Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2018 Sep 1.
Published in final edited form as: Psychiatry Res. 2017 May 22;255:219–224. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2017.05.035

A Confirmatory Factor Analysis of the PTSD Checklist 5 in Veteran and College Student Samples

Jasmine Jamison-Eddinger 1, Meghan McDevitt-Murphy 1
PMCID: PMC5991101  NIHMSID: NIHMS883911  PMID: 28578182

Abstract

An important change in the conceptualization of posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) has been the shift from a three-factor model used in the DSM-IV-TR to the current four-factor model used in DSM-5. Early research initially supported the three-factor model, but most recent data suggest a four-factor model provides the best fit. Still other research has examined evidence for a five-factor model that would include depression sequelae. By way of a confirmatory factor analysis, we demonstrate the reliability of DSM-5 PTSD criteria clustering in a sample of 124 OEF/OIF/OND Veterans treated at a VAMC (49% white, 89% men) and a sample of 737 college students (48% white, 78% women). All participants were trauma-exposed, and completed the PTSD Checklist for DSM-5. The current study shows both samples best support a five-factor model over two four factor models considered for the DSM-5, though none provided better than moderate fit. Implications of the current findings regarding the reliability of the new DSM-5 criteria of PTSD will be discussed.

1. Introduction

Posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) is a significant public health concern, particularly as veterans return home from overseas tours seeking treatment for trauma-induced injuries (Ivanova et al., 2011). In the general population, research suggests that 25% (Norris & Sloan, 2007) to 67% (Elhai, 2012) of Americans will experience a traumatic event (as defined by the DSM-IV-TR). The lifetime prevalence of PTSD in the United States is estimated to be between 3.5–7.8% (Kessler et al., 2005, 1995) in the general population, and as high as 13.8% among veterans (Tanielian, & Jaycox, 2008). Despite its prevalence, questions remain about how many dimensions best represent the latent structure of PTSD..

The PTSD model in the DSM-5 (American Psychiatric Association, 2013) is divided into four symptom-related criteria. PTSD symptoms include: intrusive symptoms such as recurrent involuntary memories, nightmares, and intense or prolonged stress in relation to reminders of the trauma (Criterion B); avoidance of thoughts, feelings, and external reminders of the trauma (Criterion C); negative alterations in cognitions and mood such as diminished interest in activities and feeling alienated from others (Criterion D); and alterations in arousal, reactivity, sleep disturbances and exaggerated startle response (Criterion E). (American Psychiatric Association, 2013).

The DSM-5 criteria for PTSD also reflect structural changes relative to the DSM-IV-TR. Originally categorized as an anxiety disorder, PTSD is now in a new category called the Trauma- and Stressor-related Disorders. Criterion A has also been changed, no longer requiring an individual to experience “fear, helplessness, or horror” to receive a PTSD diagnosis (American Psychiatric Association, 2000, 2013). DSM-5 includes three new symptoms addressing distorted sense of blame for self and others, persistent negative mood states, and engaging in reckless or destructive behaviors. One symptom was eliminated, and other symptoms have had minor clarifying revisions. Additionally, the symptoms are assigned to four symptom sets, as opposed to three. The ongoing structural research continues to inform DSM revision decisions.

At least 42 studies have been published examining the factor structure of PTSD in adults, and four models have developed strong support. However, few of these studies were conducted since the release of the DSM-5 with the new symptoms confirmed. Further, few of these studies examined the stability of these structural models across different subgroups of trauma exposed individuals, that is, examining measurement invariance, because model analyses often require larger samples and researchers often combine samples. However, Engdahl and colleagues (2011) examined both two four-factor models finding measurement noninvariance among veteran and nonveteran samples. To test for measurement invariance, they used a multigroup confirmatory factor analysis (MCFA) and found that the measure invariance was not established. This lends support to the plausibility of combining samples when researchers combine samples to evaluate model fit.

To date, two models have the most support: the modified King and colleagues (DSM-5) model (1998) and the Simms model (Simms et al., 2002). Each model was developed based on differing theoretical perspectives. For example, King and colleagues (1998) tested models based on competing theoretical perspectives finding the best fitting model was based on the theoretical framework of Litz (1992). Litz (1992) suggested emotional numbing was poorly conceptualized and likely its own entity with PTSD. The King model split the avoidance and numbing symptom set into two categories, otherwise leaving the previous DSM-IV-TR symptoms intact (King et al., 1998).

Simms and colleagues (2002) tested several models, including the King model and a previously untested model. The untested model was created because the original D factor symptoms (such as sleep disturbance) did not fit well with the other symptom factors. Simms and colleagues (2002) loosely defined these symptoms as “dysphoria,” made up of Numbing and Hyperarousal symptoms (Simms et al., 2002).

