Skip to main content
Applied Psychological Measurement logoLink to Applied Psychological Measurement
. 2016 Nov 16;41(3):178–194. doi: 10.1177/0146621616677520

Critical Values for Yen’s Q3: Identification of Local Dependence in the Rasch Model Using Residual Correlations

Karl Bang Christensen 1,, Guido Makransky 2, Mike Horton 3
PMCID: PMC5978551  PMID: 29881087

Abstract

The assumption of local independence is central to all item response theory (IRT) models. Violations can lead to inflated estimates of reliability and problems with construct validity. For the most widely used fit statistic Q3, there are currently no well-documented suggestions of the critical values which should be used to indicate local dependence (LD), and for this reason, a variety of arbitrary rules of thumb are used. In this study, an empirical data example and Monte Carlo simulation were used to investigate the different factors that can influence the null distribution of residual correlations, with the objective of proposing guidelines that researchers and practitioners can follow when making decisions about LD during scale development and validation. A parametric bootstrapping procedure should be implemented in each separate situation to obtain the critical value of LD applicable to the data set, and provide example critical values for a number of data structure situations. The results show that for the Q3 fit statistic, no single critical value is appropriate for all situations, as the percentiles in the empirical null distribution are influenced by the number of items, the sample size, and the number of response categories. Furthermore, the results show that LD should be considered relative to the average observed residual correlation, rather than to a uniform value, as this results in more stable percentiles for the null distribution of an adjusted fit statistic.

Keywords: local dependence, Rasch model, Yen’s Q3, residual correlations, Monte Carlo simulation

Introduction

Statistical independence of two variables implies that knowledge about one variable does not change the expectations about another variable. Thus, test items, X1,,XI, are not independent, because a student giving a correct answer to one test item would change the expectation of his or her probability of also giving a correct answer to another item in the same test. A fundamental assumption in the Rasch (1960) model and in other item response theory (IRT) models is that item responses are conditionally independent given the latent variable:

P(X1=x1,,XI=xI|θ)=Πi=1IP(Xi=xi|θ).

The items should only be correlated through the latent trait that the test is measuring (Lord & Novick, 1968). This is generally referred to as local independence (Lazarsfeld & Henry, 1968).

The assumptions of local independence can be violated through response dependency and multidimensionality, and these violations are often referred to under the umbrella term of “local dependence” (LD). Both of these situations yield interitem correlations beyond what can be attributed to the latent variable, but for very different reasons. Response dependency occurs when items are linked in some way, such that the response on one item governs the response on another because of similarities in, for example, item content or response format. A typical example is where several walking items are included in the same scale. If a person can walk several miles without difficulty, then that person must be able to walk 1 mile, or any lesser distance, without difficulty (Tennant & Conaghan, 2007). This is a structural dependency which is inherent within the items, because there is no other logical way in which a person may validly respond. Another form of LD could be caused by a redundancy–dependency, where the degree of overlap within the content of items is such that the items are not independent (i.e., where the same question is essentially asked twice, using slightly different language or synonymous descriptive words). Yen (1993) offered an in-depth discussion of ways that the format and presentation of items can cause LD.

Violation of the local independence assumption through multidimensionality is typically seen for instruments composed of bundles of items that measure different aspects of the latent variable or different domains of a broader latent construct. In this case, the higher order latent variable alone might not account for correlation between items in the same bundle.

Violations of local independence in a unidimensional scale will influence estimation of person parameters and can lead to inflated estimates of reliability and problems with construct validity. Consequences of LD have been described in detail elsewhere (Lucke, 2005; Marais, 2009; Marais & Andrich, 2008a; Scott & Ip, 2002; Yen, 1993). Ignoring LD in a unidimensional scale thus leads to reporting of inflated reliability giving a false impression of the accuracy and precision of estimates (Marais, 2013). For a discussion of the effect of multidimensionality on estimates of reliability, see Marais and Andrich (2008b).

Detecting LD

One of the earliest methods for detecting LD in the Rasch model is the fit measure Q2 (van den Wollenberg, 1982), which was derived from contingency tables and used the sufficiency properties of the Rasch model. Kelderman (1984) expressed the Rasch model as a log-linear model in which LD can be shown to correspond to interactions between items. Log-linear Rasch models have also been considered by Haberman (2007) and by Kreiner and Christensen (2004, 2007), who proposed to test for LD by evaluating partial correlations using approach similar to the Mantel–Haenszel analysis of differential item functioning (DIF; Holland & Thayer, 1988). The latter approach is readily implemented in standard software such as SAS or SPSS. Notably, Kreiner and Christensen (2007) argued that the log-linear Rasch models proposed by Kelderman that incorporate LD still provide essentially valid and objective measurement and described the measurement properties of such models. Furthermore, a way of quantifying LD has been proposed by Andrich and Kreiner (2010) for two dichotomous items. It is based on splitting a dependent item into two new ones, according to the responses to the other item within the dependent pair. LD is then easily quantified by estimating the difference d between the item locations of the two new items. However, Andrich and Kreiner do not go on to investigate whether d is statistically significant. For the partial credit model (Masters, 1982) and the rating scale model (Andrich, 1978), a generalized version of this methodology exists (Andrich, Humphry, & Marais, 2012).

