Abstract
The vagus nerve is the primary means of neural communication between the gastrointestinal (GI) tract and the brain. Vagally mediated GI signals activate the hippocampus (HPC), a brain region classically linked with memory function. However, the endogenous relevance of GI-derived vagal HPC communication is unknown. Here we utilize a saporin (SAP)-based lesioning procedure to reveal that selective GI vagal sensory/afferent ablation in rats impairs HPC-dependent episodic and spatial memory, effects associated with reduced HPC neurotrophic and neurogenesis markers. To determine the neural pathways connecting the gut to the HPC, we utilize monosynaptic and multisynaptic virus-based tracing methods to identify the medial septum as a relay connecting the medial nucleus tractus solitarius (where GI vagal afferents synapse) to dorsal HPC glutamatergic neurons. We conclude that endogenous GI-derived vagal sensory signaling promotes HPC-dependent memory function via a multi-order brainstem–septal pathway, thereby identifying a previously unknown role for the gut–brain axis in memory control.
Feeding-relevant vagal signaling occurs between the gastrointestinal tract and the brain, but it is unclear if this pathway influences cognitive processes. This study shows that endogenous gastrointestinal derived vagal sensory signaling promotes hippocampal-dependent memory function via a multi-order brainstem–septal pathway.
Introduction
Energy balance and metabolic-relevant communication between the gastrointestinal (GI) tract and the brain is mediated largely by the vagus nerve. Vagal afferent/sensory information is received first in the brain within the medial nucleus of the solitary tract (mNTS) in the caudal brainstem and then relayed to various hindbrain and forebrain regions via ascending neural pathways1. Neurons in the hippocampus (HPC), a brain region traditionally linked with learning and memory control and more recently with feeding behavior2, are activated by direct vagal nerve stimulation and by GI vagally mediated signals such as mechanical distension of the stomach and intestinal nutrient infusion3–5. In addition, rats with selective HPC lesions are impaired in utilizing interoceptive hunger and satiety cues to guide learned anticipatory appetitive outcomes6, suggesting that the HPC functionally integrates GI energy balance-relevant cues. Unknown is whether feeding-relevant GI vagal afferent signaling endogenously impacts cognitive and mnemonic processes that are regulated by the HPC.
Consistent with a role for vagal signaling in memory function, vagus nerve stimulation enhances memory7, 8, facilitates HPC neurogenesis, and increases HPC expression of brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF)9, 10, a neurotrophin that promotes neuronal survival and differentiation, as well as synaptic plasticity11. These findings suggest that the vagus nerve promotes neurogenic and neurotrophic signaling. However, these findings involve non-physiological electrical stimulation of the cervical vagus nerve. The endogenous relevance of vagal signaling, especially gut-innervating vagal afferent pathways, to mnemonic and cognitive control is poorly understood. Furthermore, the neural pathways through which vagally mediated energy-state signals are transmitted between the GI tract and hippocampal neurons remains to be fully understood. The mNTS, where GI-derived vagal sensory inputs synapse, sends projections to many brainstem and forebrain sites, but none directly to the HPC12–14. Thus, the neural communication between the gut and the HPC must involve a yet unidentified multi-order neural pathway.
The present study investigated the endogenous role of GI-derived (subdiaphragmatic) vagus nerve signaling on a variety of HPC-dependent memory processes that involve the following: (1) processing of external visuospatial stimuli (i.e., spatial working memory and contextual episodic memory)15; (2) discrimination learning based on interoceptive energy status cues (food restriction vs. satiety)6; and (3) social transmission of olfactory-related food cues16. To dissociate between the role of GI vagal sensory vs. motor signaling on HPC-dependent memory, we utilized total subdiaphragmatic vagotomy (SDV; eliminates all GI vagal afferents and efferents) and a novel rodent surgical approach for selective GI vagal deafferentation in which a SAP conjugated to cholecystokinin (CCK-SAP) is injected into the nodose ganglia (overview of approaches in Fig. 1a, b). This recently established procedure eliminates ~ 80% of GI-derived vagal sensory input to the brain while leaving intact all brain-to-gut vagal motor signaling, and supradiaphragmatic and colonic vagal sensory signaling17. Results show that vagal gut–brain sensory signaling is required for hippocampal-dependent learning processes based on external and visuospatial cues, effects accompanied by reduced hippocampal expression of neurotrophic (BDNF) and neurogenic (doublecortin, DCX) markers. Using monosynaptic and multisynaptic virus-based neural pathway tracing methods, we also identified a multi-order pathway connecting the medullary mNTS to the dorsal HPC via medial septum (MS) input to HPC glutamatergic neurons.
Results
SDV and CCK-SAP impair contextual episodic memory
Novel object in context (NOIC) learning is a rodent model of contextual episodic memory. During day 1 of training, SDV and sham groups exhibited similar object discrimination indices (DIs; a measure of exploration time of both objects, Fig. 2a, left), indicating that baseline preference for objects A and B did not differ by group. On test day, SDV animals had impaired contextual episodic memory, demonstrated by a significantly reduced DI relative to sham animals (Fig. 2a, right; a DI above 0.50 means animals spent more time exploring the novel object for the test context). Repeated-measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) analyses across days revealed a significant day × group interaction (F[1,13] = 5.564, p = 0.0347), with Newman–Keuls’ post hoc analyses confirming a significant sham vs. SDV group difference on day 3 (p = 0.0047) but not on day 1.
Analogous to the SDV and sham group, CCK-SAP and SAP (controls) groups demonstrated similar DIs on day 1 of NOIC training (Fig. 2g, left). On test day, however, the CCK-SAP group had a significantly reduced DI relative to the sham group (Fig. 2g, right). Repeated-measures ANOVA analyses revealed a significant day × group interaction (F [1,15] = 6.496, p = 0.0223), with Newman–Keuls’ post hoc analyses confirming a significant SAP vs. CCK-SAP group interaction on day 3 (p = 0.0241) but not on day 1. Thus, hippocampal-dependent contextual episodic memory in rats requires intact GI vagal afferent signaling.
SDV and CCK-SAP impair spatial working memory
We developed a modified Barnes maze procedure to assess hippocampal-dependent spatial working memory in rats in which the spatial location of an escape hole is constant across two consecutive trials per day, but changes each subsequent training day. The index of learning on this task is the difference in the number of errors (exploration of escape holes that do not contain the escape box) from trial 2 to trial 1 for each individual training day. Results show that the SDV group was impaired in spatial working memory performance relative to shams (Fig. 2b, right). Repeated-measures ANOVA analyses of average difference in number of errors from trial 2 to trial 1 revealed significant group main effect across the 5 training days (F[1,19] = 6.8565, p = 0.0169). Individual training day analyses showed trends toward a group effect statistical significance on Day 2 (F[1,14] = 3.626, p = 0.0776, ANOVA), Day 4 (F[1,14] = 3.842, p = 0.0702, ANOVA), and Day 5 (F[1,14] = 3.555, p = 0.0803, ANOVA) compared with shams (Fig. 2b, left).
Similar to the SDV and sham groups, the CCK-SAP group was also impaired in this task relative to SAP controls, with repeated-measures ANOVA analyses of average error difference (T2–T1) revealing a significant group main effect across the 5 training days (F[1,13] = 8.66, p = 0.0114) (Fig. 2h, left). Individual training day analyses also indicated a significant group effect in error difference (T2–T1) on Day 2 (F[1,13] = 6.824, p = 0.0215, ANOVA) relative to sham (Fig. 2h, right). Overall, these findings indicate that spatial working memory in rats is impaired following GI vagal afferent ablation.
