Skip to main content
SSM - Population Health logoLink to SSM - Population Health
. 2018 Mar 10;4:280–290. doi: 10.1016/j.ssmph.2018.03.002

Family income, maternal psychological distress and child socio-emotional behaviour: Longitudinal findings from the UK Millennium Cohort Study☆☆

Katharine Noonan 1, Richéal Burns 1, Mara Violato 1,
PMCID: PMC5976845  PMID: 29854912

Abstract

The association between low family income and socio-emotional behaviour problems in early childhood has been well-documented, and maternal psychological distress is highlighted as central in mediating this relationship. However, whether this relationship holds for older children, and the precise mechanisms by which income may influence child behaviour is unclear.

This study investigated the relationship between family income and child socio-emotional behaviour at 11 years of age, and examined the mediating role of maternal psychological distress over time using the UK Millennium Cohort Study.

The primary outcome was parent-reported behavioural problems, as captured by the Total Difficulties Score (TDS), derived from the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ). Secondary outcomes were the emotional, peer-related, conduct, and hyperactivity/inattention problems subscales of the SDQ; and teacher-reported TDS. Permanent family income was the primary exposure variable; frequency of poverty up to age 11 years was the secondary exposure variable. Maternal psychological distress was operationalised to reflect the trajectory from child birth to age 11. Multivariable logistic regression models were used to estimate the effect of permanent family income on child behaviour at age 11, controlling for maternal psychological distress and other relevant covariates.

Results showed a statistically significant protective effect of increased permanent family income on the likelihood of behavioural problems at age 11. This finding was consistent for all SDQ subscales apart from emotional problems, and was strongest for teacher-reported behavioural problems. Maternal distress was an important mediator in the income-child behaviour relationship for parent-reported, but not teacher-reported, behavioural problems.

The results of this study strengthen empirical evidence that the child behaviour-income gradient is maintained in older childhood. Mother’s psychological distress, particularly longstanding or recurrent, appears to contribute to this relationship. These findings may validate calls for psychosocial and financial supports for families affected by parental mental health issues.

Keywords: UK, Child health and development, Income gradient, Maternal psychological distress, Health inequalities

Highlights

  • Higher family income is associated with fewer child behavioural problems at age 11.

  • Maternal distress mediates the relationship between child behaviour and income.

  • Persistent rather than sporadic distress is more detrimental to child behaviour.

Introduction

Child health and development, and their relationship with socio-economic status, are complex and important issues spanning an array of disciplines including public health, economics, developmental psychology, and social policy. Health experienced in childhood and adolescence affects human capital accumulation, as well as health and labour market status in adulthood (Currie et al., 2008, Lundborg et al., 2014). In the UK, stark inequalities in child physical and mental health have been linked to poverty and social disadvantage (Social Mobility and Child Poverty Commission, 2017). However, measures to financially assist families with children have been reduced, and the government’s dedicated unit to eliminate child poverty was abolished in 2016 (Social Mobility and Child Poverty Commission, 2017, Social Mobility and Child Poverty Commission, 2015, Merrick, 2017). Although studies have consistently documented poorer behavioural outcomes in children with economic deprivation, the precise mechanisms by which income affects health, and how this association may change with age, is less well-understood. Identifying the factors through which income affects both physical and mental health may provide alternative ways to improve health which complement financial contributions.

Increasing attention in recent years has been paid to child development, including behavioural outcomes, and their relationship with economic hardship. Child behavioural difficulties not only reflect impaired mental health, but they are also linked to adult outcomes, including academic attainment and employment (Najman, Aird, Bor, O’Callaghan, Williams, and Shuttlewood, 2004). Problems in child socio-emotional behaviour have been associated with poorer health and behavioural outcomes, such as: obesity; substance abuse; juvenile delinquency; and criminality (Fergusson et al., 2005). Socio-emotional development encompasses externalising behaviours, such as conduct and hyperactivity problems, and internalising behaviours, including emotional and peer problems (Goodman, Lamping, and Ploubidis, 2010). This study seeks to add to the current body of evidence by, firstly, establishing whether the observed association between family income and behaviour in early childhood is maintained in later childhood; secondly, by exploring the role of maternal mental health as a potential mediating mechanism, examining different trajectories of maternal psychological distress over time; and thirdly, building on knowledge of early childhood development, exploring the extent to which later childhood factors may shape child behaviour at age 11. In theory, the relationship may strengthen as a child becomes older, as observed by Currie and Stabile (2003). They suggested that the harmful effects of economic hardship accumulate over time, and that low income children are subject to a greater number of adverse health events than children from higher-income families. A steepening effect of family resources on socio-emotional behaviour over time has been replicated in several more recent studies, possibly reflecting a cumulative effect of economic deprivation (Condliffe and Link, 2008, Khanam et al., 2009, Fletcher and Wolfe, 2014, Murasko, 2008, Fletcher and Wolfe, 2016). Heckman and colleagues’ dynamic framework for capability formation (Conti & Heckman, 2012) suggests that capabilities (including socio-emotional skills) are self-productive, such that better capabilities in earlier years enhance development of capabilities at future ages. They also propose sensitive and critical periods, referring to ages at which parental inputs, enabled by higher family income, are more productive in improving capabilities (Conti & Heckman, 2012). It is therefore possible that socio-emotional skills at age 11 are more important than early years for long-term outcomes, for example due to the age at which children are involved in education.

The findings of this study may be relevant to policy makers regarding financial assistance for families at different stages of child development, as well as to clinicians in supporting the mental health of mothers.

Previous research

The child health-family income gradient refers to the positive association between health and wealth and its origins in childhood (Case et al., 2002), and has been the subject of a growing body of literature within the field of health economics (Propper et al., 2007, Currie et al., 2007). It was explored in the influential studies by Case et al. (2002) and Currie and Stabile (2003) using US and Canadian data of children up to age 17, respectively.

Across the literature, it has been consistently demonstrated that children from higher income families also exhibit better behavioural outcomes, from as early as 3 years of age (Najman et al., 2004, Brooks-Gunn and Duncan, 1997, Taylor et al., 2004, Bradshaw and Holmes, 2008, Kiernan and Huerta, 2008, Korenman et al., 1995, Kiernan and Mensah, 2009). However, the magnitude of this effect and the pathways through which it operates are less clear. Permanent income has been more strongly linked with behavioural problems than current income, (Taylor et al., 2004, Blau, 1999, Curtis et al., 2001) and persistent financial adversity is consistently associated with childhood behavioural problems (Fletcher and Wolfe, 2016, Brooks-Gunn and Duncan, 1997, Korenman et al., 1995, Ackerman et al., 2004, Holmes and Kiernan, 2013). In two recent studies using Millennium Cohort Study (MCS) data when children were aged 11 years, both persistent poverty and first transitions into poverty were strongly linked to childhood mental health problems (Fitzsimons et al., 2017, Wickham et al., 2017).

The mechanisms by which family income exerts an impact on child development has been investigated through both psychological and economic frameworks, namely the parental stress and parental investment theories. The parental investment theory takes a human capital approach, reflecting the ability of parents to invest resources to support their child’s development, particularly with learning and cognition (Yeung et al., 2002, Duncan et al., 2014). The family stress theory suggests that adverse circumstances such as poverty may create a stressful home environment, which contributes to psychological distress in parents and children (Kiernan & Huerta, 2008; Holmes & Kiernan, 2013). This distress may filter into parenting practices, which tend to be punitive, inconsistent and less nurturing (Duncan et al., 2014). Studies using MCS data for children at 3 and 5 years of age have found that a significant proportion of the effect of economic deprivation on externalising behaviour was accounted for by parenting behaviours (Kiernan and Huerta, 2008, Holmes and Kiernan, 2013). Yeung et al., (2002) found that family income was associated with children’s externalising behaviour primarily via maternal emotional distress and parenting practices, in US children aged 3 to 5 years. Both the parental stress and parental investment pathways were explored by Violato et al. (2011) in British children aged 3 and 5 years, finding that the direct effect of income on socio-emotional behaviour reduced substantially when parental stress and investment factors were adjusted for. An Australian study by Khanam and Nghiem (2016) found that family income was no longer significantly associated with non-cognitive development of children up to age 7 after controlling for parental stress and investment variables. A 2015 study by Akee, Simeonova, Costello, and Copeland (2015) in US adolescents found that parenting and family relationships were important potential channels through which household income influences emotional-behavioural outcomes.