Beyond the Rasch model, Yen (1984) proposed the Q3 statistic for detecting LD in the three parameter logistics (3PL) model. This fit statistic is based on the item residuals,

di=XiE(Xi|θ^),

and computed as the Pearson correlation (taken over examinees),

Q3,ij=rdidj,

where di and dj are item residuals for items i and j, respectively. This method is often used for the Rasch model, the partial credit model, and the rating scale model.

Chen and Thissen (1997) discussed X2 and G2 LD statistics that, although not more powerful than the Q3, have null distributions very similar to the chi-square distribution with one degree of freedom. Other methods for detecting LD are standardized bivariate residuals for dichotomous or multinomial IRT models (Maydeu-Olivares & Liu, 2015), the use of conditional covariances (Douglas, Kim, Habing, & Gao, 1998), or the use of Mantel–Haenszel type tests (Ip, 2001). Tests based on parametric models are also a possibility: Glas and Suarez-Falcon (2003) proposed Lagrange multiplier (LM) tests based on a threshold shift model, but bifactor models (Liu & Thissen, 2012, 2014), specification of other models that incorporate LD (Hoskens & De Boeck, 1997; Ip, 2002), or limited information goodness-of-fit tests (Liu & Maydeu-Olivares, 2013) are also possible.

The Use of the Q3 Fit Statistic

Yen’s Q3 is probably the most often reported index in published Rasch analyses due to its inclusion (in the form of the residual correlation matrix) in widely used software such as RUMM (Andrich, Sheridan, & Luo, 2010). Yen (1984) argued that if the IRT model is correct, then the distribution of the Q3 is known, and proposed that p values could be based on the Fisher (1915)z-transform. Chen and Thissen (1997) stated, “In using Q3 to screen items for local dependence, it is more common to use a uniform critical value of an absolute value of 0.2 for the Q3 statistic itself” (pp. 284-285). They went on to present results showing that, although the sampling distribution under the Rasch model is bell shaped, it is not well approximated by the standard normal distribution, especially in the tails (Chen & Thissen, 1997, Figure 3).

Figure 3.

Figure 3.

The empirical 95th and 99th percentiles in the empirical distribution of Q3,* for dichotomous items.

Note. Gray horizontal dashed lines indicate 0.2 and 0.3, respectively.

In practical applications of the Q3 test statistic researchers will often compute the complete correlation matrix of residuals and look at the maximum value:

Q3,max=maxi>jQ3,ij.

Critical Values of Residual Correlations

When investigating LD based on Yen’s Q3, residuals for any pair of items should be uncorrelated, and generally close to zero. Residual correlations that are high indicate a violation of the local independence assumption, and this suggests that the pair of items have something more in common than the rest of the item set have in common with each other (Marais, 2013).

As noted by Yen (1984), a negative bias is built into Q3. This problem is due to the fact that measures of association will be biased away from zero even though the assumption of local independence applies, due to the conditioning on a proxy variable instead of the latent variable (Rosenbaum, 1984). A second problem is that the way the residuals are computed induces a bias (Kreiner & Christensen, 2011). Marais (2013) recognized that the sampling properties among residuals are unknown; therefore, these statistics cannot be used for formal tests of LD. A third, and perhaps the most important, problem in applications is that there are currently no well-documented suggestions of the critical values which should be used to indicate LD, and for this reason, arbitrary rules of thumb are used when evaluating whether an observed correlation is such that it can be reasonably supposed to have arisen from random sampling.

Standards often reported in the literature include looking at fit residuals over the critical value of 0.2, as proposed by Chen and Thissen (1997). For examples of this, see Reeve et al. (2007); Hissbach, Klusmann, and Hampe (2011); Makransky and Bilenberg (2014); and Makransky, Rogers, and Creed (2014). However, other critical values are also used, and there seems to be a wide variation in what is seen as indicative of dependence. Marais and Andrich (2008a) investigated dependence at a critical value of 0.1, but a value of 0.3 has also often been used (see, for example, das Nair, Moreton, & Lincoln, 2011; La Porta et al., 2011; Ramp, Khan, Misajon, & Pallant, 2009; Røe, Damsgård, Fors, & Anke, 2014), and critical values of 0.5 (Davidson, Keating, & Eyres, 2004; Ten Klooster, Taal, & van de Laar, 2008) and even 0.7 (González-de Paz et al., 2015) can be found in use.

There are two fundamental problems with this use of standard critical values: (a) there is limited evidence of their validity and often no reference of where values come from, and (b) they are not sensitive to specific characteristics of the data.

Marais (2013) not only identified that the residual correlations are difficult to directly interpret confidently when there are fewer than 20 items in the item set but also stated that the correlations should always be considered relative to the overall set of correlations. This is because of the magnitude of a residual correlation value, which indicates LD will vary depending on the number of items in a data set. Instead of an absolute critical value, Marais (2013) suggested that residual correlation values should be compared with the average item residual correlation of the complete data set to give a truer picture of the LD within a data set. It was concluded that when diagnosing response dependence, item residual correlations should be considered relative to each other and in light of the number of items, although there is no indication of a relative critical value (above the average residual correlation) that could indicate LD.

Thus, under the null hypothesis, the average correlation of residuals is negative (cf. Marais, 2013) and, ideally, observed correlations between residuals in a data set should be evaluated with reference to this average value. Marais proposes to evaluate them with reference to the average of the observed correlations rather than the average under the null hypothesis. Thus, following Marais, the average value of the observed correlations could be considered:

Q¯3=(I2)1i>jQ3,ij,

where (I2) is the number of item pairs and defines the test statistic:

Q3,*=Q3,maxQ¯3,

that compares the largest observed correlation with average of the observed correlations.