SDV does not affect interoceptive or social learning
Deprivation intensity discrimination learning is a hippocampal-dependent procedure in which rats learned to use interoceptive energy status cues (0 vs. 24 h food restriction) as discriminative stimuli for a forthcoming food reinforcer6. Repeated-measures ANOVA over six 8-trial blocks of training (before SDV and sham surgeries) showed that both Group 0 + and Group 24 + learned to respond more (anticipatory food cue entries before food pellet delivery) during the last minute of test sessions under their reinforced compared to non-reinforced food restriction level (Fig. 2c), supported by a significant deprivation state × deprivation group interaction (F[1,22] = 135.54, p < 0.0001) and a significant block × deprivation state × deprivation group interaction (F[5,110] = 13.6535, p < 0.0001). When analyzing each block individually, Newman–Keuls’ post hoc analyses indicate that Group 0 + responded significantly more under 0 h compared with 24 h food deprivation during blocks 3–6 (all ps < 0.0017, Fig. 2c, left), whereas Group 24 + responded significantly more under 24 h compared with 0 h food deprivation during blocks 2–6 (all ps < 0.000178, Fig. 2c, right). Testing of deprivation intensity discrimination retention occurred following recovery from SDV and sham surgeries. Results confirmed that SDV lesions had no impact on retention of this type of interoceptive-based discrimination. In Fig. 2d, the effects of total SDV vs. sham surgery on food cup entry during the last minute of each test session performance when both Groups 0 + and 24 + were tested under 0 h and 24 h food deprivation are shown. Repeated-measures ANOVA revealed no significant surgery group effect (F[1,22] = 0.0123, p = 0.9126) or deprivation level × surgery group interaction (F[1,22] = 0.1665, p = 0.687). Animals in Group 0 + responded significantly more under 0 h than under 24 h food deprivation (Fig. 2d, left) and those in Group 24 + responded significantly more under 24 h than 0 h food deprivation (Fig. 2d, right), regardless of surgical group. These conclusions are supported by Newman–Keuls’ post hoc analyses revealing a significant 0 h vs. 24 h deprivation state interaction for Group 0 + SDV (p = 0.0028), Group 0 + sham (p = 0.004), Group 24 + SDV (p = 0.000589), and Group 24 + sham (p = 0.000894). These results suggest that in the absence of GI vagal signaling via SDV, non-vagal cues are sufficient to sustain the learned ability to use interoceptive energy status cues as discriminative stimuli for food reinforcement.
Social transmission of food preference (STFP) is a hippocampal-dependent procedure involving social-based learning using olfactory cues16. Percent paired flavor preference at testing was above 50% chance for both sham and SDV groups (Fig. 2e, left). Both sham and SDV group significantly preferred the paired flavored chow to the non-paired flavored chow when tested 24 h after the social interaction (Fig. 2e, right; 30 min cumulative food intake). One-way ANOVA analyses revealed no significant SDV vs. sham group effect for 30 min percent paired flavor preference (F[1,12] = 0.0225, p = 0.883). Paired Student’s t-test analysis indicated a significant preference for paired vs. non-paired flavored chow for both SDV (p = 0.014) and sham (p = 0.014) groups. Thus, GI vagal signaling has minimal impact on hippocampal-dependent social olfactory-based learning.
Neither SDV nor CCK-SAP affect innate anxiety or body weight
The Zero maze procedure is an established rodent model of anxiety-like behavior that is similar to the elevated plus maze procedure. ANOVA revealed no significant surgical group main effect between SDV and shams for time spent in the open section (F[1,19] = 0.0454, p = 0.833) (Fig. 2f; left) and number of open section entries (F[1,19] = 4.861, p = 0.731) (Fig. 2f; right). Similarly, ANOVA revealed no significant surgical group main effect between CCK-SAP vs. SAP for: time in open section (F[1, 15] = 0.0103, p = 0.92) and number of open section entries (F[1,15] = 0, p = 1.0; groups had equal means) in the zero maze test of innate anxiety (Supplementary Fig. 1a), as well as center zone distance (F[1,15] = 0.198, p = 0.663), number of center zone entries (F[1,15] = 0.6269, p = 0.441), and total distance (F[1,15] = 0.1784, p = 0.679) in the open field test (Supplementary Fig. 1b). Thus, observed contextual episodic and spatial working memory impairments observed in SDV and CCK-SAP rats are unlikely to be secondary to effects on anxiety-like behavior. Overall, there were three cohorts of SDV and sham animals, and terminal body weights did not differ between surgical groups in any cohort: cohort 1 underwent deprivation intensity discrimination task (sham: 413.54 ± 10.74, SDV: 396.78 ± 11.62; p = 0.329, ANOVA), cohort 2 tested in the Barnes task (sham: 391.19 ± 8.33, SDV: 365.09 ± 11.95; p = 0.095, ANOVA), and cohort 3 tested in NOIC and STFP tasks (sham: 371.69 ± 7.16, SDV: 354.47 ± 11.42; p = 0.199, ANOVA). Similarly, the cohort of SAP and CCK-SAP animals used in this study showed no significant group differences in terminal body weight (SAP: 394.16 ± 11.04, CCK-SAP: 383.28 ± 11.53; p = 0.508, ANOVA). Therefore, we conclude that the observed contextual episodic and spatial working memory impairments observed in SDV and CCK-SAP rats are unlikely to be secondary to surgical effects on body weight regulation.
SDV and CCK-SAP reduce BDNF and DCX in the dHP
Immunoblot analyses from dorsal HPC lysates revealed that total SDV reduced BDNF and DCX protein expression in the dorsal HPC relative to sham controls (Fig. 3a, b), with a significant main effect of surgical group observed for BDNF (F[1,14] = 4.609, p = 0.049, ANOVA) and DCX (F[1,14] = 5.5133, p = 0.034, ANOVA). Similar to SDV, the CCK-SAP significantly reduced levels of both proteins in the dorsal HPC relative to SAP controls (Fig. 3c, d). This conclusion is supported by one-way ANOVA analyses indicating a significant main effect of surgical group for both dorsal hippocampal BDNF (F[1,13] = 4.881, p = 0.0457) and DCX (F [1,14] = 5.494, p = 0.034) levels. On the other hand, immunoblot analyses from whole hypothalamic lysates revealed no significant group differences for both BDNF (F[1,13] = 0.26, p = 0.619, ANOVA) and DCX (F[1,14] = 0.4955, p = 0.493, ANOVA) levels in CCK-SAP vs. control SAP animals (Supplementary Fig. 2 a, b), indicating that GI vagal afferent ablation is unlikely to have systemic brain-wide impact of these neurotrophic (BDNF) and neurogenic (DCX) markers. Hypothalamic tissue was not collected in SDV and sham groups, and therefore analyses for these groups could not be included.
Following Shapiro–Wilk test of normality to confirm that the data were normally distributed [CCK-SAP vs. SAP controls: BDNF, W = 0.75377; DCX, W = 0.90026; DI, W = 0.96494; spatial working memory, W = 0.94624; for SDV vs. sham controls: BDNF, W = 0.88919; DCX, W = 0.92599; DI, W = 0.93305; spatial working memory, W = 0.91989], linear regression analyses were conducted to examine whether dorsal hippocampal BDNF and DCX protein expression were functionally correlated with HPC-dependent spatial working memory (Fig. 3e, f) and NOIC (Fig. 3g, h) task performance. For Barnes maze, analyses included all groups (sham, SDV, SAP, and CCK-SAP) and revealed that (1) dorsal HPC BDNF levels are negatively correlated with average error difference (T2–T1) (F[1,28] = 4.211, R2 = 0.1307, p = 0.0496) and (2) dorsal HPC DCX levels showed a trend toward significant correlation with Barnes performance (F[1,29] = 3.546, R2 = 0.1089, p = 0.0698). These results indicate that lower levels of BDNF and DCX are associated with poorer performance in the Barnes task. For NOIC, analyses of SAP and CCK-SAP groups only (as HPC lysates were not collected from SDV and sham rats that performed NOIC) reveal a significant positive correlation for both BDNF (F[1,13] = 5.277, R2 = 0.2887, p = 0.0389) and DCX (F[1,13] = 7.36, R2 = 0.3615, p = 0.0178) levels with DI, indicating that lower levels of BDNF and DCX are associated with poorer performance in contextual episodic memory.
CCK activates c-Fos in dCA3 and DG glutamatergic neurons
Intraperitoneal (i.p.) injections of CCK-8 (8 μg/kg) increased the number of c-Fos protein immunoreactive cells expressed in the dorsal CA3 (dCA3; F[1,9] = 20.236, p = 0.001492, ANOVA) (Fig. 4a, c) and dentate gyrus (DG; F[1,9] = 37.917, p = 0.000167, ANOVA) (Fig. 4b, d) relative to i.p. saline treatment. In addition, i.p. CCK injections increased the number of labeled cells for c-Fos messenger RNA (fluorescent in situ hybridization, FISH) expressed in the dCA3 (F[1,9] = 53.093, p = 0.000046, ANOVA) (Fig. 5a) and DG (F[1,9] = 40.496, p = 0.000131, ANOVA) (Fig. 5b) relative to saline treatment, with 93.11% and 94.35% of c-Fos mRNA-positive cells in dCA3 (Fig. 5c) and DG (Fig. 5d) being VGLUT1 positive, respectively, and only 4.29% (Figs. 5e) and 8.44% (Fig. 5f) of c-Fos mRNA-positive cells being GAD2 mRNA positive, respectively.
