on these contrast estimates to determine overall differences in connectivity by condition. In addition, we also estimated the correlation between these contrasts and the corresponding change in behavior weights (e.g. Δ Healthiness weight, HC > [NC +TC]), to determine whether changes in functional connectivity predicted regulatory success for attributes in the food choice task. Since this was an exploratory and supplemental analysis, we report results at a significance level of p < 0.005, uncorrected for voxels falling within ROIs of interest (e.g. the VMPFC conjunction area).

GLM S4: Changes in connectivity of the VMPFC during cognitive regulation of food choices

This GLM was identical to GLM S3, with the exception that it used trial-by-trial responses extracted from the VMPFC during the food choice task.

GLM S5: Changes in connectivity of the TPJ during cognitive regulation of altruistic choices

This GLM was identical to GLM S2, except that it included the trial-by-trial beta estimates from the TPJ ROI during the NC condition (R13), PC condition (R14) and EC condition (R15). All other details are similar to GLM S3.

Contrast estimates were calculated at the individual subject level for the following contrasts: R14 – R13 (TPJ connectivity in PC vs. NC), R15 – R13 (TPJ connectivity in EC vs. NC), R14 – R15 (TPJ connectivity in PC vs. EC trials), and [R14 + R15] – 2*R13 (TPJ connectivity in both social focus condition vs. NC trials). Group-level t-tests were performed on these contrast estimates to determine overall differences in connectivity by condition. In addition, we also estimated the correlation between these contrasts and the corresponding change in behavior weights (e.g. ΔOther weight, PC > EC), to determine whether changes in functional connectivity predicted regulatory success for attributes in the altruistic choice task. Since this was an exploratory and supplemental analysis, we report results at a significance level of p < 0.005, uncorrected for voxels falling within ROIs of interest (e.g. the VMPFC conjunction area).

GLM S6: Changes in connectivity of the Precuneus during cognitive regulation of altruistic choice

This GLM was identical to GLM S5, with the exception that it used trial-by-trial responses extracted from the Precuneus during the altruistic choice task.

GLM S7: Changes in connectivity of the VMPFC during cognitive regulation of altruistic choice

This GLM was identical to GLM S5, with the exception that it used trial-by-trial responses extracted from the VMPFC during the altruistic choice task.

Results: Exploratory functional connectivity analyses

DLPFC connectivity during food choices

We began by examining connectivity patterns during the food choice task using the DLPFC region described by the conjunction of goal-consistent changes in Tastiness, Healthiness, and $Self attributes. We expected that altered connectivity of the DLPFC with VMPFC or visual areas during the HC focus condition, and that changes in connectivity with these areas should correlate with increases in weighting of the Healthiness attribute during the Health Focus condition, decreases in weighting of the Tastiness attribute, or both.

We did not observe a main effect of cognitive regulation of decision making on functional connectivity to either visual/motor areas or VMPFC, even at a liberal statistical threshold of p < 0.005 (uncorrected). However, we found that functional connectivity with the VMPFC correlated with regulatory success. In particular, successful up-regulation of behavioral health weights (Δw Healthiness, (HC - [NC, TC])) correlated with increased connectivity to VMPFC (p < 0.005, uncorrected, Figure 3—figure supplement 2A) when subjects focused on Health in the food task compared to natural or taste trials. No correlations emerged between connectivity to the visual cortex and regulatory success.

DLPFC connectivity during altruistic choices

Given that we observed decreased encoding of selfish outcomes in the DLPFC during both PC and EC compared to NC trials, we expected either that we should observe a decrease in connectivity between DLPFC and VMPFC or visual areas during these conditions, or that decreased connectivity should correlate with decreases in the weighting of Self during regulation trials. As in the Food Task, we observed no significant alterations in connectivity overall. However, as predicted, decreased weighting of $Self (Δw $Self, (NC – [EC, PC])) correlated with decreased coupling of the DLPFC with VMPFC (p<0.005, uncorrected, Figure 3—figure supplement 2B) during regulation trials. No correlations emerged between connectivity to the visual cortex and regulatory success.

TPJ and Precuneus connectivity during altruistic choice

We observed specific changes in coding of others’ outcomes in the TPJ and Precuneus when focusing on one’s partner’s feelings (PC) compared to focusing on ethics (EC). We thus expected that we should observe changes in coupling between TPJ or Precuneus and other areas, either overall, or as a function of changes in weighting of partner’s outcomes. We observed no overall effects on connectivity in either region. In contrast, tighter coupling between the Precuneus and VMPFC in Partner vs. Ethics trials correlated with greater weight given to $Other in Partner vs. Ethics trials (p <0.005, uncorrected, Figure 3—figure supplement 2C). No correlations emerged between connectivity to the visual cortex and regulatory success.

Univariate Analysis of fMRI Data

GLM S1. We compared the multivariate SVR results (Support Vector Regression) with those of a traditional univariate GLM based on data smoothed at 8 mm FWHM. For each subject in the food task, we estimated GLM S1 with AR(1) and the following regressors of interest: R1) A boxcar function for the choice period on Natural trials; R2) R1 modulated by the taste rating for the food on each trial; R3) R1 modulated by the health rating for the food on each trial; R4-R6) analogous regressors for HC trials; R7-9) analogous regressors for TC trials. No orthogonalization was used, allowing regressors to compete fully for explained variance. All regressors of interest were convolved with the canonical form of the hemodynamic response. The model also included motion parameters and session constants as regressors of no interest. Missed response trials were modeled as a separate regressor of no interest.

Subject-level contrast images were calculated for each of the parametric regressors (R2, R3, R5, R6, R8, and R9) and used for separate random-effects group analyses. We used one-sample t-tests as implemented in SPM to look for areas where linear variation in BOLD response correlated with trial-to-trial variability in attribute values, and paired t-tests to test for modulation of BOLD responses by condition. Repeated measures ANOVAs were used to examine if neural responses encoding choice-attributes varied across regulation conditions (p < 0.05, FWE cluster-corrected, p < 0.001 height threshold).

GLM S2. We computed a second, analogous GLM to estimate univariate responses in the altruism task. This GLM was identical to GLM S1 with the following exceptions: 1) Natural, Taste, and Health conditions were replaced with Natural, Ethics, and Partner Focus conditions; 2) parametric modulators for taste and health were replaced with the value of $Self and $Other on each trial; 3) we included a third parametric modulator in each condition to represent fairness −1*|$Self - $Other| on each trial. All other details are as in GLM S1.

See Supplementary file 1E for results for univariate analyses.

Self-reported motivation to comply with instructions and observed regulation-success

One potential concern with the findings described in the main text is that they result purely from experimental demand effects, and have no bearing on real-world behavior. Although such demand effects were likely reduced by the probabilistic implementation of participants’ choices (in effect obscuring their choice from the experimenter), we examined if individual differences in regulatory success in either choice task merely reflect peoples’ tendency to comply with demands of the current experimental condition by examining self-reported compliance for the sake of the experimenter.

We obtained self-report measures of experimental demand by asking participants ‘How much did you try to choose based on the experimenters expectations?’ (5-point scale, 1=’not at all’, 5=’absolutely’). These indicated that experimenter demand effects were generally low in our sample of participants (M ± SD: 1.83 ± 1.21, range of 1 to 4). Moreover, we found no significant correlation with regulation success in the food task (all p’s > 0.14, uncorrected) or the altruism task (all p’s > 0.16, uncorrected). This suggests that individual differences in observed healthy and generous choices were not explained purely by demand characteristics of the tasks.

Funding Statement

This research was supported by funding from NIMH Conte Center 2P50 MH094258. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Contributor Information

Anita Tusche, Email: anita.tusche@gmail.com.

Geoffrey Schoenbaum, National Institute on Drug Abuse, National Institutes of Health, United States.

Funding Information

This paper was supported by the following grant:

  • National Institute of Mental Health NIMH Conte Center 2P50 MH094258 to Cendri A Hutcherson.

Additional information

Competing interests

No competing interests declared.

Author contributions

Conceptualization, Data curation, Software, Formal analysis, Validation, Investigation, Visualization, Methodology, Writing—original draft, Project administration, Writing—review and editing, designed the study, collected the data, performed data analysis, and wrote the manuscript.

Conceptualization, Data curation, Software, Formal analysis, Validation, Investigation, Visualization, Methodology, Writing—original draft, Project administration, Writing—review and editing, designed the study, collected the data, performed data analysis, and wrote the manuscript.

Ethics

Human subjects: All subjects gave written informed consent, and consent to publish, and the Internal Review Board of the California Institute of Technology approved the study (AR-346).

Additional files

Supplementary file 1. Supplemental tables.

(A) Drift diffusion model (DDM) parameters and RTs. Table reports Mean and Standard Deviations (±SD). b = initial height of the barrier; d = rate of decay for collapsing barriers; NDT = non decision time.; RTs = reaction times. Results for the Altruism Task are reported both for full sample (N = 49), and for the subset of participants that also completing the Food Task (N = 36).

(B) Support vector regressions (SVRs) of decision values (DV) in both choice tasks. Results for goal-independent encoding of DVs (Main effect for DV, averaged information content across three conditions in each task) are reported at a statistical threshold of p < 0.05, FWE corrected at voxel-level; indicates results for goal-dependent variations in DV (repeated measures ANOVAs across three conditions in each task) reported at p < 0.001, FWE cluster-corrected at p < 0.05, k = 10 voxels; only peak activations of clusters are reported; L = left hemisphere, R = right hemisphere, k = cluster size in voxels, MNI = Montreal Neurological Institute.

(C) Correlation of regulatory success in goal-dependent attribute weighting across choice domains. Correlation coefficients for change scores in attribute weights (w) in food choices and altruistic choices estimated in two separate DDMs. Successful reduction in the weight on selfish considerations (Δw $Self) in altruistic choices was correlated with successfully amplifying the weight on health considerations (Δw Healthiness) suppressing the weight of taste considerations (Δw Tastiness) in food choices. Changes in the ability to increase social considerations (Δw Other) were not correlated to goal-consistent changes in food attributes. Δw Tastiness [NC - TC] is not displayed, as estimated attribute weights did not significantly differ between conditions. Note also that differences scores in Δw Healthiness [NC - TC] (last column) were minimal, limiting the interpretability of the respective correlation analyses. 

(D) Decoding of individual differences in regulatory success in DLPFC (altruism task). Decoding of individual differences in regulation success based on response patterns in right DLPFC (Figure 5A) obtained in the altruism task. Response patterns reliably predicted the extent of increased generous choice behavior. Consistent with key results reported in the main text, neural activation patterns also predicted individual’s increased healthy choices in a separate food task. Regarding altered attributes weights, predictive information in DLPFC was selective for subjects’ inhibition of $Self weights, but did not extend to altered weights on $Other or Fairness, confirming results reported in the main text. Higher-than-chance predictions are reported when decoding accuracy values exceeded the 95th percentile of empirical null-distribution (cutoff), obtained with 1000 replications of the analysis on permuted data sets. 

(E) Univariate encoding of attributes in food task and altruism task. Regions reported as significant if they passed a cluster-corrected threshold p < 0.05, with a voxel-defining threshold of p < 0.001, uncorrected, unless otherwise noted. * Illustrates results significant at p < 0.001, uncorrected, reported for completeness: subgenual area did not overlap with the area of vmPFC that displayed overlapping representations of all attributes; only peak activations of clusters are reported; L = left hemisphere, R = right hemisphere, MNI = Montreal Neurological Institute, k = cluster size in voxels.

elife-31185-supp1.docx (55.1KB, docx)
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.31185.015
Transparent reporting form
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.31185.016

Data availability

Functional imaging and behavioral data is deposited at the project's Open Science Framework (OSF) page (osf.io/wa4cs). The project page also makes available the derived statistical maps (univariate and multivariate decoding analyses), regions of interest (ROIs) used in analyses of functional imaging data, processed behavioural data, and details on the experimental procedure.