The problem with the currently used critical values is that they are neither theoretically nor empirically based. Researchers and practitioners faced with making scale validation, and development decisions need to know what level of LD could be expected, given the properties of their items and data.

A possible solution would be to use a parametric bootstrap approach and simulate the residual correlation matrix several times under the assumption of fit to the Rasch model. This would provide information about the level of residual correlation that could be expected for the particular case, given that the Rasch model fits. To the authors’ knowledge, there is no existing research that describes how important characteristics such as the number of items, number of response categories, number of respondents, the distribution of items and persons, and the targeting of the items affect residual correlations expected, given fit to the Rasch model. In the current study, the possibility of identifying critical values of LD is investigated by examining the distribution of Q3 under the null hypothesis, where the data fit the model. This is done using an empirical example along with a simulation study.

Given the existence of the wide range of fit statistics with known sampling distributions outlined above, it is surprising that Rasch model applications abound with reporting of Q3 using arbitrary cut-points without theoretical or empirical justification. The reason for this is that the theoretically sound LD indices are not included in the software packages used by practitioners. For this reason, this article presents extensive simulation studies that will (a) illustrate that Q3 should be interpreted with caution and (b) allow researchers to know what level of LD could be expected, given properties of their items and data. Furthermore, these simulation studies will be used to study whether the maximum correlation, or the difference between the maximum correlation and the average correlation, as suggested by Marais (2013), is the most informative. Thus, the objectives of this article are (a) to provide an overview of the influence of different factors upon the null distribution of residual correlations and (b) to propose guidelines that researchers and practitioners can follow when making decisions about LD during scale development and validation. Two different situations are addressed: first, the situation where the test statistic is computed for all item pairs and only the strongest evidence (the largest correlation) is considered, and second, the less common case where only a single a priori defined item pair is considered.

Simulation Study

Methods

The simulated data sets used are as follows: (a) I dichotomous items simulated from

P(Xi=x|θ)=exp(x(θβi))1+exp(θβi)(i=1,,I),

with evenly spaced item difficulties βi ranging from −2 to 2,

βi=2(i1I1)(i=1,,I),

or (b) I polytomous items simulated from

P(Xi=x|θ)=exp(h=1x(θβih))1+l=13exp(h=1l(θβih))(x=0,1,2,3;i=1,,I),

with item parameters defined by

βih=2(i1I1)+(h1)(i=1,,I;h=1,2,3).

The person locations were simulated from a normal distribution with mean μ and SD 1. All combinations of the four conditions—(a) number of items (I = 10, 15, 20), (b) number of persons (N = 200, 250, . . . , 1,000), (c) number of response categories (two, four), and (d) mean value in the distribution of the latent variable θ (μ = 0, 2)—were simulated. This yielded 204 different setups, and for each of these, 10,000 data sets were simulated and the steps followed to find the empirical 95th and 99th percentiles:

  1. Estimating item parameters using pairwise conditional estimation (Andrich & Luo, 2003; Zwinderman, 1995),

  2. Estimating person parameters using weighted maximum likelihood (WML; Warm, 1989),

  3. Computing the residuals (Equation 2),

  4. Computing the empirical correlation matrix,

  5. Extracting the largest value from the correlation matrix.

Note that only data sets under the null hypothesis are simulated; there is no LD in the simulated data sets.

Results

Figure 1 reports the empirical 95th and 99th percentiles in the empirical distribution of the maximum residual correlation for dichotomous items. The top panel shows μ = 0 (labeled “good targeting”) and the bottom panel shows μ = 2 (labeled “bad targeting”). The reason for this labeling is that the average of the item locations (the item difficulties) is zero.

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

The empirical 95th and 99th percentiles in the empirical distribution of Q3,max for dichotomous items.

Note. Gray horizontal dashed lines indicate 0.2 and 0.3, respectively.

The percentiles decrease as the sample size increases, and they increase with the number of items. The latter finding is hardly surprising in a comparison of the maximum of 45, 105, and 190 item pairs, respectively. However, it is evident that the targeting does not have an impact on the percentiles. Figure 2 reports the empirical 95th and 99th percentiles in the empirical distribution of the maximum residual correlation for polytomous items. Again the top panel labeled “good targeting” shows μ = 0 and the bottom panel labeled “bad targeting” shows μ = 2.

Figure 2.

Figure 2.

The empirical 95th and 99th percentiles in the empirical distribution of Q3,max for polytomous items.

Note. Gray horizontal dashed lines indicate 0.2 and 0.3, respectively.

For N = 200, some of these percentiles were very large. Again, the percentiles decrease as sample size increases and the mean μ had little impact on the percentiles.

When item pairs are considered individually and computed the empirical distribution Q3 for selected item pair, there was quite a big difference across item pairs and, again, the percentiles decrease as sample size increases while the mean μ had little impact on the percentiles. Comparing the percentiles in the distribution of the correlation for a single a priori specified item pair shows that percentiles increase with the number of items (results not shown). Thus, the above finding that the percentiles in the distribution of the maximum correlation increase with the number of items is not solely due to the increase in the number of item pairs. Figures 3 and 4 show the empirical distribution of Q3,* for dichotomous and polytomous items, respectively.

Figure 4.

Figure 4.