The Elhai model has also received significant attention, presenting an alternative to the four-factor model. The model was based on Watson's (2005) research suggesting that sleep disturbance, irritability, and difficulty concentrating represent a general dysphoria more consistent with depression symptoms and not like fear-based hyperarousal symptoms.. It is similar to the DSM-5, though the Hyperarousal symptom set is split between Watson’s (2005) general dysphoria symptoms and call Dysphoric Arousal (Elhai, et al., 2011). The remaining hyperarousal symptoms such as “being “superalert” or watchful or on guard being called Anxious Arousal. Watson’s (2005) research is also the foundation of increasingly complex models that have received significant support, but present statistical challenges due to factors containing only two symptoms to represent the symptom sets (Armour, et al., 2015).

Here we briefly review the representative literature on these models: the modified King and colleagues (DSM-5) model (1998), and the five-factor Elhai model (Elhai et al., 2011). The review examines research using confirmatory factor analyses. Table 1 presents the previous DSM-IV model and DSM-IV-based symptoms for reference. It also contains the DSM-5 model and the alternative models with the new DSM-5 symptoms.

Table 1.

PTSD Symptoms by Model

DSM Symptoms DSM-IV DSM-5 Simms Elhai
1. Repeated, disturbing, and unwanted memories? R R R R
2. Repeated, disturbing dreams? R R R R
3. Suddenly feeling or acting as if the stressful experience were happening? R R R R
4. Feeling very upset when something reminds you of the stressful experience? R R R R
5. Having strong physical reactions to experience? R R R R
6. Avoiding memories, thoughts or feelings A/N A A A
7. Avoiding external reminder of the stressful experience? A/N A A A
8. Trouble remembering important parts of the stressful experience? A/N NACM D NACM
9. Having strong negative beliefs about yourself, others, or the world? A/N NACM D NACM
10. Blaming yourself or someone else for the stressful experience or what happened after it? - NACM D NACM
11. Having strong negative feelings such as fear, horror, anger, guilt, or shame? - NACM D NACM
12. Loss of interest in activities that you used to enjoy? A/N NACM D NACM
13. Feeling distant or cut off from other people? A/N NACM D NACM
14. Trouble experiencing positive feelings? A/N NACM D NACM
15. Irritable behavior, angry outburst, or acting aggressively? H AR D DA
16. Taking too many risks or doing things that could cause you harm? - AR AR DA
17. Being “superalert” or watchful or on guard? H AR AR AA
18. Feeling jumpy or easily startled? H AR AR AA
19. Having difficulty concentrating? H AR D DA
20. Trouble falling or staying asleep? H AR D DA

Note. R = re-experiencing, A/N = avoidance and numbing, NACM = negative alterations in cognitions and mood, H = arousal and reactivity, D = dysphoria, DA = dysphoric arousal, AA = anxious arousal.

In the DSM-IV-TR, PTSD symptoms were presented as three symptom sets: Re-experiencing, Avoidance/Emotional Numbing, Hyperarousal (American Psychiatric Association, 2000). However, factor analytic studies suggested that there were a number of alternatives to the DSM-IV-TR model (Elhai, et al., 2011; Gentes et al., 2014; King et al., 1998; Simms et al., 2002). Models consisting of at least four factors seem to show superior fit to the three-factor model (King et al., 1998; Simms et al., 2002; Elhai et al., 2011).

Though much of the evidence suggests the Simms model provides a better fit in several studies, overall, results have been mixed (Yufik and Simms, 2010). McWilliams, Cox, and Asmundson (2005) conducted research on a non-institutionalized civilian sample using data from the National Comorbidity Survey (Kessler et al., 1995). Based on Diagnostic Interview Schedule’s PTSD module (Breslau, Davis, Andreski, & Peterson, 1991) results, the data showed that the King model had the best fit based on the goodness of fit index (GFI), the comparative fit index (CFI), and the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA). However, the model did not meet criteria for good fit until elements (such as concentration difficulties and irritability) of the King model were adapted to fit more closely to the Simms model’s dysphoria factor. Baschnagel, O’Connor, Colder, and Hawk (2005) examined PTSD symptoms using the Posttraumatic Diagnostic Scale administered at one and three months after trauma exposure in undergraduate students following the September 11th World Trade Center attack. Ultimately the Simms model fit was better than the King model, but the fit strength of the Simms model was inconsistent at the one and three-month time points. Also, Zelazny and Simms (2015) conducted a follow up confirmatory factor analysis study based on a sample of mental health treatment seeking participants. They used the Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview 6 (Sheehan et al., 1998) that was modified to include the new symptoms of the DSM-5. They compared the fit across a number of other models including the King/DSM-5 and Elhai model and more complex 6 and 7 factor models. The study found that the four and five-factor models provided acceptable fit.

Despite mixed findings, the Simms model has support across multiple samples, including combat veterans (e.g., Williams, et al., 2011). A meta-analysis looking at different measures and sample sizes across 40 PTSD studies using DSM-IV criteria found the Simms model provided superior fit to several proposed PTSD models including the King model (Yufik and Simms, 2010). Another study also using DSM-IV criteria compared the symptom structure between the Simms model and King model in deployed and non-deployed veterans. Though no significant difference in model fit was found in the non-deployed group, the Simms model provided a better model fit in the deployed veteran sample (Engdahl, et al., 2011).