References

  1. Adolphs R, Tusche A. From faces to prosocial behavior: cues, tools, and mechanisms. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2017;26:282–287. doi: 10.1177/0963721417694656. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Anderson NH. Integration theory and attitude change. Psychological Review. 1971;78:171. doi: 10.1037/h0030834. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  3. Bartra O, McGuire JT, Kable JW. The valuation system: a coordinate-based meta-analysis of BOLD fMRI experiments examining neural correlates of subjective value. NeuroImage. 2013;76:412–427. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.02.063. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Basten U, Biele G, Heekeren HR, Fiebach CJ. How the brain integrates costs and benefits during decision making. PNAS. 2010;107:21767–21772. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0908104107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bettman JR, Luce MF, Payne JW. Constructive consumer choice processes. Journal of Consumer Research. 1998;25:187–217. doi: 10.1086/209535. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bhanji JP, Beer JS. Taking a different perspective: mindset influences neural regions that represent value and choice. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 2012;7:782–793. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsr062. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Brainard DH. The psychophysics toolbox. Spatial Vision. 1997;10:433–436. doi: 10.1163/156856897X00357. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Brunton BW, Botvinick MM, Brody CD. Rats and humans can optimally accumulate evidence for decision-making. Science. 2013;340:95–98. doi: 10.1126/science.1233912. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Bzdok D, Schilbach L, Vogeley K, Schneider K, Laird AR, Langner R, Eickhoff SB. Parsing the neural correlates of moral cognition: ALE meta-analysis on morality, theory of mind, and empathy. Brain Structure and Function. 2012;217:783–796. doi: 10.1007/s00429-012-0380-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Carroll KM, Onken LS. Behavioral therapies for drug abuse. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2005;162:1452–1460. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.8.1452. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Charness G, Rabin M. Understanding social preferences with simple tests. The Quarterly Journal of Economics. 2002;117:817–869. doi: 10.1162/003355302760193904. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  12. Cisler JM, Bush K, Steele JS. A comparison of statistical methods for detecting context-modulated functional connectivity in fMRI. NeuroImage. 2014;84:1042–1052. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.09.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Clithero JA, Rangel A. Informatic parcellation of the network involved in the computation of subjective value. Social cognitive and affective neuroscience. 2014;9:1289–1302. doi: 10.1093/scan/nst106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Corradi-Dell'Acqua C, Tusche A, Vuilleumier P, Singer T. Cross-modal representations of first-hand and vicarious pain, disgust and fairness in insular and cingulate cortex. Nature Communications. 2016;7:10904. doi: 10.1038/ncomms10904. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Crockett MJ. How formal models can illuminate mechanisms of moral judgment and decision making. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2016;25:85–90. doi: 10.1177/0963721415624012. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  16. Egner T, Hirsch J. Cognitive control mechanisms resolve conflict through cortical amplification of task-relevant information. Nature Neuroscience. 2005;8:1784–1790. doi: 10.1038/nn1594. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Etkin A, Egner T, Peraza DM, Kandel ER, Hirsch J. Resolving emotional conflict: a role for the rostral anterior cingulate cortex in modulating activity in the amygdala. Neuron. 2006;51:871–882. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.07.029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Gross J, Woelbert E, Zimmermann J, Okamoto-Barth S, Riedl A, Goebel R. Value signals in the prefrontal cortex predict individual preferences across reward categories. Journal of Neuroscience. 2014;34:7580–7586. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5082-13.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Grueschow M, Polania R, Hare TA, Ruff CC. Automatic versus choice-dependent value representations in the human brain. Neuron. 2015;85:874–885. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.12.054. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Hare TA, Camerer CF, Knoepfle DT, Rangel A. Value computations in ventral medial prefrontal cortex during charitable decision making incorporate input from regions involved in social cognition. Journal of Neuroscience. 2010;30:583–590. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4089-09.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Hare TA, Camerer CF, Rangel A, Hare C. Self-control in decision-making involves modulation of the vmPFC valuation system. Science. 2009;324:646–648. doi: 10.1126/science.1168450. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Hare TA, Malmaud J, Rangel A, Hare M. Focusing attention on the health aspects of foods changes value signals in vmPFC and improves dietary choice. Journal of Neuroscience. 2011a;31:11077–11087. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6383-10.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Hare TA, Schultz W, Camerer CF, O'Doherty JP, Rangel A. Transformation of stimulus value signals into motor commands during simple choice. PNAS. 2011;108:18120–18125. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1109322108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Haynes JD, Rees G. Decoding mental states from brain activity in humans. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2006;7:523–534. doi: 10.1038/nrn1931. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Haynes JD, Sakai K, Rees G, Gilbert S, Frith C, Passingham RE. Reading hidden intentions in the human brain. Current Biology. 2007;17:323–328. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2006.11.072. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Heinzle J, Anders S, Bode S, Bogler C, Chen Y, Cichy RM, Hackmack K, Kahnt T, Kalberlah C, Reverberi C, Soon CS, Tusche A, Weygandt M, Haynes J-D. Multivariate decoding of fMRI data. E-Neuroforum. 2012;18:1–16. doi: 10.1007/s13295-012-0026-9. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  27. Hollmann M, Hellrung L, Pleger B, Schlögl H, Kabisch S, Stumvoll M, Villringer A, Horstmann A. Neural correlates of the volitional regulation of the desire for food. International Journal of Obesity. 2012;36:648–655. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2011.125. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Hutcherson CA, Bushong B, Rangel A. A Neurocomputational Model of Altruistic Choice and Its Implications. Neuron. 2015b;87:451–462. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.06.031. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Hutcherson CA, Montaser-Kouhsari L, Woodward J, Rangel A. Emotional and Utilitarian Appraisals of Moral Dilemmas Are Encoded in Separate Areas and Integrated in Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortex. Journal of Neuroscience. 2015a;35:12593–12605. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3402-14.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Hutcherson CA, Plassmann H, Gross JJ, Rangel A, Gross H. Cognitive regulation during decision making shifts behavioral control between ventromedial and dorsolateral prefrontal value systems. Journal of Neuroscience. 2012;32:13543–13554. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6387-11.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Kable JW, Glimcher PW. The neural correlates of subjective value during intertemporal choice. Nature Neuroscience. 2007;10:1625–1633. doi: 10.1038/nn2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Kahnt T, Grueschow M, Speck O, Haynes JD. Perceptual learning and decision-making in human medial frontal cortex. Neuron. 2011;70:549–559. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.02.054. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Kahnt T, Park SQ, Haynes JD, Tobler PN. Disentangling neural representations of value and salience in the human brain. PNAS. 2014;111:5000–5005. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1320189111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Keeney RL, Raiffa H. Decisions with Multiple Objectives: Preferences and Value Trade-Offs. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press; 1993. [Google Scholar]
  35. Knutson B, Rick S, Wimmer GE, Prelec D, Loewenstein G. Neural predictors of purchases. Neuron. 2007;53:147–156. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.11.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Kober H, Mende-Siedlecki P, Kross EF, Weber J, Mischel W, Hart CL, Ochsner KN. Prefrontal-striatal pathway underlies cognitive regulation of craving. PNAS. 2010;107:14811–14816. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1007779107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Krajbich I, Armel C, Rangel A. Visual fixations and the computation and comparison of value in simple choice. Nature Neuroscience. 2010;13:1292–1298. doi: 10.1038/nn.2635. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Kriegeskorte N, Bandettini P. Analyzing for information, not activation, to exploit high-resolution fMRI. NeuroImage. 2007;38:649–662. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.02.022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Kriegeskorte N, Goebel R, Bandettini P. Information-based functional brain mapping. PNAS. 2006;103:3863–3868. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0600244103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Kriegeskorte N, Simmons WK, Bellgowan PS, Baker CI. Circular analysis in systems neuroscience: the dangers of double dipping. Nature Neuroscience. 2009;12:535–540. doi: 10.1038/nn.2303. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Lim SL, O'Doherty JP, Rangel A. The decision value computations in the vmPFC and striatum use a relative value code that is guided by visual attention. Journal of Neuroscience. 2011;31:13214–13223. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1246-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Loewenstein G, Small DA. The scarecrow and the tin man: the vicissitudes of human sympathy and caring. Review of General Psychology. 2007;11:112–126. doi: 10.1037/1089-2680.11.2.112. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  43. Magar ECE, Phillips LH, Hosie JA. Self-regulation and risk-taking. Personality and Individual Differences. 2008;45:153–159. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2008.03.014. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  44. Metcalfe J, Mischel W. A hot/cool-system analysis of delay of gratification: dynamics of willpower. Psychological Review. 1999;106:3. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.106.1.3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Plassmann H, O'Doherty J, Rangel A. Orbitofrontal cortex encodes willingness to pay in everyday economic transactions. Journal of Neuroscience. 2007;27:9984–9988. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2131-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Plassmann H, O'Doherty JP, Rangel A. Appetitive and aversive goal values are encoded in the medial orbitofrontal cortex at the time of decision making. Journal of Neuroscience. 2010;30:10799–10808. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0788-10.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Quirk GJ, Beer JS. Prefrontal involvement in the regulation of emotion: convergence of rat and human studies. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 2006;16:723–727. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2006.07.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Ratcliff R, McKoon G. The diffusion decision model: theory and data for two-choice decision tasks. Neural Computation. 2008;20:873–922. doi: 10.1162/neco.2008.12-06-420. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Rissman J, Gazzaley A, D'Esposito M. Measuring functional connectivity during distinct stages of a cognitive task. NeuroImage. 2004;23:752–763. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.06.035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Schmidt L, Tusche A, Manoharan N, Hutcherson C, Hare T, Plassmann H. Neuroanatomy of the vmPFC and dlPFC predicts individualdifferences in cognitive regulation during dietary self-control acrossregulation strategies. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2018 doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3402-17.2018. In press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Schoemaker PJH, Waid CC. An experimental comparison of different approaches to determining weights in additive utility models. Management Science. 1982;28:182–196. doi: 10.1287/mnsc.28.2.182. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  52. Schurz M, Radua J, Aichhorn M, Richlan F, Perner J. Fractionating theory of mind: a meta-analysis of functional brain imaging studies. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2014;42:9–34. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.01.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Shaw K, O’Rourke P, Del Mar C, Kenardy J. Psychological interventions for overweight or obesity. The Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews. 2005;2 doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD003818.pub2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Smith PL, Ratcliff R. Psychology and neurobiology of simple decisions. Trends in Neurosciences. 2004;27:161–168. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2004.01.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Sokol-Hessner P, Camerer CF, Phelps EA. Emotion regulation reduces loss aversion and decreases amygdala responses to losses. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 2013;8:341–350. doi: 10.1093/scan/nss002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Sullivan N, Hutcherson C, Harris A, Rangel A. Dietary self-control is related to the speed with which attributes of healthfulness and tastiness are processed. Psychological Science. 2015;26:122–134. doi: 10.1177/0956797614559543. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Sylvain C, Ladouceur R, Boisvert JM. Cognitive and behavioral treatment of pathological gambling: a controlled study. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1997;65:727–732. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.65.5.727. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Tusche A, Böckler A, Kanske P, Trautwein FM, Singer T. Decoding the charitable brain: empathy, perspective taking, and attention shifts differentially predict altruistic giving. Journal of Neuroscience. 2016;36:4719–4732. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3392-15.2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Van Overwalle F. Social cognition and the brain: a meta-analysis. Human Brain Mapping. 2009;30:829–858. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20547. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Wisniewski D, Reverberi C, Tusche A, Haynes JD. The neural representation of voluntary Task-Set selection in dynamic environments. Cerebral Cortex. 2015;25:4715–4726. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhu155. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Yokum S, Stice E. Cognitive regulation of food craving: effects of three cognitive reappraisal strategies on neural response to palatable foods. International Journal of Obesity. 2013;37:1565–1570. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2013.39. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision letter

Editor: Geoffrey Schoenbaum1

In the interests of transparency, eLife includes the editorial decision letter and accompanying author responses. A lightly edited version of the letter sent to the authors after peer review is shown, indicating the most substantive concerns; minor comments are not usually included.

Thank you for submitting your article "Computations for altered attribute representations underlie cognitive regulation in altruistic and healthy choices" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been favorably evaluated by Michael Frank (Senior Editor) and three reviewers, one of whom is a member of our Board of Reviewing Editors. The reviewers have opted to remain anonymous.

The reviewers have discussed the reviews with one another and the Reviewing Editor has drafted this decision to help you prepare a revised submission.

Summary:

In this study, the authors explore the ability of goals or attentional focus to modulate choices in a food task and in an altruism task. In the tasks, subjects chose or rejected different foods or different monetary offers. Instructions to choose healthy or to make generous choices modulated the behavior in both tasks and also affected neural activity patterns measured using fMRI. Regulatory success – or the ability to use instructions to change default choice behavior – was correlated with DLPFC activity.

Essential revisions:

One reviewer, not someone doing fMRI work, had significant difficulty simply following how the concepts of value versus regulation were operationalized in the task and analysis. What precisely were the conditions compared to isolate regulation for example from value? Describing this in more concrete terms would be helpful. This was related to the overarching concern of the other reviewers as well, which was the distinction between the authors conceptualization versus a more trivial interpretation – namely that when participants pay attention to a particular type of information (by instruction), this information is easier to decode from their brain activity. As noted below: "This effect should be seen as the outcome (and not the process) of cognitive regulation. How cognitive regulation is implemented, and how better decoding translates into biased choices, still need to be explained." Basically what is needed is to provide a more mechanistic account for what is going on.

Suggestions are given in reviews below. For example "it would be important to show that the decoded value (i.e., the decoder output) correlates with the behavioral weights. The alternative would be that changes in decoding accuracy correspond to changes in precision (i.e., signal-to-noise ratio) and not changes in the signal itself. If correct, this would mean that a region downstream to the DLPFC could just read this value, add it to other values corresponding to other attributes, and feed the aggregate value to a selection process that makes the decision. Perhaps functional connectivity could be used to test for such a transfer of information. Thus, the neural model would parallel the behavioral model."

Another suggestion was that some sort of functional connectivity analysis could clarify how DLPFC (and potentially rTPJ) enables successful regulation mechanistically, possibly using additional ROI analyses on VMPFC to ensure this is indeed the case.

These are the essentially revisions or problems identified in our discussion – clarify the approach and provide a more mechanistic account of the proposed interaction to rule out trivial explanations of the findings.

Reviewer #1:

I think the general question is of interest and the authors approach is quite novel to me. They are basically testing how changes in goals affect behavior. This is similar to the use of devaluation in animal learning theory tasks, but here they are using simple instructions in the course of training with humans. I think this is a creative way to integrate economic decision making, which focuses on a unitary utility as guiding choices, with work from experimental psychology and computational neuroscience, which typically distinguishes different sources of information by its associative basis or computational basis. This is excellent.