Medial septum connects mNTS neurons to the dorsal HPC
The medial nucleus tractus solitarius (mNTS) is the first central nervous system (CNS) site to receive GI-derived vagal sensory input; however, the mNTS does not communicate monosynaptically with the HPC13. To identify regions of possible relay between the mNTS and HPC, a combination of retrograde and anterograde pathway tracing was used: unilateral iontophoretic injections of a retrograde pathway tracer targeted to the dCA3 (cholera toxin subunit B (CTB) AlexaFluor 488 (AF488) conjugated) (Fig. 6a, c) were combined with ipsilateral iontophoretic injections of an anterograde pathway tracer targeted to the mNTS (AAV1-hSyn-TurboRFP-WPRE-rBG) (Fig. 6b, d). Red fluorophore (red fluorescent protein (RFP) and Cy3 following immunohistochemistry (IHC)) anterogradely labeled axons originating from the mNTS were found in apposition to green fluorophore (AF488 following IHC) labeled cell bodies in the MS that were retrogradely labeled from dCA3 in each of the three animals that were confirmed as double hits in both injection sites (representative appositions in Fig. 6e–g), whereas such appositions were not observed in various control animals (n = 11) in which either (or both) injection site(s) were either undeterminable or adjacent to the intended target.
To further support this multi-order pathway, we utilized a novel dual-synaptic virus-based pathway tracing approach to examine whether mNTS neurons synpatically communicate to the HPC via a MS relay pathway18. The AAV2/1-hSyn-Cre drives Cre expression in first-order neurons infected at the injection site, as well as in second-order (but not third-order) neurons based on virion release from first-order axon terminals18. A unilateral iontophoretic co-injection of AAV2/1-hSyn-Cre (and CTB-488 to confirm injection site) targeted to the mNTS (at the level of the area postrema; Fig. 7a) was followed by a pressure injection of AAV1-CAG-FLEX-TdTomato (a Cre-dependent anterograde tracer) targeted to the MS (Fig. 7b, c). Results revealed robust axon terminal fields in the dCA3 (Fig. 7d, e) and DG (Fig. 7h, i) for each of the three animals that were confirmed as double hits in both injection sites. A schematic of the rostral-caudal distribution of axon terminal fields in the dCA3 (Fig. 7f, g) and DG (Fig. 7j, k) from a representative double hit animal are displayed. Axonal labeling in the dHPC was not observed for rats (n = 13) in which either the mNTS or MS injection was absent or adjacent to the targeted region. Overall, these results indicate the presence of synaptic connections to the dHPC from MS neurons that receive direct input from the mNTS.
Discussion
Our results reveal that GI-derived vagal sensory signaling endogenously promotes hippocampal-dependent learning and memory function in rats. Both classic nonselective (SDV; eliminates all GI vagal afferents and efferents) and novel sensory-selective (CCK-SAP approach; eliminates ~ 80% of upper GI vagal afferents) ablative methods of GI vagal disconnection impaired hippocampal (HPC)-dependent memory processes, including spatial working memory and contextual episodic memory. Moreover, both ablative methods reduced expression of neurogenic (DCX) and neurotrophic (BDNF) markers in the dorsal HPC that promote neurogenesis and plasticity, and expression of these markers in the HPC were correlated to both spatial working memory and contextual episodic memory. We further investigated HPC involvement in GI vagal sensory signaling by analyzing neuronal activation in the HPC in response to the GI-derived vagally mediated satiation signal, CCK. Expression of c-Fos in response to peripheral CCK was robust in the HPC, predominantly present in glutamatergic neurons in the dorsal HPC CA3 and DG. To identify multi-order neuronal pathways by which GI vagal sensory signaling communicates to the HPC, we employed multiple innovative multi-order neural pathway tracing strategies. These approaches identified the MS as a relay region between the mNTS (first site of vagal sensory input to the CNS) and the dorsal HPC (CA3 and DG). Overall, these results reveal a novel role for gut-to-brain communication in the control of learning and memory function and identify a putative neuronal pathway through which this communication may occur.
Previous work using electrical vagus nerve stimulation approaches involving non-physiological stimulation of the cervical vagus nerve (and is therefore not selective to GI vagal signaling) revealed improved word-recognition memory in humans following vagus nerve stimulation8. Similarly in rodents, vagus nerve stimulation improved retention of inhibitory-avoidance memory7 and facilitated extinction of conditioned fear19. Our data expand these findings by establishing a physiological role for vagal sensory signaling, specifically that originating in the GI tract, in HPC-dependent memory function. These findings may have clinical relevance in relation to current treatments for obesity that involve disruptive manipulation of the vagus nerve, such as bariatric surgeries (e.g., RYGB, vertical sleeve gastrectomy)20 and chronic electrical disruption of vagal nerve signaling (e.g., VBLOC21).
Our results support the notion that gut to brain vagally-mediated communication has an important role in protecting against neurodegenerative disorders (e.g., Alzheimer’s disease). For example, human patients with Alzheimer’s disease showed improved cognition following 6 months of chronic cervical vagus nerve stimulation treatment22. At a mechanistic level, recent studies have indicated a functional role for the gut microbiome in regulating the interaction between GI signaling and cognition23. For example, in germ-free mice that lack a microbiome, hippocampal BDNF levels are reduced24, and these effects are associated with cognitive dysfunction25. In addition, total SDV impairs the ability of butyrate, a short-chain fatty acid synthesized by colonic microbiota, to reduce appetite and prevent diet-induced obesity26, suggesting a critical need for preservation of vagal nerve signaling in the microbiome–gut–brain axis. Given that butyrate promotes hippocampal neurogenesis and memory function27, one possible mechanism linking GI vagal afferent signaling to HPC function is via gut microbial interactions with short-chain fatty acid production.
We examined the effects of SDV-mediated GI vagal ablative disconnection on a variety of HPC-dependent mnemonic and cognitive processes that rely on the utilization of different cues (external, interoceptive, social, olfactory), and that differ in their reliance on discrete HPC subregions. Of the various memory procedures assessed, SDV significantly impaired spatial working memory (Barnes maze procedure) and contextual episodic memory (NOIC), whereas performance in appetitive learning based on internal energy-state cues (deprivation intensity discrimination) and social transmission of food-related (olfactory) cues (STFP) was preserved. The lack of group differences between SDV and control animals for both deprivation intensity discrimination and STFP learning could be due to differential effects of vagal nerve signaling on two functionally and anatomically-independent subregions of the HPC, as studies have shown the dorsal (septal in rodents, posterior in primates) HPC is associated predominantly with visuospatial-based exteroceptive memory, whereas the ventral (temporal in rodents, anterior in primates) HPC is associated with conditioned appetitive and anxiety-like behaviors2, 28, 29. The contextual episodic (NOIC) and spatial working memory (Barnes maze) tasks used in the present study rely on the integration of visuospatial external environmental cues. Conversely, deprivation intensity discrimination and STFP rely on conditioned appetitive cues (internal energy-state and social cues, respectively). Deprivation intensity discrimination learning is impaired by lesions to complete, dorsal, or ventral HPC6, 29, whereas STFP is primarily linked with ventral hippocampal substrates16. Thus, overall our data suggest gut-derived vagal signaling promotes memory involving external environmental cues (that may rely predominantly on the dorsal HPC), while having less impact on conditioned appetitive and anxiety-like behaviors (that may rely predominantly on the ventral HPC). Considering these findings, we hypothesize that interoceptive processing for deprivation intensity discrimination learning is sustained in the absence of GI vagal input, and may therefore primarily involve circulating endocrine and other metabolic signals that communicate to the HPC. Consistent with this framework, injections of the gut-derived hunger hormone, ghrelin, into the ventral but not dorsal HPC increase food intake30, and intracerebroventricular ghrelin injections generalize to 24 h food restriction in non-restricted rats trained in the deprivation discrimination task31. However, one limitation of the design with regards to comparing across these different tasks is that the learning of interoceptive and social cue-based tasks occurred at different times post surgery. Further, although the experimental design was consistent across surgical groups with regards to time between surgery and behavioral testing, complete counterbalancing of behavioral experiments that involved multiple comparisons was not employed and is therefore a limitation of the study.
Episodic memory, or memory of a specific event, is HPC-dependent and was impaired by SDV (NOIC procedure). Episodic memories are important for the control of feeding behavior and energy balance, as they are critical for animals to remember aspects about where (food location), what (nutritive vs. adverse postingestive consequences), and when eating occurs. Consistent with this notion, experimental manipulations in human subjects designed to disrupt episodic memory during feeding increase hunger ratings and food intake at a subsequent eating episode32, 33. Similarly in rats, disruption of episodic meal-related memory via postprandial dorsal hippocampal infusion of muscimol decreases the latency to start the post-infusion meal and increases the size of the post-infusion meal34. From an evolutionary perspective, the physiological role of GI-derived vagal sensory signaling in HPC-dependent memory may normally function to enhance episodic memory for eating occasions, as GI vagal sensory signaling is most heavily engaged during feeding. Moreover, given that it is advantageous to remember the physical location of the food source to inform future foraging behavior, the visuospatial external environment is likely to be a critical component of episodic meal-related memories. From this perspective, GI vagal sensory signaling during meal taking represents an advantageous biological survival mechanism that promotes meal-related episodic memory to facilitate future feeding.