The King model also has strong support. Results from another study with a sample of 15,593 active duty military personnel showed the King model provided the best fit for the data (Mansfield et al., 2011), lending strong support to the model. A modified version of the King model with the three new PTSD symptoms was ultimately selected for the DSM-5 despite model fit being superior for the Simms across a variety of samples (American Psychiatric Association 2013; Gentes et al. 2014; Yufik and Simms, 2010).

Elhai et al. (2011) hypothesized a 5-factor model emphasizing the symptoms strongly correlated with depression and anxiety symptoms may provide the best model fit for the conceptualization of PTSD. The Re-experiencing, Avoidance, and Numbing symptom sets are intact, but the Hyperarousal symptom set is divided into Dysphoric Arousal and Anxious Arousal. Results showed the high comorbidity between depression and anxiety were represented structurally in a factor analysis. The results of the analysis showed the Elhai model had similar fit scores to that of the Simms and King model. A subsequent study by Armour et al. (2012) found the Elhai model had a significantly superior fit than both the King and Simms model based on a confirmatory factor analysis of Gulf war veterans and primary care patients. A recent study by McSweeney and colleagues (2016) conducted an exploratory factor analysis using the PCL-5 also supported a five-factor model.

1.2 Present Study

In order to understand whether the new DSM-5 criteria show a better fit to data derived from diverse groups of individuals with PTSD, relative to other proposed models, more empirical studies are warranted. Based on existing research, there is still some question whether the DSM-5 PTSD symptom factors provide the best fit for the data. In this work, we investigated the current model reflected in DSM-5 and alternative models for the PTSD symptom sets. The DSM-5, Simms, and Elhai models were examined. Though new 6 and 7-factor PTSD models have been developed, we focus on models with fewer factors to limit instances of factors with fewer than two items to maintain stability within our samples. The work investigated the best-fitting models in two samples, one sample of individuals with mixed civilian trauma, and one sample of veterans to assess the universality of the different models across different groups. No previous research has examined both a civilian and veteran sample across the three different models to confirm best model fit.

Based on the current literature including the extensive meta-analysis conducted by Yufik and Simms (2010), we predicted the Simms model should provide the best fit for the data in both civilian and veteran samples. This means the model that combines most of the symptoms now comprising the Numbing and Hyperarousal into one symptom set called “dysphoria” will provide a better fit for the data over the modified King/DSM-5 model, which splits the numbing and avoidance symptom set. This finding would suggest that the dysphoria construct plays a stronger role in the conceptualization of PTSD as theorized by Simms and Elhai. If the Elhai model provides a better fit, it also suggests a new direction in the conceptualization of PTSD related to dysphoria in the form of dysphoric arousal and anxious arousal. This assessment was based on RMSEA, TFI, GFI, and CFI values. Due to the emerging support for the five-factor Elhai model, it was examined to determine its strength relative to the four-factor models. Also, in the interest of replication of Engdahl and colleagues (2011) measurement invariance research, we combined our samples to examine whether we could achieve similar results.

2. Method

2.1 Participants

As represented in Table 2, the first sample included 129 Operation Iraqi Freedom/Operation Enduring Freedom/Operation New Dawn veterans recruited during primary care visits from the Veteran Affairs Medical Center in Memphis, TN as part of a larger study. Potential participants were approached at waiting areas in medical clinics throughout the main Veterans Affairs Medical Center building and at outlying clinics. The sample included 114 (88%) men and 15 (11%) women. Fifty-eight (45%) of the participants identified as white, 61 (47%) identified as African American, or at least one participant identified as Hispanic, Native American, or Asian, or multi-racial. Participant age ranged from 21 to 66 with a mean age of 35 years old (SD = 10.10). When filling out the PCL-5, the veteran participants were asked to recall their combat or exposure to a war-zone. The mean score for the PCL-5 was 48.70 (SD = 19.7) out of a possible score of 80. All participants had been deployed as part of OEF/OIF for an average of 9.9 (SD = 9.4) months. The veterans had been returned for an average of 3.63 (SD = 2.72) years.

Table 2.

Veteran and College Student Sample Demographics

Veterans College students
Age (years) 35.0 20.7
SD 9.9 4.7
Gender
  Men 110 (89%) 165 (22%)
  Women 14 (11%) 570 (78%)
Race
  White 61 (49%) 354 (48%)
  Black 60 (48%) 292 (40%)
  Other 10 (8%) 60 (8%)

Note. Some percentage do not equal 100% due to identifying in multiple categories.