The role of maternal mental health has been highlighted as central in mediating the effect of family income (Kiernan & Mensah, 2009; Violato et al., 2011; Khanam & Nghiem, 2016; Schoon, Hope, Ross & Duckworth, 2010; Washbrook et al., 2014). Persistent maternal mental health problems have been most strongly associated with child behavioural problems (Fitzsimons et al., 2017, Schoon et al., 2010, Mensah and Kiernan, 2010). While the role of maternal psychological distress is well established in the relationship between family income and early child development, there is paucity of evidence as to whether this relationship holds for older children. Furthermore, no studies explore child socio-emotional outcomes and different patterns of maternal psychological distress over time.

Methods

Data

The MCS is a longitudinal cohort study of 18,818 children born in the UK between September 2000 and January 2002 (Hansen, 2014). It is a multidisciplinary survey developed to capture the effects of social, economic and health advantages and disadvantages on childhood development and other outcomes (Hansen, 2014). There have been six surveys to date, the first collected when the children were around 9 months of age, followed by surveys at ages 3, 5, 7, 11 and 14. At the time this study was conducted (2016) only the first five surveys were available. There were 13,287 children remaining in the study at the end of the fifth survey (Hansen, 2014). The MCS oversampled from areas with higher levels of poverty, as well as higher proportions of ethnic minority groups (Hansen, 2014). At each survey, caregivers were surveyed via interview and self-completed questionnaire. Later surveys included teacher interviews and physical, cognitive and behavioural assessments of child development (Hansen, 2014).

Child-health production function

As outlined by Grossman, 2000, Jacobsen, 2000) this study estimates child behaviour as the product of parental and other inputs, assuming the child’s initial health endowment. The resulting child-health production function can be written as follows:

COt=α0+h0+α1Xt+α2LN(Y)t+z=1ZαPSzPStz+j=1JαPIjPItj+j=1SαOPsOPts+εt (1)

where CO represents the behavioural outcomes being analysed; t denotes child age; h0 is the child’s initial health endowment, such as gestational age and birthweight; X is a set of variables including family sociodemographic controls and some child characteristics; Y is family income given in its logarithmic form; the three summation terms refer to ‘parental stress’, ‘parental investment’ and ‘other pathways’ (for example, peer relationships) variables, respectively; and εt is the error term.

Family income and poverty persistence

The primary exposure was permanent family income, measured by averaging equivalised household income over the first five available surveys, expressed in its logarithmic form (Currie and Stabile, 2003, Condliffe and Link, 2008, Khanam et al., 2009, Murasko, 2008, Currie et al., 2007, Taylor et al., 2004, Dooley and Stewart, 2007). Income values from surveys 1 to 4 were adjusted for inflation using the Average Weekly Earnings (AWE) (Office for National Statistics, 2016) index, expressing income at 2011 (survey five) prices (Thompson, 2009). Frequency of poverty (defined in the MCS as having a net equivalent family income below 60 per cent of the national median, accounting for the number and age of people in the home (Hansen, 2014)) since child birth was employed as the secondary exposure of interest (Bradshaw and Holmes, 2008, Kiernan and Huerta, 2008, Kiernan and Mensah, 2009, Holmes and Kiernan, 2013, Flouri et al., 2014). As per Holmes and Kiernan (2013) a categorical variable indicating frequency of poverty across the first five surveys was constructed. The five categories were: “never experienced poverty”, “poverty at one survey only”, “poverty at two or three surveys”, and “poverty at four or five surveys”. These were generated to capture persistence of poverty, rather than timing, as existing evidence indicates that the former is more strongly associated with problematic child behaviour (Korenman et al., 1995, Holmes and Kiernan, 2013).

Child socio-emotional behaviour

Socio-emotional behaviour at 11 years of age was measured using the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) (Hansen, 2014, Goodman, 1997). The SDQ is a concise and well-validated tool used to measure prosocial behaviour and psychopathology of 3 to 16 year olds (Goodman, 2001). Responses generate scores on five subscales: conduct problems, pro-social behaviour, hyperactivity or inattention, emotional problems, and problems with peers (Goodman, 2001, Scoring the SDQ [Internet], 2017). For all subscales except prosocial behaviour, a higher score indicates more problems (Goodman, 2001). Emotional and peer-related difficulties are considered “internalising” problems, while conduct and hyperactivity/inattention behaviours are “externalising” problems.

The primary outcome measure was parent-reported socio-emotional behavioural problems at age 11, captured by the total difficulty score (TDS) (Goodman, 2001). The TDS was calculated by summing the four problem domains, excluding the pro-social domain, with a cut-off of 17 or more (out of 40) applied to generate a binary variable. Secondary outcomes were the four SDQ subscales (emotional, peer-related, conduct, and hyperactivity/inattention problems) (Scoring the SDQ, 2017). Using validated cut-offs for the scores out of 10 (5 for emotional problems, 4 for conduct problems, 7 for hyperactivity and 4 for peer problems) (Goodman, 2001), binary variables were generated for each subscale.

Parent-reported behaviour may be subject to biases, especially in the presence of mother’s psychological distress (Fergusson et al., 1993, Breslau et al., 1988). To address this point, a secondary analysis was conducted using teacher-reported TDS at age 11, where this information was available. A binary variable for problem behaviour was generated using the validated cut-off of 16 or more out of 40. (Scoring the SDQ, 2017). Binary variables were also generated for the subscales with cut-offs of 6, 4, 7 and 5 out of 10 for emotional problems, conduct problems, hyperactivity and peer problems, respectively (Goodman, 2001).

Covariates

The choice of covariates was informed by previous literature and established parental investment and parental stress theoretical frameworks. The set of standard sociodemographic controls included child age at survey five, child sex and ethnicity, number of siblings, housing tenure (i.e. tenancy status), maternal education and age at the child’s birth. The other background variables controlled for were the child’s initial endowment of health, captured by birthweight and gestational age, breastfeeding, mother’s drinking and smoking during pregnancy. Child longstanding illness was included as a pathway variable, given its relationship with income and potential to affect socio-emotional problems. These variables were included in model specification 2.

Maternal psychological distress (model specification 3)

The MCS utilised the Kessler-6 (K-6) questionnaire (Kessler, Andrews, Colpe, Hiripi, Mroczek & Normand, 2002) at surveys two to five to capture maternal psychological distress (Hansen, 2014). For the first survey (child aged 9 months), an 8-item scale based on the Rutter Malaise Inventory (RMI) was used to capture maternal distress (Schoon, Sacker, Hope, Collishaw, and Maughan, 2005). Both are validated measures, used widely as screening tools for psychological affect. For each survey, a binary variable was constructed from the K-6 or RMI scores indicating whether or not the mother suffered from psychological distress (Johnson, 2012) (1=yes; 0=no). A score of ≥6 (out of 24) and a score of ≥4 (out of 9) were taken to indicate maternal psychological distress for the K-6 questionnaire and the RMI, respectively. Maternal psychological distress trajectory was then operationalised by generating a variable reflecting the mother’s distress over the first five available surveys. The groupings were: never experienced psychological distress; distress in early years only (child age 9 months and/or 3 years); distress in middle years only (child age 5 and/or 7 years); current psychological distress (child age 11) only, recurrent distress (at least once in the early years and once in middle/later years of child’s life), persistent psychological distress (all five surveys) and other patterns (e.g. the last two or three surveys, sporadic distress).