The empirical 95th and 99th percentiles in the empirical distribution of Q3,* for polytomous items.

Note. Gray horizontal dashed lines indicate 0.2 and 0.3, respectively.

When using Q3,* rather than Q3,max, there is a smaller effect of the number of items, but again the critical values decrease as sample size increases.

Makransky and Bilenberg Data

Methods

The empirical data example uses the Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder Rating Scale–IV (ADHD-RS-IV), which has been validated using the Rasch model in a sample consisting of 566 Danish schoolchildren (52% boys), ranging from 6 to 16 years of age (M = 10.98) by Makransky and Bilenberg (2014). The parent and teacher ADHD-RS-IV (Barkley, Gwenyth, & Arthur, 1999) which is one of the most frequently used scales in treatment evaluation of children with ADHD consists of 26 items which measure across three subscales: Inattention, Hyperactivity/Impulsivity, and Conduct Problems. Parents and teachers are independently asked to rate children on the 26 items on a 4-point Likert-type scale, resulting in six subscales (three with ratings from parents and three with ratings from teachers). In this study, the authors will specifically focus on the nine items from the teacher ratings of the Hyperactivity/Impulsivity subscale. They attempted to find the empirical residual correlation critical value that should be applied to indicate LD. They did this by simulating data sets under the Rasch model, that is, data sets without LD. Using an implementation in SAS (Christensen, 2006), the simulation study was conducted by simulating 10,000 data sets under the Rasch model and, for each of these, performing the Steps (i) to (v) outlined above to find the empirical 95th and 99th percentiles.

Results

In this section, the authors describe an empirical example where they illustrate the practical challenge of deciding whether or not the evidence of LD provided by the maximum value Q3,max of Yen’s (1984)Q3 is large enough to violate the assumptions of the Rasch model. Makransky and Bilenberg (2014) reported misfit to the Rasch model using a critical value of 0.2 to indicate LD. Using this critical value, they identified LD between Item 2 (“Leaves seat”) and Item 3 (“Runs about or climbs excessively”) where Q3 was 0.26, and also between Item 7 (“Blurts out answers”) and Item 8 (“Difficulty awaiting turn”) where Q3 was 0.34 (Table 1).

Table 1.

The Observed Residual Correlation Matrix in the Makransky and Bilenberg (2014) Data for the Teacher Ratings of Hyperactivity/Impulsivity in the ADHD-RS-IV.

Item 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9
1. Fidgets or squirms 1.00
2. Leaves seat .12 1.00
3. Runs about or climbs excessively .03 .26 1.00
4. Difficulty playing quietly −.05 −.04 .04 1.00
5. On the go −.09 −.25 −.02 −.14 1.00
6. Talks excessively −.20 −.25 −.26 −.21 .03 1.00
7. Blurts out answers −.34 −.26 −.23 −.25 −.18 .00 1.00
8. Difficulty awaiting turn −.29 −.21 −.23 −.24 −.19 −.12 .34 1.00
9. Interrupts −.20 −.12 −.14 −.14 −.24 −.32 .12 .12 1.00

Note. Boldface indicates values above 0.1. ADHD-RS-IV = Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder Rating Scale–IV.

They were able to explain the LD based on the content of the items, for example, that students would have to leave their seat to run about or climb excessively within a classroom environment, where students are usually required to sit in their seat, and they went on to adjust the scale based on these results. Thus, the observed value of Q3,max is 0.34, and as the average correlation Q¯3 in Table 1 is −0.12, the observed value of Q3,* is 0.46.

As described above, there are examples in the literature where this procedure has been used with critical values of Q3 ranging from 0.1 to 0.7. The choice of the critical value has implications for the interpretation of the measurement properties of a scale. This will, in turn, impact upon any amendments that might be made, as well as the conclusions that are drawn. Using a critical value of 0.3 would lead to the conclusion that the residual correlation value of 0.26 identified between Items 2 and 3 is not in violation of the Rasch model. A critical value of 0.7 would lead to the conclusion that there is no LD in the scale. Alternatively, a critical value of 0.1 would result in the conclusion that three additional pairs of items also exhibit LD within this data set.

Based on the estimated item and person parameters in the Makransky and Bilenberg data, 10,000 data sets from a Rasch model were simulated without LD, computed residuals, and their associated correlations. The empirical distribution of the maximum value Q3,max based on these 10,000 data sets is shown in Figure 5.

Figure 5.

Figure 5.

The empirical distribution of Q3,max based on 10,000 data sets simulated using item and person parameters from the Makransky and Bilenberg (2014) data.

The 95th and 99th percentiles in this empirical distribution were 0.19, and 0.24, respectively, indicating that Makransky and Bilenberg were correct in concluding that Q3,max = 0.34 indicated misfit. Using the parametric bootstrap results reported in Figure 1, Makransky and Bilenberg could have rejected the assumption of no LD with a p value of <.001. For nine items (as in the Makransky and Bilenberg data), there are 36 item pairs, and based on the simulated data sets, the authors are able to determine critical values for Yen’s Q3 for each item pair. If a hypothesis about LD had been specified a priori for a single item pair (e.g., between Items 2 and 3), then it would make sense to compare the observed correlation with a percentile in the empirical distribution of correlations for this item pair. In Table 2, the median and four empirical percentiles are shown.

Table 2.

Empirical 95th and 99th Percentiles in the Empirical Distribution of the Correlations of Residuals.