Also represented in Table 2, the second sample included 737 college students at the University of Memphis who reported trauma exposure as defined by Criterion A in DSM-5. Potential participants who did not endorse an event that met Criterion A were not included in this investigation. Questions assessing life threat and exposure level (e.g. direct exposure, witnessing, and learning about the event) were used to screen these participants. The sample contained 165 (22%) men, and 570 (78%) women and 2 individuals who did not report their gender. Nearly half of the sample, 354 (48%) identified as white, 292 (40%) as African American or at least one participant identified as Hispanic, Native American, Asian, or multi-racial. Participants’ ages ranged from 18 to 54 with a mean age of 20.7 (SD = 4.7). The mean score on the PCL-5 was 31.2 (SD = 14.01) out of a possible score of 80. Participants were asked to indicate which traumatic event they considered to be the “worst” event, and were asked to base their PCL-5 responses on this event. The most frequently identified “worst” traumatic event category was motor vehicle accident (MVA, 17.8%), followed by sexual assault (11.8%), and a sudden violent death of someone close to them (8.4%).

2.2 Procedure

Participants in the veterans sample were approached by research project staff at VAMC clinics and invited to participate. Interested veterans completed a brief packet of questionnaires that included the PCL-5. These participants were invited to participate in a larger study that entailed attending two assessment sessions as well, but data used in the present analyses were derived from the initial packet. The Institutional Review Boards at both the Memphis VA Medical Center and The University of Memphis approved this study.

The civilian sample data were collected using the Psychology Research Participation System (Sona system) at the University of Memphis as part of a larger study. Participants completed a set of self-report measures online and were awarded one credit hour toward any psychology course requiring research credit.

2.3 Measures

Sociodemographic questionnaire. A sociodemographic questionnaire assessed general demographic information in both samples. It was modified to ask sample-specific information (e.g., questions about military experience for the veteran sample and about academic progress for college students).

Life Events Checklist (LEC). The LEC is a self-reported measure assessing exposure to 17 potentially traumatic events. Individuals were asked to indicate whether they had experienced the event directly, indirectly, or if they had learned about it happening to a close other (Blake et al., 1995).

PTSD Checklist for DSM-5 (PCL-5). The PCL-5 is a 21-item self-reported measure assessing PTSD symptoms based on DSM-5 criteria (Weathers et al., 2013). It is a revision of the previous PCL scale that corresponded to DSM-IV-TR symptoms. The PCL-5 describes symptoms such as “Repeated, disturbing dreams of the stressful experience” and “Feeling very upset when something reminded you of a stressful experience,” with anchors ranging from 0 (not at all) to 4 (extremely). The PCL-M, PCL-C, and PCL-S based on the DSM-IV-TR has been shown to have a high convergent validity with the Clinician Administered PTSD Scale (CAPS), considered to be the “gold standard” for PTSD diagnosis and assessment (Wilkins, 2011). The scores range from 0 to 80, with a score of 38 or greater indicating likely PTSD (Blevins et al., 2015) in college students and a score between 31–33 in veterans (Bovin, 2015). Blevins and colleagues also demonstrated the PCL-5 exhibited strong internal consistency (α = 0.94), test-retest reliability (r = 0.82), and convergent (r’s = 0.74 to 0.85) and discriminant (r’s = 0.31 to 0.60) validity in college students. In veterans, Bovin and colleagues (2015) found PCL-5 test scores demonstrated strong internal consistency (α = 0.96), test-retest reliability (r = 0.84), and convergent and discriminant validity. In the present study, the PCL-5 demonstrated high internal consistency (α = 0.96 for the veteran sample and 0.97 for the civilian sample).

2.4 Data Analysis Plan

IBM AMOS 23 was used to conduct the model analyses (Arbuckle, 2014). Prior to conducting our analyses, data were examined for missing values. Due to completing less 50% of the PCL items, ten participants in the Veteran sample were removed from the original 139 participants leaving 129 for analysis. No student participants that met Criterion A required removal. In the veteran sample, missing data for each variable ranged from 4–6% and in the student sample. Missing data were 0.01% across all variables. Missing data were replaced using Full Information Maximum Likelihood (FIML). The next step was to conduct confirmatory factory analysis using Maximum Likelihood estimation based on modified King model/DSM-5, Simms, and Elhai models for goodness of fit with the two samples. First the sample model fit was assessed separately and then the data were combined and we examined the model fit in the combined sample. A confirmatory factor analysis has been shown to be an effective method of testing model fit (Marsh et al., 1988). For the current study, RMSEA, TFI, GFI, CFI, and AIC were used as fit indices. The RMSEA is measured on a scale from one to one with a score closer to zero being ideal. A good fit falls below 0.08, with 0.08–.10 being considered mediocre, and above 0.10 considered poor fit (MacCullum et al., 1996). The TFI, GFI, and CFI are measured on a scale from zero to one with scores closer to one being ideal. For TFI, 0.90 and greater are generally considered to be a good fit, however researchers have argued for a more restrictive value of 0.95 (Hu and Bentler, 1999). For GFI, 0.90 is generally considered an acceptable cutoff for good fit, however due to its sensitivity to sample size, it is recommended to use a .95 cutoff for smaller sample sizes (Miles and Shevlin, 1998). CFI has a recommended cutoff of 0.90, though some researchers have argued for 0.95 to ensure poor models are not accepted (Hu and Bentler, 1999). Models with lower AIC values are considered an improvement over models with higher AIC. We also examined whether we were measuring the same construct of PTSD across both samples using measurement invariance. Data were analyzed at the item level.