However, beyond that I had difficulty following the authors framing, predictions and understanding the outcomes of the neural activity analysis. The DLPFC is the key "regulatory" area. But what does that mean? It seems to me that I would expect some areas to represent value independent of goals and some areas to represent that value only when goal relevant. Is this what is meant by regulatory? Does the DLPFC represents value relevant to goals and some other area does not – VMPFC?

Generally I think the question is very interesting and the approach is attractive, but I simply could not follow how the authors framed and then conducted their analysis. I will be interested to see what the other reviewers say. I might grasp things better if it were more clear how the theoretical concepts were operationalized for the analyses – precisely what is "regulatory" for example and how is it distinguished from non-regulatory versus just not involved by the task and then in the data analysis.

Reviewer #2:

Tusche and Hutcherson present a thought-provoking and methodologically impressive study on cognitive regulation of dietary and altruistic choices, a topic of broad interest. The analyses, which combine a drift diffusion computational model of attribute-weighted choice and trial-by-trial MVPA decoding, are sophisticated, appropriate, and comprehensive. They show that while attribute values across choice and regulatory goal contexts can be decoded in VMPFC, they do not appear to be modulated by regulatory goal. Conversely, attribute values in DLPFC are modulated consistently with model-derived behavioral weights across regulatory goals that emphasize either healthiness or tastiness during dietary choice, and personal gain in altruistic choice contexts. Regulatory goals that emphasize another's benefits (e.g. feelings), however, could instead be decoded from right TPJ and precuneus, but not DLPFC, suggesting representations of others' wellbeing is modulated according to the prosocial regulatory goal but only when it requires theory of mind. These findings speak to both the domain-generality and domain-selectivity of cognitive regulation of decision making and importantly advance our understanding of the neural systems important for cognitive control and decision making more generally. In general I am supportive of this paper but think some outstanding issues could be better addressed to confirm some of their interpretations and rule out others.

It is notable that DLPFC flexibly encoded values of tastiness, healthiness, and $Self but not $Other or $Fairness on the one hand but strongly predicted altruistic choices on the other hand. Is the DLPFC prediction of altruistic choices then mediated especially by the change in $Self during altruistic choices, but not the change to $Other or $Fairness?

A related question: Do the rTPJ and precuneus group effects predict individual differences in regulatory success during altruistic choices when goals depended on another's thoughts or feelings? Such evidence would tie together their argument that these latter regions "assume responsibility" for regulatory success when DLPFC does not because of the component process required to meet that goal.

Given the prior literature on this topic, and in order to rule out a model whereby VMPFC value representations are modulated in an attribute-specific manner that depends on regulatory goals, it would be informative to see an ROI-based analysis of VMPFC. I may have missed something but I could not find one. The ROI from the conjunction presented in Figure 3 could be used or prior literature could be used.

One question which is unclear from the conjunction analyses is whether it is the same neural code (e.g. in DLPFC) that is used across task contexts, or whether the code is distinct (context-dependent) but found in the same brain region. What do the authors find if the SVR is trained on one attribute (e.g. tastiness) and tested on the other two (e.g. healthiness and $self)? Can the overlap between representations be better visualized?

It would be informative to visualize the feature weights for the key areas (e.g. VMPFC, DLPFC) across voxel space in every subject. This procedure should help to assess to what extent any decoding effects are due to hard anatomical boundaries between subareas (e.g. dorsal and ventral aspects of DLPFC) or to distributed patterns within areas.

How do the authors interpret the altered representations in some primary sensory and motor areas, e.g. less strong decoding of tastiness values for NC than HC and TC? Have the authors considered a model whereby coupling between DLPFC and distinct regions of sensory cortex is modulated according to the regulatory goal? The DLPFC must get its attribute representations from someplace.

The one comparison in which a match with the behavioral analyses failed to emerge was for ethical considerations compared to normal or personal considerations. I do not view this null finding as problematic, but out of curiosity do the authors have any thoughts as to what is going on when regulatory success depended on changes to fairness due to the goal of complying with social norms? This null finding should be addressed in the Discussion.

Reviewer #3:

In this manuscript, Tusche and Hutcherson report an fMRI study on how cognitive regulation affects the neural representation of choice-relevant attributes. They look for mechanisms that may generalize across two types of choices, one involving conflict between healthiness and tastiness of food items, the other involving conflict between self-interests and altruistic concerns. In different conditions, participants are asked to focus on one or the other attribute, which regulates the weights assigned to the targeted attributes, as shown via computational modeling of choice data. The key findings are the links between these changes in attribute weights and the decoding accuracy obtained for these attributes using multivariate pattern analysis (MVPA) in various cortical regions. The results are quite convincing, with successful decoding across tasks and individuals. There is no clear conclusion about whether the regulation is centralized or distributed though, since changes in decoding accuracy are observed in the DLPFC for most of the attributes but not all.

The role of cognitive control in economic choice is poorly understood and this study brings valuable insights by applying MVPA to standard choice paradigms. My main concern is the absence of a mechanistic account linking brain activity to behavioral output. In a sense, the results seem a bit trivial: when participants pay attention to a particular type of information (by instruction), this information is easier to decode from their brain activity. This effect should be seen as the outcome (and not the process) of cognitive regulation. How cognitive regulation is implemented, and how better decoding translates into biased choices, still need to be explained.

For the latter point, it would be important to show that the decoded value (i.e., the decoder output) correlates with the behavioral weights. The alternative would be that changes in decoding accuracy correspond to changes in precision (i.e., signal-to-noise ratio) and not changes in the signal itself. If correct, this would mean that a region downstream to the DLPFC could just read this value, add it to other values corresponding to other attributes, and feed the aggregate value to a selection process that makes the decision. Perhaps functional connectivity could be used to test for such a transfer of information. Thus, the neural model would parallel the behavioral model.

Other points:

- The correlation across individuals could reflect compliance to the instructions rather than self-regulation capacity. The arguments taken from subjective report and from body-mass index are quite weak. For subjective report it could be that the rating scale is not reflecting the propensity to comply with the instructions. For body-mass index the opposite correlation could be expected: those who regulates food intake in real life should not need instructions in the lab.

- The observation that all attributes are represented in the VMPFC but inaccessible to cognitive regulation is super interesting (and novel, to my knowledge). The dissociation with DLPFC should be more emphasized and discussed. Would this mean that VMPFC representations are closer to stimuli and DLPFC to responses?

- To compare the pattern of attribute weights and the pattern of decoding accuracy across conditions, the authors intend to reproduce significance of pair-wise comparisons. As they know this approach heavily depends on the statistical threshold, which may be matter of debate when multiple comparisons are made. I would favor a straight regression of decoding accuracy against weight (across conditions).

eLife. 2018 May 29;7:e31185. doi: 10.7554/eLife.31185.030

Author response


Essential revisions:

One reviewer, not someone doing fMRI work, had significant difficulty simply following how the concepts of value versus regulation were operationalized in the task and analysis. What precisely were the conditions compared to isolate regulation for example from value? Describing this in more concrete terms would be helpful.

We thank the editor for the opportunity to resubmit our paper, and for the thoughtful comments and suggestions. We agree with reviewer 1 that we were not as clear as we could have been in the previous instantiation of the manuscript about the focus of our paper, and on the distinctions between value and regulation. We have substantially revised the Introduction (as described in greater detail below, see our first response to reviewer 1) to make this clearer. In brief, we have more clearly specified that we are focused on disentangling different hypotheses about what stage of the processing stream regulation acts on to modify value computations and have explicitly spelled out different possibilities along with the predictions they make at the neural level. Moreover, we have substantially revised the Results section of the manuscript to orient the reader to how different analyses address these different questions. Finally, we added a figure to illustrate the decoding analysis that aimed to identify brain regions that encode value in a goal-dependent manner (Figure 4) as well as a panel in Figure 1 to illustrate the behavioral computational modeling approach (Figure 1D). We feel that these revisions have significantly strengthened the clarity of the manuscript.

This was related to the overarching concern of the other reviewers as well, which was the distinction between the authors conceptualization versus a more trivial interpretation – namely that when participants pay attention to a particular type of information (by instruction), this information is easier to decode from their brain activity. As noted below: "This effect should be seen as the outcome (and not the process) of cognitive regulation. How cognitive regulation is implemented, and how better decoding translates into biased choices, still need to be explained. " Basically what is needed is to provide a more mechanistic account for what is going on.

Regarding the first point, we agree that in some sense it should be “trivial” to observe changes in information decoded from brain activity that matches changes at the behavioral level. Nevertheless, as we make clear in the revised Introduction and Discussion sections, extant research has actually not consistently found such patterns despite clear changes in behavior, raising an important puzzle to be explained. Our results are the first to identify a clear representation of all choice-relevant attributes across multiple contexts, and to show clear modulation of these choice-relevant attributes in a manner consistent with regulatory goals.

We also think our specific pattern of results clearly speak against the trivial interpretation that one would have to observe changes in attribute coding in the brain, and that this is an uninteresting reflection of changes in behavior that gives no insight into mechanism. We observe a highly specific pattern of altered and unaltered representations of attribute values, which reveals important dissociations across different kinds of attributes and different individuals. This is anything but trivial, and, we believe opens up crucially important lines of questioning that will be the basis for a better understanding of cognitive regulation and its mechanistic implementation. We have substantially revised the Introduction and Discussion to highlight these important points (see our specific responses to reviewers and our next response below).

Regarding the second point (that this should be seen as the outcome and not the process, and that this does not reveal mechanism), we respectfully disagree that no information about mechanism can be derived from a detailed description of where such changes are observed. We have tried to make this clearer in the following ways:

1) We have now further clarified in the new version of the paper in the Introduction, Results, and Discussion how identifying where attribute encoding changes occur may allow us to distinguish between different theories about computational process and mechanisms. Specifically, we make clearer that regulation could occur via two mechanisms: modulation at the level of specific attribute representations in non-overlapping, attribute-specific regions, or at the level of weighting in the value integration process.

2) We also make clearer that there are important outstanding questions about the domain-generality or domain-specificity of regulation effects. On the one hand, if different attributes are computed in different regions with different computational properties, regulation could operate through different channels in different domains. If, instead, regulation operates at a more domain-general level, then regulation might influence attribute representations in a common region across multiple different attributes and multiple choice contexts. Understanding whether regulation operates in a domain-general or domain-specific way can help reveal both the targets of regulation, as well as the mechanism of action.

3) In the Discussion, we highlight how these questions are not of purely theoretical importance: the locus of action (attribute level or integration level, domain specific or domain general) predicts non-trivial differences in patterns of regulatory success, which we observe in our own data. For attributes where regulation influences representation in a common region (i.e., DLPFC) regulatory success tracks together, but for attributes represented elsewhere (i.e., $Other in TPJ and precuneus), regulatory success appears to operate independently. Our results also may help to explain when and why regulatory success might feel difficult: if, as we observe, there are many regions of the brain where attribute representations are not altered by regulation, then these altered representations may continue to leak into the choice process and interfere with regulatory success.

4) We have also included several supplementary analyses, including functional connectivity results, which shed some light on mechanistic processes.

For these reasons, we believe this paper will be of considerable interest to the readership of eLife, and have tried to articulate this reasoning better in the paper. We hope the reviewers and editor will agree.

Suggestions are given in reviews below. For example "it would be important to show that the decoded value (i.e., the decoder output) correlates with the behavioral weights. The alternative would be that changes in decoding accuracy correspond to changes in precision (i.e., signal-to-noise ratio) and not changes in the signal itself. If correct, this would mean that a region downstream to the DLPFC could just read this value, add it to other values corresponding to other attributes, and feed the aggregate value to a selection process that makes the decision. Perhaps functional connectivity could be used to test for such a transfer of information. Thus, the neural model would parallel the behavioral model."

Another suggestion was that some sort of functional connectivity analysis could clarify how DLPFC (and potentially rTPJ) enables successful regulation mechanistically, possibly using additional ROI analyses on VMPFC to ensure this is indeed the case.

We agree with the editor and reviewer that each of these issues is incredibly important, and have sought to address them in a series of supplementary analyses.

1) First, we used regression of decoding accuracies against behavioral weights (as suggested by reviewer 3). We show that results corroborate findings from the ANOVAs and paint essentially the same picture in our regions of interest. Specifically, we find goal-consistent coding of attributes that match predictions of the behavioral model. We therefore believe that these results are not simply a reflection of changes in signal-to-noise. However, we also believe that the ANOVAs actually give us greater power to detect effects, particularly in the altruistic choice task, where it seems clear that our behavioral weights (particularly for a construct like other’s welfare) reflect different considerations in the different regulatory focus conditions. These ANOVAs reveal, for instance, that TPJ and Precuneus seem to code for others’ welfare when focused on partner feelings, but not when focused on ethical reasons for giving, despite increased behavioral weighting on $Other in both. This is an important result in its own right, and sheds light on the computational function of these regions. This result would not have been clear using the regression analysis proposed by the reviewer. We have thus opted to retain the original analyses, but provide a detailed description of the regression analyses in our point-by-point response, to corroborate and extend the conclusions that we come to using the ANOVAs. We opted for this course to maintain a clear and streamlined set of results, and to avoid over-taxing the reader’s working memory, since the paper is already rich in terms of methodological approaches and results. If the editor or reviewers deem it appropriate and necessary, we can of course include the regression analyses in the main body of the paper.