Percentile
Item 1 Item 2 Median IQR 95th 99th
1 2 −0.07 −0.11 to −0.02 0.06 0.12
3 −0.06 −0.11 to −0.02 0.06 0.12
4 −0.08 −0.13 to −0.02 0.06 0.12
5 −0.07 −0.12 to −0.02 0.06 0.13
6 −0.07 −0.11 to −0.02 0.07 0.14
7 −0.07 −0.12 to −0.02 0.06 0.12
8 −0.07 −0.12 to −0.02 0.06 0.13
9 −0.07 −0.12 to −0.02 0.06 0.13
2 3 −0.08 −0.12 to −0.03 0.05 0.10
4 −0.09 −0.15 to −0.04 0.05 0.10
5 −0.08 −0.13 to −0.03 0.06 0.13
6 −0.08 −0.13 to −0.03 0.06 0.12
7 −0.08 −0.13 to −0.03 0.05 0.12
8 −0.08 −0.12 to −0.03 0.05 0.12
9 −0.08 −0.13 to −0.03 0.05 0.12
3 4 −0.10 −0.15 to −0.05 0.03 0.09
5 −0.08 −0.13 to −0.03 0.05 0.11
6 −0.07 −0.13 to −0.02 0.06 0.12
7 −0.08 −0.13 to −0.03 0.05 0.11
8 −0.08 −0.13 to −0.03 0.05 0.11
9 −0.08 −0.13 to −0.03 0.05 0.11
4 5 −0.09 −0.15 to −0.04 0.05 0.12
6 −0.08 −0.14 to −0.03 0.06 0.12
7 −0.10 −0.15 to −0.04 0.04 0.11
8 −0.09 −0.15 to −0.04 0.05 0.10
9 −0.09 −0.15 to −0.04 0.05 0.12
5 6 −0.08 −0.13 to −0.02 0.06 0.13
7 −0.08 −0.13 to −0.03 0.06 0.12
8 −0.08 −0.13 to −0.03 0.06 0.13
9 −0.09 −0.14 to −0.03 0.06 0.13
6 7 −0.08 −0.13 to −0.02 0.06 0.13
8 −0.08 −0.13 to −0.02 0.06 0.13
9 −0.08 −0.13 to −0.02 0.06 0.13
7 8 −0.08 −0.13 to −0.03 0.06 0.12
9 −0.08 −0.13 to −0.03 0.06 0.12
8 9 −0.08 −0.13 to −0.03 0.06 0.12

Note. Based on 10,000 data sets simulated under the Rasch model using estimated parameters from the Makransky and Bilenberg (2014) data. IQR = interquartile range.

Table 2 illustrates that the median value of the Q3 test statistic for any item pair is negative. Table 2 further outlines the critical values that could be used for tests at the 5% and 1% level, respectively, if the hypothesis about LD was specified a priori for an item pair. These values ranged from 0.05 to 0.07 with a mean of 0.06 for the 95th, and from 0.09 to 0.14 with a mean of 0.12 for the 99th percentiles. As no a priori hypotheses about LD were made in the Makransky and Bilenberg study, the results indicate that the conclusions made using a critical value of 0.2 were reasonable. As the simulation performed is based on the estimated item and person parameters in the Makransky and Bilenberg data, it can be viewed as a parametric bootstrap approach.

The empirical distribution of Q3,* (the difference between Q3,max and the average correlation Q¯3) based on these 10,000 data sets is shown in Figure 6.

Figure 6.

Figure 6.

The empirical distribution of Q3,* based on 10,000 data sets simulated using item and person parameters from the Makransky and Bilenberg (2014) data.

As the average value Q¯3 is negative, it is not surprising that the distribution of the Q3,* is shifted to the right compared with the distribution of Q3,max. The relevant critical value for a test at the 5% level is 0.26, and the relevant critical value for a test at the 1% level is 0.31. The observed value of the average correlation being Q¯3=.12, as computed from Table 1, we see that Q3,*=0.46. Based on this, Makransky and Bilenberg were correct in concluding that LD exists in the data.

Formally, the results in Figures 1 and 2 would enable us to reject the overall hypothesis about absence of LD and conclude that there is LD for the Item Pair 7 and 8. Of course, a parametric bootstrap approach like this could be extended from looking at the maximum value Q3,max to looking at the empirical distribution of largest and the second largest Q3 value. Makransky and Bilenberg report that LD between the items was successfully dealt with by combining the item pairs with LD into single combination items, and evaluating fit for the resulting seven-item scale. They further argue that item deletion is not desirable because the Hyperactivity/Impulsivity subscale in the ADHD-RS-IV is “. . . developed to assess the diagnosis in the DSM-IV and DSM-5, and the elimination of the items would decrease the content validity of the scale” (Makransky & Bilenberg, 2014, p. 702). A third alternative is to model the LD using log-linear Rasch models (Kelderman, 1984). Table 3 outlines the result obtained using item deletion and combining items, respectively. Item fit was evaluated using comparison of observed and expected item-restscore correlation (Kreiner, 2011), while Andersen’s (1973) conditional likelihood ratio test was used to evaluate scale fit.

Table 3.

Evaluation of Item and Scale Fit in Four Models With Item Deletion and a Model With Item Combination: The Makransky and Bilenberg Data.