Parental investment, parental stress and external factors variables (model specification 4)

To operationalise the parental investment and family stress theories (Holmes and Kiernan, 2013, Yeung et al., 2002, Duncan et al., 2014), several variables describing parenting practices and home environment were identified. Information was extracted on change in family structure and parenting style, including discipline measures, structure, and time spent with the child in activities such as reading and playing. Current alcohol intake and smoking, examples of parent’s child-health related behaviours (Violato et al., 2011; Dooley & Stewart, 2007; Jones, Gutman & Platt, 2013), were also controlled for. Additionally, the analysis sought to account for external factors that are important in behavioural outcomes in later childhood, such as peer relationships (Jones et al., 2013). Hours of sedentary screen time have been cited as having small but consistent detrimental effects on childhood mental health (Biddle & Asare, 2011), while lower odds of emotional problems have been observed in youth with higher levels of physical activity (Kremer et al. 2014). As such, variables measuring time spent playing sport and screen time (i.e. computer/video usage) were included. The group of covariates incorporating family, parenting, and later childhood factors is hereafter denoted ‘family and external factors’. These variables were extracted from the age 7 survey (survey four) to build a longitudinal picture of the relationship with behaviour at age 11 and to limit reverse causality concerns.

Statistical analyses

Children were eligible for inclusion if they were singletons recruited at 9 months of age, were present for all first five surveys and had responses recorded for all relevant questions. Only mother interviews were used, including natural, adoptive, and foster mothers, if present in the first five surveys of the study. The sample size after exclusions was 8499. Survey weights were included in the analyses to account for the stratified cluster sample design of the study, and attrition bias due to non-response across the surveys (Millennium Cohort Study Initial findings from the Age 11 survey, 2014, Ketende and Jones, 2011).

As the behavioural outcomes in this study are dichotomous, the child health production function (1) was estimated for each outcome of interest using multivariable logistic regression with permanent income in its logarithmic form as the primary exposure. Associations between outcome variables, exposures and other covariates were expressed as partial effects (marginal effects for continuous variables; average effects for categorical variables) measured at the mean of each covariate. Secondary analyses used poverty persistence as the exposure. The primary outcome was parent-assessed problem behaviour, measured by the TDS. The secondary outcomes were the four subscales of the SDQ (emotional, peer-related, conduct and hyperactivity/inattention problems) and teacher-reported behavioural problems, measured by the TDS.

Empirical selection was conducted by assessing conceptually similar variables for collinearity and statistical significance. Covariates were then added to the model in a stepwise fashion in groups reflecting theoretical frameworks. The variables included in the four model specifications are described in Table 1. The same model specifications were adopted for secondary exposures and secondary outcomes as the main model. Teacher-reported TDS was available only for a subsample of the children for whom parent-reported TDS was available, hence the sample size for this secondary outcome was smaller than in the primary analyses. The same covariates were included in this analysis to aid comparability.

Table 1.

Variables included in the four model specifications of the child-health production function.

Model specifications Variables
Model specification 1 Raw model of family income and child behavioural outcome
Model specification 2 Models included family income, standard sociodemographic controls (current age, sex, ethnicity, number of siblings, maternal education, maternal age at child birth, housing tenure) and ‘child’s health endowment’ (birth weight, gestational age, breastfeeding, mother’s smoking and alcohol during pregnancy, child’s longstanding illness)
Model specification 3 Models included the same variables as in model specification 2 with the addition of ‘maternal s psychological distress pattern’
Model specification 4 Models included the same variables as in model specification 3 with the addition of ‘family and external factors’:
  • Family stress and parental investment frameworks (change in mother’s relationship from wave 4 to 5, indicators of routine/structure, discipline measures, indicators of mother’s time spent with the child)

  • Mother’s child-health-related behaviours (mother longstanding illness, smoking and alcohol intake)

  • Older child variables (time spent with friends, time playing sport)

The adjusted Wald test was used to assess significance of covariates. All statistical analysis was conducted using Stata version 12.1. StataCorp (2011).

This study was a secondary analysis of the Millennium Cohort Study, using de-identified data available in the public domain. As the project falls within the remit of the original parent ethics approval (Hansen, 2014), no additional ethics clearance was required.

Results

Descriptive characteristics

Baseline characteristics of the final cohort are summarised in Table 2; statistical significance of the association between covariates and parent-reported problem behaviour are also presented, whilst the association between covariates and family income (Baron & Kenny, 1986) are reported in Table A1.2. There was an equal distribution of males and females in the final sample, which was a predominately White population. The mean age at interview for survey five was 10.7 years. The median permanent income, adjusted for inflation over the years of the data and equivalised for household size, was £23,072 (IQR £15,545 to £31,491). 54.8% of the children’s families had never experienced poverty, 15.2% of children lived in persistent poverty, and 13.8% had been under the poverty line once (Millennium Cohort Study Initial findings from the Age 11 survey, 2014).

Table 2.

Descriptive characteristics and univariate associations with problem behaviour at age 11.

Variable N (Total = 8499) % total N Problem behaviour % Problem behaviour N No problem behaviour % No problem behaviour p-value
Sociodemographic variables
Age 0.020
10 2800 34.2 246 40.0 2554 33.6
11 5666 65.5 444 59.7 5222 66.1
12 33 0.4 2 0.4 31 0.4
Sex <0.001
Male 4260 50.9 422 62.9 3838 49.7
Female 4239 49.1 270 37.1 3969 50.3
Ethnicity 0.721
White 7650 90.6 623 91.1 7027 90.6
Non-white 849 9.4 69 8.9 780 9.5
Siblings b <0.001
0 968 11.6 106 16.0 862 11.1
1 4041 47.3 273 39.0 3768 48.2
2 2381 28.0 184 27.0 2197 28.1
≥3 1109 13.1 129 18.0 980 12.6


 

 

 

 

 

 


Mother’s academic qualification a
Tertiary degree 2599 26.8 109 13.3 2490 28.1
A-levels 926 10.0 56 7.4 870 10.3
GCSE 3994 50.1 391 58.0 3603 49.3
None of these 980 13.1 136 21.3 844 12.3
Mother’s age at child’s birth <0.001
<20 years 507 7.7 71 12.3 436 7.2
20–24 years 1260 15.9 166 26.3 1094 14.9
25–29 years 2410 29.2 212 31.9 2198 28.9
30–34 years 2797 30.7 161 20.1 2636 31.7
≥35 years 1516 16.6 82 9.4 1434 17.3
Housing tenure b <0.001
Own/mortgage 6177 67.2 357 44.9 5820 69.4
Council 938 13.3 161 25.1 777 12.1
Rent/other 1384 19.5 174 30.0 1210 18.45


 

 

 

 

 

 


Child health endowment variables
Birthweight 0.015
>2.5 kg 8009 94.2 635 91.2 7374 94.1
≤2.5 kg 490 5.8 57 8.8 433 5.9
Gestational age 0.012
≥37 weeks 7886 92.3 628 88.7 7258 92.6
<37 weeks 613 7.7 64 11.3 549 7.4
Child longstanding illness b <0.001
No 6928 81.2 462 66.8 6466 82.7
Yes 1571 18.8 230 33.2 1341 17.3
Mother’s alcohol intake during pregnancy <0.001
Never 5715 66.4 504 71.0 5211 65.9
Light 2164 26.2 124 18.6 2040 26.9
Moderate 449 5.3 45 7.3 404 5.1
Heavy/binge 171 2.1 19 3.1 152 2.0
Mother’s smoking in pregnancy <0.001
Never smoked 5690 64.5 349 47.9 5341 66.2
Stopped smoking during pregnancy 1076 13.3 93 12.2 983 13.4
Smoked throughout pregnancy 1733 22.2 250 40.0 1483 20.4
Breastfeeding duration <0.001
No breastfeeding 2482 32.2 248 38.6 2234 31.6
<7 days 979 9.8 107 12.9 872 9.6
1 week to 3 months (inclusive) 2127 24.8 191 29.1 1936 24.4
3 to 6 months (inclusive) 1225 14.0 54 7.9 1171 14.6
>6 months 1686 19.1 92 11.8 1594 19.8
Maternal psychological distress pattern <0.001
Never 3185 36.3 81 10.9 3104 38.9
Early years only 520 6.1 19 3.0 501 6.4
Middle years only 604 7.2 44 6.5 560 7.24
Age 11 only 637 7.6 48 6.1 589 7.8
Other pattern 1237 14.3 169 23.3 1068 13.4
Recurrent 1930 23.8 243 37.6 1687 22.4
Persistent 386 4.7 88 12.7 298 3.9