2) Second, we have now performed a detailed set of functional connectivity analyses to test for different theories about the role of information transfer in producing the observed pattern of results. Although we believe that functional connectivity analyses are somewhat complicated to interpret in the context of multivariate rather than univariate analysis (see our more detailed response to reviewer 3), we have also taken to heart the reviewers’ suggestions to dig deeper into our data. Thus, we have now conducted exploratory functional connectivity analyses to test whether and how changes in patterns of functional connectivity between regions are linked to regulatory success. To do this, we used a beta series approach (Rissman et al., 2004), which research suggests may be more sensitive to modulation of functional connectivity by context in studies like ours that use an event-related design with many trial repetitions (Cisler et al., 2014).

In brief, we extracted trial-by-trial fluctuations of BOLD response in regions of interest (i.e., DLPFC, precuneus, TPJ, and VMPFC) and then looked for areas of the brain where correlation with this trial-by-trial variation (after controlling for whole-brain response) varied as a function of regulatory goal. We looked both for main effects at the group level, as well as effects that correlated with regulatory success (e.g. identifying functional connectivity differences in HC vs. NC that correlated with change in behavioral weights of the healthiness attribute in HC vs. NC). At the suggestion of reviewer 2, we also specifically focused on two possibilities in terms of altered functional connectivity. First we tested the hypothesis that DLPFC, TPJ, and/or precuneus show altered connectivity with visual and/or or motor areas, and that this explains observed differences in regulation success. Second, we tested the hypothesis that DLPFC, TPJ and/or precuneus show altered connectivity with the VMPFC.

Our results most clearly support the latter hypothesis. At admittedly-somewhat-liberal thresholds, we find hints that connectivity patterns between VMPFC and DLPFC as well as between VMPFC and Precuneus correlate with changes in behavioral weighting of choice attributes in a manner that is consistent with predictions of our behavioral computational model. Using the DLPFC as a seed region, we find that increased connectivity with VMPFC correlates with greater weighting of the healthiness attribute when focused on health during food choice, and lower weighting of the self outcomes when focused on pro-social motives in the altruism task. Using the precuneus as a seed region, we find that increased connectivity with the VMPFC correlates with greater weighting of partner outcomes in the contrast of Partner vs. Ethics focus (which is also the contrast that most clearly differentiates encoded neural information on partner outcomes ($Other) in precuneus). A similar analysis using the VMPFC as a seed region revealed similar results.

What might these results mean in terms of mechanism? Despite the functional coupling with the VMPFC, representations of these attributes values within the VMPFC did not change (as confirmed by three supplemental analyses, see our fourth response to reviewer 2). This finding suggests that altered functional connectivity did not result in altered goal-consistent attribute representations in the VMPFC. Instead, our results raise an intriguing (albeit speculative) possibility as an alternative mechanism: signals from the VMPFC may actually be used to alter computations within DLPFC and Precuneus to amplify or diminish attribute values computed in these areas. Although this is not the modal view of the function of the VMPFC, it is actually consistent with some early work on emotional conflict resolution suggesting that the VMPFC may be involved in modulating affective representations in lower-level areas such as the amygdala in a goal-consistent way (Etkin et al., 2006; Quirk and Beer, 2006).

Because these results were observed only at relatively liberal thresholds, we have opted not to make them a primary focus of the main paper. However, we have included them in the Appendix and refer the interested reader to them in the appropriate portions of the manuscript. We hope that this approach balances our twin goals of keeping the main paper streamlined and informative, while also providing full disclosure about analyses that may be of interest.

These are the essentially revisions or problems identified in our discussion – clarify the approach and provide a more mechanistic account of the proposed interaction to rule out trivial explanations of the findings.

As outlined above, we have thoroughly revised the manuscript to further clarify our approach. We have also added several new analyses to provide a more mechanistic account of the proposed interaction of identified brain regions, and to rule out other explanations of the findings. We believe that these revisions have substantially improved the manuscript and we truly appreciate the editors’ and reviewers’ constructive and thoughtful suggestions. Please find below the point-by-point reply to each comment of the reviewers.

Reviewer #1:

I think the general question is of interest and the authors approach is quite novel to me. They are basically testing how changes in goals affect behavior. This is similar to the use of devaluation in animal learning theory tasks, but here they are using simple instructions in the course of training with humans. I think this is a creative way to integrate economic decision making, which focuses on a unitary utility as guiding choices, with work from experimental psychology and computational neuroscience, which typically distinguishes different sources of information by its associative basis or computational basis. This is excellent.

However, beyond that I had difficulty following the authors framing, predictions and understanding the outcomes of the neural activity analysis. The DLPFC is the key "regulatory" area. But what does that mean? It seems to me that I would expect some areas to represent value independent of goals and some areas to represent that value only when goal relevant. Is this what is meant by regulatory? Does the DLPFC represents value relevant to goals and some other area does not – VMPFC?

We thank the reviewer for his/her positive comments and enthusiasm for the approach! We apologize for the lack of clarity in the previous version of the manuscript and appreciate that the reviewer brought this to our attention. In order to fully address the comments, we have broken down our responses into three separate but related issues: framing, predictions, and understanding of outcomes.

A) Framing.

Our usage of the term “regulatory” in the original version of our manuscript needed further clarification. In any study of cognitive regulation (e.g. in the context of research on emotion, or decision making, or perceptual tasks), it is important to distinguish between an effector and an effect of regulation. The former refers to the causal driver of changes, while the latter is in essence the result of regulatory efforts. Revealing the neural effectors and effects of regulation are related but separate goals in neuroeconomics. Our analyses were designed specifically to focus on the latter: how do value representations (either at the level of individual choice-relevant attributes or at the level of integrated decision values) change as a function of regulatory goals?

We have substantially revised and reframed the introduction to the paper to clarify this point and to highlight what we might learn by understanding how regulation changes value representation. More specifically, we now spell out in greater detail two different hypotheses about the effects of regulation:

The attribute-level hypothesis suggests that regulatory goals operate by altering computations in brain areas specialized for processing specific attributes. For example in the food task, there might be specific regions that represent the current value of a food’s tastiness and other regions that represent the value of its healthiness. If regulation operates at this attribute level, then each of these specific regions would be independently modulated depending on goals. This view suggests that we should see alterations in attribute-specific areas. On the other hand, the integration-level hypothesis suggests that regulatory goals operate by altering weighting of those attributes only in an area (or areas) representing decision values. If regulation operates only at the integration-level, then goal-consistent alterations in attribute representation should be observed only within centralized, domain-general regions associated with value integration and decision value computations (i.e., VMPFC). This hypothesis thus predicts that attribute-specific areas should represent attributes regardless of regulation, while value integration regions might shift to flexibly represent attribute in a goal-consistent manner.

We have now worked to re-frame the Introduction, focusing on this distinction, as well as two corollary questions: 1) Does the level at which regulation operates (i.e., attribute-or integration-level) depend on the specific attribute and/or domain? 2) What role do the VMPFC and DLPFC play in these processes? We believe that these revisions have substantially improved the clarity and focus of the manuscript, as well as highlighting its major contribution to the literature on regulation.

The relevant section of the revised manuscript now reads:

“A key goal of neuroeconomics is to describe how these attribute and decision value computations change as a function of regulatory goals and contexts, and to link such changes to regulatory success. Here, we sought to address three important questions about this process […] However, several failures to observe changes in the VMPFC during cognitive regulation of decision making (Hollmann et al., 2012; Hutcherson et al., 2012; Yokum and Stice, 2013) suggest the need to either measure value computation in a more sensitive way, or to identify alternate routes to behavioral change.”

We have also revised the title of the manuscript and the Abstract to further clarify that the study is interested in the effect of cognitive regulation on value representations.

The revised title is:

“Cognitive regulation alters social and dietary choice by changing attribute representations in domain-general and domain-specific brain circuits”

B) Predictions.

We have substantially revised the manuscript to better link the analyses and our hypotheses. Both the attribute-level and integration-level theories predict that goal-dependent value signals will be represented more strongly when participants’ regulatory goals emphasize those attributes. The critical question is where this modulation is observed, as this allows us to distinguish between the two hypotheses outlined above. We have added three overview paragraphs to the revised Results section that emphasize the overall aim of the subsequent set of analyses and the predictions more clearly.

“Behavior

To identify how value computations change to accommodate regulatory goals, our analysis strategy proceeded in the several steps. […] We also used our computational behavioral models (multi-attribute drift diffusion models, DDMs) to link these alterations to amplification or suppression of the influence of specific choice-relevant attributes on choices.”

“Neural encoding of choice attributes and effects of regulation

Next, we examined neural underpinnings of goal-consistent increases/decreases in the influence of attributes on altered choices in both tasks. […] We also explicitly tested whether the locus of effect differed across attributes (e.g. tastiness/healthiness, $Self/$Other/Fairness) or choice domain (e.g. social, non-social).”

“Individual differences in regulatory success

Are some people generally more successful using cognitive regulation of decision making or does it depend on the choice domain? […] It should also be predicted by neural activation in distinct, non-overlapping brain regions.”

C) Understanding of Outcomes.

We hope that the greater clarity of our framing and predictions now make clearer the implications of our results for understanding how regulation affects value representations at a mechanistic level. We have substantially revised the Discussion section to address the three questions we raise in the Introduction: 1) does cognitive regulation of decision making operate by changing attribute-level or integration-level representations? 2) Does the level at which it operates vary by domain? 3) What role do VMPFC and DLPFC play in this process?

Generally I think the question is very interesting and the approach is attractive, but I simply could not follow how the authors framed and then conducted their analysis. I will be interested to see what the other reviewers say. I might grasp things better if it were more clear how the theoretical concepts were operationalized for the analyses – precisely what is "regulatory" for example and how is it distinguished from non-regulatory versus just not involved by the task and then in the data analysis.

Again, we thank the reviewer for the positive assessment of the importance and interest value of this question. Please see our second response (above) for a more detailed discussion of the ways in which we have changed the framing, as well as discussion of the hypotheses and results, to make it clearer what we predicted and what we found, as well as how this relates questions of regulatory vs. non-regulatory regions.

We also thank the reviewer for bringing to our attention the lack of clarity in the description of our analyses (as did reviewer #2). In addition to revisions of the Results section, we have now also included a figure that further illustrates the key aspects of the multi-voxel decoding approach that we used to identify brain regions that differentially encode attribute values (or decision values) across goals/task conditions (Figure 4). We hope that this will make it easier for the broad and heterogeneous readership of eLife to follow the logic of this central analysis of our paper.

Reviewer #2:

Tusche and Hutcherson present a thought-provoking and methodologically impressive study on cognitive regulation of dietary and altruistic choices, a topic of broad interest. The analyses, which combine a drift diffusion computational model of attribute-weighted choice and trial-by-trial MVPA decoding, are sophisticated, appropriate, and comprehensive. They show that while attribute values across choice and regulatory goal contexts can be decoded in VMPFC, they do not appear to be modulated by regulatory goal. Conversely, attribute values in DLPFC are modulated consistently with model-derived behavioral weights across regulatory goals that emphasize either healthiness or tastiness during dietary choice, and personal gain in altruistic choice contexts. Regulatory goals that emphasize another's benefits (e.g. feelings), however, could instead be decoded from right TPJ and precuneus, but not DLPFC, suggesting representations of others' wellbeing is modulated according to the prosocial regulatory goal but only when it requires theory of mind. These findings speak to both the domain-generality and domain-selectivity of cognitive regulation of decision making and importantly advance our understanding of the neural systems important for cognitive control and decision making more generally. In general I am supportive of this paper but think some outstanding issues could be better addressed to confirm some of their interpretations and rule out others.

We thank the reviewer for this positive evaluation of our study and the approach, as well as for the constructive comments and suggestions! We have performed several supplemental analyses in response to his/her suggestions, which have significantly strengthened the manuscript and provided further support for our interpretations. We detail these analyses below in our response to the reviewer’s specific comments.

It is notable that DLPFC flexibly encoded values of tastiness, healthiness, and $Self but not $Other or $Fairness on the one hand but strongly predicted altruistic choices on the other hand. Is the DLPFC prediction of altruistic choices then mediated especially by the change in $Self during altruistic choices, but not the change to $Other or $Fairness?

The reviewer raises an interesting question! We addressed this idea in the following way. First, at the behavioral level, we regressed changes in generosity (ΔGenerous Choices [(PC, EC) - NC]) on changes in model-based weights for subject’s own benefits (Δw $Self [NC - (EC, PC)]), and calculated the residuals. This gave us a measure of change in generosity after controlling for change in weight on $Self. Next, we repeated the cross‐subject decoding analysis (SVR) using neural response patterns in the DLPFC as features and the estimated residuals as labels for the prediction. This tells us whether the DLPFC continues to predict goal-consistent increases in generosity after controlling for change in the input of $Self on choices. Our results are consistent with the idea that the DLPFC predicts change in generosity because it encodes changes in weight on $Self: activation patterns in the DLPFC no longer predicted individuals’ goal-consistent changes in generosity when we controlled for changes in the weight of self-related benefits on choices (r=0.11, p=0.317, permutation test, [CI: -0.41, 0.41]). We next sought to show that this effect is specific to changes in self-related considerations. To do this, we repeated this supplemental analysis for each of the other choice attributes (i.e., regressing out change in weight on $Other or change in weight on Fairness, and then asking whether DLPFC predicts residual changes in generosity). We found that DLPFC prediction of altered generosity remained highly significant when we controlled for altered inputs of other’s benefits (r=0.51, p=0.016, [CI -0.41, 0.40]) or fairness considerations (r= 0.56, p=0.007, [CI -0.39, 0.39]) on choices. Taken together, these findings indicate that predictions of goal-consistent changes in generosity in the DLPFC are mediated specifically by altered weighs of subjects’ own monetary benefits on choices (Δw $Self). This finding is also consistent with evidence of a specific functional role of the DLPFC for altered representations for self-related considerations, while changes in attributes of $Other and Fairness are encoded elsewhere.