Deleting items
Original scale 2 and 7 3 and 7 2 and 8 3 and 8 Combining items
Item fit
1. (Fidgets or squirms) 0.981 0.880 0.251 0.697 0.428 0.297
2. (Leaves seat) 0.989 0.558 0.695 0.650
3. (Runs about or climbs excessively) 0.005 0.008 0.009
4. (Difficulty playing quietly) 0.001 0.010 0.016 0.005 0.010 0.037
5. (On the go) 0.716 0.123 0.208 0.180 0.319 0.168
6. (Talks excessively) 0.030 0.124 0.171 0.108 0.150 0.345
7. (Blurts out answers) 0.023 0.116 0.095 0.735
8. (Difficulty awaiting turn) 0.394 0.761 0.613
9. (Interrupts) 0.772 0.996 0.782 0.854 0.906 0.196
Scale Fit 0.036 0.019 0.218 0.002 0.045 0.081

Note. Boldface indicates p < .05. Item fit evaluated using comparison of observed and expected item-restscore correlations, total fit based on Andersen’s (1973) conditional likelihood ratio test, p values reported.

Based on the results in Table 3, we see that combining items yields the best item and scale fit. The four models are also compared with respect to the test information function (Figure 7).

Figure 7.

Figure 7.

The test information in four models with item deletion and in the model with item combination.

Figure 7 shows that combining items yields the highest test information.

Discussion

Local independence implies that, having extracted the unidimensional latent variable, there should be no leftover patterns in the residuals (Tennant & Conaghan, 2007). The authors simulated the distribution of residuals that can be expected between two items when the data fit the Rasch model under a number of different conditions. In all instances, the critical values used to indicate LD were shown to be lower when there are fewer items, and more cases within a data set. Similar patterns were observed for dichotomous and polytomous items.

In the first part of this study, empirical percentiles were reported from the empirical distribution of the Q3,max test statistic and the Q3,* test statistic. We reported critical values across a number of situations with differing numbers of items, response options, and respondents and with different targeting. Each of these conditions was based on 10,000 data sets simulated under the Rasch model. The outlined parametric bootstrap method could be applied on a case-by-case basis to inform research about a reasonable choice of cut-point for the maximum value of the Q3,max and for the Q3,* test statistics. The second part of this study made it clear that the critical value of the Q3,max test statistic depends heavily on the number of items, but that the Q3,* test statistics are more stable.

In the second part of this study, the empirical 95th and 99th percentiles were reported from the empirical distribution of the maximum value Q3,max of Yen’s (1984)Q3 test statistic in 10,000 data sets, which were simulated under the Rasch model using the estimated item and person parameters from the Makransky and Bilenberg (2014) data. Based on this, a critical value of 0.19 was observed at the 95th percentile, and a critical value of 0.24 was observed at the 99th percentile. As the observed value was Q3,max=0.34, it is reasonable to conclude that there is LD in the data set.

Having disclosed evidence of LD when it is found to exist, several ways of dealing with it have been suggested. These include the deletion of one of the LD items or by fitting the partial credit model to polytomous items resulting from summation locally dependent Rasch items (Andrich, 1985; Kreiner & Christensen, 2007; Makransky & Bilenberg, 2014). Other approaches include using testlet models (Wang & Wilson, 2005; Wilson & Adams, 1995) or a bifactor model (Reise, 2012). In the analysis of the Makransky and Bilenberg (2014) data, we found that combining items yielded the best item and scale fit and the highest test information.

Summary and Recommendations

In summary, several methods for identifying LD have been suggested, but the most frequently used one appears to be Yen’s Q3 based on computing residuals (observed item responses minus their expected values), and then correlating these residuals. Thus, in practice, LD is identified through the observed correlation matrix of residuals based on estimated item and person parameters, and residual correlations above a certain value are used to identify items that appear to be locally dependent.

It was shown that a singular critical value for the Q3,max test statistics is not appropriate for all situations, as the range of residual correlations values is influenced by a number of factors. The critical value which indicates LD will always be relative to the parameters of the specific data set, and various factors should be considered when assessing LD. For this reason, the recommendation by Marais (2013) was that LD should be considered relative to the average residual correlation, and thus that the Q3,* test statistic should be used. For neither of the test statistics, a single stand-alone critical value exists.

Despite no single critical value being appropriate, the simulations show that the Q3,* critical value appears to be reasonably stable around a value of 0.2 above the average correlation. Within the parameter ranges that were tested, any residual correlation >0.2 above the average correlation would appear to indicate LD, and any residual correlation of independent items at a value >0.3 above the average would seem unlikely.

Finch and Jeffers (2016) proposed a permutation test for LD based on the Q3 and found it to have good Type I error control, while also yielding more power for detecting LD than the use of the 0.2 cut-value. Bootstrapping and determining critical values for the Q3 is one option, but using one of the statistics with known null distribution listed in the introduction is a better option. For researchers for whom these tests are not available, the results presented in Figures 3 and 4 yield guideline for choosing a critical value of the Q3,max and the results presented in Figures 5 and 6 yield guideline for choosing a critical value of the Q3,* for certain data structure situations, and the parametric bootstrap approach outlined illustrates how a precise critical value can be ascertained. A complete summary of the simulation studies is available online on the home page (http://publicifsv.sund.ku.dk/~kach/Q3/critical_values_Yens_Q3.html).