 

 

 

 

 

 


Family/External factors variables
Change in mother’s relationship, wave 4–5 0.001
Became single 531 6.4 61 8.1 470 6.22
Became partnered 358 4.8 56 7.9 302 4.4
No change 7610 88.9 575 84.0 7035 89.3
Regular bedtime on weekdays b 0.036
Never/almost never 288 3.4 36 4.8 252 3.2
Sometimes 435 5.0 51 6.9 384 4.9
Usually 2699 31.7 226 31.8 2473 31.7
Always 5077 59.9 379 56.5 4698 60.2
Hours spent playing computer/video games during week b <0.001
None 866 10.4 91 13.6 775 10.1
<1 hour 4644 53.7 322 44.5 4322 5.5
1–3 hours 2668 32.0 229 34.4 2439 31.8
>3 hours 321 3.9 50 7.5 271 3.52
Mother's satisfaction with time spent with child b 0.001
Enough or more than enough 2029 24.1 195 27.8 1834 23.7
Just enough 3682 43.6 243 35.3 3439 4.4
Not enough 2788 32.3 254 36.9 2534 31.9
Tells off when naughty b <0.001
Never 48 0.5 1 0.2 47 0.5
Rarely 1019 12.1 51 7.9 968 12.5
Sometimes 3003 35.1 146 20.4 2857 36.6
Often 4429 52.3 494 71.6 3935 50.4
Sends to room when naughty b <0.001
Never 1037 11.4 39 5.3 998 12.0
Rarely 2498 29.5 126 15.7 2372 15.7
Sometimes 3406 39.5 227 33.3 3179 33.3
Often 1558 19.6 300 45.7 1258 45.7
Takes things away when naughty b <0.001
Never 687 7.4 39 4.8 648 7.7
Rarely 2414 28.6 109 15.0 2305 29.9
Sometimes 3931 46.1 271 39.2 3660 46.8
Often 1467 17.9 273 41.0 1194 15.5
Amount of time reading to child b <0.001
Not at all 176 2.0 19 2.2 157 1.9
Approximately monthly 615 7.2 69 12.7 546 6.7
Weekly or more 7708 90.8 604 85.1 7104 91.4
Time spent playing games with child b <0.001
Not at all 385 4.4 61 7.2 324 4.14
Approximately monthly 2226 26.5 154 22.2 2072 26.9
Weekly or more 5888 69.1 477 70.6 5411 68.9
Mother longstanding health condition b <0.001
No 6382 75.1 427 61.4 5955 76.4
Yes 2117 24.9 265 38.6 1852 23.6
Mother smoking <0.001
Non-smoker 6383 73.0 417 57.4 5966 74.6
Smoker 2116 27.0 275 42.6 1841 25.4
Mother’s alcohol intake b <0.001
5+ weekly 576 7.3 30 4.4 546 7.6
3–4 times weekly 1069 12.7 49 6.2 1020 13.3
1–2 times weekly 2445 28.1 172 23.8 2273 28.6
Monthly 3067 36.8 283 42.0 2784 36.3
Never/almost never 1342 15.2 158 23.7 1184 14.3
Time spent with friends outside school b <0.001
Not at all 440 5.1 94 13.2 346 4.23
Approximately monthly 1532 19.0 120 18.2 1412 19.0
Regularly, 1–3 times per week 4667 55.7 284 41.7 4383 5.71
Most days 1860 20.3 194 26.9 1666 19.64
Time playing sport b <0.001
Never/rarely 2334 29.1 313 47.2 2021 27.2
Once per week 2309 26.9 175 25.8 2134 26.9
2–3 times per week 3154 36.2 174 21.9 2980 3.8
≥4 times per week 702 7.8 30 5.1 672 8.1

Note: Unweighted counts and survey-weighted proportions reported; numbers may not add up to 8499 due to individuals with missing information; p-values calculated using chi-square tests

a

Information from wave 1 (age 9 months)

b

Information from wave 4 (age 7 years)

A total of 692 children (8.14%) were reported to have behavioural problems at age 11, measured by parent-reported TDS. Examining SDQ subscales, 10.68% of children had emotional difficulties, 10.54% peer related problems, 9.07% conduct issues, and 9.85% had hyperactivity or inattention. Only 1.48% of children were reported to have impaired pro-social skills. Teachers reported proportionately fewer socio-emotional problems than parents. Of the 4784 children for whom teacher-reported TDS was available, 6.22% were reported to have behavioural difficulties.

Using the Rutter Malaise Inventory, 14.2% of mothers reported high psychological distress at 9 months of child age. The proportion of mothers indicating medium to very high psychological distress, via the Kessler-6 scale, was markedly higher in subsequent surveys. When the cohort was 3, 5 and 7 years old, 33.8%, 32.5% and 32.7% of mothers reported medium or high psychological distress, respectively. At the age 11 survey, this increased to 41.6% of mothers. In this sample, 36.3% of mothers never reported psychological distress in the first five surveys. Looking at stages of child development, 6.1% of mothers reported psychological distress only in the early years, 7.2% only in the middle years, and 7.6% for the first time at child age 11. Only 4.7% of mothers reported significant distress at all five surveys, while 23.8% experienced recurrent psychological distress, and 14.3% demonstrated other patterns.

As shown in Table A1.2, most covariates, and notably maternal psychological distress, were predicted by family income. The same socio-economic pattern was found also when family income was replaced by poverty (results not shown).

Effect of permanent income

The marginal effects (ME) of permanent income on the six indicators of child behaviour at age 11 are summarised in Table 3, which shows the effect of a 1% increase in permanent family income on the likelihood of behaviour problems, in model specification 4. A 1% increase in family income was associated with 4.4% (SE 0.014) and 5.2% (SE 0.016) lower probabilities of behavioural problems being reported by parents and teachers, respectively. The effect of income was smaller in magnitude for the SDQ subscales and was not significant for emotional problems.

Table 3.

The impact of permanent family income and maternal psychological distress on childhood behavioural outcomes at age 11 years: logit models - partial effects (SE).

Parent TDSa,b Teacher TDSa,b Internalising c
Externalising c
Emotional problems a,b Peer problemsa,b Conduct problems a,b Hyperactivity/Inattention problems a,b
log of permanent income -0.044*** (0.014) -0.052*** (0.016) -0.022 (0.016) -0.037** (0.016) -0.039*** (0.014) -0.026* (0.014)
Maternal psychological distress patternd
Never #
Early years only 0.002 (0.013) 0.002 (0.016) 0.031** (0.015) 0.019 (0.016) -0.001 (0.013) 0.003 (0.014)
Middle years only 0.039**(0.014) 0.035*(0.017) 0.025* (0.013) 0.063***(0.015) 0.023 (0.015) 0.027** (0.012)
Current (age 11) only 0.035**(0.013) 0.033 (0.021) 0.075*** (0.017) 0.048***(0.017) 0.050*** (0.016) 0.056*** (0.014)
Other pattern 0.079**(0.012) 0.024*(0.011) 0.116*** (0.014) 0.088***(0.014) 0.060*** (0.010) 0.085*** (0.013)
Recurrent 0.071**(0.009) 0.015 (0.010) 0.089*** (0.011) 0.076***(0.011) 0.058*** (0.009) 0.077*** (0.010)
Persistent 0.116**(0.018) 0.026 (0.018) 0.150*** (0.023) 0.107***(0.021) 0.121*** (0.021) 0.115*** (0.020)
Sample size 8499 4784 8499 8499 8499 8499

Notes: # Reference case;

a

Partial effects with standard errors in parentheses

b

Each regression included standard socioeconomic controls, child health endowment variables, family and external factor variables (see Methods section on data and variable definitions for details)

c

Individual subscales of SDQ from parent-completed survey

d

Maternal psychological distress measured using Kessler-6 scale at wave 2–5 and Rutter Malaise Inventory at wave 1

*

Significant at the 10-percent level;

**

Significant at the 5-percent level;

***

Significant at the 1-percent level.