We now refer to this supplemental evidence in the Results section of the manuscript:

“Supplemental analyses suggest that predictive information on altered generosity was driven by neural information on changes in the attribute encoded in the DLPFC ($Self) and not by other attributes of the altruistic choice task (e.g., $Other, fairness) (see Appendix 1 – 1.5).”

We also present the details of these supplemental analyses in the Appendix. Due to the close match of this section with our description above, we refrain from re-iterating the details here and refer reviewer #2 to the relevant section of the supplemental material:

Appendix 1 – 1.5. DLPFC-based prediction of goal-consistent changes of generosity is driven by goal-consistent changes in attribute representations of $Self (but not $Other or Fairness).

A related question: Do the rTPJ and precuneus group effects predict individual differences in regulatory success during altruistic choices when goals depended on another's thoughts or feelings? Such evidence would tie together their argument that these latter regions "assume responsibility" for regulatory success when DLPFC does not because of the component process required to meet that goal.

We thank the reviewer for this suggestion. To address it, we ran supplemental decoding analyses to explicitly test if neural response patterns in the Precuneus and/or rTPJ predict individual differences in regulatory success in the altruism task. The analyses closely matched the prediction of regulatory success based on neural responses in the DLPFC with the following differences: First, ROIs were based on clusters in Precuneus and rTPJ in which information about other’s benefits ($Other) varied depending on the behavioral goal (for details see Table 4, for illustration see Figure 2H and Figure 2—figure supplement 1). Second, neural activation patterns for this prediction were extracted from subject-specific contrast images in GLM2 (altruism task) based on the contrast image of [PC > EC], since this was the contrast that yielded reliable differences in attribute representations for both ROIs (see Author response image 1 for illustration).

Author response image 1. Multi-voxel response patterns in the precuneus reliably predicted individuals’ altered generosity in the altruism task (ΔGenerous Choices [PC – EC]: r = 0.57, p = 0.002 [CI: -0.41, 0.38]; ΔGenerous Choices [(NC, PC) – EC]: r = 0.61, p = 0.004 [CI: -0.41, 0.41]).

Author response image 1.

This finding is consistent with the pattern of results on goal-dependent encoding of others benefits in this brain region (left panel). Response patterns in the rTPJ, on the other hand, did not significantly predict individual’s goal-dependent changes in generosity (all p’s ≥ 0.065).

This latter null result has to be interpreted with some caution however. Depending on details of the ROI selection and parameters of the decoding approach, we did observe significant predictions of regulatory success based on rTPJ responses. However, given the variability in these results, the lack of an independent replication sample to further validate the modulated decoding approach, and the complexity of the manuscript, we prefer not to expand on this TPJ finding in the manuscript. Should the reviewer or the editor feel that this complementary evidence is necessary to further validate our interpretation, we are happy to provide details of this complementary analysis that hints at prediction of regulatory success based on response patterns in the rTPJ.

Overall, these findings are in line with our interpretation that other regions such as the Precuneus might ‘assume responsibility’ for goal-consistent regulatory success when the DLPFC does not. We now refer to this supplemental evidence in the Results section of the revised manuscript.

Specifically we now report:

“Precuneus encodes individual differences in regulatory success in altruistic choice.

Strikingly, patterns in the DLPFC did not decode regulatory success for social attributes that were flexibly encoded in other regions of the brain (i.e., $Other, Fairness). […] We found that response patterns in the precuneus reliably predicted individuals’ altered generosity in the altruism task (ΔGenerous Choices [PC - EC]: r = 0.57, p = 0.002 [CI: -0.41, 0.38]; ΔGenerous Choices [(NC, PC) - EC]: r = 0.61, p = 0.004 [CI: -0.41, 0.41]), suggesting that domain-specific attribute coding contributes to individual differences in regulatory control.”

Given the prior literature on this topic, and in order to rule out a model whereby VMPFC value representations are modulated in an attribute-specific manner that depends on regulatory goals, it would be informative to see an ROI-based analysis of VMPFC. I may have missed something but I could not find one. The ROI from the conjunction presented in Figure 3 could be used or prior literature could be used.

Following the reviewers’ suggestion, we used an ROI-based approach to further examine if neural response patterns in the VMPFC encoded goal-consistent change in attribute representations. The ROI consisted of voxels in the VMPFC identified in the conjunction analyses (see Figure 3 for illustration, upper panel in Author response image 2). We tested this question in three different sets of analyses:

Author response image 2. The figure illustrates two VMPFC-ROIs used for the supplemental analyses.

Author response image 2.

1) For the first approach, we examined average predictive information in the VMPFC-ROI identified by the whole brain searchlight decoding analyses described in the paper: For each participant and each condition, we extracted decoding accuracies for each voxel in the VMPFC-ROI and estimated an ROI-based average for each choice-attribute. Next, for each attribute, we ran a repeated measures ANOVA to explicitly test for altered information content. Confirming results from whole-brain analyses, information about the values of choice attributes (as measured by decoding accuracies) was not significantly modulated by the regulatory goal (all p’s > 0.13, uncorrected). This finding is in line with our whole-brain results (implemented in SPM), and suggests that the lack of results was not due to overly stringent statistical thresholds. This provides further support for our interpretation that value representations in the VMPFC on the attribute level are unaffected at a group level by our implemented regulatory goals.

2) However, this analysis leaves open the possibility that local response patterns in the VMPFC (rather than average decoding accuracies) might differentiate attributes by regulatory goal. Thus, we ran supplemental ROI-based within‐subject decoding analyses to predict trial-wise values of an attribute based on local response patterns in the VMPFC. These analyses were similar to the searchlight decoding approach with the difference that ROI-wise response patterns in VMPFC served as neural features for the predictions. Thus, instead of extracting multi-voxel patterns from a sphere around individual voxels in the ROI, we use one response pattern consisting of all voxels of the ROI to define neural response patterns. For each attribute separately, we estimated how well a subject’s multi-voxel response patterns encoded attribute values in a particular task condition. Next, on a group level, we used repeated measures ANOVA to examine whether neural information on an attribute varied depending on the behavioral goal. Consistent with the searchlight decoding approach, predictive information encoded in the VMPFC did not significantly vary cross task conditions (all p’s > 0.29, uncorrected).

3) Finally, we examined if response patterns in the VMPFC encoded goal-dependent changes across participants. Such a pattern might suggest that even though we did not observe main effects, VMPFC attribute representations still contribute in some way to regulatory success. Thus, in addition to the within-subject analyses described above (1-2), we also ran cross-subject decoding analyses. These analyses were identical to the decoding analyses of individual regulatory success from neural response patterns of the DLPFC, with the exception that response patterns were extracted from the VMPFC cluster identified in the conjunction analysis. Consistent with the lack of main effects for the supplemental analyses reported above, cross-subject decoding analyses did not yield significant predictions of participants’ regulatory success for any attribute or for choice behavior (i.e. healthy or generous choices) in the food task or altruism task (all p’s > 0.31, uncorrected).

Finally, to address potential concerns that the selected ROI might have been too small for the multi-voxel analyses approach, we also repeated these analyses (1-3) using a larger VMPFC‐ROI (see Author response image 2, lower panel). The original conjunction area in the VMPFC (upper panel) represents only the central voxels of the searchlight spheres that encoded all attribute values in the attribute-specific searchlight decoding analyses. The lower panel illustrates the VMPFC-ROI based on all voxels within spheres that predicted all attribute values. Results of the analyses (1-3) matched those for the original VMPFC ROI, suggesting that null-findings in supplemental analyses are not merely driven by the comparatively small number of voxels identified in the conjunction analyses (Tastiness, Healthiness, $Self, $ Other, Fairness).

Taken together, these more sensitive and fine-grained ROI analyses confirm our conclusion that, in this study, our VMPFC ROI encodes the values of each of the individual attributes, but not in a manner that is sensitive to regulatory goals. We have highlighted these additional analyses briefly in the Results section of the main paper, and expanded on them in the Appendix.

The relevant paragraphs in the revised Results section now reads:

“No evidence for goal-dependent coding of attribute values and decision values in the VMPFC. […] While activation patterns in the VMPFC (as well as several other regions) reliably predicted overall decision values in both tasks, regulation failed to modulate decoding accuracies for decision value (Supplementary file 2) or for any specific attribute (Appendix 1 – 1.3), and did not predict individual differences in regulatory success (Appendix 1 – 1.3).”

“VMPFC does not encode individual differences in regulatory success.

Because of its hypothesized role in valuation, a post-hoc analyses also examined whether the VMPFC region that encoded all attributes predicted regulatory success in either choice task. […] However, exploratory functional connectivity analyses provided subtle hints that the VMPFC could be indirectly related to regulatory success through its modulation of both DLPFC and precuneus (see Figure 3—figure supplement 2 and Appendix 1 – 1.6 for details).”

For detailed descriptions (matching those presented above) we refer reviewer #2 to the respective section of the Appendix:

“Appendix 1 – 1.3 ROI-based post-hoc tests to identify goal-consistent value coding in the VMPFC.”

One question which is unclear from the conjunction analyses is whether it is the same neural code (e.g. in DLPFC) that is used across task contexts, or whether the code is distinct (context-dependent) but found in the same brain region. What do the authors find if the SVR is trained on one attribute (e.g. tastiness) and tested on the other two (e.g. healthiness and $self)?

The question of how the DLPFC encodes different attributes is extremely interesting! The reviewer is also correct that the main analyses of the manuscript do not address this issue. We therefore ran a supplemental decoding analysis to explicitly test if neural codes in the DLPFC are similar or distinct across contexts, attributes, and tasks. More precisely, we tested if a decoding model trained on neural data in one context (or attribute, or task) allows decoding the attribute values in a different context (or attribute, or task) based on neural data observed in these trials. Significant decoding would suggest that the neural code in one context is reinstated (at least in part) in the different context.

To do this, we used a post-hoc ROI-based approach using voxels in the DLPFC (Figure 5A) together with a within‐subject decoding approach (as described in the manuscript). However, instead of a leave-one-run-out cross-validation, we now trained the SVR model on data of one condition (e.g. Tastiness-HC) and tested it on data of another condition (e.g. Tastiness-TC). The procedure was then repeated, reversing training and testing data (e.g. train on Tastiness-TC, test on Tastiness‐HC). Decoding accuracies were estimated by averaging predictions across this 2-fold cross-validation. As a sanity check, we also ran the analysis for data within a condition (e.g. train on 1st half of data in Tastiness-HC, test on 2nd half of data in Tastiness-HC, and vice versa). Statistical significance was assessed at the group level using permutation tests in which we examined how likely observed decoding accuracies were compared to an empirical null-distribution of these predictions. Null-distributions were estimated by breaking up the matching of trial-wise neural activation patterns and attribute values (1000 folds). Upper cutoff-values for significance were the 95th percentile of null-distributions.

Our results suggest that the DLPFC maintains distinct neural codes for different attributes, but that such codes may be similar across different contexts. Figure 5B illustrates the results of this analyses for attributes encoded in the DLPFC (Tastiness, Healthiness, $Self).

First, we examined whether values of a particular attribute (e.g. Tastiness) relied on common neural codes across contexts/regulatory conditions (within-attribute decoding, yellow frames). For each attribute, we found evidence in favor of common coding of attribute values across contexts (illustrated in red, signifying significant cross-condition classification, p < 0.05 in stringent permutation tests). For example, similar neural codes seem to represent values of Tastiness in TC and HC. However, a lack of significant cross-condition classification in 2 of the 18 tests (e.g. Tastiness in NC and HC) might also point to some degree of context-dependent differences in activation patterns for attribute values, or to the much weaker representation of some attributes in some conditions.

Second, and more importantly, the analyses enabled us to explicitly test whether neural codes in the DLPFC generalize across attributes (cross-attribute decoding). Response patterns that coded for values of one attribute did not allow predicting values of another attribute (illustrated in blue). This was found to hold true across regulatory conditions. This finding suggests that distinct neural codes within the DLPFC are used to encode values of individual attributes in both choice tasks (Tastiness, Healthiness, and $Self).

We now report these findings in the revised Results sections of the manuscript:

“This finding suggests that the DLPFC acts as a domain-general circuit for goal-sensitive value representations. But what does this convergence in the DLPFC signify? […] This supports the idea that the DLPFC acts as a domain-general mechanism for representing different attributes in a goal-sensitive manner, using unique codes for each attribute.”