Footnotes

Declaration of Conflicting Interests: The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

Funding: The author(s) received no financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

References

  1. Andersen E. B. (1973). A goodness of fit test for the Rasch model. Psychometrika, 38, 123-140. [Google Scholar]
  2. Andrich D. (1978). A rating formulation for ordered response categories. Psychometrika, 43, 561-573. [Google Scholar]
  3. Andrich D. (1985). A latent trait model for items with response dependencies: Implications for test construction and analysis. In Embretson S. E. (Ed.), Test design: Developments in psychology and psychometrics (pp. 245-275). New York, NY: Academic Press. [Google Scholar]
  4. Andrich D., Humphry S., Marais I. (2012). Quantifying local, response dependence between two polytomous items using the Rasch model. Applied Psychological Measurement, 36, 309-324. [Google Scholar]
  5. Andrich D., Kreiner S. (2010). Quantifying response dependence between two dichotomous items using the Rasch model. Applied Psychological Measurement, 34, 181-192. [Google Scholar]
  6. Andrich D., Luo G. (2003). Conditional pairwise estimation in the Rasch model for ordered response categories using principal components. Journal of Applied Measurement, 4, 205-221. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Andrich D., Sheridan B., Luo G. (2010). RUMM2030 [Computer software and manual]. Perth, Australia: RUMM Laboratory. [Google Scholar]
  8. Barkley R., Gwenyth E. H., Arthur L. R. (1999). Defiant teens: A clinician’s manual for assessment and family intervention. New York, NY: Guilford Press. [Google Scholar]
  9. Chen W.-H., Thissen D. (1997). Local dependence indexes for item pairs using item response theory. Journal of Educational and Behavioral Statistics, 22, 265-289. [Google Scholar]
  10. Christensen K. B. (2006). Fitting polytomous Rasch models in SAS. Journal of Applied Measurement, 7, 407-417. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. das Nair R., Moreton B. J., Lincoln N. B. (2011). Rasch analysis of the Nottingham extended activities of daily living scale. Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine, 43, 944-950. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Davidson M., Keating J. L., Eyres S. (2004). A low back-specific version of the SF-36 Physical Functioning Scale. Spine, 29, 586-594. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Douglas J., Kim H. R., Habing B., Gao F. (1998). Investigating local dependence with conditional covariance functions. Journal of Educational and Behavioral Statistics, 23, 129-151. [Google Scholar]
  14. Finch W. H., Jeffers H. (2016). A Q3-based permutation test for assessing local independence. Applied Psychological Measurement, 40, 157-160. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Fisher R. A. (1915). Frequency distribution of the values of the correlation coefficient in samples of an indefinitely large population. Biometrika, 10, 507-521. [Google Scholar]
  16. Glas C. A. W., Suarez-Falcon J. C. (2003). A comparison of item-fit statistics for the three-parameter logistic model. Applied Psychological Measurement, 27, 87-106. [Google Scholar]
  17. González-de Paz L., Kostov B., López-Pina J. A., Solans-Julián P., Navarro-Rubio M. D., Sisó-Almirall A. (2015). A Rasch analysis of patients’ opinions of primary health care professionals’ ethical behaviour with respect to communication issues. Family Practice, 32, 237-243. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Haberman S. J. (2007). The interaction model. In von Davier M., Carstensen C. H. (Eds.), Multivariate and mixture distribution Rasch models: Extensions and applications (pp. 201-216). New York, NY: Springer. [Google Scholar]
  19. Hissbach J. C., Klusmann D., Hampe W. (2011). Dimensionality and predictive validity of the HAM-Nat, a test of natural sciences for medical school admission. BMC Medical Education, 11, Article 83. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Holland P. W., Thayer D. T. (1988). Differential item performance and the Mantel-Haenszel procedure. In Wainer H., Braun H. I. (Eds.), Test validity (pp. 129-145). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. [Google Scholar]
  21. Hoskens M., De Boeck P. (1997). A parametric model for local dependence among test items. Psychological Methods, 2, 261-277. [Google Scholar]
  22. Ip E. H. (2001). Testing for local dependency in dichotomous and polytomous item response models. Psychometrika, 66, 109-132. [Google Scholar]
  23. Ip E. H. (2002). Locally dependent latent trait model and the Dutch identity revisited. Psychometrika, 67, 367-386. [Google Scholar]
  24. Kelderman H. (1984). Loglinear Rasch model tests. Psychometrika, 49, 223-245. [Google Scholar]
  25. Kreiner S. (2011). A note on item-restscore association in Rasch models. Applied Psychological Measurement, 35, 557-561. [Google Scholar]
  26. Kreiner S., Christensen K. B. (2004). Analysis of local dependence and multidimensionality in graphical loglinear Rasch models. Communications in Statistics—Theory and Methods, 33, 1239-1276. [Google Scholar]
  27. Kreiner S., Christensen K. B. (2007). Validity and objectivity in health-related scales: Analysis by graphical loglinear Rasch models. In von Davier M., Carstensen C. H. (Eds.), Multivariate and mixture distribution Rasch models: Extensions and applications (pp. 329-346). New York, NY: Springer-Verlag. [Google Scholar]
  28. Kreiner S., Christensen K. B. (2011). Exact evaluation of bias in Rasch model residuals. In Baswell A. R. (Ed.), Advances in mathematics research (Vol. 12, pp. 19-40). New York, NY: Nova. [Google Scholar]
  29. La Porta F., Franceschini M., Caselli S., Cavallini P., Susassi S., Tennant A. (2011). Unified balance scale: An activity-based, bed to community, and aetiology-independent measure of balance calibrated with Rasch analysis. Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine, 43, 435-444. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Lazarsfeld P. F., Henry N. W. (1968). Latent structure analysis. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin. [Google Scholar]