As shown in Table A1.1, the marginal effect of permanent income was attenuated in magnitude across model specifications, but it remained statistically significant at the 1% level. In model specification 1, a 1% increase in permanent income was associated with a reduction in the probability of parent-reported problem behaviour of 0.104 (SE 0.009, p<0.01). Accounting for sociodemographic and health endowment factors (model specification 2), the marginal effect of family income was -0.077 ± SE 0.013 (p<0.01). This effect was reduced by a further 24.7% (ME -0.058 ± SE 0.016, p<0.01) by adjusting for maternal psychological distress pattern (model specification 3). Model specification 4 showed a reduction in the likelihood of socio-emotional behaviour problems of 0.044 (SE 0.014, p<0.01) with each percentage increase in income.

There were distinct effects of family income on the different subscales of the SDQ (Table A2). The strength of association was substantially diminished across subsequent model specifications, with small in magnitude but statistically significant relationships seen for all but the emotional subscale in model specification 4.

As detailed in Table A3, the unadjusted marginal effect of income on teacher-assessed behavioural problems (model specification 1) was slightly weaker in magnitude than for parent-completed behavioural problems (ME -0.086 ± SE 0.012, p<0.01 vs. ME -0.104 ± SE 0.009, p<0.01). However, the association was not attenuated to the same extent as parent-reported TDS (Table A1.1) over the subsequent model specifications. When fully adjusted (model specification 4), a 1% increase in income was associated with a reduction in the probability of teacher-assessed problem behaviour of 0.052 (SE 0.016, p<0.01), compared to a 0.044 reduction for parent-reported TDS.

Effect of poverty

The partial effects of poverty across the five waves of the MCS on child socio-emotional behaviour at age 11 are shown in Table 4. The relationships between family poverty patterns and child socio-emotional behaviour are analogous to that of permanent income. Persistent poverty was associated with the greatest increase in the probability of behavioural problems at age 11, compared with never experiencing poverty. This was consistent for parent and teacher TDS, as well as internalising subscales, but the relationship was less significant for externalising problems. The association between poverty and child behavioural problems was greater in magnitude for teacher-reported than parent-reported TDS. For children who experienced poverty at 4 or 5 surveys, there was a partial effect of 0.061 (SE 0.020, p<0.01) for teacher-assessed behaviour, compared to 0.038 (SE 0.015, p<0.05) for parent-reported behaviour. The likelihood of behavioural problems, as reported by teachers, increased with poverty frequency. This pattern was not consistent with the analysis using parent-reported TDS.

Table 4.

The impact of family poverty and maternal psychological distress on childhood behavioural outcomes at age 11 years: logit models - partial effects (SE).

Parent TDSa,b Teacher TDSa,b Internalising c
Externalising c
Emotional problemsa,b Peer problemsa,b Conduct problems a,b Hyperactivity/Inattention problems a,b
Poverty pattern
Never#
Once only 0.023** (0.011) 0.010 (0.011) 0.041*** (0.014) 0.001 (0.011) -0.001 (0.013) 0.004 (0.011)
2–3 waves 0.018 (0.011) 0.033** (0.014) 0.023* (0.014) 0.033** (0.015) -0.004 (0.012) 0.008 (0.012)
4–5 waves 0.038** (0.015) 0.061*** (0.020) 0.061*** (0.019) 0.046** (0.019) 0.022 (0.016) 0.034* (0.017)


 

 

 

 

 


Maternal psychological distress patternd
Never #
Early years only 0.002 (0.013) 0.003 (0.016) 0.031** (0.015) 0.020 (0.016) 0.000 (0.013) 0.004 (0.014)
Middle years only 0.039*** (0.014) 0.037*(0.017) 0.026** (0.014) 0.064***(0.016) 0.024 (0.015) 0.027** (0.012)
Current (age 11) only 0.035*** (0.013) 0.035 (0.022) 0.076** (0.017) 0.048***(0.017) 0.052*** (0.016) 0.058*** (0.014)
Other pattern 0.080*** (0.012) 0.026*(0.011) 0.114*** (0.014) 0.089***(0.014) 0.062*** (0.011) 0.086*** (0.013)
Recurrent 0.073*** (0.009) 0.018*(0.010) 0.089*** (0.010) 0.078***(0.011) 0.060*** (0.009) 0.078*** (0.010)
Persistent 0.119*** (0.018) 0.029 (0.018) 0.149*** (0.023) 0.109***(0.021) 0.125*** (0.021) 0.117*** (0.020)
Sample size 8499 4784 8499 8499 8499 8499

Notes: # Reference case;

“Wave” denotes data collection points, i.e. waves 1, 2, 3, 4, and 5 collected at ages 9 months, 3, 5, 7, and 11 years respectively

Maternal psychological distress measured using Kessler-6 scale at wave 2–5 and Rutter Malaise Inventory at wave 1

a

Partial effects with standard errors in parentheses

b

Each regression included standard socioeconomic controls, child health endowment variables, family and external factor variables (see Methods section on data and variable definitions for details)

c

Individual subscales of SDQ from mother-completed survey

*

Significant at the 10-percent level;

**

Significant at the 5-percent level;

***

Significant at the 1-percent level.

Role of maternal psychological distress

Maternal distress pattern was an important mediating factor in the relationship between income and parent-reported child behaviour. The marginal effect of income in model specification 2 was attenuated by 24.7% after controlling for maternal psychological distress in model specification 3 (Table A1.1). The size and statistical significance of the association of poverty with parent-reported behaviour also decreased between model specifications 2 and 3. The partial effect of poverty was reduced by 18.2%, 22.9% and 21.3% in children who experienced poverty once, twice or three times, and four or five times over the period covered by the survey, respectively (Table A4). A similar pattern was observed in the income and poverty models with SDQ subscales (Table A2 and A5). The marginal effects of income on emotional and peer problems were reduced by 41.3% and 27.9%, respectively, between model specification 2 and 3. The association between income and conduct and hyperactivity/inattention problems was attenuated by 23.4% and 35.2%, respectively, between model specification 2 and 3.

Conversely, maternal psychological distress had little impact on the association between income and teacher-assessed child behaviour. The marginal effect of income was attenuated by only 8.0% between model specification 2 and 3 (Table A3).

Regarding the direct association of maternal psychological distress with parent-reported TDS (Table 3), children whose mothers were persistently distressed had 11.6% greater probability of being reported as having socio-emotional problems than children whose mothers had never reported psychological distress. Maternal psychological distress only in the early years (age 9 months and/or 3 years) of the child’s life was not significantly associated with problem behaviour at age 11. Persistent and recurrent distress patterns were also significantly associated with all four SDQ subscale models, with the largest effect on emotional symptoms.

Other covariates

A substantial mediating effect of family and external factors (representing parental stress and parental investment constructs) on the child behaviour-family income gradient was observed in model specification 4. The marginal effect of income on parent-assessed behaviour was reduced by 24.1% between model specifications 3 and 4 (Table A1.1). Similarly, the association of poverty with parent-reported behavioural problems became smaller in magnitude after adjusting for these factors.

There were statistically significant protective effects of both time spent with friends and siblings on behavioural issues for parent-reported TDS, internalising and externalising problems (Table A1.1 and A2). High frequency of punitive parenting practices, such as sending the child to their room, or confiscating belongings when naughty, was associated with higher levels of problem behaviour, particularly externalising problems (Table A2). In contrast, there was little effect of early life influences such as gestational age, birthweight, and mother’s education on behaviour at age 11 (Table A1.1).

Discussion

This study demonstrated that the relationship between permanent family income and socio-emotional behaviour holds at age 11. The results reinforce the importance of financial stability and a family’s material wealth in the behavioural outcomes of their child. The effect of a 1% (10%) increase in permanent income was associated with 7.7 (77) percentage points reduction in child behavioural problems when accounting for standard socio-demographic controls (model specification 2, Table A1.1). The direct effect of income was stronger for children of poorer families: at the 25th percentile of income distribution, a 1% increase in permanent income was associated with 9.3 percentage points reduction in child behavioural problems, and 5.6 percentage points at the 75th percentile. These results may be significant for policy makers, in that children may experience worse outcomes because they are poorer, not only through the effect of income on parenting and household characteristics. The income-child behaviour relationship remained significant even when accounting for factors known to influence child behaviour, which differs from findings of several studies involving children at younger ages (Yeung et al., 2002; Violato et al., 2011; Khanam & Nghiem, 2016; Dooley & Stewart, 2007; Kelly, Sacker, Del Bono, Francesconi & Marmot, 2011).