Based on the impaired spatial working and contextual episodic memory following SDV (that eliminates both GI sensory and motor signaling), we next demonstrated that a novel selective GI vagal sensory ablation surgical method (CCK-SAP) also impaired both of these HPC-dependent memory processes. These findings suggest that GI vagal sensory/afferent (and not motor/efferent) signaling promotes HPC-dependent memory. The CCK-SAP approach selectively eliminates ~ 80% of GI-derived vagal afferent signaling below the diaphragm17, which differs from the established surgical subdiaphragmatic deafferentation procedure (SDA), which involves cutting the vagal sensory (afferent) rootlets unilaterally near the brainstem interface and then ablating the contralateral subdiaphragmatic vagal trunk. SDA eliminates all GI vagal to mNTS sensory signaling, while leaving 50% of supradiaphragmatic sensory and 50% of vagal motor (efferent) signaling intact35. Previous work has shown that SDA has no effect on novel object recognition memory or working memory in a non-spatial alternation task36. In contrast, here we show that both SDV and CCK-SAP impairs a similar NOIC task. Although novel object recognition relies on visual object recognition memory (i.e., the animal must remember, which object it has previously seen), the NOIC test relies on external visuospatial and/or contextual memory (i.e., animal must remember the location in which it previously encountered an object). Based on previous work, it remains controversial whether the HPC is critically involved in novel object recognition learning, but rather mediates recognition memory when it requires remembering that a stimulus occurred in a certain place or time37. The perirhinal cortical area, on the other hand, is more strongly linked novel object recognition memory, as lesions to this brain region impair the ability of animals to discriminate between familiar and novel objects37–39. Moreover, rats presented with novel versus familiar objects or pictures have increased c-Fos expression in perirhinal cortex but not HPC, whereas the HPC, but not perirhinal cortex shows increased c-Fos expression in response to novel spatial arrangements of familiar objects40, 41. Whether or not the SDA approach, like SDV and CCK-SAP approaches, impairs spatial and/or contextual-based HPC-dependent memory requires further study.
STFP and NOIC involve consumption of novel foods and exposure to novel objects, respectively, which are also used to assess anxiety-like behavior in rodents (neophobia). Thus, we tested both SDV and CCK-SAP rats in the zero maze anxiety test. Moreover, we also tested the CCK-SAP animals in an additional anxiety-relevant task, the open field test. Results revealed no significant group differences in either SDV or CCK-SAP groups relative to controls in these anxiety-related tests. These results differ from a previous study that demonstrated reduced innate anxiety-like behavior in SDA rats42. These effects could be due to procedural differences, as unlike the individually housed rats in the present study, the rats in the previous study were group-housed, which has been shown to demonstrate a decrease in mean arterial blood pressure and heart rate relative to isolated (single-housed) male and female Sprague–Dawley rats43, 44. In addition, we tested the SDV and CCK-SAP groups in an elevated zero maze, whereas the SDA rats in the previous study were tested in an elevated plus maze. Although both the zero maze and the elevated plus maze are well accepted tests for measuring anxiety-like behavior in rodents, untreated/normal rats show increased exploration time of the open areas in the elevated plus vs. zero maze, potentially due to the time spent in the center (neutral) region of the plus maze, to which these is no equivalent in the zero maze45, 46. Although a more extensive analysis of anxiety was conducted in this previous study (we did not perform the food neophagia test), it is worth noting that the elimination of 50% of the vagal afferents above the diaphragm via SDA could be a potential reason for different innate anxiety-like effects of SDA relative to SDV and CCK-SAP animals, as the supradiaphramatic vagal afferents are preserved in these two latter approaches. Consistent with this possibility, optogenetic activation of nonselective vagal afferents (including those innervating cardiac systems) robustly reduces heart rate in mice47, and transgenic overexpression of angiotensin-(1–7) in mice chronically reduces heart rate and is accompanied by reduced anxiety-like behavior48. Future research is needed to directly examine the role of different vagal afferent neuron populations in anxiety-like behavior.
The mNTS, where GI vagal sensory input arrives in the CNS, does not send direct projections to the HPC13. Here we show that peripheral injections of CCK, a vagally mediated GI-derived satiation signal, significantly increased c-Fos protein and mRNA expression in the dorsal HPC CA3 and DG, suggesting a multi-order connection between mNTS and the dHPC. We utilized a monosynaptic co-injection neural pathway tracing method to identify the MS as a possible relay region connecting mNTS to the HPC49. As this method does not determine synaptic communication between immunoreactive cell bodies (back-labeled from the CA3) in apposition to axon terminals emanating from the mNTS, we employed a dual-synaptic viral-mediated anterograde tracing method and confirmed the MS as a mNTS to HPC relay region18. Although the dual-synaptic AAV1-Cre can be transported in both the anterograde and retrograde (from axon terminals) direction18, we limited the application to a pathway where there are no reciprocal connections between targeted pre- and postsynaptic regions. While the mNTS projects to the MS, descending MS projections do not project to the caudal brainstem50. Previous studies have established a role of septal cholinergic function in HPC-dependent learning and memory processes. Future work could investigate whether cholinergic signaling is critical in regulating GI vagal modulation of HPC-dependent spatial working and episodic memory function.
Collective results from the present study demonstrate that endogenous vagal afferent signaling from the GI tract regulates HPC-dependent contextual episodic and spatial working memory, potentially by driving the expression of memory-related neurotrophic (BDNF) and neurogenic (DCX) signaling pathways. These findings compliment and expand previous work using cervical vagus nerve stimulation, as well as a recent report showing that total SDV in mice reduces HPC DCX51. We further identify the MS as a likely relay connecting the gut to glutamatergic dorsal HPC neurons. Our results further expand previous work by revealing that gut-derived signals, either vagal36, 42 or endocrine52, interact with higher-order brain regions to regulate memory and cognition. These findings have direct clinical relevance, as common bariatric surgeries partially denervate vagal signaling20, 53, 54 and chronic vagus nerve blockade (VBLOC) was recently Food and Drug Administration-approved for obesity treatment21. Future studies investigating the neuroendocrine and neural pathways conveying energy-relevant signals between the GI tract and the HPC (and other regions of the telencephalon and cortex) will provide additional insight into the complex role of gut-to-brain communication in cognitive control.
Methods
Animals
Male Sprague–Dawley rats (Envigo; 320–450 g on arrival) were individually housed with ad libitum access (except where noted) to water and chow (LabDiet 5001, LabDiet, St. Louis, MO) on 12 h:12 h light/dark cycle (lights on at 08:00 h). All procedures involving animals were approved by the University of Southern California Institute of Animal Care and Use Committee.
Total SDV
Rats were habituated to liquid diet (Research Diets; AIN76A) for five days before surgery. Following a 24 h fast and under ketamine (90 mg/kg), xylazine (2.7 mg/kg), and acepromazine (0.64 mg/kg) anesthesia and analgesia (Metacam 2 mg/kg), the trunks of the subdiaphragmatic vagus nerve were transected as described previously55. A midline abdominal incision was made and then the stomach was retracted caudally and the liver was retracted cranially to expose the esophagus. The dorsal and ventral branches of the vagus were then dissected from the esophagus. Each vagal branch was ligated twice with a surgical thread at an interval of 1–2 cm, and then cauterized between the ligatures. In sham surgeries, the trunks were exposed but the vagus nerve was not ligated or cauterized. The incision was then closed with running sutures along the abdominal wall and stop sutures along the skin. Rats were allowed to recover until liquid diet intake stabilized (at least 1 week) and were then returned to a standard chow diet. After behavioral testing, SDV was verified functionally with i.p. CCK-induced food intake reduction as described56, 57. Briefly, the functional verification consists of analysis of food intake following i.p. CCK-8 (2 μg/kg; Bachem) or saline injections (treatments given counterbalanced on separate days) after an overnight fast. SDV rats were included in the statistical analysis if CCK treatment resulted in a < 30% reduction of their food intake, as described57, 58. Of the three separate cohorts of rats that underwent SDV surgery and subsequent behavioral testing, four animals were removed from deprivation intensity discrimination analyses and one animal was removed from NOIC analyses based on these criteria.