  31. Liu Y., Maydeu-Olivares A. (2013). Local dependence diagnostics in IRT modeling of binary data. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 73, 254-274. [Google Scholar]
  32. Liu Y., Thissen D. (2012). Identifying local dependence with a score test statistic based on the bifactor logistic model. Applied Psychological Measurement, 36, 670-688. [Google Scholar]
  33. Liu Y., Thissen D. (2014). Comparing score tests and other local dependence diagnostics for the graded response model. British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology, 67, 496-513. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Lord F. M., Novick M. R. (1968). Statistical theories of mental test scores. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley. [Google Scholar]
  35. Lucke J. F. (2005). “Rassling the hog”: The influence of correlated item error on internal consistency, classical reliability, and congeneric reliability. Applied Psychological Measurement, 29, 106-125. [Google Scholar]
  36. Makransky G., Bilenberg N. (2014). Psychometric properties of the parent and teacher ADHD rating scale (ADHD-RS): Measurement invariance across gender, age, and informant. Assessment, 21, 694-705. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Makransky G., Rogers M. E., Creed P. A. (2014). Analysis of the construct validity and measurement invariance of the career decision self-efficacy scale: A Rasch model approach. Journal of Career Assessment, 21, 694-705. [Google Scholar]
  38. Marais I. (2009). Response dependence and the measurement of change. Journal of Applied Measurement, 10, 17-29. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Marais I. (2013). Local dependence. In Christensen K. B., Kreiner S., Mesbah M. (Eds.), Rasch models in health (pp. 111-130). London, England: Wiley. [Google Scholar]
  40. Marais I., Andrich D. (2008a). Effects of varying magnitude and patterns of local dependence in the unidimensional Rasch model. Journal of Applied Measurement, 9, 105-124. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Marais I., Andrich D. (2008b). Formalising dimension and response violations of local independence in the unidimensional Rasch model. Journal of Applied Measurement, 9, 1-16. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Masters G. N. (1982). A Rasch model for partial credit scoring. Psychometrika, 47, 149-174. [Google Scholar]
  43. Maydeu-Olivares A., Liu Y. (2015). Item diagnostics in multivariate discrete data. Psychological Methods, 20, 276-292. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Ramp M., Khan F., Misajon R. A., Pallant J. F. (2009). Rasch analysis of the Multiple Sclerosis Impact Scale (MSIS-29). Health and Quality of Life Outcomes, 7, Article 58. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Rasch G. (1960). Probabilistic models for some intelligence and attainment tests. Copenhagen: Danish Institute for Educational Research. [Google Scholar]
  46. Reeve B. B., Hays R. D., Bjorner J. B., Cook K. F., Crane P. K., Teresi J. A., . . . Hambleton R. K. (2007). Psychometric evaluation and calibration of health-related quality of life item banks. Medical Care, 45(Suppl. 1), S22-S31. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Reise S. P. (2012). The rediscovery of bifactor measurement models. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 47, 667-696. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Røe C., Damsgård E., Fors T., Anke A. (2014). Psychometric properties of the pain stages of change questionnaire as evaluated by Rasch analysis in patients with chronic musculoskeletal pain. BMC Musculoskeletal Disorders, 15, Article 95. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Rosenbaum P. R. (1984). Testing the conditional independence and monotonicity assumptions of item response theory. Psychometrika, 49, 425-435. [Google Scholar]
  50. Scott S. L., Ip E. H. (2002). Empirical Bayes and item-clustering effects in a latent variable hierarchical model. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 97, 409-419. [Google Scholar]
  51. Ten Klooster P. M., Taal E., van de Laar M. A. F. J. (2008). Rasch analysis of the Dutch health assessment questionnaire disability index and the Health Assessment Questionnaire II in patients with rheumatoid arthritis. Arthritis & Rheumatism, 59, 1721-1728. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Tennant A., Conaghan P. G. (2007). The Rasch measurement model in rheumatology: What is it and why use it? When should it be applied, and what should one look for in a Rasch paper? Arthritis Care & Research, 57, 1358-1362. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. van den Wollenberg A. L. (1982). Two new test statistics for the Rasch model. Psychometrika, 47, 123-139. [Google Scholar]
  54. Wang W. C., Wilson M. (2005). The Rasch testlet model. Applied Psychological Measurement, 29, 126-149. [Google Scholar]
  55. Warm T. (1989). Weighted likelihood estimation of ability in item response theory. Psychometrika, 54, 427-450. [Google Scholar]
  56. Wilson M., Adams R. J. (1995). Rasch models for item bundles. Psychometrika, 60, 181-198. [Google Scholar]
  57. Yen W. M. (1984). Effects of local item dependence on the fit and equating performance of the three-parameter logistic model. Applied Psychological Measurement, 8, 125-145. [Google Scholar]
  58. Yen W. M. (1993). Scaling performance assessments: Strategies for managing local item dependence. Journal of Educational Measurement, 30, 187-213. [Google Scholar]
  59. Zwinderman A. H. (1995). Pairwise parameter estimation in Rasch models. Applied Psychological Measurement, 19, 369-375. [Google Scholar]

Articles from Applied Psychological Measurement are provided here courtesy of SAGE Publications

RESOURCES