Unsurprisingly, the associations between poverty and child behavioural problems reinforced the results for income, more so as reported by teachers than parents. This finding is consistent with literature examining this relationship at younger child ages (Bradshaw and Holmes, 2008, Kiernan and Huerta, 2008, Kiernan and Mensah, 2009).

The emotional problems subscale was most weakly affected by family income, but most strongly associated with poverty. This may be driven by non-linearity in the family income effects, in that effects on emotional problems are strongest at the lower end of the income distribution. A 2013 systematic review (Cooper & Stewart, 2013) found strong evidence of a greater impact of money on behavioural and emotional outcomes of children whose families were poorest (Taylor et al., 2004, Dearing et al., 2001, Dearing et al., 2006, Akee et al., 2010, Isaacs and Magnuson, 2011). This finding is also consistent with Plewis and Kallis (2008) study using the MCS at child age 3 and Taylor et al. (2004) study of US children aged 1 to 3 years.

Consistent with findings in the UK and USA (Holmes and Kiernan, 2013, Wickham et al., 2017, Yeung et al., 2002, Schoon et al., 2010), there was a striking effect of maternal mental health on the relationship between family income and child behaviour. There were also marked differences in the size of the effect of maternal psychological distress with child behavioural problems depending on trajectories of distress over time. The overall pattern was reflective of both duration and temporality of mother’s psychological distress, exerting the strongest effect for the longest durations.

An interesting finding was the discrepancy in the association of maternal psychological distress with child behaviour between parent- and teacher-reported behaviour. It is possible that the constricted sample size for teacher-reported behaviour may not have provided adequate power to detect this effect, or that the children with available teacher-reported TDS were systematically different from those without. School quality could impact on teacher rating, which is tied to income and area of deprivation (Washbrook et al., 2014). Additionally, there is potential for bias in parent reports of behaviour (Fergusson et al., 1993). The analysis using teacher-reported TDS aimed to assess the extent of such bias. The parent-reported analysis was repeated for the restricted sample of children with teacher-completed TDS. For those children, a 1% increase in income was associated with a statistically significant reduction in probability of behavioural issues (ME -0.059 ± SE 0.016, p<0.01). This is similar to the teacher-reported effect of income (-0.052 ± SE 0.016, p<0.01). Mothers experiencing high psychological distress may perceive their children to be more difficult. Indeed, a positive association has been observed between maternal levels of distress, usually measured through self-report, and discrepancies between mothers’ assessment of their child and the ratings of other informants, such as teachers (Breslau et al., 1988, Collishaw et al., 2009). Najman et al. (2001) found that, in mothers rating their child’s behaviour, as the mother’s current emotional impairment increased, so did her reports of the child’s behavioural problems, compared with child self-reports. Nonetheless, the assessment of problem behaviour by teachers should be interpreted with a degree of caution. It is possible that a child's behaviour at school may be systematically different from that at home (Washbrook et al., 2014). Furthermore, there is invariable discrepancy between informants when evaluating youth behaviour (De Los Reyes and Kazdin, 2005). Agreement between informants tends to be better in younger children than older children, and in reporting externalising than internalising problems (De Los Reyes and Kazdin, 2005). There can be marked gender differences in behaviour assessment by teachers, as well as influences of socioeconomic status, academic attainment and neighbourhood disadvantage (Collishaw et al., 2009), the so called “negative halo effect” (Abikoff, Courtney, Pelham, and Koplewicz, 1993).

Family and external factors, representing the parental stress and investment frameworks, substantially mitigated the effect of income and poverty on children’s behaviour. This corroborates Australian findings (Khanam and Nghiem, 2016) and results from earlier surveys of the MCS (Kiernan & Huerta 2008; Violato et al., 2011). It suggests that it is factors affecting parents’ capacity to promote positive development, in combination with financial hardship, which contributes to variation in behaviour. Treating a mother’s mental health may be a beneficial way to also improve child behaviour, as shown in a study by Creswell et al. (2015). Identification of key mediating mechanisms through further research may guide more service-oriented policy, beyond what income distribution itself can achieve. Our findings also support the importance of peer relationships in child behavioural development, as time with friends consistently emerged as beneficial for socio-emotional behaviour. There is potential for a degree of reverse causality, in that disruptive or antisocial children may seek fewer friendships, or be excluded by their peers (Laird, Jordan, Dodge, Pettit & Bates, 2001).

The study findings need to be interpreted considering some limitations. Firstly, loss to follow up and the list-wise deletion approach of the study meant that the sample size was reduced by almost half of the original cohort. It may be possible that children who were not present at all five surveys are systematically different to those for whom we had complete data. Loss to follow up, however, is commonplace in longitudinal cohort designs and some previous studies using MCS data in similar areas of investigation indicated that the latter circumstance does not bias results (Wickham et al., 2017; Violato et al., 2011; Violato et al., 2009). Secondly, the role of fathers has not been explored in this study. Violato et al., (2011) assessed the impact of fathers in the relationship between income and child development, finding that paternal attitudes towards parenting and their involvement in child-rearing were significantly associated with child behaviour. Thirdly, we included maternal psychological distress at age 11 and family income at age 11 to operationalise the maternal psychological distress trajectory and the permanent income measure, respectively. However, there may be potential for reverse causality and the results need to be interpreted with caution in light of this potential limitation. Also, the number of sibling variable was measured when the child was aged 7, but income may affect family planning and number of children, which is a potential further limitation.

Given the wealth of data collected in the MCS, a range of important covariates were included in the model, reducing residual unobserved heterogeneity. However, unobserved family background characteristics may lead to inflated effects as they potentially increase family income and reduce the likelihood of socio-emotional problems. Area-level socioeconomic data would be a valuable inclusion in future analysis using the MCS in order to further control for social deprivation. Previous papers examining parental income-child outcome relationships have used instrumental variable (IV), fixed-effect methods, and natural experiments to correct for this endogeneity, but those were beyond the scope of this study. Future studies using the MCS would warrant use of these methods. Finally, although associations between income and child behaviour have been observed, these effects may not be causal.

Conclusions and policy implications

This work contributes to an established, albeit limited, body of literature in the health economics discipline. The findings highlight the importance of maternal mental health in the child behaviour-family income relationship and have potential implications for priorities in the treatment of childhood socio-emotional problems.

Consistent with developmental psychology literature (Yeung et al., 2002), there were strong associations between mothers’ psychological distress and parent-reported behavioural problems, with greater likelihood of difficulties with more persistent psychological distress. These results may validate calls for further financial and psychosocial supports for families affected by parental mental health issues.

The issue of child poverty is particularly topical, with UK levels increasing for the first time in almost two decades (Social Mobility and Child Poverty Commission, 2017). As of 2013-14, 3.7 million children were living in poverty (Department for Work and Pensions, 2015), but former child poverty targets were abandoned in 2015 and the dedicated Child Poverty Unit was abolished in 2016 (Merrick, 2017). Proposed reductions to tax credits and benefits are also likely to reduce the income of some households considerably (Taylor-Robinson et al., 2015). In early 2016, the Social Mobility and Child Poverty Commission compared the prospects of UK children from poorer backgrounds in terms of academic attainment, future employment and standard of living, demonstrating considerable differences in terms of life chances (Social Mobility and Child Poverty Commission, 2017). This study supports these findings from the perspective of child behavioural problems, and calls for improved government intervention to ameliorate health and developmental outcomes of children from deprived backgrounds.

Ethical statement

This study was a secondary analysis of the Millennium Cohort Study, using de-identified data available in the public domain. As the project falls within the remit of the original parent ethics approval (Hansen K (editor). Millennium Cohort Study: A Guide to the Datasets (Eighth Edition) First, Second, Third, Fourth and Fifth Surveys. London: Centre for Longitudinal Studies, 2014) no additional ethics clearance was required.