CCK-SAP nodose ganglia injection
CCK-SAP targets CCK receptor expressing cells, which are localized on vagal afferent neurons that specifically innervate the upper GI tract59, 60. As recently confirmed, CCK-SAP injection in the nodose ganglia (vagal afferent cell bodies) selectively eliminates ~ 80% of GI-derived vagal afferent signaling, while preserving colonic and supradiaphragmatic vagal sensory pathways, as well as all vagal efferents17. Twenty-four hours before surgery, rats were given 15 ml of condensed milk in addition to their normal ad libitum access to chow and water, and were fasted before lights went off (18:00 h). Twenty minutes before surgery, rats received an i.p. injection of atropine sulfate (0.05 mg/kg) and carprofen (5.0 mg/kg; Henry Schein), and then anesthetized with a ketamine (90 mg/kg), xylazine (2.7 mg/kg), and acepromazine (0.64 mg/kg) cocktail. A midline incision was made along the length of the neck. The vagus nerve was separated from the carotid artery with Graefe forceps until the nodose ganglion was visible and accessible. A glass capillary (20 μm tip, beveled 30° angle) attached to a micromanipulator was used to position and puncture the nodose ganglion and 1 µl volume of CCK-SAP (250 ng/µl) or SAP (250 ng/µl) was injected with a Picospritzer III injector (Parker Hannifin) at two sites: 0.5 µl rostral and 0.5 µl caudal to the laryngeal nerve branch. The same procedure was performed for both nodose ganglia on either side before the skin was closed with sterile and stop sutures along the skin. Rats were returned to their home cage and deprived of water for 6 h and food overnight. The post-op care was optimized to avoid excessive weight loss post surgery and increase survival rate, as previously described61. The schedule was as follows: Day 1 post-op rats received carprofen (5.0 mg/kg; SQ) and were given ad libitum access to condensed milk; day 2 post-op rats received mash (10 g powdered chow mixed with 20 ml condensed milk diluted as described above); day 3 post-op rats received mash and solid chow pellets; day 4 and onwards, rats were given ad libitum access to chow. After behavioral testing, CCK-SAP was verified functionally with i.p. CCK-induced food intake reduction. The functional verification modeled that of the SDV approach described above and that published for CCK-SAP in17. Based on this verification test, four CCK-SAP rats were removed from all analyses.
General research design
Three separate cohorts of rats underwent SDV (or sham) surgery and subsequent behavioral testing (described below), beginning 7 days post surgery. Cohort 1 underwent deprivation intensity discrimination training (described below) in which rats were assigned to one of two groups (group assignment matched based on body weight): Group 0 + (n = 16) or Group 24 + (n = 11). After asymptotic discrimination was reached, animals were then subdivided (matched based on body weight and performance over the last 8-trial block of training) into two additional groups to receive SDV (Group 0 + , n = 7; Group 24 + , n = 5) or sham (Group 0 + , n = 8; Group 24 + , n = 6) surgery 4 days after the last training day. After 7 days of post-surgery recovery, the animals were tested on deprivation discrimination performance for one 8-trial block (see Supplementary Table 1). Seven days after deprivation discrimination testing, animals in Cohort 1 were tested in the zero maze task. Cohort 2 (SDV n = 8; sham n = 8) was tested in the spatial working memory task (5 days, described below) 7 days post surgery. Five to 6 days later, dHPC tissue was harvested from Cohort 2 for immunblot analyses of BDNF and DCX (SDV, n = 8; sham, n = 8) (see Supplementary Methods). Cohort 3 (SDV n = 6, sham n = 9) was tested in the NOIC (5 days) task 14 days post surgery, followed by STFP 21 days post surgery (STFP; 3 days). For STFP (described below), the SDV and sham groups were observers, whereas demonstrator rats (non-operated, n = 8) were housed in a separate room from the observers. Based on results from our SDV experiments, CCK-SAP (n = 9) and SAP (control, n = 8)] rats were tested in the Barnes task (beginning 7 days post surgery; matching the timeline of post surgery SDV Barnes testing) followed by the NOIC task (beginning 14 days post surgery; matching the timeline of post surgery SDV NOIC testing). Seven days after NOIC testing, animals were tested in the zero maze task followed by the open field task 3 days later. Five to 6 days later, dHPC tissue was harvested for immunblot analyses of BDNF (CCK-SAP, n = 8; SAP, n = 7) and DCX (CCK-SAP, n = 8; SAP, n = 8) (see Supplementary Methods; see Supplementary Fig. 3 for uncropped scan of CCK-SAP vs. SAP BDNF blot). Groups were assigned matched according to body weight at the beginning of each experiment. For all video analyses for behavioral variables, experimenters were blinded to group assignments of the animals.
Novel object in context
NOIC incorporates object recognition within a specific context, a test of contextual episodic memory. Procedures followed those previously described15, 62. Rats undergo 2 days of habituation: half of the rats (counterbalanced within surgical groups) are able to freely explore Context 1, a semi-transparent box (15 in W × 24 in L × 12 in H) with orange stripes, for 5 min, whereas the other half are habituated to Context 2, a gray opaque box (17 in W × 17 in L × 16 in H). The following day, groups are switched and habituated to the other context under the same conditions. Twenty-four hours later, on day 1 of NOIC, each animal is exposed to two distinct objects: a 500 ml jar filled with blue water (object A) and a square glass container (object B) in Context 1. The next day, the animals are placed in Context 2 with duplicates of object A. Twenty-four hours later the animals are placed in the previous day’s location (Context 2) with objects A & B and investigation of both objects is measured. Each day consists of 5 min sessions and contexts are cleaned with 10% ETOH between each animal. Exploration is defined as sniffing or touching the object with the nose or forepaws. The task scoring involves recording of the time spent exploring an object novel to the context vs. time spent exploring an object familiar to the context, and calculation of a novelty or DI is based on these measurements and calculated as [Exploration of object B/(Exploration of object A + object B)]. Rats normally will preferentially investigate object B given that object B is a familiar object that is now presented in a novel context. NOIC is hippocampal dependent, as performance is impaired in rats with reversible muscimol-induced inactivation of the dorsal HPC CA115.
Spatial working memory
Spatial working memory is a component of short-term memory involving encoding and remembering information about one’s environment and spatial orientation. The spatial working memory task used in the present study involves an elevated white circular Barnes maze (Diameter: 122 cm, Height: 140 cm) with 18 holes evenly spaced around the outer edge of the table’s circumference. A hidden black escape box (38.73 cm × 11.43 cm × 7.62 cm) is placed under one of the holes. There are four sets of visuospatial cues (black and white stripes, a white circle, a red triangle, and an assortment of irregular shapes) placed on each of the walls surrounding the table. One day after the habituation session, spatial working memory training begins. Rats utilize visuospatial cues to learn the location of the escape box, while being exposed to mildly aversive stimuli (120 W bright overhead light, 75 db white noise), which motivates them to find the escape box to avoid these stimuli63. Each rat receives two trials per day for five training days. The two trials are separated by 2 min (during which the maze is cleaned and rotated). Importantly, the escape box is placed in the same location for both trials that occur on each individual training day, but is placed in a new location at the beginning of the first trial for each subsequent training day. Errors are measured as investigation of holes that do not contain the escape box, and spatial working memory is measured as the difference in the number of errors from trial 2 to trial 1 on each separate training day (which is then averaged over the five training days). Control rats (Controls, Fig. 2b below) show improved performance (fewer errors) on the second trial of each training day compared to the 1st, indicating that the spatial location of the escape hole can be integrated into working memory capacity in control rats.
Deprivation intensity discrimination
Deprivation intensity discrimination involves learning to use different levels of food restriction as interoceptive discriminative stimuli for sucrose reinforcement. The behavioral paradigm follows that from previous publications6, 31, 64. Training: Rats are divided into Group 0 + or Group 24 + and food deprivation levels alternate each day between 0 and 24 h for the entire experiment. Group 0 + receives a reinforcement of five sucrose pellets (45 mg, Product F06233, Bio-Serv) at the end of 4 min training sessions that take place under 0 h food deprivation, and receive no pellets during training sessions that take place under 24 h deprivation. Group 24 + receives the opposite contingency between food deprivation level and pellet delivery. During sessions in which rats were trained under their non-rewarded deprivation condition, the feeders operated but no pellets were delivered. The rats were then given 2 min to consume the pellets before being removed from the conditioning chambers and returned back to their home cages. Training sessions always occurred at the same time of day (10:00 h), but not every day to avoid a single-alternating schedule of pellet delivery. The index of learning in the deprivation intensity discrimination task is food-anticipatory responding (head pokes in sucrose pellet delivery location; detected with photobeam interruptions/magazine entries) during the 1 minute prior to pellet delivery. More specifically, the mean of the percentage of the three 20 s intervals during the last minute of the session in which the magazine photobeam was interrupted was calculated as the dependent variable (as we’ve previously described6). Rats with HPC lesions are impaired in learning and retention of this discrimination problem6, 65, indicating that the HPC is critical for this type of learning process. Training consisted of six eight-trial blocks (eight trials for each deprivation state per training block), and testing (same procedures as training) consisted of one eight-trial block 7 days after surgical recovery.