3. Results

Results from the confirmatory factor analysis suggested the Elhai model had marginally superior fit statistics over both the Simms and DSM-5 models in both the college student sample and the veteran sample with the exception of the AIC value (Simms=411.10, 706.57; Elhai 577.56, 1132.11). As represented in Table 3, we used the following cut-off scores to identify good fit: A non-significant Chi-Square test, GFI, CFI, and TFI scores equal to or greater than 0.90, and RMSEA scores of 0.08 or less. AIC was used to compare model parsimony. Across the three models, none provided better than moderate fit in either sample.

Table 3.

Model Fit Indices

Veteran College

X2 df AIC CFI TFI RMSEA (90%
CI)
X2 df AIC CFI TFI RMSEA
(90%
CI)
DSM5 475.76 164 607.75 0.88 0.85 0.12(0.11–0.14) 1106.62 164 1238.62 0.91* 0.89 0.09(0.08–0.09)
Simms 463.47 164 411.10 0.92* 0.89 0.11(0.10–0.13) 1160.10 164 706.57 0.95* 0.93 0.08(0.07–0.08)
Elhai 434.06 160 577.56 0.89 0.86 0.12(0.10–0.13) 992.11 160 1132.11 0.92* 0.90 0.08(0.08–0.09)

Note. X2 = chi square test; df = degrees of freedom; CFI = Comparative Fit Index; TLI = Tucker Lewis Index; RMSEA = Root Mean Square Error of Approximation.

*

= >0.90 for CFI and TFI.

We investigated measurement invariance across the two samples, and found the factor loadings did not meet criteria for measurement invariance using both configural and metric tests across all three models (Vandenberg and Lance, 2000). The AIC statistics were also worse across the three models The King model AIC for the combined sample was 1847.93 compared to 607.75 for veterans and 1238.62 for college students alone. The Simms model AIC for the combined sample was 1900.00 and 411.10 for the veteran sample and 706.57 for the college student sample. The Elhai model AIC was 1711.128 for the combined sample, and 577.56 for veterans, and 1132.11 for college students. Table 4 displays the standardized factor loadings across each item.

Table 4.

Veteran and College Student Sample Factor Loadings Across Models

DSM-5
Veterans
DSM 5 College Simms
Veterans
Simms College Elhai
Veterans
Elhai College
Item 1 0.908a 0.858a 0.909a 0.858a 0.908a 0.858a
Item 2 0.843a 0.751a 0.842a 0.751a 0.838a 0.751a
Item 3 0.870a 0.751a 0.870a 0.752a 0.870a 0.752a
Item 4 0.916a 0.819a 0.916a 0.818a 0.918a 0.818a
Item 5 0.843a 0.779a 0.843a 0.780a 0.844a 0.779a
Item 6 0.918b 0.909b 0.921b 0.909b 0.920b 0.909b
Item 7 0.945b 0.898b 0.942b 0.897b 0.943b 0.897b
Item 8 0.662c 0.485c 0.662e 0.483e 0.666c 0.485c
Item 9 0.809c 0.802c 0.804e 0.790e 0.812c 0.805c
Item 10 0.766c 0.769c 0.745e 0.751e 0.753c 0.763c
Item 11 0.871c 0.820c 0.866e 0.810e 0.868c 0.817c
Item 12 0.849c 0.768c 0.851e 0.763e 0.847c 0.769c
Item 13 0.899c 0.830c 0.898e 0.830e 0.897c 0.833c
Item 14 0.881c 0.847c 0.886e 0.839e 0.887c 0.848c
Item 15 0.822d 0.836d 0.824e 0.814e 0.825f 0.843f
Item 16 0.679d 0.614d 0.647d 0.612d 0.675f 0.614f
Item 17 0.868d 0.625d 0.903d 0.717d 0.909g 0.732g
Item 18 0.888d 0.748d 0.925d 0.834d 0.932g 0.887g
Item 19 0.796d 0.828d 0.803e 0.798e 0.796f 0.831f
Item 20 0.726d 0.695d 0.701e 0.654e 0.703f 0.696f

Note. Reference Table 1 for item number descriptions.

a

= re-experiencing,

b

= avoidance and numbing,

c

= negative alterations in cognitions and mood,

d

= arousal and reactivity,

e

= dysphoria,

f

= dysphoric arousal,

g

= anxious arousal.

Between the two samples evaluated in the current study, the college student sample showed better overall fit, even across the TFI and CFI fit indices that are more resistant to smaller sample size type I or type II errors. The veteran sample also had poor fit across all models, particularly for RMSEA values despite low Chi-Square values (DSM-5, 0.12; Simms, 0.11; Elhai, 0.12). Though both the RMSEA and Chi-Square values are affected by sample size, the acceptable range Chi-square versus degrees of freedom ratios falls between two (Tabachnick and Fidell, 2007) to five (Wheaton et al, 1977). All Chi-Square tests were significant at p < .001.