Footnotes

All authors report no conflicts of interest relevant to this article.

☆☆

This research was not funded by external funding bodies.

Appendix A

Supplementary data associated with this article can be found in the online version at doi:10.1016/j.ssmph.2018.03.002.

Appendix A. Supplementary material

Supplementary material

mmc1.docx (125.3KB, docx)

.

References

  1. Currie J., Decker S., Lin W. Has public health insurance for older children reduced disparities in access to care and health outcomes? Journal of Health Economics. 2008;27(6):1567–1581. doi: 10.1016/j.jhealeco.2008.07.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Lundborg P., Nilsson A., Rooth d-O. Adolescent health and adult labor market outcomes. Journal of Health Economics. 2014;37:25–40. doi: 10.1016/j.jhealeco.2014.05.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Social Mobility and Child Poverty Commission (2017). The Social Mobility Index [Internet]. 2016 [cited August 10]. Available from: 〈https://www.gov.uk/government/publications/social-mobility-index〉.
  4. Social Mobility and Child Poverty Commission (2015). State of the Nation 2015 report [Internet] [cited 2017 August 10]. Available from: 〈https://www.gov.uk/government/news/state-of-the-nation-2015-report-published〉.
  5. Merrick R. (2017). Theresa May accused of abandoning fight against child poverty, after axing Whitehall unit devoted to it 2016 [cited August 10]. Available from: 〈http://www.independent.co.uk/news/uk/politics/theresa-may-child-poverty-unit-axe-whitehall-department-work-pensions-dwp-prime-minister-a7486131.html〉.
  6. Najman J.M., Aird R., Bor W., O’Callaghan M., Williams G.M., Shuttlewood G.J. The generational transmission of socioeconomic inequalities in child cognitive development and emotional health. Social Science Medicine. 2004;58(6):1147–1158. doi: 10.1016/s0277-9536(03)00286-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Fergusson D.M., Horwood L.J., Ridder E.M. Show me the child at seven: The consequences of conduct problems in childhood for psychosocial functioning in adulthood. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2005;46(8):837–849. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00387.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Goodman A., Lamping D., Ploubidis G. When to use broader internalising and externalising subscales instead of the hypothesised five subscales on the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ): Data from British parents. Teachers and Children. 2010:1179–1191. doi: 10.1007/s10802-010-9434-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Currie J., Stabile M. Socioeconomic status and child health: Why is the relationship stronger for older children? The American Economic Review. 2003;93(5):1813–1823. doi: 10.1257/000282803322655563. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Condliffe S., Link C.R. The relationship between economic status and child health: Evidence from the United States. The American Economic Review. 2008;98(4):1605–1618. doi: 10.1257/aer.98.4.1605. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Khanam R., Nghiem H.S., Connelly L.B. Child health and the income gradient: Evidence from Australia. Journal of Health Economics. 2009;28(4):805–817. doi: 10.1016/j.jhealeco.2009.05.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Fletcher J., Wolfe B. Increasing our understanding of the health-income gradient in children. Health Economics. 2014;23(4):473–486. doi: 10.1002/hec.2969. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Murasko J.E. An evaluation of the age-profile in the relationship between household income and the health of children in the United States. Journal of Health Economics. 2008;27(6):1489–1502. doi: 10.1016/j.jhealeco.2008.07.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Fletcher J., Wolfe B. (2016). The importance of family income in the formation and evolution of non-cognitive skills in childhood. In National Bureau of Economic Research, editor. NBER Working Paper Series. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  15. Conti G., Heckman J. (2012). The economics of child well-being. In National Bureau of Economic Research, editor. NBER Working Paper Series.
  16. Case A., Lubotsky D., Paxson C. Economic status and health in childhood: The origins of the gradient. The American Economic Review. 2002;92(5):1308–1334. doi: 10.1257/000282802762024520. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Propper C., Rigg J., Burgess S. Child health: Evidence on the roles of family income and maternal mental health from a UK birth cohort. Health Economics. 2007;16(11):1245–1269. doi: 10.1002/hec.1221. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Currie A., Shields M.A., Price S.W. The child health/family income gradient: Evidence from England. Journal of Health Economics. 2007;26(2):213–232. doi: 10.1016/j.jhealeco.2006.08.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Brooks-Gunn J., Duncan G.T. The effects of poverty on children. The Future of Children. 1997;7(2):55–71. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Taylor B.A., Dearing E., McCartney K. Incomes and outcomes in early childhood. The Journal of Human Resources. 2004;29(4) [Google Scholar]
  21. Bradshaw J., Holmes J. Social Policy Research Unit: University of York; 2008. Family poverty assessed at three years old. [Google Scholar]
  22. Kiernan K.E., Huerta M.C. Economic deprivation, maternal depression, parenting and children’s cognitive and emotional development in early childhood. British Journal of Sociology. 2008;59(4):783–806. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-4446.2008.00219.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Korenman S., Miller J.E., Sjaastad J.E. Long-term poverty and child development in the United States: Results from the NLSY. Children and Youth Services Review. 1995;17(1/2):127–155. [Google Scholar]
  24. Kiernan K.E., Mensah F.K. Poverty, maternal depression, family status and children’s cognitive and behavioural development in early childhood: A longitudinal study. Journal of Social Policy. 2009;38(04):569. [Google Scholar]
  25. Blau D.M. The effect of income on child development. The Review of Economics and Statistics. 1999;81(2):261–276. [Google Scholar]
  26. Curtis L.J., Dooley M.D., Lipman E.L., Feeny D.H. The role of permanent income and family structure in the determination of child health in Canada. Health Economics. 2001;10(4):287–302. doi: 10.1002/hec.591. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Ackerman B.P., Brown E.D., Izard C.E. The relations between persistent poverty and contextual risk and children’s behavior in elementary school. Development Psychology. 2004;40(3):367–377. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.40.3.367. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Holmes J., Kiernan K. Persistent poverty and children’s development in the early years of childhood. Policy Politics. 2013;41(1):19–42. [Google Scholar]
  29. Fitzsimons E., Goodman A., Kelly E., Smith J.P. Poverty dynamics and parental mental health: Determinants of childhood mental health in the UK. Social Science and Medicine. 2017;175:43–51. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2016.12.040. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Wickham S., Whitehead M., Taylor-Robinson D., Barr B. The effect of a transition into poverty on child and maternal mental health: A longitudinal analysis of the UK Millennium Cohort Study. Lancet Public Heatlh. 2017;2:e141–e148. doi: 10.1016/S2468-2667(17)30011-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Yeung W.J., Linver M.R., Brooks-Gunn J. How money matters for young children’s development: Parental investment and family processes. Child Development. 2002;73(6):1861–1879. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.t01-1-00511. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Duncan G.T., Magnuson K., Votruba-Drzal E. Boosting family income to promote child development. The Future of Children. 2014;24(1):99–120. doi: 10.1353/foc.2014.0008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Violato M., Petrou S., Gray R., Redshaw M. Family income and child cognitive and behavioural development in the United Kingdom: does money matter? Health Econ. 2011;20(10):1201–1225. doi: 10.1002/hec.1665. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Khanam R., Nghiem S. Family income and child cognitive and noncognitive development in Australia: Does money matter? Demography. 2016;53(3):597–621. doi: 10.1007/s13524-016-0466-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Akee R., Simeonova E., Costello E., Copeland W. (2015). How does household income affect child personality traits and behaviors? In National Bureau of Economic Research, editor. NBER Working Paper Series. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  36. Schoon I., Hope S., Ross A., Duckworth K. Family hardship and children’s development: The early years. Longitudinal and Life Course Studies. 2010;1(3):209–222. [Google Scholar]
  37. Washbrook E., Gregg P., Propper C. A decomposition analysis of the relationship between parental income and multiple child outcomes. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society. 2014;177:4. [Google Scholar]