Social transmission of food preference
STFP learning involves the utilization of olfactory cues experienced during a social encounter to guide subsequent preference for a scented food. STFP task procedures were adapted from16. Briefly, demonstrators (non-treated rats) and observers (surgicated rats, controls and experimental) are first habituated to an unscented powdered rodent chow [LabDiet 5001 (ground pellets)] overnight. Twenty-four hours later, observers are then assigned to demonstrators (1 demonstrator assigned for 2 observers, counterbalanced by observer group) and are habituated to social interaction by being placed in a social interaction arena (23.5 cm W × 44.45 cm L × 27 cm H; clear plastic bin with Sani-chip bedding covering the bottom) to interact with each other for 30 min. The next day following a 23 h period of food restriction for all rats, demonstrators are given access to one of two flavors of powdered chow (flavored with 2.5% marjoram or 0.5% thyme; counterbalanced) for 30 min in a separate room from observers. Consistent with previous published work66, our pilot studies demonstrated that animals equally prefer these flavors of chow. The demonstrator is then immediately placed in the social interaction arena with one of their two assigned observer rats and allowed to socially interact for 30 min, followed immediately by a 30 min interaction with the second assigned observer rat (observer rat order counterbalanced across experimental groups). Immediately after social interactions, observers are then returned to their home cages and allowed to eat ad libitum for 1 h. Twenty-four hours later, the 23 h food-restricted observers are tested for STFP by placing two jars in the home cage: one containing paired flavor (flavored chow that was given to the demonstrator) and the other with non-paired flavor (novel flavored chow not given to the demonstrator animal). Rats are then allowed to eat for 1 h and % preference for the paired flavor is calculated by: 100*Demonstrator-paired flavored chow intake/Demonstrator + Novel flavored chow intake. Cumulative food intake for paired and non-paired flavors are calculated. Untreated control animals learn to prefer the demonstrator-paired flavor based on social interaction and olfactory food cues from the breath of the demonstrator rat; however, lesioning the dorsal HPC impairs consolidation of this preference learning.
Zero maze
Seven days following memory test procedures, all groups of rats (SDV, sham, CCK-SAP, and SAP) were tested in the zero maze task to examine anxiety-like behavior. The zero maze is an elevated circular track, divided into four equal length sections. Two sections were open with 3 cm high curbs, whereas the two other closed sections contained 17.5 cm high walls. Animals were placed in the maze for 5 min before being returned to the home cage. After the trial, the maze was cleaned with 10% ethanol. Innate anxiety was measured as the number of open section entries and total time spent in open sections, defined as the head and front two paws in open sections. Previous studies have demonstrated that animals with high levels of innate anxiety (based on anxiolytic and anxiogenic experimental manipulations) spend less time in the open sections compared with animals with low levels of innate anxiety67.
Open field
Three days following zero maze procedures, the CCK-SAP rats were then tested in the open field task, another behavioral paradigm used in rodents to evaluate innate anxiety-like behavior67. Open field procedures are derived from42. The apparatus consists of a gray Plexiglas arena (60 cm × 56 cm), and a designated center zone within the arena (19 cm × 17.5 cm), placed under diffused lighting (30 lux). Animals are placed in one of the bin’s four corners and allowed to freely explore for 10 minutes. The apparatus is cleaned with 10% ethanol between rats. Innate anxiety was measured as the number of entries into the center zone, distance moved in center zone, and total distance moved in the entire arena. Previous work has shown that animals with innate anxiety-like behavior will spend less time in the center zone and more time in close proximity to the walls compared with animals with low levels of innate anxiety67.
c-Fos protein and mRNA expression
Nonsurgicated rats (n = 11) received i.p. injections of either saline (n = 5) or CCK (n = 6) (CCK-8, 8 μg/kg; Bachem), 90 min before perfusion and tissue was collected and processed as described above for IHC and FISH processing. IHC detection of c-Fos was performed using rabbit anti-c-Fos primary antibody (1:500, Cell Signaling, Catalog number: 2250 s) followed by a donkey anti-rabbit IgG-AlexaFluor AF488 secondary antibody (1:500, Jackson Immunoresearch, RRID: AB_2340619). In the same animals, mRNA detection followed FISH procedures (see Supplementary Methods) and used c-Fos (ACD, Catalog number: 403591), VGLUT1 (ACD, Catalog number: 317001), or GAD2 (ACD, Catalog number: 435801) probes. Representative images and quantification for c-Fos protein and mRNA expression in dCA3 and DG obtained from both i.p. saline and CCK injected animals were confined to Swanson Atlas level 28–3068.
Co-injection neural tracing
Neural pathway tracing experiments utilized two tracing techniques: co-injection monosynaptic neural tracing to identify second-order relay connections (COIN)49 and Cre-mediated dual-synaptic anterograde tracing (below)18. Iontophoresis was performed using a precision current source (Digital Midgard Precision Current Source, Stoelting) as described previously69. Rats (n = 14) received unilateral iontophoretic injections of AAV1-TurboRFP (AAV1-hSyn-TurboRFP-WPRE-rBG; Penn Vector Core, Catalog number: V5574L) targeting the mNTS and, in the same animal, CTB-488 (CTB, AlexaFluor 488 conjugate; ThermoFisher, Catalog number: C22841) targeting the dCA3. Following a 12-day survival period, animals were fixation-perfused and tissue was collected and processed as described in Supplementary Methods. The native fluorescent signal was amplified using a cocktail of mouse anti-CTB (1:500, Abcam, Catalog number: ab62429) and rabbit anti-RFP (1:2000, Rockland, RRID: AB_2209751) primary antibodies followed by a cocktail of donkey anti-mouse IgG-AF488 secondary antibody (1:500, Jackson Immunoresearch, RRID: AB_2341099) and donkey anti-rabbit IgG-Cy3 (1:500, Jackson Immunoresearch, RRID: AB_2307443). Detection of AAV1 and CTB was by visualization of red (TurboRFP and Cy3) and green (AlexaFluor 488) fluorescence. In 3 of the 14 animals, injection sites were confined to the mNTS (AAV1) and dCA3 (CTB) in the same animal. Similar neuroanatomical labeling was observed in each of these three animals and representative images were obtained from one of these animals. Experimental controls (n = 11) were included when neither anterograde nor retrograde labeling was observed in the MS following injection sites adjacent to (respectively) the mNTS or dCA3 that did not include these regions.
Cre-mediated multisynaptic anterograde tracing
Rats (n = 16) received a unilateral iontophoretic co-injection of AAV-Cre (4:5; AAV1-hSyn-Cre-WPRE-hGH; Penn Vector Core, Catalog number: CS1087) and, to determine injection site, CTB-488 (1:5; ThermoFisher, Catalog number: C22841) in the mNTS. Next, the rats received a 200 nl pressure injection of AAV-Flex-tdTomato (diluted 1:2 in 0.1 M sodium phosphate-buffered saline, pH 7.4, AAV1-CAG-Flex-tdTomato-WPRE-rBG; Penn Vector Core) in the MS. Following a 3-week survival period, animals were perfused and tissue was collected and processed as described in Supplementary Methods. AAV1-Flex-TdTomato and CTB-488 was detected based on a combination of native fluorescence of RFP and green fluorescent protein, respectively, and amplification of signal using the same cocktail of primary and secondary antibodies as described in the co-injection neural tracing section. Representative images were obtained from one of the three animals with both CTB-488 and AAV1-Flex-tdTomato injection sites that were predominantly confined to the mNTS (CTB) and MS (AAV1), respectively (similar labeling was observed in the other two animals that were double hits). The specificity of this connection was further supported by the absence of axon labeling in dCA3 and DG following MS injections of AAV-FLEX-TdTomato alone (n = 3; w/AAV2/1-hSyn-Cre injections adjacent to mNTS), mNTS injections of AAV2/1-hSyn-Cre alone (n = 4; w/AAV-FLEX-TdTomato injections adjacent to the MS), or in rats in which either injection site was off target (misses, n = 6).