We also refer to these findings in the revised Discussion section of the manuscript:

“Third, although we observed converging effects of regulation for a subset of attributes in the DLPFC (including tastiness, healthiness, and self-related benefits), representations of these attributes utilized distinct, differentiated codes. Taken together, although our results do not preclude the possibility that in other contexts cognitive regulation of decision making might operate on a single, centralized value integration mechanism, they suggest that it may often operate by changing distinct attribute representations.”

We believe that this set of supplemental analyses has significantly strengthened the manuscript and has shed further light on the neural computations in the DLPFC that underlie cognitive regulation of value-based choice.

Can the overlap between representations be better visualized?

It would be informative to visualize the feature weights for the key areas (e.g. VMPFC, DLPFC) across voxel space in every subject. This procedure should help to assess to what extent any decoding effects are due to hard anatomical boundaries between subareas (e.g. dorsal and ventral aspects of DLPFC) or to distributed patterns within areas.

The reviewer makes an interesting (but challenging!) suggestion. Using a post-hoc ROI approach based on voxels in the DLPFC (Figure 5B), we re-ran the within-subject decoding analyses of values of tastiness, healthiness, and $Self (separately for each condition). We then extracted feature weights of the prediction for each of the attributes encoded in the DLPFC, subject, and condition and projected them back into MNI space (3 attributes x 36 subjects x 3 conditions = 324 images). Visual inspection of feature weights did not reveal systematic clustering of feature weights across voxel space or hard anatomical boundaries. Because of the large amount of data, we have opted for now to leave out a presentation of every attribute in every subject and condition, as suggested by the reviewer. The full set of the 324 images of brain images in the MNI space (nifti format) can be found here: https://www.dropbox.com/sh/7s7edpdvq9zjgmd/AADDm1SC8Y_yPGi0bCH18kuFa?dl=0

See Author response image 3 for an illustration of the results for one representative subject (subject 1, [MNI 45, 38, 22], xjview (http://www.alivelearn.net/xjview/)).

Author response image 3.

Author response image 3.

In addition, to provide the reviewer with a summary of the extracted feature weights, we have prepared several author response images. For each subject (s1 to s36), we plotted feature weights (color-coded) of all voxels in the DLPFC-‐ROI (vox) for Tastiness, Healthiness and $Self (columns 1-3). Separate figures were created for task-conditions: NC (Author response image 4), HC and EC (Author response image 5), and TC and PC (Author response image 6). As noted above, visual inspection of feature weights did not reveal hard anatomical boundaries (i.e., systematic clustering of voxels with high positive/negative weights in the SVM model as indicated in darker red and/or blue) but rather point towards distributed coding for all three attributes encoded in the DLPFC.

Author response image 4. Voxel-wise feature weights of the Support Vector Regression Model in Natural trials [NC] in both choice tasks.

Author response image 4.

Author response image 5. Voxel-wise feature weights of the Support Vector Regression Model in Health [HC] and Ethics trials [EC].

Author response image 5.

Author response image 6. Voxel-wise feature weights of the Support Vector Regression Model in Taste [HC] and Partner trials [PC].

Author response image 6.

We also note that the current manuscript focuses on brain regions that show goal-dependent coding of attribute values. The issue of predictive features across multiple attributes in the VMPFC will be reported elsewhere in greater detail. The latter touches on the popular notion of so called ‘common currency’ coding in value integration areas of the brain that will be presented together with data of another fMRI task not included in the current manuscript.

How do the authors interpret the altered representations in some primary sensory and motor areas, e.g. less strong decoding of tastiness values for NC than HC and TC? Have the authors considered a model whereby coupling between DLPFC and distinct regions of sensory cortex is modulated according to the regulatory goal? The DLPFC must get its attribute representations from someplace.

We speculate that part of the altered representations in visual and motor cortex may be due to differences in patterns of eye movements, visual attention, and the vigor of motor responding. While these effects are not uninteresting, we suspect they are a corollary of changes happening in other areas, rather than drivers of regulation per se.

We wholeheartedly agree with the reviewer that, in theory, information transfer between the DLPFC and other areas could account for changes in attribute representations. However, it is not entirely clear what the appropriate method is to test for this coupling, given that attribute encoding effects in the DLPFC appeared only in multivariate response patterns, not univariate response, while current functional connectivity methods assume that connectivity will manifest in the coupling of univariate responses in one region to univariate responses in another. We suspect that functionally coupling actually occurs at the level of distributed representations within and across regions, which complicates the interpretation of effects in univariate connectivity analyses.

Nevertheless, and with that caveat in mind, we performed a complete set of connectivity analyses for four regions of interest (ROIs) identified in our dataset: 1) the DLPFC area showing a conjunction between regulatory effects on representations of Tastiness, Healthiness, and $Self, 2) the TPJ and 3) the precuneus showing altered attribute encoding for $Other, and 4) the VMPFC area showing a conjunction across all attribute representations. For each of these regions, we performed a beta series functional connectivity analysis (Rissman et al., 2004), a method that has more power to detect context-specific changes in functional connectivity for event-related designs (Cisler et al., 2014). These supplemental analyses explicitly tested for changes in functional connectivity that might explain altered representations in these regions:

1) Based on the reviewers’ comment, we first tested the possibility that DLPFC might be more connected with visual or motor areas during regulation, either on average, or as a function of regulatory success. To this end, we defined the DLPFC as seed region, extracted trial-by-trial responses from this region, and then used these responses as parametric modulators in a GLM to identify areas where response covaried with DLPFC differently as a function of regulation. These analyses did not reveal goal-dependent functional connectivity with either primary visual or motor areas that showed goal-dependent attribute coding, even at liberal statistical thresholds (p < 0.005, uncorrected).

2) Following up on the notion that the DLPFC must get its attribute representations from someplace, we then tested the possibility of functional coupling between the DLPFC and the VMPFC region shown to encode all attributes in a goal-independent manner. We observed the following patterns:

a) During food choices, increased coupling between VMPFC and DLPFC during Health vs. Natural trials [HC > NC] reflected the extent to which participants increased the weight of healthiness in Health vs. Natural trials (Δw Healthiness [HC > NC], p < 0.005, uncorrected).

b) During altruistic choices, decreased coupling between VMPFC and DLPFC during Natural vs. Regulatory trials [NC > (EC, PC)] predicted the extent to which the weight on $Self decreased in Natural vs. Regulatory trials (Δw $Self [NC > (EC, PC)], p < 0.005, uncorrected).

These results are detailed in the revised manuscript and the Appendix:

“Figure 3—figure supplement 2. Exploratory functional connectivity analyses.

A. Region of the VMPFC (red) where increased connectivity with the DLPFC during Health vs. Natural and Taste focus conditions correlates with Δw Healthiness in Health vs. Natural and Taste Focus. […] All results are shown thresholded at p < 0.005 uncorrected.”

“Appendix 1 – 1.6 Changes in functional connectivity with the VMPFC correlate with regulatory success”.

In addition, we have now included a brief reference to this issue in the revised Results section and Discussion section of the manuscript.

“However, exploratory functional connectivity analyses provided subtle hints that the VMPFC could be indirectly related to regulatory success through its modulation of both DLPFC and precuneus (see Figure 3—figure supplement 2 and Appendix 1 – 1.6 for details).”

“This raises an important question: what determines the capacity of the DLPFC to properly represent these different attributes? […] Future work including the use of measures with higher temporal precision may help to elucidate when and how interactions between the VMPFC and DLPFC determine regulatory success in different contexts.”

The one comparison in which a match with the behavioral analyses failed to emerge was for ethical considerations compared to normal or personal considerations. I do not view this null finding as problematic, but out of curiosity do the authors have any thoughts as to what is going on when regulatory success depended on changes to fairness due to the goal of complying with social norms? This null finding should be addressed in the Discussion.

We fully share the reviewer’s curiosity about the meaning of this result! We think there are two possibilities here that are not mutually exclusive. First, we speculate that the instructions to focus on ethical reasons for giving are less constrained than instructions to focus on one’s partner’s feelings. Ethical values can be derived from many sources (including fairness, deontological views about the correct action, consequentialist views about the correct outcome, etc.). Prior research suggests that there is considerable individual heterogeneity in which constructs people deem relevant, and we suspect that is in part what is happening here. In contrast, focusing on one’s partner’s feelings may result in more consistent focus on specific considerations and attributes related to empathy and perspective taking across subjects.

However, we also believe our results are just as consistent with a second interpretation: that the lack of a correspondence between behavioral weights and neural response in the ethics condition speaks to the idea that the behavioral weights that we estimate, despite giving a single number for a specific attribute, may actually result from the consideration of multiple sub-attributes served by specific computations being performed in specific regions. In other words, the increased weight on $Other in both Partner and Ethics focus conditions may reflect empathetic considerations of consequences in the Partner condition, but may reflect consideration of norms and rules (e.g., do unto others as you would have done unto you) in the Ethics condition. That neural activation patterns in the TPJ and precuneus encode $Other in the Partner focus condition and not in the Ethics condition suggests that their computations may support the former more directly than the latter.

We have revised the Discussion section to focus on this second possibility, specifically considering its implications for the computations occurring in the TPJ and precuneus:

“At the same time, goal-consistent changes in pro-social attributes (e.g. others benefits) appeared in areas like the TPJ and precuneus, especially when focused on the partner’s thoughts and feelings. […] Thus, the TPJ and precuneus appear to encode features specifically related to representing others’ outcomes in a goal-sensitive manner, pointing to specialized loci of cognitive regulation in social choice domains.”

We have opted not to include the first point (heterogeneity of strategies) because the Discussion is already a bit long, and we feel that other points are more critical to make. However, if the reviewer or editor feels that a discussion of this point is worth mentioning, we are of course very happy to include it.

Reviewer #3:

In this manuscript, Tusche and Hutcherson report an fMRI study on how cognitive regulation affects the neural representation of choice-relevant attributes. They look for mechanisms that may generalize across two types of choices, one involving conflict between healthiness and tastiness of food items, the other involving conflict between self-interests and altruistic concerns. In different conditions, participants are asked to focus on one or the other attribute, which regulates the weights assigned to the targeted attributes, as shown via computational modeling of choice data. The key findings are the links between these changes in attribute weights and the decoding accuracy obtained for these attributes using multivariate pattern analysis (MVPA) in various cortical regions. The results are quite convincing, with successful decoding across tasks and individuals. There is no clear conclusion about whether the regulation is centralized or distributed though, since changes in decoding accuracy are observed in the DLPFC for most of the attributes but not all.

We fully concur that the DLPFC finding is convincing and intriguing, but not entirely consistent with either a fully centralized or fully distributed model. Response patterns in the DLPFC encoded values of three choice attributes (tastiness, healthiness, $Self) in a goal-consistent manner, while attribute values of Fairness and $Other were flexibly encoded in attribute-specific brain regions. Although this could be seen as providing “no clear conclusion” we believe our data, especially with the addition of new analyses suggested by our three reviewers, allows us to make a number of important and clear inferences. Specifically, we believe the paper presents convincing evidence in favor of a “hybrid model” in which goal-dependent changes in attribute values occur at the attribute level in attribute-specific loci for some attributes, but in a common hub in the DLPFC for other attributes. We believe that this is an important finding in its own right that will inspire future research on adaptive decision making. We have substantially modified the Introduction and Discussion to further clarify this point. More importantly, we now also report additional analyses of DLPFC responses that suggest that this common hub flexibly represents distinct attributes using distinct neural codes (as opposed to a unified value signal that generalizes across attributes and domains), shedding more light about how the DLPFC encodes goal-dependent attribute values. We now refer to these additional analyses and findings (suggested by reviewer #2) in the revised Results section of the manuscript:

“This finding suggests that the DLPFC acts as a domain-general circuit for goal-sensitive value representations. […] This supports the idea that the DLPFC acts as a domain-‐general mechanism for representing different attributes in a goal-‐ sensitive manner, using unique codes for each attribute.”

“Figure 5. Domain-general locus of goal-dependent attribute coding. […] This pattern of results indicates that goal-sensitive representations of attribute values in DLPFC rely on attribute-specific neural codes.”

Taken together, the sophisticated analyses and novel results significantly improve our understanding of when and why an individual will show specific or more global deficits in regulatory success in dietary and social choice, making our paper of high interest to the broad readership of eLife. Moreover, they open up several novel and interesting questions. The most prominent one is certainly the following question: what distinguishes attributes whose representations converge in DLPFC from those that did not? Are attributes that are flexibly encoded in the DLPFC simply less social in nature, less abstract and thus easier/faster to construct, or do they influence outcomes for the participant more directly? Identifying the precise factors that determine whether and when the DLPFC acts as the site for cognitive regulation of value opens up exciting new avenues for future research. We believe that the revised discussion of centralized and distributed loci of goal-consistent attribute coding has significantly strengthened the manuscript.

The role of cognitive control in economic choice is poorly understood and this study brings valuable insights by applying MVPA to standard choice paradigms. My main concern is the absence of a mechanistic account linking brain activity to behavioral output. In a sense, the results seem a bit trivial: when participants pay attention to a particular type of information (by instruction), this information is easier to decode from their brain activity. This effect should be seen as the outcome (and not the process) of cognitive regulation. How cognitive regulation is implemented, and how better decoding translates into biased choices, still need to be explained.