4. Discussion

The current research adds to the body of evidence supporting the similar, but better overall fit of the Simms over the DSM-5 model across both samples as represented in Table 3. However, our research shows only poor to moderate support across the fit indices. The Elhai model, with five factors rather than the four of the DSM-5 and Simms model, provided better fit, though it still only provides moderate fit across both samples. This lends support to the conceptual validity of the “dysphoria” construct within PTSD. Despite the DSM-5 modifications changing the diagnosis of PTSD from three factors in DSM-IV to four factors, it still may not address the complexity of PTSD and the construct of dysphoria.

Also, consistent with Engdahl et al. (2011), the current research did not show evidence of measurement invariance between college students and veterans. The initial configural invariance test did not yield similar fit (fit was poorer) indices compared to those in the individual samples across all models. A follow up metric test examining Chi-square test results between the unconstrained and constrained models was used to determine whether the latent variables were similar across the samples. Measurement invariance was also not found with the metric test approach. This may be something to consider when combining datasets to satisfy the relatively large sample size requirements for CFA.

One of the limitations of this study is the relative lack of gender diversity within the samples. In the college student sample, the majority of participants were women while the veteran sample was almost exclusively men. There was also a substantial age difference between the two samples with the veteran sample having a mean age of 35 years old (SD = 9.9) and the college students having a mean age of 20.7 (SD = 4.7). Additionally, the veterans were asked to relate their PCL-5 responses to wartime experiences, whereas the college students picked their worst lifetime event where they endorsed motor vehicle accidents, sexual assaults, and witnessing a homicide most frequently. Also, the lower overall PCL-5 scores (indicating level of severity of PTSD symptoms) in college students may have led to restriction of range when comparing model fit with the veteran sample. We may have also encountered ceiling effects based on higher veterans PCL-5 scores. Thus, several important differences between these samples, in addition to their roles as college student or veterans. Also, we used the PCL-5, a self-report measure, and not an interview measure like the Clinician Administered PTSD Scale, which is considered to be the gold standard for PTSD diagnosis (Weathers, et al. 2013).

Another limitation is the size of the samples. The college student sample was larger than the veteran sample, which may have had some effect on the RMSEA and Chi-square tests, which are sensitive to sample size. Whenever possible, we used fit indices that were less sensitive to sample size to mitigate this concern. Though the research is mixed on the sample sizes required for CFA, Wolf and colleagues (2013) suggest that higher factor loadings, such as those above 0.8, stabilize the analyses. The size of the veteran sample is within the margin of acceptability according to MacCallum and colleagues (1999). Also, due to the statistical software used, we were unable to conduct analyses that test model fit and relied on visual inspections of the data.

This research supports the strength of the current model of PTSD as conceptualized in Simms model over the DSM model. However, the fit statistics for the Simms model suggest it still leaves room for improvement, particularly as it relates to dysphoria. More complex models are being developed that address continue to examine the role of dysphoria and the structure of PTSD. However, it is important researchers continue examining models and symptom reduction methodologies to find a balance between addressing the complexity of PTSD while maintaining a practical, parsimonious diagnosis. Bovin and colleagues (2015) performed the PCL-5 psychometric tests across several models for PTSD, including more complex six and seven models, also suggesting the current DSM-5 may still need to be reconfigured. However, with each additional factor, diagnosis becomes more complicated, with unknown consequences. More complicated factor structures could lead to an increasingly heterogeneous profile for PTSD. It may make creating stable criteria for diagnosis possible.

It is also important that the diagnostic PTSD model captures different traumas across multiple groups. Our research shows that consistent with other findings (Engdahl, et al, 2011), there is still some question whether the symptoms of different samples are represented in the models of PTSD with the most empirical and consensus-based support. This is especially critical as veterans return from conflicts seeking mental health services and college students potentially receive PTSD diagnoses more frequently under the DSM-5 criteria (Elhai, et al., 2012). Our research suggests that not only is there potential to improve the overall diagnosis of PTSD, it may also make sense to explore the utility in a uniform PTSD structure across samples.

The implications for finding only marginally fitting models for PTSD and the acceptance of increasingly complex structures suggest examining different approaches to understanding and diagnosing PTSD. Network analysis is one such approach where researchers examine PTSD through individual symptom influences and central symptom relationships (McNally et al., 2015).

As the structural nature of PTSD becomes clearer and diagnostic measures improve, clinicians and researchers will be able to address the growing need of trauma survivors, particularly as dysphoria remains complex due to its role in several disorders. Refining the methodologies in place and incorporating innovative approaches will bring researchers closer to untangling key aspects of PTSD so that researchers can enhance mental health services. The role of CFAs will continue to shed new light of the question of PTSD’s structure.

Highlights.

The Simms PTSD model provides the best fit.