Statistical analyses
Terminal body weights and zero maze performance data were analyzed using one-way ANOVA, with surgery group as a between-subjects factor for both SDV and CCK-SAP groups. NOIC was analyzed using repeated-measures ANOVA with surgical group (between-subjects) and testing day (within-subjects) as factors for both SDV and CCK-SAP experiments, with Newman–Keuls post hoc comparisons to evaluate group differences on each separate day. Spatial working memory data (Barnes maze) were analyzed using repeated-measures ANOVA across training days with surgical group as a between-subjects variable and training day as a within-subjects variable. The deprivation intensity discrimination test data were analyzed using repeated-measures ANOVA with surgical and deprivation (0 + , 24 + ) assignment group as between-subjects factors, with deprivation state as a within-subjects factor, and Newman–Keuls post hoc tests for individual group × deprivation state comparisons. The STFP percent paired flavor preference data was analyzed using one-way ANOVA with surgical group as a within-subjects factor. Postmortem immunoblot for both SDV and CCK-SAP groups were analyzed using ANOVA with surgical group as a between-subjects factor. Linear regressions were used to calculate p-values, R2 goodness-of-fit, 95% confidence bands of the best-fit line, and linear equations between NOIC and Barnes data with protein level data by surgical group. All groups (SDV, CCK-SAP, and their controls) were analyzed for linear regression of HPC protein expression vs. Barnes performance, whereas only CCK-SAP and their controls were analyzed for HPC protein expression vs. NOIC performance (as immunoblot HPC tissue was not extracted from SDV and sham/controls that underwent NOIC testing). Normality was confirmed for all analyses using Shapiro–Wilk’s test. c-Fos protein and mRNA expression levels in dCA3 and DG were analyzed using one-way ANOVA with drug treatment group as a between-subject factor. All statistical analyses were performed using the statistical software Statistica (Version 7; Statsoft) and linear regressions analyses were performed using Prism 7 (GraphPad) statistical software. Sample size was chosen based on a priori power analyses (conducted in Statistica V7) to ensure sufficient power to detect a pre-specified effect size. Pre-established exclusion criteria used was the Grubbs test for outliers (conducted in Prism 7). ANOVAs were followed by Newman–Keuls post hoc comparisons when significant main effects or interactions were obtained. Results are presented as mean ± SE. Statistical significance was set at p < 0.05.
Data availability
The data that support the findings of this study are available in the Open Science Framework Repository [10.17605/OSF.IO/UYMBQ]70.
Electronic supplementary material
Acknowledgements
We thank the following individuals for notable contributions to this work: Dr. Wolfgang Langhans, Brian Zingg, and Vaibhav Konanur. This study was supported by the National Institute of Health grants: DK104897 (SEK) and DK094871 (GDL).
Author contributions
A.N.S., S.E.K., and G.D.L. designed the experiments. A.N.S., T.M.H., C.M.L., E.E.N., and E.M.N performed behavioral experiments. A.M.C. performed immunohistochemistry, immunoblotting, and fluorescent in situ hybridization experiments. J.D.H. and A.N.S. performed iontophoresis and neural tract tracing procedures. G.D.L. performed CCK-SAP nodose ganglia injections. A.N.S. and S.E.K. wrote the manuscript, and all of the authors helped with the revision of the manuscript.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Electronic supplementary material
Supplementary Information accompanies this paper at 10.1038/s41467-018-04639-1.
Publisher's note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Contributor Information
Guillaume de Lartigue, Email: gdelartigue@jbpierce.org.
Scott E. Kanoski, Email: kanoski@usc.edu
References
- 1.Grill HJ, Hayes MR. Hindbrain neurons as an essential hub in the neuroanatomically distributed control of energy balance. Cell. Metab. 2012;16:296–309. doi: 10.1016/j.cmet.2012.06.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Kanoski SE, Grill HJ. Hippocampus contributions to food intake control: mnemonic, neuroanatomical, and endocrine mechanisms. Biol. Psychiatry. 2017;81:748–756. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.09.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Min DK, Tuor UI, Chelikani PK. Gastric distention induced functional magnetic resonance signal changes in the rodent brain. Neuroscience. 2011;179:151–158. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2011.01.051. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Min DK, Tuor UI, Koopmans HS, Chelikani PK. Changes in differential functional magnetic resonance signals in the rodent brain elicited by mixed-nutrient or protein-enriched meals. Gastroenterology. 2011;141:1832–1841. doi: 10.1053/j.gastro.2011.07.034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Wang GJ, et al. Gastric stimulation in obese subjects activates the hippocampus and other regions involved in brain reward circuitry. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2006;103:15641–15645. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0601977103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Davidson TL, et al. Hippocampal lesions impair retention of discriminative responding based on energy state cues. Behav. Neurosci. 2010;124:97–105. doi: 10.1037/a0018402. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Clark KB, et al. Posttraining electrical stimulation of vagal afferents with concomitant vagal efferent inactivation enhances memory storage processes in the rat. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 1998;70:364–373. doi: 10.1006/nlme.1998.3863. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Clark KB, Naritoku DK, Smith DC, Browning RA, Jensen RA. Enhanced recognition memory following vagus nerve stimulation in human subjects. Nat. Neurosci. 1999;2:94–98. doi: 10.1038/4600. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Follesa P, et al. Vagus nerve stimulation increases norepinephrine concentration and the gene expression of BDNF and bFGF in the rat brain. Brain Res. 2007;1179:28–34. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2007.08.045. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Biggio F, et al. Chronic vagus nerve stimulation induces neuronal plasticity in the rat hippocampus. Int. J. Neuropsychopharmacol. 2009;12:1209–1221. doi: 10.1017/S1461145709000200. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Noble EE, Billington CJ, Kotz CM, Wang C. The lighter side of BDNF. Am. J. Physiol. Regul. Integr. Comp. Physiol. 2011;300:R1053–R1069. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.00776.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Castle M, Comoli E, Loewy AD. Autonomic brainstem nuclei are linked to the hippocampus. Neuroscience. 2005;134:657–669. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.04.031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Rinaman L. Ascending projections from the caudal visceral nucleus of the solitary tract to brain regions involved in food intake and energy expenditure. Brain Res. 2010;1350:18–34. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2010.03.059. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Mello-Carpes PB, Izquierdo I. The nucleus of the solitary tract -- nucleus paragigantocellularis -- locus coeruleus -- CA1 region of dorsal hippocampus pathway is important for consolidation of object recognition memory. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 2013;100:56–63. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2012.12.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Martinez MC, Villar ME, Ballarini F, Viola H. Retroactive interference of object-in-context long-term memory: role of dorsal hippocampus and medial prefrontal cortex. Hippocampus. 2014;24:1482–1492. doi: 10.1002/hipo.22328. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Carballo-Marquez A, Vale-Martinez A, Guillazo-Blanch G, Marti-Nicolovius M. Muscarinic receptor blockade in ventral hippocampus and prelimbic cortex impairs memory for socially transmitted food preference. Hippocampus. 2009;19:446–455. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20530. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Diepenbroek C, Quinn D, Stephens R, Zollinger B, Anderson S, de Lartigue G. Validation and characterization of a novel method for selective vagal deafferentation of the gut. Am. J. Physiol. Gastrointest. Liver Physiol. 2017;313:G342–G352. doi: 10.1152/ajpgi.00095.2017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Zingg B, et al. AAV-mediated anterograde transsynaptic tagging: mapping corticocollicular input-defined neural pathways for defense behaviors. Neuron. 2017;93:33–47. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.11.045. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Pena DF, et al. Vagus nerve stimulation enhances extinction of conditioned fear and modulates plasticity in the pathway from the ventromedial prefrontal cortex to the amygdala. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 2014;8:327. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00327. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Picot J, et al. The clinical effectiveness and cost-effectiveness of bariatric (weight loss) surgery for obesity: a systematic review and economic evaluation. Health Technol. Assess. 2009;13:1–190. doi: 10.3310/hta13410. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Shikora SA, et al. Sustained weight loss with vagal nerve blockade but not with Sham: 18-month results of the ReCharge Trial. J. Obes. 2015;2015:365604. doi: 10.1155/2015/365604. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Merrill CA, et al. Vagus nerve stimulation in patients with Alzheimer’s disease: additional follow-up results of a pilot study through 1 year. J. Clin. Psychiatry. 2006;67:1171–1178. doi: 10.4088/JCP.v67n0801. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Noble EE, Hsu TM, Kanoski SE. Gut to brain dysbiosis: mechanisms linking western diet consumption, the microbiome, and cognitive impairment. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 2017;11:9. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2017.00009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Sudo N, et al. Postnatal microbial colonization programs the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal system for stress response in mice. J. Physiol. 2004;558:263–275. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2004.063388. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Gareau MG, et al. Bacterial infection causes stress-induced memory dysfunction in mice. Gut. 2011;60:307–317. doi: 10.1136/gut.2009.202515. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Li Z., et al. Butyrate reduces appetite and activates brown adipose tissue via the gut-brain neural circuit. Preprint at https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/29101261 (2017). [DOI] [PubMed]