We thank the reviewer for his/her critical comments and suggestions, and for the assessment that this study yields valuable insights! We agree with the reviewer that the following assumption is extremely intuitive: when participants pay attention to a particular type of information (by instruction), this information is easier to decode from their brain activity. However, for several reasons we believe that evidence in support of this notion is by no means trivial and that our findings provide valuable insights into the mechanisms that underlie cognitive regulation of value in the brain.

First, as we make clear in the revised Introduction and Discussion sections, extant research has actually not consistently found such patterns despite clear changes in behavior, raising an important puzzle to be explained. Our results are the first to identify a clear representation of all choice-relevant attributes across multiple contexts, and to show clear modulation of all choice-relevant attributes in a manner depending on regulatory goals. Second, we also think our specific pattern of results clearly speak against the trivial interpretation that one would have to observe changes in attribute coding in the brain, and that this is an uninteresting reflection of changes in behavior. We observe a highly specific pattern of altered and unaltered representations of attribute values: for each attribute, we identified brain regions that reliably encoded current attribute values independent of regulatory condition. The VMPFC is one example for this finding, highlighted in the manuscript. In fact, a lack of modulation was actually the rule rather than the exception. Only a limited number of brain regions contained information on attribute values that varied significantly across task conditions. More importantly, we find a coherent set of diverging and converging changes in in attribute representation: while some attribute changes can be decoded from the DLPFC, changes in social attribute representation appear specifically in social cognitive areas. This is anything but trivial, and, we believe opens up crucially important lines of questioning that will be the basis for a better understanding of cognitive regulation and its mechanistic implementation. We have substantially revised the Introduction and Discussion to highlight these important points.

Third, we completely agree with the reviewer that our results focus more on outcome (and not the process) of cognitive regulation. We have substantially revised the Introduction to clarify this point further. However, we respectfully disagree that no information about process or mechanism can be derived from a detailed description of where such changes are observed. We have tried to make this clearer in the following ways:

1) We have now clarified in the new version of the paper in the Introduction, Results, and Discussion how identifying where attribute encoding changes occur may allow us to distinguish between different theories about computational process and mechanism. Specifically, we make clearer that regulation could occur via two processes: modulation at the level of specific attribute representations in non-overlapping, attribute-specific regions, or at the level of weighting in the value integration process. We show how our results are clearly consistent with attribute-level modulation for some attributes (i.e., $Other in TPJ and precuneus, fairness in middle frontal gyrus), and how our results are more ambiguous for other attributes (i.e., modulation of Tastiness, Healthiness, and $Self in DLPFC).

2) We also make clearer that there are important outstanding questions about the domain-generality or domain-specificity of regulation effects. On the one hand, if different attributes are computed in different regions with different computational properties, regulation could operate through different channels in different domains. If, instead, regulation operates at a more domain-general level, then regulation might influence attribute representations in a common region across multiple different attributes and multiple choice contexts. Understanding whether regulation changes attribute representations in a domain-general or domain-specific way can help reveal both the targets of regulation, as well as the mechanism of action.

In the Discussion, we also highlight how these questions are not of purely theoretical importance: the locus of action (attribute or integration level, domain specific or domain general) predicts non-trivial differences in patterns of regulatory success, which we observe in our own data. For attributes where regulation influences representation in a common region (i.e., DLPFC) regulatory success tracks together, but for attributes represented elsewhere (i.e., $Other in TPJ and precuneus), regulatory success appears to operate independently. Our results also may help to explain when and why regulatory success might feel difficult: if, as we observe, there are many regions of the brain where attribute representations are not altered by regulation, then these altered representations may continue to leak into the choice process and interfere with regulatory success.

3) Finally, we have included several supplementary analyses, including analyses that tested for commonalities of goal-consistent attribute coding in the DLPFC as well as functional connectivity results, which shed some light on how these computational processes are implemented in the brain. Please see our detailed first response to reviewer 3. above (neural attribute coding in the DLPFC across attributes and tasks) and our response to the next point below for more information (exploratory functional connectivity).

Taken together, we believe that these revisions have significantly strengthened the manuscript and provide valuable insights into neural computations in cognitive regulation of decision making across contexts and domains.

For the latter point, it would be important to show that the decoded value (i.e., the decoder output) correlates with the behavioral weights. The alternative would be that changes in decoding accuracy correspond to changes in precision (i.e., signal-to-noise ratio) and not changes in the signal itself. If correct, this would mean that a region downstream to the DLPFC could just read this value, add it to other values corresponding to other attributes, and feed the aggregate value to a selection process that makes the decision. Perhaps functional connectivity could be used to test for such a transfer of information. Thus, the neural model would parallel the behavioral model.

The reviewer suggests two supplemental analyses. First, we examined the link of decoding accuracy and estimated behavioral weights. To this end we ran supplemental regression analyses. Please see our sixth response to reviewer 3 below for details.

Second, we wholeheartedly agree with the reviewer that, in theory, information transfer between the DLPFC and other areas could account for changes in attribute representations. However, it is not entirely clear what the appropriate method is to test for this coupling, given that attribute encoding effects in the DLPFC appeared only in multivariate response patterns, not univariate response, while current functional connectivity methods assume that connectivity will manifest in the coupling of univariate responses in one region to univariate responses in another. We suspect that functionally coupling actually occurs at the level of distributed representations within and across regions, which complicates the interpretation of effects in univariate connectivity analyses.

Nevertheless, and with that caveat in mind, we performed a complete set of connectivity analyses for four regions of interest (ROIs) identified in our dataset (as also suggested by reviewer 2, see our seventh response to reviewer 2): 1) the DLPFC area showing a conjunction between regulatory effects on representations of Tastiness, Healthiness, and $Self, 2) the TPJ and 3) the precuneus showing altered attribute encoding for $Other, and 4) the VMPFC area showing a conjunction across all attribute representations. For each of these regions, we performed a beta series functional connectivity analysis (Rissman et al., 2004), a method suggested to have more power to detect context-specific changes in functional connectivity for event-related designs (Cisler et al., 2014). These supplemental analyses explicitly tested for changes in functional connectivity that might explain altered representations in these regions:

1) Using the DLPFC conjunction area as seed region, we first tested the possibility of functional coupling between the DLPFC and the VMPFC region shown to encode all attributes in a goal-independent manner would change either overall, or as a function of regulatory success. Consistent with this notion, we observed the following patterns:

During food choices, increased coupling between VMPFC and DLPFC during Health vs. Natural trials [HC > NC] reflected the extent to which participants increased the weight of healthiness in Health vs. Natural trials (Δw Healthiness [HC > NC], p < 0.005, uncorrected).

During altruistic choices, decreased coupling between VMPFC and DLPFC during Natural vs. Regulatory trials [NC > (EC, PC)] predicted the extent to which the weight on $Self decreased in Natural vs. Regulatory trials (Δw $Self [NC > (EC, PC)], p < 0.005, uncorrected).

2) We also examined coupling using Precuneus and TPJ as seed regions. This analysis suggested that, during altruistic choices, increased coupling between the VMPFC and Precuneus on Partner vs. Ethics trials also correlated with greater weighting of $Other in Partner vs. Ethics trials (Δw $Other [PC > EC], p < 0.005, uncorrected).

3) Complementary analyses using the VMPFC as a seed region yielded similar patterns.

Our results thus suggest precisely the relationship that the reviewer is suggesting: the DLPFC, TPJ, and Precuneus may be receiving information from – or sending information to – the VMPFC. We now report results for these exploratory, complementary connectivity analyses in the revised version of the manuscript and the Appendix:

“Figure 3—figure supplement 2. Exploratory functional connectivity analyses.[…] All results are shown thresholded at p < 0.005 uncorrected.”

Detailed descriptions of the methods and results of this exploratory functional connectivity analyses can be found here:

“Appendix 1 – 1.6 Changes in functional connectivity with the VMPFC correlate with regulatory success”.

In addition, we refer to these exploratory findings in the Results section and Discussion section of the revised manuscript. Notably the section also acknowledges another reviewer comment (see our fifth response to reviewer

3 below) as well as the issue of directionality for the interpretation of functional coupling between brain regions (following up on comments of reviewer #2):

“However, exploratory functional connectivity analyses provided subtle hints that the VMPFC could be indirectly related to regulatory success through its modulation of both DLPFC and precuneus (see Figure 3—figure supplement 2 and Appendix 1 – 1.6 for details).”

“This raises an important question: what determines the capacity of the DLPFC to properly represent these different attributes? […] Future work including the use of measures with higher temporal precision may help to elucidate when and how interactions between the VMPFC and DLPFC determine regulatory success in different contexts.”

Other points:

- The correlation across individuals could reflect compliance to the instructions rather than self-regulation capacity. The arguments taken from subjective report and from body-mass index are quite weak. For subjective report it could be that the rating scale is not reflecting the propensity to comply with the instructions. For body-mass index the opposite correlation could be expected: those who regulates food intake in real life should not need instructions in the lab.

We agree with the reviewer that compliance with reviewer instructions could be driving these results, either in part or in whole. This criticism can be leveled at the majority of studies on cognitive regulation, in both the decision making and emotion-regulation literature. However, we believe the specific pattern of results that we find argues against the simplest version of an experimental demand explanation. Specifically, we hypothesize that the motivation to comply with instructions likely affects both the motivation to suppress concerns about your own payoff during altruistic choices as well as increase those for another person (if not more so, as regulatory goals in the PC Partner Condition explicitly focused on $Other). However, behavioral evidence specifically links the ability to decrease self-related monetary consideration ($Self) to goal-consistent changes of food attributes (but not changes in $Other or Fairness), a finding mirrored in identified neural substrates of goal-consistent attribute coding.

However, the reviewer makes a valid point and we agree that we cannot fully distinguish between capacity and motivation explanations in this dataset. We now explicitly acknowledge this limitation in the revised Discussion section of the manuscript:

“We cannot completely rule out that regulatory effects on behavior and attribute representations might partly reflect differences in motivation to satisfy expectations of the experimenter. […] Tying laboratory measures of regulation to real-world consequences also remains a necessary future step in understanding the significance of these findings.”

We are also happy to include any specific reviewer suggestions in this paragraph on how to further reduce motivational or experimenter demand effects in both choice paradigms, in addition to our probabilistic implementation of choices (ensuring anonymity with regard to the randomly drawn and implemented trial at the end of both tasks, as both the experimenter and the partner are unaware if the outcome was based on a choice or a choice reversal), collected self-report measures of subjects’ motivation to comply with task instructions, and collected real-‐world measures of dietary success (BMI). These concrete suggestions might benefit future studies that aim to further elucidate the neural basis of cognitive regulation of decision making.

We also would like to briefly mention evidence on structural brain correlates of dietary regulatory success reported elsewhere (Schmidt et al., in press), because we believe these results may in part address the reviewer’s concern. Using voxel-based morphometry, this paper combines four independent datasets that examine goal-dependent changes in two different forms of dietary self-regulation. Note that in this independent paper, dietary success refers to altered weights of food attributes as estimated in a behavioral regression model (instead of a drift diffusion model as used in the current paper). We show that individual differences in dietary success correlate with differences in grey matter volume in the DLPFC, identified in a whole-brain regression analyses without any a priori assumption about regions of interests. Importantly, this localized neuronatomical correlate of dietary regulatory success matches our functional DLPFC ROI. See Author response image 7 for illustration. This finding indicates that goal-consistent changes in dietary choice at the behavioral level are likely not due solely to situational motivational effects, but might rather reflect more stable individual differences (as reflected in neuroanatomical markers in key regions of regulatory control). Upon acceptance of this manuscript, we intend to refer to this evidence in the respective Discussion section (see above) as additional reference.

Author response image 7.

Author response image 7.

Finally, we have taken the comment of reviewer #2 to heart and have removed the description of the BMI related evidence from the revised manuscript and have moved the description of self-report data on subjects’ motivation to comply with experimental instructions to the Appendix:

“Appendix 1 – 1.8 Self-reported motivation to comply with instructions and observed regulation- success.”

- The observation that all attributes are represented in the VMPFC but inaccessible to cognitive regulation is super interesting (and novel, to my knowledge). The dissociation with DLPFC should be more emphasized and discussed. Would this mean that VMPFC representations are closer to stimuli and DLPFC to responses?

We wholeheartedly agree with the reviewer about the novelty and importance of our VMPFC findings. Although there are hints in previous literature that the VMPFC may not always be the direct precursor of regulatory success, our study confirms and extends these earlier results in important ways. We have now performed a considerably more extensive analysis of VMPFC responses, using a post-hoc ROI-based approach as suggested by reviewer #2, confirming that decoding of attribute values in VMPFC doesn’t vary across regulatory goals and also fails to predict individual differences in success (in contrast to DLPFC, which does both). We have revised both the Introduction and Discussion sections to highlight the importance of understanding the roles of VMPFC and DLPFC in enabling regulatory success, and discuss the significance of the VMPFC null result more carefully in the Discussion section, incorporating the reviewer’s idea that the VMPFC may represent an earlier stage in the process of value construction:

The relevant sections in the revised Discussion section now read:

“Unexpectedly, cognitive regulation of decision making did not reliably modulate value signals within the VMPFC. Instead, regulatory effects converged to modulate a subset of distinct attribute representations in both the social and non‐social domain within a region of the DLPFC that has previously been implicated in value-based choice (Plassmann et al., 2007; Plassmann et al., 2010; Hutcherson et al., 2015)”

“The role of VMPFC and DLPFC in valuation and cognitive regulation.