  38. Mensah F.K., Kiernan K.E. Maternal general health and children’s cognitive development and behaviour in the early years: Findings from the Millennium Cohort Study. Child Care Health Dev. 2010;37(1):44–54. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2214.2010.01150.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Hansen K. (editor). Millennium Cohort Study: A Guide to the Datasets (Eighth Edition) First, Second, Third, Fourth and Fifth Surveys. London: Centre for Longitudinal Studies, 2014.
  40. Grossman M. The human capital model. In: Culyer A.J., Newhouse J., editors. The handbook of health economics. Elsevier; North-Holland, Amsterdam: 2000. [Google Scholar]
  41. Jacobsen L. The family as producer of health – an extended grossman model. Journal of Health Economics. 2000;19(5):611–637. doi: 10.1016/s0167-6296(99)00041-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Dooley M., Stewart J. Family income, parenting styles and child behavioural-emotional outcomes. Health Econcomy. 2007;16(2):145–162. doi: 10.1002/hec.1142. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Office for National Statistics (2016). Average Weekly Earnings time series dataset [Internet]. 2016 [cited July 30]. Available from: 〈http://www.ons.gov.uk/employmentandlabourmarket/peopleinwork/earningsandworkinghours/datasets/averageweeklyearnings〉.
  44. Thompson G. Statistical literacy guide – How to adjust for inflation. House of Commons Library. 2009 [Google Scholar]
  45. Flouri E., Midouhas E., Joshi H. Family poverty and trajectories of children’s emotional and behavioural problems: The moderating roles of self-regulation and verbal cognitive ability. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2014;42(6):1043–1056. doi: 10.1007/s10802-013-9848-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Goodman R. The strengths and difficulties questionnaire: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 1997;38(5):581–586. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1997.tb01545.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Goodman R. Psychometric properties of the strengths and difficulties questionnaire. Journal of the American Academy of Child Adolescent Psychiatry. 2001;40(11):1337–1345. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200111000-00015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Scoring the SDQ [Internet] (2017). Youthinmind Ltd; 2016 [cited August 10] [Available from: 〈http://www.sdqinfo.org/py/sdqinfo/c0.py〉.
  49. Fergusson D., Lynskey M., Horwood L. The effect of maternal depression on maternal ratings of child behavior. An Official Publication of the International Society for Research in Child and Adolescent Psychopathology. 1993;21(3):245–269. doi: 10.1007/BF00917534. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Breslau N., Davis G.C., Prabucki K. Depressed mothers as informants in family history research—Are they accurate? Psychiatry Research. 1988;24(3):345–359. doi: 10.1016/0165-1781(88)90115-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Kessler R.C., Andrews G., Colpe L.J., Hiripi E., Mroczek D.K., Normand S.-L.T. Short screening scales to monitor population prevalences and trends in non-specific psychological distress. Psychological Medicine. 2002;32(06):959–976. doi: 10.1017/s0033291702006074. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Schoon I., Sacker A., Hope S., Collishaw S., Maughan B. Children’s development in the family environment. In: Dex S., Joshi H., editors. Children of the 21st century – From birth to nine months. The UK Millennium Cohort Study Series. The Policy Press; Bristol, United Kingdom: 2005. pp. 159–174. [Google Scholar]
  53. Johnson J., editor. Millennium Cohort Study Psychological, Developmental and Health Inventories. Developmental and Health Inventories, Centre for Longitudinal Studies: Institute of Education, University of London; 2012. [Google Scholar]
  54. Jones E, Gutman L, Platt L. Family stressors and children's outcomes. Childhood Wellbeing Research Centre, 2013.
  55. Biddle S.J.H., Asare M. Physical activity and mental health in children and adolescents: A review of reviews. British Journal of Sports Medicine. 2011;45(11):886–895. doi: 10.1136/bjsports-2011-090185. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Kremer P., Elshaug C., Leslie E., Toumbourou J.W., Patton G.C., Williams J. Physical activity, leisure-time screen use and depression among children and young adolescents. Journal of Science and Medicine in Sport/Sports Medicine Australia. 2014;17(2):183–187. doi: 10.1016/j.jsams.2013.03.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Millennium Cohort Study Initial findings from the Age 11 survey (2014). London: Centre for Longitudinal Studies.
  58. Ketende S., Jones E. Centre for Longitudinal Studies, Institute of Education, University of London; London: 2011. The Millennium Cohort Study – user guide to analysing MCS data using Stata. [Google Scholar]
  59. StataCorp . StataCorp LP; College Station, TX: 2011. Stata Statistical Software: Release 12. [Google Scholar]
  60. Baron R.M., Kenny D.A. The moderator–mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1986;51(6):1173–1182. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.51.6.1173. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Kelly Y., Sacker A., Del Bono E., Francesconi M., Marmot M. What role for the home learning environment and parenting in reducing the socioeconomic gradient in child development? Findings from the Millennium Cohort Study. Archives of Disease in Childhood. 2011;96(9):832–837. doi: 10.1136/adc.2010.195917. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Cooper K., Stewart K. Does money affect children’s outcomes? A systematic review. Joseph Rowntree Foundation. 2013 [Google Scholar]
  63. Dearing E., McCartney K., Taylor B. Change in family income-to-needs matters more for children with less. Child Development. 2001;72(6):1779–1793. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00378. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Dearing E., McCartney K., Taylor B. Within-child associations between family income and externalizing and internalizing problems. Developmenal Psychology. 2006;42:237–252. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.42.2.237. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Akee R.K.Q., Copeland W.E., Keeler G., Angold A., Costello E.J. Parents' incomes and children's outcomes: A quasi-experiment using transfer payments from casino profits. American Economic Journal: Applied Economics. 2010;2:86–115. doi: 10.1257/app.2.1.86. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Isaacs J., Magnuson K. Center on Children and Families at Brookings; Washington, DC: 2011. Income and education as predictors of children's school readiness. [Google Scholar]
  67. Plewis I., Kallis C. Department of Work and Pensions: Centre for Longitudinal Studies, University of London; 2008. Changing economic circumstances in childhood and their effects on subsequent educational and other outcomes. [Google Scholar]
  68. Collishaw S., Goodman R., Ford T., Rabe-Hesketh S., Pickles A. How far are associations between child, family and community factors and child psychopathology informant-specific and informant-general? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2009;50(5):571–580. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.02026.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Najman J.M., Williams G.M., Nikles J., Spence S., Bor W., O’Callaghan M. Bias influencing maternal reports of child behaviour and emotional state. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology. 2001;36(4):186–194. doi: 10.1007/s001270170062. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. De Los Reyes A., Kazdin A.E. Informant discrepancies in the assessment of childhood psychopathology: A critical review, theoretical framework, and recommendations for further study. Psychology Bulletin. 2005:483–509. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.131.4.483. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Abikoff H., Courtney M., Pelham W.E., Koplewicz H.S. Teachers' ratings of disruptive behaviors: The influence of halo effects. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 1993;21(5):519–533. doi: 10.1007/BF00916317. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Creswell C., Cruddace S., Gerry S., Gitau R., McIntosh E., Mollison J. (2015)., et al. Treatment of childhood anxiety disorder in the context of maternal anxiety disorder: a randomised controlled trial and economic analysis. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  73. Laird R.D., Jordan K.Y., Dodge K.A., Pettit G.S., Bates J.E. Peer rejection in childhood, involvement with antisocial peers in early adolescence, and the development of externalizing behavior problems. Development and Psychopathology. 2001;13(2):337–354. doi: 10.1017/s0954579401002085. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Violato M., Petrou S., Gray R. The relationship between household income and childhood respiratory health in the United Kingdom. Soc. Sci. Med. 2009;69(6):955–963. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2009.06.036. (1982) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Department for Work and Pensions (2015). Households Below Average Income, An analysis of the income distribution 1994/95–2013/14, Tables 4a and 4b. 〈https://www.gov.uk/government/statistics/〉.
  76. Taylor-Robinson D., Wickham S., Barr B. Child health at risk from welfare cuts. BMJ. 2015:351. doi: 10.1136/bmj.h5330. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplementary material

mmc1.docx (125.3KB, docx)

Articles from SSM - Population Health are provided here courtesy of Elsevier

RESOURCES