- 27.Intlekofer KA, et al. Exercise and sodium butyrate transform a subthreshold learning event into long-term memory via a brain-derived neurotrophic factor-dependent mechanism. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2013;38:2027–2034. doi: 10.1038/npp.2013.104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Fanselow MS, Dong HW. Are the dorsal and ventral hippocampus functionally distinct structures? Neuron. 2010;65:7–19. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.11.031. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Hock BJ, Jr., Bunsey MD. Differential effects of dorsal and ventral hippocampal lesions. J. Neurosci. 1998;18:7027–7032. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.18-17-07027.1998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Kanoski SE, Fortin SM, Ricks KM, Grill HJ. Ghrelin signaling in the ventral hippocampus stimulates learned and motivational aspects of feeding via PI3K-Akt signaling. Biol. Psychiatry. 2013;73:915–923. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.07.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Davidson TL, et al. The interoceptive cue properties of ghrelin generalize to cues produced by food deprivation. Peptides. 2005;26:1602–1610. doi: 10.1016/j.peptides.2005.02.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Brunstrom JM, et al. Episodic memory and appetite regulation in humans. PLoS ONE. 2012;7:e50707. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0050707. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Higgs S, Donohoe JE. Focusing on food during lunch enhances lunch memory and decreases later snack intake. Appetite. 2011;57:202–206. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2011.04.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Henderson YO, Smith GP, Parent MB. Hippocampal neurons inhibit meal onset. Hippocampus. 2013;23:100–107. doi: 10.1002/hipo.22062. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Norgren R, Smith GP. A method for selective section of vagal afferent or efferent axons in the rat. Am. J. Physiol. 1994;267:R1136–R1141. doi: 10.1152/ajpcell.1994.267.4.C1136. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Klarer M, Weber-Stadlbauer U, Arnold M, Langhans W, Meyer U. Cognitive effects of subdiaphragmatic vagal deafferentation in rats. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 2017;142:190–199. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2017.05.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Oliveira AM, Hawk JD, Abel T, Havekes R. Post-training reversible inactivation of the hippocampus enhances novel object recognition memory. Learn. Mem. 2010;17:155–160. doi: 10.1101/lm.1625310. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Wan H, Aggleton JP, Brown MW. Different contributions of the hippocampus and perirhinal cortex to recognition memory. J. Neurosci. 1999;19:1142–1148. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-03-01142.1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Aggleton JP, Brown MW. Contrasting hippocampal and perirhinal cortex function using immediate early gene imaging. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. B. 2005;58:218–233. doi: 10.1080/02724990444000131. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Albasser MM, et al. Perirhinal cortex lesions uncover subsidiary systems in the rat for the detection of novel and familiar objects. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2011;34:331–342. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2011.07755.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Albasser MM, Poirier GL, Aggleton JP. Qualitatively different modes of perirhinal-hippocampal engagement when rats explore novel vs. familiar objects as revealed by c-Fos imaging. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2010;31:134–147. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2009.07042.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Klarer M, et al. Gut vagal afferents differentially modulate innate anxiety and learned fear. J. Neurosci. 2014;34:7067–7076. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0252-14.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Sharp JL, Zammit TG, Azar TA, Lawson DM. Stress-like responses to common procedures in male rats housed alone or with other rats. Contemp. Top. Lab. Anim. Sci. 2002;41:8–14. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Sharp J, Zammit T, Azar T, Lawson D. Stress-like responses to common procedures in individually and group-housed female rats. Contemp. Top. Lab. Anim. Sci. 2003;42:9–18. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Braun AA, Skelton MR, Vorhees CV, Williams MT. Comparison of the elevated plus and elevated zero mazes in treated and untreated male Sprague-Dawley rats: effects of anxiolytic and anxiogenic agents. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 2011;97:406–415. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2010.09.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Shepherd JK, Grewal SS, Fletcher A, Bill DJ, Dourish CT. Behavioural and pharmacological characterisation of the elevated “zero-maze” as an animal model of anxiety. Psychopharmacol. (Berl.) 1994;116:56–64. doi: 10.1007/BF02244871. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Chang RB, Strochlic DE, Williams EK, Umans BD, Liberles SD. Vagal sensory neuron subtypes that differentially control breathing. Cell. 2015;161:622–633. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2015.03.022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Moura Santos D, et al. Chronic overexpression of angiotensin-(1-7) in rats reduces cardiac reactivity to acute stress and dampens anxious behavior. Stress. 2017;20:189–196. doi: 10.1080/10253890.2017.1296949. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Thompson RH, Swanson LW. Hypothesis-driven structural connectivity analysis supports network over hierarchical model of brain architecture. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2010;107:15235–15239. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1009112107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Swanson LW, Cowan WM. The connections of the septal region in the rat. J. Comp. Neurol. 1979;186:621–655. doi: 10.1002/cne.901860408. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.O’Leary OF, et al. The vagus nerve modulates BDNF expression and neurogenesis in the hippocampus. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 2018;28:307–316. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2017.12.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.McGregor G, Malekizadeh Y, Harvey J. Minireview: food for thought: regulation of synaptic function by metabolic hormones. Mol. Endocrinol. 2015;29:3–13. doi: 10.1210/me.2014-1328. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Gautron L, Zechner JF, Aguirre V. Vagal innervation patterns following Roux-en-Y gastric bypass in the mouse. Int. J. Obes. (Lond.). 2013;37:1603–1607. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2013.48. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Bueter M, et al. Vagal sparing surgical technique but not stoma size affects body weight loss in rodent model of gastric bypass. Obes. Surg. 2010;20:616–622. doi: 10.1007/s11695-010-0075-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Kanoski SE, Rupprecht LE, Fortin SM, De Jonghe BC, Hayes MR. The role of nausea in food intake and body weight suppression by peripheral GLP-1 receptor agonists, exendin-4 and liraglutide. Neuropharmacology. 2012;62:1916–1927. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2011.12.022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Moran TH, Baldessarini AR, Salorio CF, Lowery T, Schwartz GJ. Vagal afferent and efferent contributions to the inhibition of food intake by cholecystokinin. Am. J. Physiol. 1997;272:R1245–R1251. doi: 10.1152/ajpcell.1997.272.6.C1804. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Williams DL, Kaplan JM, Grill HJ. The role of the dorsal vagal complex and the vagus nerve in feeding effects of melanocortin-3/4 receptor stimulation. Endocrinology. 2000;141:1332–1337. doi: 10.1210/endo.141.4.7410. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Kanoski SE, Fortin SM, Arnold M, Grill HJ, Hayes MR. Peripheral and central GLP-1 receptor populations mediate the anorectic effects of peripherally administered GLP-1 receptor agonists, liraglutide and exendin-4. Endocrinology. 2011;152:3103–3112. doi: 10.1210/en.2011-0174. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Moran TH, Norgren R, Crosby RJ, McHugh PR. Central and peripheral vagal transport of cholecystokinin binding sites occurs in afferent fibers. Brain Res. 1990;526:95–102. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(90)90253-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Peters JH, Simasko SM, Ritter RC. Modulation of vagal afferent excitation and reduction of food intake by leptin and cholecystokinin. Physiol. Behav. 2006;89:477–485. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2006.06.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Arnold M, Mura A, Langhans W, Geary N. Gut vagal afferents are not necessary for the eating-stimulatory effect of intraperitoneally injected ghrelin in the rat. J. Neurosci. 2006;26:11052–11060. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2606-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Balderas I, et al. The consolidation of object and context recognition memory involve different regions of the temporal lobe. Learn. Mem. 2008;15:618–624. doi: 10.1101/lm.1028008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Rosenfeld C. S., Ferguson S. A. Barnes maze testing strategies with small and large rodent models. J. Vis. Exp. e51194 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 64.Kanoski SE, Walls EK, Davidson TL. Interoceptive “satiety” signals produced by leptin and CCK. Peptides. 2007;28:988–1002. doi: 10.1016/j.peptides.2007.02.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Kennedy PJ, Shapiro ML. Retrieving memories via internal context requires the hippocampus. J. Neurosci. 2004;24:6979–6985. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1388-04.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Galef BG, Jr., Whiskin EE. Socially transmitted food preferences can be used to study long-term memory in rats. Learn. Behav. 2003;31:160–164. doi: 10.3758/BF03195978. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Belzung C, Griebel G. Measuring normal and pathological anxiety-like behaviour in mice: a review. Behav. Brain. Res. 2001;125:141–149. doi: 10.1016/S0166-4328(01)00291-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Swanson L. W. Brain Maps: Structure of the Rat Brain (Vol 3), 3rd edn. (Academic Press, 2003).
- 69.Hahn JD, Swanson LW. Distinct patterns of neuronal inputs and outputs of the juxtaparaventricular and suprafornical regions of the lateral hypothalamic area in the male rat. Brain. Res. Rev. 2010;64:14–103. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2010.02.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Suarez A. N. Vagus-Hippocampus. Open Science Framework. 10.17605/OSF/IO/UYMBQ (2018).
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The data that support the findings of this study are available in the Open Science Framework Repository [10.17605/OSF.IO/UYMBQ]70.