Our study adds to a growing body of experimental work finding that behavioral effects of regulation can occur in the absence of corresponding changes to either overall levels of VMPFC response (Hollmann et al., 2012; Hutcherson et al., 2012; Yokum and Stice, 2013), or VMPFC representation of specific attributes like taste (Hare et al., 2011a). […] Future work including the use of measures with higher temporal precision may help to elucidate when and how interactions between the VMPFC and DLPFC determine regulatory success in different contexts.”

We also highlight the potentially distinct role of VMPFC and DLPFC in goal-consistent value coding in a separate goal of the study in the revised Introduction of the manuscript (Goal 3):

“Finally, we sought to shed light on whether information represented in VMPFC and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) supports either attribute-level or integration-level changes in value during cognitive regulation of decision making. […] However, several failures to observe changes in the VMPFC during cognitive regulation of decision making (Hollmann et al., 2012; Hutcherson et al., 2012; Yokum and Stice, 2013) suggest the need to either measure value computation in a more sensitive way, or to identify alternate routes to behavioral change.”

- To compare the pattern of attribute weights and the pattern of decoding accuracy across conditions, the authors intend to reproduce significance of pair-wise comparisons. As they know this approach heavily depends on the statistical threshold, which may be matter of debate when multiple comparisons are made. I would favor a straight regression of decoding accuracy against weight (across conditions).

The reviewer raises an interesting point. A regression of decoding accuracy against weight (across conditions) allows testing whether the amount of local neural information on attribute values reflects individual differences in the behavioral weights of this attribute. We expected that this alternative analysis approach would confirm our main findings for attributes for which neural evidence closely matched predictions of the behavioral computational models (e.g. for taste and health). However, one major potential concern with the suggested regression approach occurs in cases in which neural evidence diverges from predictions of the behavioral model (such as for $Others in the rTPJ and Precuneus). For those key findings of our study, estimated behavioral weights (specifically for the ethics condition) clearly don’t match the neural substrate for goal-dependent coding of attribute values. Thus, while our repeated measures ANOVAs identify any brain region in which information content on attribute values varies across condition (independent of whether it is consistent with predictions of the behavioral model or not), the regression approach identifies only brain regions in which neural decoding accuracies fully and closely match predictions of the behavioral model. Thus, if the behavioral model is even slightly miss-specified, or if more than one region contributes independently to the single weight estimated by the behavioral model, the neural results could fail to conform to predictions. While we are sensitive to potential concerns related to the issue of multiple comparisons, we respectfully argue that our original analysis approach is more appropriate to study goal-dependent changes across a wide variety of attributes, based on these theoretical considerations.

This being said however, we agree with the reviewer that it is important to know whether decoding accuracies also match the behaviorally estimated weights. We ran several supplemental analyses to provide further evidence on our key findings (also addressing reviewer 3’s third comment). More precisely, following the suggestion of reviewer #3, we used straight regression analyses of decoding accuracies against weights (across conditions). This set of complimentary analyses adopted a whole-brain approach that examined whether behavioral attribute weights covary with local neural decoding accuracies using parametric regression analyses as implemented in SPM: For each attribute (e.g. for the neural coding of tastiness values), these group-level analyses estimated a general linear model in SPM (simple one-sample t-test against 0) that used whole-brain decoding accuracy maps of each subject (collapsed across conditions) as the dependent variable and the attribute weights as the predictor variable. Contrast images of the parametric regressor against implicit baseline identify brain regions in which decoding accuracies covary linearly with behavioral attribute weights.

First, we examined attributes in the food choice task. Confirming results of our main analyses, the whole-brain analyses found that neural decoding accuracies on healthiness values in the right DLPFC were modulated by the weight of healthiness on choices as estimated in the behavioral model (p<0.001, cluster-level corrected at p<0.05 FWE). Likewise, in line with our previous results, we found that decoding accuracies on tastiness values in the right DLPFC (peak in the right superior frontal gyrus, SFG) covaried with the weight that this attribute had in food choices (p<0.001, cluster-level corrected at p<0.05 FWE). No brain region showed a negative correlation of decoding accuracies and behavioral attribute weights. See Table below for details on the clusters.

Author response table 1: Brain regions in which neural decoding accuracies covaried with individuals attribute weights in the food task across conditions.

Attribute Brain region Side T k MNI
x

y

z
Healthiness
Positive

(D)LPFC

R

4.37

133

51

23

28
Visual Cortex L/R 6.46 1346 ‐3 ‐76 7
Cerebellum L/R 4.84 147 9 ‐37 ‐38

Negative
Motor Cortex
L 4.65 217 ‐45 ‐19 70
Tastiness
Positive

(D)LPFC (peak in SFG)

L/R

5.73

2795

18

35

46
Visual Cortex R 5.21 992 45 ‐82 13
Negative L 4.49 415 ‐54 ‐73 22

Results are reported at a statistical threshold of p<0.05, FWE corrected at cluster-‐level (height threshold of p<0.001), only peak activations of clusters are reported; L=left hemisphere, R=right hemisphere, MNI=Montreal Neurological Institute, k=cluster size in voxels.

Importantly, for both choice-relevant attributes in the food task, we found that the cluster in the DLPFC closely matched the cluster identified in our previous approach (see Author response image 8 for illustration of overlap). The close match of identified brain regions for goal-dependent coding of attribute values provides further evidence for robustness of our previous evidence.

Author response image 8.

Author response image 8.

On a more general level, these findings suggest that decoding accuracies in goal-dependent brain regions reflect individual differences in attribute weights (across subjects and conditions), rather than only overall changes in signal‐to-noise due to attention to an attribute.

Next, we implemented a similar analysis for the altruism task. Consistent with our results suggesting that Altruistic Choice may be a more heterogeneous process, results of the regression analysis for $Self, $Other, and Fairness were weaker and did not survive correction for multiple comparisons in this whole- brain analyses. However, at a slightly more lenient statistical threshold (p < 0.001, uncorrected), neural information in the right DLPFC on trial-wise values of $Self indeed reflected individual weights assigned to $Self during choices as estimated in the behavioral model (w $Self). This result further supports the notion that neural activation patterns in the right DLPFC are involved in goal-consistent coding for peoples own monetary benefits in the altruism task. A matching analyses that tested for brain regions in which neural information is negatively correlated with attribute weights for $Self did not yield any significant results at this statistical threshold. See Author Response Table 2 for complete list of results for $Self.

Taken together with the results of the regression analyses for food attributes reported above, these supplemental analyses provide further support for the notion that flexible attribute representations in the right DLPFC generalize across task domains (i.e., $Self, Tastiness, Healthiness). Please see Author response image 9 for an illustration of the overlap of the DLPFC cluster identified in the whole brain regression analyses (green, for illustration purposes displayed at p < 0.005) with the DLPFC cluster identified in the conjunction analysis of goal-consistent attribute representations of regulatory vs. natural task conditions (orange).

Author response image 9.

Author response image 9.

Similar whole-brain regression analyses were implemented for Fairness and $Other (for complete list of results at p < 0.001, uncorrected, see Author response table 2). Interestingly, for $Other, we found that at this threshold neural information on $Other significantly covaried with the subject’s w$Other in the left pSTS/TPJ (while the right pSTS/TPJ only became significant at a threshold of p < 0.005, uncorrected). For illustration of both clusters (and the PCC/Precunes) at p < 0.005, uncorrected, see panel A). Notably, the cluster in the right pSTS/TPJ closely matched the brain region identified using our previous approach in which we compared regulatory and natural conditions (see panel B for illustration). While results at this liberal statistical threshold have to be interpreted with extreme caution, these findings suggest that goal-sensitive neural representations of $Other might be encoded bilaterally in the pSTS/TPJ.

Taken together, these results suggest that our results are largely robust to different ways of conceptualizing the correspondence between decoding accuracies and behaviorally estimated attribute weights. As mentioned above, we have chosen to report the original ANOVAs in the paper because of their greater flexibility in identifying patterns that do not fully conform to the predictions of the behavioral model. We hope that the reviewer’s concerns on this regard have been fully addressed.

Author response image 10.

Author response image 10.

Author response table 2: Brain regions in which neural decoding accuracies for trial-wise attribute values covaried with the weights (estimated in the DDM) in the altruism task across conditions.

Attribute Brain region Side T k MNI
x

y

z
$Self
Positive (D)LPFC R 3.88 9 36 11 34
Superior Frontal Gyrus/ L 3.56 8 -9 26 31
Paracingulate Cortex
Visual Cortex R 3.69 11 12 -97 4
Precuneus R 3.83 22 18 -46 43
Cerebellum L 4.48 67 -12 -70 -47
Cerebellum L 4.48 67 -12 -70 -47
Negative -
Fairness
Positive Inferior Parietal Cortex * R 4.03 192 57 -58 52
Inferior Frontal Gyrus/Rolandic operculum R 3.34 12 57 5 16
Thalamus L 3.66 15 -9 -4 10
Inferior Temporal Gyrus L 3.69 29 -51 -31 -20
Visual Cortex L/R 3.83 130 -3 -70 1
Cerebellum L/R 3.61 22 0 -46 -17
Angular Gyrus R 3.51 16 27 -67 46
Negative -
$Other
Positive Posterior Superior Temporal Gyrus L 85 4.30 -42 -61 10
DMPFC L 48 3.95 -6 38 70
R 6 3.18 18 47 43
Inferior Temporal Gyrus R 22 3.65 63 -40 -29
Cerebellum L/R 111 3.84 3 -46 -8
Visual Cortex L/R 9 3.36 3 -73 22
Negative -

Results are reported at a statistical threshold of p<0.001 (uncorrected) with a cluster-size threshold of k > 5 voxels, only peak activations of clusters within brain mask are reported; * indicates FWE corrected at cluster-level; L=left hemisphere, R=right hemisphere, MNI=Montreal Neurological Institute, k=cluster size in voxels.

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    Supplementary file 1. Supplemental tables.

    (A) Drift diffusion model (DDM) parameters and RTs. Table reports Mean and Standard Deviations (±SD). b = initial height of the barrier; d = rate of decay for collapsing barriers; NDT = non decision time.; RTs = reaction times. Results for the Altruism Task are reported both for full sample (N = 49), and for the subset of participants that also completing the Food Task (N = 36).

    (B) Support vector regressions (SVRs) of decision values (DV) in both choice tasks. Results for goal-independent encoding of DVs (Main effect for DV, averaged information content across three conditions in each task) are reported at a statistical threshold of p < 0.05, FWE corrected at voxel-level; indicates results for goal-dependent variations in DV (repeated measures ANOVAs across three conditions in each task) reported at p < 0.001, FWE cluster-corrected at p < 0.05, k = 10 voxels; only peak activations of clusters are reported; L = left hemisphere, R = right hemisphere, k = cluster size in voxels, MNI = Montreal Neurological Institute.

    (C) Correlation of regulatory success in goal-dependent attribute weighting across choice domains. Correlation coefficients for change scores in attribute weights (w) in food choices and altruistic choices estimated in two separate DDMs. Successful reduction in the weight on selfish considerations (Δw $Self) in altruistic choices was correlated with successfully amplifying the weight on health considerations (Δw Healthiness) suppressing the weight of taste considerations (Δw Tastiness) in food choices. Changes in the ability to increase social considerations (Δw Other) were not correlated to goal-consistent changes in food attributes. Δw Tastiness [NC - TC] is not displayed, as estimated attribute weights did not significantly differ between conditions. Note also that differences scores in Δw Healthiness [NC - TC] (last column) were minimal, limiting the interpretability of the respective correlation analyses. 

    (D) Decoding of individual differences in regulatory success in DLPFC (altruism task). Decoding of individual differences in regulation success based on response patterns in right DLPFC (Figure 5A) obtained in the altruism task. Response patterns reliably predicted the extent of increased generous choice behavior. Consistent with key results reported in the main text, neural activation patterns also predicted individual’s increased healthy choices in a separate food task. Regarding altered attributes weights, predictive information in DLPFC was selective for subjects’ inhibition of $Self weights, but did not extend to altered weights on $Other or Fairness, confirming results reported in the main text. Higher-than-chance predictions are reported when decoding accuracy values exceeded the 95th percentile of empirical null-distribution (cutoff), obtained with 1000 replications of the analysis on permuted data sets. 

    (E) Univariate encoding of attributes in food task and altruism task. Regions reported as significant if they passed a cluster-corrected threshold p < 0.05, with a voxel-defining threshold of p < 0.001, uncorrected, unless otherwise noted. * Illustrates results significant at p < 0.001, uncorrected, reported for completeness: subgenual area did not overlap with the area of vmPFC that displayed overlapping representations of all attributes; only peak activations of clusters are reported; L = left hemisphere, R = right hemisphere, MNI = Montreal Neurological Institute, k = cluster size in voxels.

    elife-31185-supp1.docx (55.1KB, docx)
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.31185.015
    Transparent reporting form
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.31185.016

    Data Availability Statement

    Functional imaging and behavioral data is deposited at the project's Open Science Framework (OSF) page (osf.io/wa4cs). The project page also makes available the derived statistical maps (univariate and multivariate decoding analyses), regions of interest (ROIs) used in analyses of functional imaging data, processed behavioural data, and details on the experimental procedure.


    Articles from eLife are provided here courtesy of eLife Sciences Publications, Ltd

    RESOURCES