In the present sample, none of the fit statistics for any model were better than moderate.

We did not find evidence of measurement invariance between the college student and veteran samples.

Acknowledgments

Funding

This research was supported by a grant from the National Institute of Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism: K23 AA 016120-01 and by support from the Memphis Veterans Affairs Medical Center Office of Research and Development.

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

Declaration of interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, DSM-IV-TR. 4. American Psychiatric Publishing; Washington, DC: 2000. (text revision) [Google Scholar]
  2. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, DSM-5. 5. American Psychiatric Publishing; Arlington, VA: 2000. [Google Scholar]
  3. Arbuckle JL. AMOS (Version 23) [Computer software] Chicago, IL: IBM SPSS Inc.; 2014. [Google Scholar]
  4. Armour C, Elhai JD, Richardson D, Ractliffe K, Wang L, Elklit A. Assessing a five factor model of PTSD: Is dysphoric arousal a unique PTSD construct showing differential relationships with anxiety and depression? J. of Anxiety Disorders. 2012;26(2):368–376. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2011.12.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Armour C, Tsai J, Durham TA, Charak R, Biehn TL, Elhai JD, Pietrzak RH. Dimensional structure of DSM-5 posttraumatic stress symptoms: Support for a hybrid Anhedonia and Externalizing Behaviors model. J. of Psychiatric Research. 2015;61:106–113. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2014.10.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Baschnagel JS, O'Connor RM, Colder CR, Hawk LW. Factor structure of posttraumatic stress among Western New York undergraduates following the September 11th terrorist attack on the World Trade Center. J. of Traumatic Stress. 2005;18(6):677–684. doi: 10.1002/jts.20076. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Blanchard EB, Jones-Alexander J, Buckley TC, Forneris CA. Psychometric properties of the PTSD Checklist (PCL) Behaviour Research and Therapy. 1996;34(8):669–673. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(96)00033-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Blake DD, Weathers FW, Nagy LM, Kaloupek DG, Gusman FD, Charney DS, Keane TM. The development of a clinician-administered PTSD scale. J. of Traumatic Stress. 1995;8(1):75–90. doi: 10.1007/BF02105408. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Blevins CA, Weathers FW, Davis MT, Witte TK, Domino JL. The Posttraumatic Stress Disorder Checklist for DSM-5 (PCL-5): Development and Initial Psychometric Evaluation. J. of Traumatic Stress. 2015;28(6):489–498. doi: 10.1002/jts.22059. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Breslau N, Davis GC, Andreski P, Peterson E. Traumatic events and posttraumatic stress disorder in an urban population of young adults. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1991;48(3):216–222. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1991.01810270028003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Brown TA. Confirmatory factor analysis for applied research. Guilford Publications; 2015. [Google Scholar]
  12. Dong Y, Peng CYJ. Principled missing data methods for researchers. SpringerPlus. 2013;2(1):222. doi: 10.1186/2193-1801-2-222. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Elhai JD, Contractor AA, Palmieri PA, Forbes D, Richardson JD. Exploring the relationship between underlying dimensions of posttraumatic stress disorder and depression in a national, trauma-exposed military sample. J. of Affective Disorders. 2011;133(3):477–480. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2011.04.035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Elhai JD, Miller ME, Ford JD, Biehn TL, Palmieri PA, Frueh BC. Posttraumatic stress disorder in DSM-5: Estimates of prevalence and symptom structure in a nonclinical sample of college students. J. of Anxiety Disorders. 2012;26(1):58–64. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2011.08.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Engdahl RM, Elhai JD, Richardson JD, Frueh BC. Comparing posttraumatic stress disorder's symptom structure between deployed and nondeployed veterans. Psychological Assessment. 2011;23(1):1. doi: 10.1037/a0020045. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Foa EB, Cashman L, Jaycox L, Perry K. The validation of a self-report measure of posttraumatic stress disorder: The Posttraumatic Diagnostic Scale. Psychological Assessment. 1997;9(4):445. [Google Scholar]
  17. Friedman MJ, Resick PA, Bryant RA, Brewin CR. Considering PTSD for DSM-5. Depression and Anxiety. 2011;28(9):750–769. doi: 10.1002/da.20767. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Gentes EL, Dennis PA, Kimbrel NA, Rissling MB, Beckham JC, Workgroup, V.M.A.M. Calhoun PS. DSM-5 posttraumatic stress disorder: Factor structure and rates of diagnosis. J. of Psychiatric Research. 2014;59:60–67. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2014.08.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Hu LT, Bentler PM. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural equation modeling: a multidisciplinary journal. 1999;6(1):1–55. [Google Scholar]
  20. Ivanova JI, Birnbaum HG, Chen L, Duhig AM, Dayoub EJ, Kantor ES, Schiller MB, Phillips GA. Cost of post-traumatic stress disorder vs major depressive disorder among patients covered by Medicaid or private insurance. The American Journal of Managed Care. 2010;17(8):e314–23. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Kessler RC, Chiu WT, Demler O, Walters EE. Prevalence, severity, and comorbidity of 12-month DSM-IV disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2005;62(6):617–627. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.62.6.617. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Kessler RC, Sonnega A, Bromet E, Hughes M, Nelson CB. Posttraumatic stress disorder in the National Comorbidity Survey. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1995;52(12):1048–1060. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1995.03950240066012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. King DW, Leskin GA, King LA, Weathers FW. Confirmatory factor analysis of the clinician-administered PTSD Scale: Evidence for the dimensionality of posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychological Assessment. 1998;10(2) [Google Scholar]
  24. Litz BT. Emotional numbing in combat-related post-traumatic stress disorder: A critical review and reformulation. Clinical Psychology Review. 1992;12(4):417–432. [Google Scholar]
  25. MacCallum RC, Browne MW, Sugawara HM. Power analysis and determination of sample size for covariance structure modeling. Psychological Methods. 1996;1(2):130. [Google Scholar]
  26. MacCallum RC, Widaman KF, Zhang S, Hong S. Sample size in factor analysis. Psychological Methods. 1999;4(1):84. [Google Scholar]
  27. Mansfield AJ, Williams J, Hourani LL, Babeu LA. Measurement invariance of posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms among US military personnel. J. of Traumatic Stress. 2010;23(1):91–99. doi: 10.1002/jts.20492. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. McNally RJ, Robinaugh DJ, Wu GW, Wang L, Deserno MK, Borsboom D. Mental disorders as causal systems a network approach to posttraumatic stress disorder. Clinical Psychological Science. 2015;3(6):836–849. [Google Scholar]
  29. McSweeney LB, Koch EI, Saules KK, Jefferson S. Exploratory factor analysis of Diagnostic and Statistical Manual, criteria for posttraumatic stress disorder. J of Nervous and Mental Disease. 2016;204(1):9–14. doi: 10.1097/NMD.0000000000000390. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. McWilliams LA, Cox BJ, Asmundson GJ. Symptom structure of posttraumatic stress disorder in a nationally representative sample. J. of Anxiety Disorders. 2005;19(6):626–641. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2004.06.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Norris FH, Slone LB. The epidemiology of trauma and PTSD. Handbook of PTSD: Science and practice. 2007:78–98. [Google Scholar]
  32. Sheehan D, Lecrubier Y, Sheehan KH, Sheehan K, Amorim P, Janavs J, Weiller E, Hergueta T, Baker R, Dunbar G. Diagnostic Psychiatric Interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. J. Clin. Psychiatry. 1998;59:22–33. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Shevlin M, Miles JN. Effects of sample size, model specification and factor loadings on the GFI in confirmatory factor analysis. Personality and Individual Differences. 1998;25(1):85–90. [Google Scholar]
  34. Simms LJ, Watson D, Doebbelling BN. Confirmatory factor analyses of posttraumatic stress symptoms in deployed and nondeployed veterans of the Gulf War. J. of Abnormal Psychology. 2002;111(4):637. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.111.4.637. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Tabachnick BG, Fidell LS, Osterlind SJ. Using multivariate statistics 2001 [Google Scholar]
  36. Vandenberg RJ, Lance CE. A review and synthesis of the measurement invariance literature: Suggestions, practices, and recommendations for organizational research. Organizational Research Methods. 2000;3(1):4–70. [Google Scholar]
  37. Watson D. Rethinking the mood and anxiety disorders: a quantitative hierarchical model for DSM-V. J. of Abnormal Psychology. 2005;114(4) doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.114.4.522. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Weathers FW, Litz BT, Keane TM, Palmieri PA, Marx BP, Schnurr PP. The ptsd checklist for dsm-5 (pcl-5) Scale available from the National Center for PTSD. 2013 at www.ptsd.va.gov.
  39. Wheaton B, Muthen B, Alwin DF, Summers GF. Assessing reliability and stability in panel models. Sociological Methodology. 1977;8(1):84–136. [Google Scholar]
  40. Williams JL, Monahan CJ, McDevitt-Murphy ME. Factor structure of the PTSD checklist in a sample of OEF/OIF veterans presenting to primary care: Specific and nonspecific aspects of dysphoria. J. of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment. 2011;33(4):514–522. doi: 10.1007/s10862-011-9248-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Wolf EJ, Harrington KM, Clark SL, Miller MW. Sample size requirements for structural equation models an evaluation of power, bias, and solution propriety. Educational and Psychological Measurement. 2013;73(6):913–934. doi: 10.1177/0013164413495237. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Yufik T, Simms LJ. A meta-analytic investigation of the structure of posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms. J. of Abnormal Psychology. 2010;119(4):764. doi: 10.1037/a0020981. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Zelazny K, Simms LJ. Confirmatory factor analyses of DSM-5 posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms in psychiatric samples differing in Criterion A status. J. of Anxiety Disorders. 2015;34:15–23. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2015.05.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES