Abstract
Should excessive and problematic engagement in non-substance use behaviors be mental disorders? The fifth revision of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual for Mental Disorders (DSM-5) repositioned gambling disorder in the substance use disorders section and introduced Internet gaming disorder in the research appendix; the International Classification of Diseases (ICD-11) is also considering it. This article outlines pros and cons of considering “behavioral addictions” as mental disorders and the DSM-5 decision making processes. It focuses upon three conditions: gambling disorder, Internet gaming disorder, and Internet addiction. The paper details assessment methods and prevalence rates for these conditions and outlines psychiatric comorbidities, demographic and biological risk factors, and promising treatment approaches. The paper also briefly discusses other putative behavioral addictions: eating/food, sex, exercise, shopping, and tanning “addictions.” Overall, data are inconclusive, and consistent terminology and methodology are needed to define and evaluate these conditions more fully prior to considering them mental disorders.
Keywords: DSM-5, gambling disorder, Internet gaming disorder, gaming addiction, Internet addiction
I. Introduction
A. The debate about behavioral addictions
Researchers and clinicians have long deliberated the existence of behavioral addictions. The term “addiction” itself is a loaded one. Some feel this term is stigmatizing, while others propose it appropriately describes the nature of symptoms associated with excessive and problematic use of a substance or engagement in an activity. Not only does the term “addiction” instigate controversy but so does consideration of new mental disorders. In particular, those related to excessive engagement in non-substance activities stimulate considerable debate.
On the one hand, recognizing a constellation of symptoms as a mental disorder allows the condition to be diagnosed and classified consistently. Diagnosis also facilitates study of etiology, prevention, and treatment, which in turn can reduce morbidity and mortality. On the other hand, proliferation of mental disorders can minimize experiences of individuals with pronounced impairments. If chocolate or love addiction are mental disorders, how do they compare to schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, or generalized anxiety disorders with respect to priorities for funding research and treatment?
As society evolves, the expression of mental disorders changes, but is technology creating new disorders or addictions? Are we all addicted to our smartphones? Should a behavior that occurs outside the realm of “normal,” or even on the high end of normal, be considered an illness? Are excessive behavior patterns simply “bad habits,” or are they reflections of underlying neurobiological abnormalities or “hijacked brains?”
These are important questions when considering a new mental disorder, and they are perhaps particularly pertinent in the context of addictions. For centuries, the medical profession overlooked substance use disorders, the classic “addiction.” Still today, substance use disorders are not given the legitimacy of other mental disorders. Treatment and insurance coverage for addictions is separated from other medical conditions, and even from other mental disorders. Insurers may have distinct deductibles and reimbursement maximums for substance use treatment. For non-substance or behavioral addictions, treatments in most countries, if available at all, occur in highly specialized sectors. Most insurers in the United States (US) do not cover their costs at all.
This paper describes the research and debate surrounding behavioral addictions. As we review the evidence for non-substance addictions, we consider issues related to psychiatric nosology in general. By using a broad framework, future research may inform the basic issues surrounding behavioral addictions.
B. The DSM-5 process and decisions
The American Psychiatric Association (APA) convened Workgroups to review research and suggest changes for conditions listed in the DSM-IV and to consider inclusion of new mental disorders. Stein et al. (2010) proposed guidelines for what constitutes a mental disorder: (1) a pattern of symptoms or syndrome, that (2) leads to clinically significant distress or disability; (3) it may stem from psychobiological dysfunction (although exact mechanism may be unknown); but it must not (4) reflect an expected response to stressors or a culturally sanctioned response (e.g. trances during religious rituals) or (5) result primarily from social conflicts or deviance. New mental disorders should have diagnostic validity, defined by demonstrations of prognostic significance, psychobiological disruption and/or treatment response; clinical utility; and diagnostic validators to differentiate disorders from similar conditions. Stein and colleagues (2010) also recognize there are rarely precise boundaries for mental, or medical, disorders. When considering a new condition, potential for benefits such as improved care or outcomes, should outweigh potential harms, such as stigmatization.
The Substance Use and Related Disorders Workgroup evaluated the literature regarding several behavioral, or non-substance, addictions using this framework. These included gambling, Internet gaming, Internet use generally, eating, shopping, sex, exercise, and tanning. After considerable review and consultation with experts, this Workgroup ultimately recommended only one should be included alongside substance use disorders—gambling disorder. This condition was introduced in the DSM in 1980, and substantial data exist regarding its pattern of symptoms, associated distress and disability, differentiation from similar conditions, neurobiology, and treatment response (Petry et al. 2014a). Of the other possible behavioral addictions, the Workgroup recommended including only Internet gaming disorder in Section 3, the research appendix, as a condition requiring further study (Petry et al. 2015). The Workgroup concluded that existing data on other conditions were too preliminary to fit Stein’s (2010) criteria as new mental disorders.
This paper focuses on gambling disorder, gaming disorder, and Internet addiction more generally, because these conditions have the largest evidence base. For each, methods of assessment and prevalence rates are presented. The paper also outlines psychiatric comorbidities and risk factors, such as demographic characteristics, neurobiology, and genetics. It reviews promising treatment approaches and suggests next steps. The paper also describes briefly other putative behavioral addictions prior to outlining specific and common issues for consideration.
II. Specific conditions
A. Gambling disorder
1. Defining features and methods for assessment
Gambling disorder was termed “pathological gambling” in earlier versions of the DSM and was included as an “Impulse control disorder not otherwise specified” before the DSM-5. There are now nine criteria, and a diagnosis necessitates meeting at least four in a one-year period. The criteria are: thinking about gambling excessively (i.e., “pre-occupation”); betting greater amounts (i.e., tolerance); being unable to cease or reduce gambling; exhibiting withdrawal symptoms (restlessness, irritability) when not gambling; gambling to escape adverse moods or problems; attempting to win back losses (i.e., “chasing”); financially relying on others to cover losses; lying about or covering up gambling; and losing important relationships, or a career or educational opportunity, because of gambling. These criteria accurately distinguish persons with clinically significant impairment, and although persons with the disorder endorse some of these criteria more than others, each lends to diagnosis (Petry et al. 2013).
Structured clinical interviews exist for assessing DSM criteria for gambling disorder such as the National Opinion Research Center Screen. It has acceptable to excellent internal consistency, test-retest reliability, construct, prospective, and discriminative validity (Gerstein et al. 1999, Hodgins 2004, Wickwire et al. 2008). The Canadian Problem Gambling Index (CPGI), a commonly used instrument, contains nine items assessing gambling harms, but they are not aligned with the DSM criteria and therefore preclude diagnosis (Currie et al. 2013). Another popular screening instrument is The South Oaks Gambling Screen, but it has high false-positive rates (Stinchfield 2002), so DSM-based instruments are better suited toward establishing prevalence rates. It is also important to note that quantity or frequency items do not predict gambling disorder, and the DSM-5 criteria for gambling disorder, similar to substance use disorders, do not employ quantity or frequency indices (Hasin et al. 2013).
2. Prevalence rates
Many countries have undertaken surveys to determine the prevalence rate of gambling disorder. Table 1 outlines nationally representative studies that utilized DSM-based criteria to assess past-year gambling disorder in 3000 or more respondents through 2016. Rates range from 0.1% to 0.7%, with most estimates indicating about 2 in 1000 persons have clinically significant problems with gambling.
Table 1.
First author (or study), year | Country | N | Ages | Instrument | % | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Bonke, 2006 | Denmark | 8,153 | 18-74 | National Opinion Research Center Screen | 0.1 | ||||||
TNS EMNID, 2011 | Germany | 15,002 | ≥ 18 | Diagnostic Interview Schedule-IV | 0.2 | ||||||
Sproston, 2000 | Great Britain | 7,680 | ≥ 16 | DSM-IV | 0.2 | ||||||
National Council on Problem Gambling, 2015 | Singapore | 3,000 | ≥ 18 | DSM-IV | 0.2 | ||||||
Petry, 2005 | USA | 42,989 | ≥ 18 | NIAAA Alcohol Use Disorder and Associated Interview Schedule-DSM-IV (AUDADIS-IV) | 0.2 | ||||||
Kessler, 2008 | USA | 3,435 | ≥ 18 | Composite International Diagnostic Interview | 0.3 | ||||||
Wardle, 2011 | Great Britain | 7,756 | ≥ 16 | DSM-IV | 0.4 | ||||||
Ólason, 2005 | Iceland | 5,000 | 18-70 | Diagnostic Interview for Gambling Screen | 0.5 | ||||||
Ferris, 2001 | Canada | 3,120 | ≥ 18 | DSM-IV | 0.7 |
3. Comorbidities
Epidemiological studies that assess gambling and other psychiatric disorders ubiquitously find high rates of comorbidity. For example, one nationally representative US study (Kessler et al. 2008) found that 76.3% of individuals with gambling disorder also had a substance use disorder, a rate 5.5 times higher than those without gambling disorder. Similarly, Petry et al. (2005) noted that nearly half the persons with gambling disorder also had alcohol dependence, and over a third an illicit drug use disorder.
Rates of comorbities with other non-addictive psychiatric disorders are also high, including depression and anxiety disorders (Kessler et al. 2008, Petry et al. 2005). A meta-analysis of epidemiological studies (Lorains et al. 2011) found gambling disorder was significantly related to nicotine, substance use, mood, and anxiety disorders, with 60.2%, 57.5%, 37.9% and 37.4% of persons with gambling disorder experiencing these respective conditions. Severity of gambling problems appears to increase with severity of substance use problems, but similar patterns are not evident with other mental health disorders (Rush et al. 2008), suggesting associations between gambling and substance use disorders are unique.
4. Risk factors
Several demographic characteristics are risk factors for gambling disorder, including male gender (Kessler et al. 2008, Petry et al. 2005), and younger age (Kessler et al. 2008). Gambling disorder also occurs more often in lower socioeconomic groups and among racial/ethnic minorities (Kessler et al. 2008, Petry et al. 2005).
Impulsivity is linked with both substance use and gambling disorders. Persons with gambling and substance use disorders are more impulsive than their counterparts with neither condition, and those with both disorders have the highest rates of impulsivity (Petry 2001). Longitudinal studies demonstrate that impulsive behaviors early in childhood are associated with development of these disorders (Pagani et al. 2009, Slutske et al. 2012, Vitaro et al. 1999).
5. Neurobiology and genetics
The reward deficiency theory attempts to explain susceptibility to addictions through impaired functioning in brain regions that control impulsive decision making. It posits that hypoactive reward systems in the striatum and medial prefrontal cortex (PFC) require stronger stimuli such as drugs or gambling to activate them. Compared to controls, persons with gambling disorder have reductions in grey matter volume, particularly within the PFC (Zois et al. 2017). They also have white matter abnormalities in regions including but not limited to the corpus callosum, cingulum, and inferior fronto-occipital fascicle, consistent with findings of substance use disorders (Joutsa et al. 2011). Many studies suggest functional impairment in persons with gambling disorder as well, generally decreased activation in the PFC, but the directionality of effects and regions of impairment sometimes vary. For example, reduced functioning in the ventral striatum is noted in some studies of gamblers relative to controls (Balodis et al. 2012) while others find greater activation (van Holst et al. 2012). The discrepancies may relate to the nature of the tasks or rewards. Gamblers appear particularly sensitive to high-risk rewards; some studies find no differences or reduced neural response when tasks involve low risk or non-monetary rewards, but enhanced activity during presentation of high-risk rewards (Limbrick-Olfield et al. 2013). In some cases, the pattern of results is similar between persons with gambling and substance use disorders (Limbrick-Olfield et al. 2013), but given different methodologies and results, it is difficult to state conclusively the extent to which substance use and gambling disorders have similar or distinct neurobiological features.
Gambling disorder, like substance use disorders, tends to run in families. Twin studies indicate strong evidence for heritability of gambling disorder and between alcohol and gambling disorders (e.g., Slutske et al. 2013). Studies of the molecular genetics of gambling disorder focus primarily on dopamine genes, with some finding an association with a polymorphism of the dopamine 2 receptor gene (DRD2) but not others (see Argawal et al. 2012 for review). Lind et al. (2013) conducted a genome-wide association study of gambling disorder in a large sample of twins and implicated four single nucleotide polymorphisms that appeared to relate to gambling as well as substance use disorders. Overall, molecular genetics findings support epidemiological data on the association of gambling and substance use disorders.
6. Treatments
Six placebo controlled trials of pharmacotherapies for gambling disorder randomized over 25 patients per condition (Table 2). One study of an opioid antagonist, nalmafene, found some benefits relative to placebo (Grant et al. 2006), but follow-up studies yielded no advantage (Grant et al. 2010, Toneatto et al. 2009). In these studies, placebo response rates ranged from 31% (Grant et al. 2006) to 63% (Saiz-Ruiz et al. 2005), underscoring the need to include attention- and expectation-matched control conditions in psychotherapy trials as well.
Table 2.
First author, year | Treatments | N | Weeks | Sessions | Gambling outcomes | Significant effects relative to control or other conditions |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Pharmacotherapies | ||||||
Grant, 2006 | Placebo | 51 | 16 | 8 | PG-YBOCS, CGI | Benefits of nalmafene (25 mg/day) and nalmafene (50 mg/day) vs placebo on PG-YBOCS. Benefits of nalmafene (25 mg/day) vs placebo on CGI. |
Nalmafene (25 mg/day) | 52 | 16 | 8 | |||
Nalmafene (50 mg/day) | 52 | 16 | 8 | |||
Nalmafene (100 mg/day) | 52 | 16 | 8 | |||
Grant, 2010 | Placebo | 74 | 16 | nr | PG-YBOCS, CGI | No differences. |
Nalmafene (20 mg/day) | 77 | 16 | ||||
Nalmafene (40 mg/day) | 82 | 16 | ||||
Toneatto, 2009 | Placebo | 25 | 11 | 7 | $, Days | No differences. |
Naltrexone (25-250mg/day) | 27 | 11 | 7 | |||
Kovanen, 2016 | Placebo | 51 | 20 | 8 | $, Days, PG-YBOCS | No differences. |
Naltrexone (50mg/day) | 50 | 20 | 8 | |||
Grant, 2003 | Placebo | 40 | 16 | 6 | PC-YBOCS, CGI | No differences. |
Paroxetine (10-60 mg/day) | 36 | 16 | 6 | |||
Saiz-Ruiz, 2005 | Placebo | 33 | 24 | nr | CCPGQ, CGI | No differences. |
Sertraline (M = 95 mg/day) | 33 | 24 | ||||
Psychotherapies | ||||||
Smith, 2015 | Exposure therapy | 43 | 12 | 12 | % Improved and VGS | No differences. |
Cognitive therapy | 44 | 12 | 12 | |||
LaBrie, 2012 | Waitlist | 102 | 12 | – | % Abstinent, $, Days | No differences on % Abstinent, $ or Days. |
MI/CB workbook | 108 | 12 | – | |||
MI/CB workbook + call | 105 | 12 | 1 | |||
Petry, 2006 | GA referral | 63 | – | 1 | % Improved, $, Days, SOGS, ASI-G | Benefits of CB therapy on % Improved and $. Benefits of both CB formats on SOGS, ASI-G, Days. |
CB workbook | 84 | 8 | 8 | |||
CB therapy (individual) | 84 | 8 | 8 | |||
Hodgins, 2009 | Waitlist | 65 | 6 | – | % Improved, $, Days, G-SAS | Benefits of CB workbook + both call conditions on $, Days, and G-SAS but not % Improved. |
CB workbook | 82 | 6 | – | |||
CB workbook + MI call | 83 | 6 | 1 | |||
CB workbook + MI calls | 84 | 36 | 7 | |||
Oei, 2010 | Waitlist | 28 | – | – | % Improved, $, Days | No differences on % Improved, $, or Days. |
CB therapy (group) | 37 | 6 | 6 | |||
CB therapy (individual) | 37 | 6 | 6 | |||
Luquiens, 2016 | Waitlist | 264 | 6 | – | PGSI | No differences. |
293 | 6 | – | ||||
Email + CB workbook | 264 | 6 | – | |||
Email, CB workbook & guidance | 301 | 6 | – | |||
Casey, 2017 | Waitlist | 55 | 6 | – | G-SAS,SOGS | Benefits of CB Internet and Feedback + support over waitlist on G-SAS and SOGS. Benefit of CB Internet vs Feedback + support on SOGS. |
Feedback + support by Internet | 59 | 6 | 6 | |||
CB treatment by Internet | 60 | 6 | 6 | |||
Hodgins, 2001 | Waitlist | 35 | 4 | – | % Improved, $ and Days | Benefits of CB workbook + MI call on % Improved, $ and Days. |
CB workbook | 35 | 4 | – | |||
CB workbook + MI call | 32 | 4 | 1 | |||
Petry, 2008 | Assessment | 48 | – | – | % Improved, $, ASI-G | Benefits of Brief Advice on % Improved, $ and ASI-G. |
Brief advice | 37 | 1 | 1 | |||
MI | 55 | 1 | 1 | |||
MI + CB therapy | 40 | 4 | 4 | |||
Petry, 2016a | Brief education | 69 | 1 | 1 | % Improved, $, Days, SOGS | Benefits of Brief Advice on Days. Benefits of MI + CB vs Brief Advice on % Improved, $, SOGS. |
Brief advice | 66 | 1 | 1 | |||
MI + CB therapy | 82 | 4 | 4 | |||
Diskin, 2009 | CB workbook + control interview | 39 | 1 | 1 | % Improved, $, Days, | Benefits on $ but not % Improved or Days. |
CB workbook + MI | 42 | 1 | 1 | |||
Cunning-ham, 2012 | Waitlist | 69 | – | – | $, Days | No differences. |
Partial feedback | 70 | 1 | 1 | |||
Full feedback | 70 | 1 | 1 | |||
Neighbors, 2015 | Attention control | 128 | 1 | 1 | $, Days, SOGS | Benefits on $ and SOGS but not Days. |
Feedback via computer | 124 | 1 | 1 | |||
Martens, 2015 | Assessment | 109 | – | – | $, Days, CPGI | Benefits of Personalized Feedback on $ and CPGI but not Days. Education vs Personalized feedback: no differences. |
Education | 113 | 1 | 1 | |||
Personalized feedback | 111 | 1 | 1 |
Notes: ASI-G = Addiction Severity Index- Gambling scale; CB = Cognitive Behavioral; CCPGQ = Criteria for Control of Pathological Gambling Questionnaire; CGI = Clinical Global Impression Scale; CPGI = Canadian Problem Gambling Inventory; DSM = Diagnostics and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (4th ed.) criteria for pathological gambling; GA = Gamblers Anonymous; G-SAS = Gambling Symptom Assessment Scale; MI = Motivational Interviewing; NODS = National Opinion Research Center DSM-IV Screen for Gambling Problems; PGSI = Problem Gambling Severity Index; PG-YBOCS: Yale-Brown Obsessive-Compulsive Scale for Pathological Gambling; SOGS = South Oaks Gambling Screen; VGS = Victorian Gambling Screen harm to self scale; nr = not reported.
Table 2 outlines psychotherapy trials with a time and attention matched control for at least one of the experimental conditions and randomized 25 or more persons per condition, allowing a reasonably stable estimate of effects (Chambless & Hollon 1998). Some trials of cognitive-behavioral therapy (CBT) alone (Casey et al. 2017, Petry et al. 2006) or combined with motivational interviewing (Petry et al. 2016a) demonstrated benefits relative to control conditions, and therapist contact generally improved outcomes compared to entirely self-directed CBT via workbooks or the Internet (Diskin & Hodgins 2009, Hodgins et al. 2001, 2009, Petry et al. 2006). Still, many studies evaluating interventions comparable in intensity found limited, or no, differences between them (Cunningham et al. 2012, LaBrie et al. 2012, Luquiens et al. 2016, Oei et al. 2010, Petry et al. 2016a, Martens et al. 2015, Smith et al. 2015). No intervention meets criteria as empirically validated (Chambless & Hollon 1998) for gambling disorder.
7. Summary
Gambling disorder is an established mental condition and the American Psychiatric Association recognizes it as the first non-substance “addiction.” It has well-defined criteria and instruments with established psychometric properties, and it is readily distinguishable from other conditions that commonly co-occur, i.e., substance use disorders, depression, bipolar disorder. Reliable risk factors have been identified, and ongoing research is evaluating the neurobiological and genetic basis of gambling disorder. Adequately powered studies with high quality designs suggest some potentially promising interventions to guide treatment efforts, although none yet rise to the level of empirically validated (Chambless & Hollon 1998).
B. Internet gaming disorder
The proposed DSM-5 criteria for Internet gaming disorder (IGD) have some parallels to gambling disorder, and they share some other features as well. The modality of gambling or gaming is not relevant to classifying the conditions, although some forms of gambling (e.g., slot machines; Petry 2007) and gaming (e.g., online games like massively multiplayer online roleplaying games; Rehbein et al. 2010) are more likely to lead to problems than others (e.g., lotteries or offline single player games). Underscoring a common misperception, the DSM-5 text states clearly that gaming offline or on devices that do not connect to the Internet can also constitute the condition. Because Internet-based applications of videogames are most linked with problems and to better distinguish the title of the condition from “gambling disorder,” the term Internet was included, i.e., Internet gaming disorder.
Although the two conditions may overlap on some levels, there are important distinctions as well. First, gambling refers to risking money or items of value in hopes of a larger payout; financially-based gains and losses are less central for gaming. In some videogames, players uncover items or powers that have monetary value, but a primary purpose of playing is not for financial gain, and money is typically not risked, or at least not frequently as is the case for gambling. Second, gambling outcomes are only partially skill dependent (i.e., some card or sports games) or not at all (i.e., lotteries, slot machines), whereas videogame outcomes are largely based on skill or knowledge.
1. Defining features and methods for assessment
Gaming is a popular pastime, especially in male adolescents, many of whom play videogames frequently, with estimates indicating over 90% of boys play videogames and an average of 12 hours per week (Gentile 2009). However, spending a lot of time gaming is not analogous to experiencing problems. Similar to gambling and substance use disorders, time and frequency descriptors are not included in diagnostic criteria (Hasin et al. 2013). The DSM-5 lists nine possible criteria for IGD, with a recommended cut-point of meeting five or more. The criteria are intended to reflect clinically significant distress along these dimensions: pre-occupation with games; tolerance toward games or gaming; inability to cease or reduce gaming; withdrawal symptoms when gaming is not possible; gaming to escape adverse moods or problems; loss of interests in other activities; continued excessive gaming despite knowledge of problems; lying about or covering up gaming; and risking or losing a relationship, job, or vocational or education opportunity because of gaming. Throughout this paper, the term IGD is intended to reflect a severe form of gaming problems, with the understanding that not all studies applied the DSM-5 or any specific criteria.
The DSM-5 establishes a standard for classifying IGD, but it also notes that the criteria and cut-point may not reflect a unique mental disorder, nor may these criteria be optimal. Even the suggested criteria themselves need consistent operationalization. Consensus-derived meanings of the criteria exist (Petry et al. 2014b), and although not all agree with these descriptors (Griffiths et al. 2016, but see Petry et al. 2016b), more consistent and precise measurement of IGD appears to have arisen since the DSM-5.
Some clinical assessments based on the DSM-5 criteria have been tested preliminarily. For example, Ko and colleagues (2014) administered clinical interviews based on the DSM-5 criteria to three groups (N = 75/group): those with current gaming problems; those with past but not current gaming problems; and a control group. Most criteria contributed substantially to classification status, and the cut point of five criteria had the greatest sensitivity and specificity for distinguishing those with normal levels of play from those who had experienced clinically significant problems. Koo et al. (2017) administered a structured assessment to 236 adolescents and found good test-retest reliability over one month, good concordance with clinical impression, and adequate discriminative validity. Although promising, more work is needed evaluating criteria with larger cross cultural samples that span general population, high risk, and clinical groups.
Brief screening instruments based on these DSM-5 criteria also exist, but psychometric testing is limited. For example, Rehbein et al. (2015) administered the Video Game Dependency Scale to 11,003 ninth-graders and found concurrent validity with indices of self-reported play durations, problems in school and sleep difficulties. Students most frequently endorsed criteria for escape and pre-occupation, but these rarely related to diagnosis, while giving up other activities, tolerance and withdrawal criteria were most associated with IGD classification in this sample. Pontes et al. (2014) administered a 20-item scale (the IGD-20) to 1003 gamers recruited from online gaming forums. Items had good internal consistency, and a score of 71 of 100 points maximized sensitivity and specificity for distinguishing problematic from non-problematic players as ascertained from response patterns on this same scale. One concern about evaluating screening instruments in non-clinical contexts is that no “gold standard” exists by which to compare scores on a self-report measure. It is imperative that some clinical or objective index of harm is assessed prior to considering any instrument validated.
2. Prevalence
Determining prevalence rates is clearly difficult when criteria are not established and instruments with sound psychometric properties are lacking. Table 3 outlines estimated rates of IGD from large-scale studies containing at least 3,000 respondents conducted in school or general population samples through 2016. It does not include surveys of convenience samples, such as online gamers, nor those that assessed a myriad of online activities of which gaming was one. Prevalence rates ranged from 0.3% to up to 4.9%, with over half the studies finding rates below 2.0%. Rates varied across studies, in part because of differences across samples, instruments and criteria. Given concerns about classification, these rates should be interpreted cautiously.
Table 3.
First author, year | Country | N | Ages | Instrument | % | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Wittek, 2016 | Norway | 10,081 | 16-74 | Game Addiction Scale | 0.3 – 0.5 | ||||||
Rehbein, 2015 | Germany | 11,003 | 13-18 | Video Game Dependency Scale | 1.2 | ||||||
Van Rooij, 2011 | Netherlands | 4,559 | 13-16 | Compulsive Internet Use Scale | 1.5 | ||||||
Muller, 2015 | Europe | 12,938 | 14-17 | Assessment of Internet and Computer Game Addiction | 1.6 | ||||||
Rehbein, 2010 | Germany | 15,168 | Average 15 | Video Game Dependency Scale | 1.7 | ||||||
Johansson, 2004 | Norway | 3,237 | 12-18 | Young Internet Addiction scale- revised for gaming | 2.7 | ||||||
Festl, 2013 | Germany | 4,382 | 14-90 | Game Addiction Scale | 3.7 | ||||||
Papay, 2013 | Hungary | 5,045 | Average 16 | Problematic Online Gaming Questionnaire | 4.6 | ||||||
Desai, 2010 | USA | 4,028 | 14-18 | Impulse Control Disorder- Revised for Gaming Scale | 4.9 |
Percentages reported reflect people with Internet gaming disorder or a similar condition reflecting relatively severe problems.
3. Comorbidities
Depression and IGD co-occur in multiple studies (Desai et al. 2010, Gentile et al. 2011, van Rooij et al. 2011). Individuals with IGD also exhibit high rates of anxiety, especially social anxiety (Gentile et al. 2011, van Rooij et al. 2014). Impulsivity and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) also appear at high rates with IGD (Choo et al. 2010, Gentile 2009, Walther et al. 2012). Chan and Rabinowitz (2006) surveyed adolescents and their parents about electronic game use, Internet use more generally, and symptoms of ADHD. Compared with adolescents who played games less than one hour a day, those who played more than an hour a day scored higher on scales assessing inattention and ADHD, but ADHD scores did not differ with respect to use of other electronic media (television or Internet use generally), suggesting the association between heavy electronic use and inattention may be specific to gaming.
Some research links IGD with substance use disorders. In US high school students (Desai et al. 2010), those with gaming problems had higher rates of smoking and some forms of illicit drug use. In Dutch students, excessive online gaming was associated with smoking, drinking and marijuana use (van Rooij et al. 2014). A study of German students (Walther et al. 2012) also noted a relation between marijuana use and problem gaming. This study also evaluated associations between personality characteristics and substance use, gambling, and game playing. It found that these behaviors had the feature of impulsivity in common. Impulsivity is a core aspect of substance use disorders and may play a role in the development or maintenance of other excessive behavioral patterns as well.
4. Risk factors
Males are much more likely than females to have IGD in most all prevalence studies, and adolescents develop this condition at higher rates than adults (Festl et al. 2013, Wittek et al. 2016). The media depicts extraordinarily problems with Internet gaming in some Southeast Asian countries, and some data suggest that US youth who are of racial and ethnic minority backgrounds, particularly Asian (Desai et al. 2010), have higher rates of IGD than Caucasians. Nevertheless, studies using parallel procedures in the US and Singapore (Choo et al. 2010, Gentile 2009) found similar rates.
Longitudinal studies find that attention problems, depression and anxiety may arise from excessive gaming, while impulsivity and low social competence appear to predict development of IGD over time. For example, Swing et al. (2010) found that children who increased gaming over a one-year period developed more attention problems than those who did not. Gentile et al. (2011) found impulsivity, low social competence, and poor emotion regulation predicted development of Internet gaming problems over time; in this study depression and anxiety more often arose from, rather than lead to, IGD. Rehbein and Baier (2013) noted that 10-year olds from single parent families who were not well integrated at school were more likely to develop gaming problems over the next 5 years than their counterparts without these characteristics. Additional systematic investigation is needed along with a greater understanding of the directionality between IGD and other psychological conditions, and whether these relationships differ in youth compared to adults.
5. Neurobiology and genetics
Neurobiological studies of IGD, similarly to gambling and substance use disorders, focus on the PFC. Some studies find changes in grey matter in the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC), bilateral insula, and supplemental motor area (Weng et al. 2013, Yuan et al. 2013). Others find differences in regions linked more broadly to decision making, emotion, and cognitive demand including decreased grey matter in the inferior frontal and cingulate gyrus, lingual, hippocampus, and precuneus, as well as white matter changes in the inferior frontal and lingual gyrus, insula, precuneus, anterior cingulate, and amygdala (Lin et al. 2015). Weng and colleagues (2013) reported an inverse relation between right OFC and bilateral insula grey matter atrophy and severity of gaming problems, while Dong et al. (2012a) noted increased white matter integrity in the thalamus and the posterior cingulate cortex was associated with severity of gaming problems. Thus, not all studies yield consistent findings.
Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies in IGD commonly apply cue-induced craving tasks, in which participants view game-related stimuli while recording brain responses. Findings from participants with gaming problems resemble, to some degree, those with substance use disorders, with greater activation in the PFC as well as the anterior cingulate cortex and insula (Ko et al. 2013). During resting state in IGD (Dong et al. 2012b), anomalies in the cerebellum and inferior parietal lobule, regions related to sensory-motor coordination have been reported, and similar patterns in these areas may also be related to predisposition for substance use disorders (Moulton et al. 2014).
Not surprisingly, little data are available regarding the genetics of IGD. One study (Han et al. 2007) reports that, compared to controls, youth with IGD were more likely to have the Taq1A1 allele of the DRD2 gene and the low activity allele Val158Met of the Catecholamine-O-Methyltransferase gene, that regulates dopamine. The degree to which IGD is heritable and linked with genes that increase risk for substance use and other problems requires further study.
6. Treatments
Few controlled trials for IGD treatments exist. Table 4 outlines the six randomized trials available that included more than 25 participants per condition. Two pharmacotherapy trials (Han & Renshaw 2012, Song et al. 2016) found benefits of bupropion, an antidepressant, relative to placebo or a no treatment control condition, respectively. However, one of them (Han & Renshaw 2012) included only patients with comorbid IGD and major depressive disorder, so one possibility is that the medication primarily impacted depression and only improved gaming indirectly. Another found that adding CBT to bupropion improved outcomes relative to bupropion alone (Kim et al. 2012) but had no control condition. No differences on gaming outcomes were noted between two medications for ADHD in adolescents with IGD (Park et al. 2016) or between a game-related speaking and writing intervention and a general educational intervention (Kim et al. 2013). These data suggest that bupropion may have benefits in reducing gaming. However, these results should be considered preliminary due to the small number of trials, short durations of treatment and follow-up, inclusion of comorbid conditions that on their own may respond to the therapies, and in some cases lack of matched control conditions.
Table 4.
First author, year | Treatments | N | Weeks | Sessions | Gaming outcomes | Significant effects relative to control or other conditions |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Pharmacotherapies | ||||||
Park, 2016 | Atomexetine | 42 | 12 | – | Young Internet Addiction Scale | No differences. |
Methylphenidate | 44 | 12 | – | |||
Han, 2012 | Placebo + education | 25 | 8 | – | Young Internet Addiction Scale, time gambling | Benefits of bupropion on both indices. |
Bupropion + education | 25 | 8 | – | |||
Song, 2016 | No treatment control | 33 | 6 | – | Young Internet Addiction Scale | Benefits of both medications compared to control. Benefits of bupropion compared to escitalopram. |
Bupropion | 44 | 6 | – | |||
Escitalopram | 42 | 6 | – | |||
Psychotherapies | ||||||
Kim, 2012 | Bupropion | 33 | 8 | – | Young Internet Addiction Scale, time gaming | Benefit of added CBT on both indices. |
Bupropion + cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT) | 32 | 8 | 8 | |||
Kim, 2013 | General education course | 32 | 8 | 21 | Time gaming | No differences. |
Massive Multiplayer Online Role-Playing Games speaking and writing course | 27 | 8 | 21 |
7. Summary
Inclusion of IGD in the DSM-5 provides a uniform direction for studying this condition, but a greater understanding of the criteria that underlie it are needed, along with development and assessment of psychometric properties of clinical interviews and screening tools. Instruments should: reflect criteria in a comprehensive, and understandable, manner; apply to persons across the age span; be relevant cross culturally; assess online as well as offline gaming behaviors and problems; and allow for a categorical decision based on the number of criteria endorsed. Determining internal consistency, factor structure, and concurrent validity with other indices of distress are necessary, but not sufficient, steps to establishing psychometrically sound instruments. Evaluating test-retest reliability, validity with collateral reports and objective indices, and prospective validity in multiple samples, including clinical populations, is critical. Once psychometrically sound instruments exist, epidemiological surveys can ascertain prevalence rates.
Another important consideration relates to the relationship of IGD and other mental disorders, including substance use disorders. For IGD to be a mental disorder, it must be unique from other psychiatric conditions, and if it is a behavioral addiction then it should be aligned more closely with substance use disorders and other behavioral addictions (i.e., gambling disorder) than other mental disorders such as ADHD. Ultimately, neurobiological and genetic studies may guide an understanding of these conditions, and longitudinal studies are important for examining their natural course, including the extent to which persons “mature out” of gaming problems on their own. Growing evidence suggests clinically significant harms arise in at least a small subset of gamers, but the extent to which these problems are transient or foretell long-term adverse effects when left untreated remains unknown.
C. Internet addiction
People can use the Internet problematically for many activities, including gambling, gaming, socializing (e.g., Facebook), information gathering, shopping, and pornography viewing. This section outlines research related to a condition known as Internet addiction (IA). Unless otherwise stated, IA is intended to relate to problematic usage of the Internet for a range of activities. It is meant to exclude problematic Internet-based gambling and gaming for the purposes of this paper, although oftentimes reports do not distinguish activities.
1. Defining features and methods for assessment
The proposed DSM-5 criteria for IGD could be applied to problems that arise from extensive Internet use beyond gaming, but the DSM-5 cautions that care should be taken in making direct applications of IGD criteria toward other Internet uses. Simply adapting existing criteria to new indications may overlook important aspects, and psychometric testing of other applications of the criteria is necessary prior to considering them appropriate for classifying addictions to the Internet or other activities.
Similar to IGD, much research on IA suffers from non standardized definitions and applications of multiple instruments addressing different symptoms and constructs. One review (Laconi et al. 2014) identified 45 different instruments to assess IA. Relatively little information on their psychometric properties is available, and what does exist is derived primarily from cross-sectional survey studies, often devoid of testing in clinical contexts. Among the most commonly used instruments is the Internet Addiction Test (IAT; Young 1998), a 20-item measure based on the DSM-IV criteria for substance dependence and pathological gambling, derived from the Young Internet Addiction Diagnostic Questionnaire. Some studies indicate that the IAT has adequate internal consistency, test-retest reliability, convergent validity with other IA measures, and convergent validity with time spent online (see Laconi et al. 2014 for a review). However, comparisons across studies highlight potential concerns about reliability and stability of the factor structure. There are also insufficient data on its validity and cut-off scores.
Many instruments are also designed to tap specific problematic aspects of Internet use, with social networks being one, or methods of accessing the Internet, such as smartphone addiction. For example, The Bergen Facebook Addiction Scale (BFAS; Andreassen et al. 2012) addresses problems with this social media site; it has good internal consistency and test-retest reliability and correlates with other problem indices, but there are limited data on its association with objective or clinical indices of harm. Likewise, several smartphone addiction scales exist (e.g., Kwon et al. 2013), but evaluation of their validity is lacking.
It is unclear whether discrete items and instruments are needed to address each specific form of excessive Internet use, or if these putative conditions could be assessed with a more parsimonious approach by a single tool. On one hand, some applications of the Internet seem to attract quite distinct groups of persons and may lead to discrete problems. For example, people who use social networks excessively may have different risk factors and comorbidities and they appear to develop different problems than those who view pornography online problematically. On the other hand, given the extent to which applications on the Internet change over time, developing and assessing instruments for each new site or function will become unwieldy and quickly outdated.
A consensus on defining features of IA is required to move this field forward and may best follow by thoughtfully considering research related to IGD. A point of caution is that clinically significant harms must be documented for any condition to be considered a mental disorder, and criteria and instruments that assess heavy usage, or cognitions alone irrespective of harms are unlikely to be assessing “addiction” as a clinical construct.
2. Prevalence rates
Prevalence rates of IA generally, or its specific formats, are difficult to estimate because of inconsistencies in instruments and thresholds for classifications. Because hundreds of studies have been published, Table 5 focuses on surveys of over 3,000 respondents drawn from nationally representative general population samples conducted through 2016. Across these studies, prevalence rates range from 0.6% to over 22%. These 17 studies utilized 10 different instruments, clearly leading to the discrepancies in rates.
Table 5.
First author, year | Country | N | Ages | Instrument | % |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Kim, 2016 | Korea | 6510 | 18-64 | Internet Addiction Test | 0.6 |
Bakken, 2009 | Norway | 3399 | 16-74 | Young Diagnostic Questionnaire | 1.0 |
Rumpf, 2014 | Germany | 15023 | 14-64 | Compulsive Internet Use Scale | 1.0 |
Tsitsika, 2014 | 7 European countries * | 13284 | 14-17 | Internet Addiction Test | 1.2 |
Mak, 2014 | 6 Asian countries** | 5366 | 12-18 | Internet Addiction Test | 1.2-4.9 |
Johanssson, 2004 | Norway | 3237 | 12-18 | Young Diagnostic Questionnaire | 2.0 |
Müller, 2017 | Germany | 9293 | 12-19 | Assessment of Internet and Computer Game Addiction | 2.6 |
Ha, 2014 | Korea | 56086 | 12-18 | Korean-Internet addiction short form | 2.8 |
Yoo, 2014 | Korea | 73328 | 12-19 | Internet Addiction Self-Diagnosis Brief Scale | 3.0 |
Macur, 2016 | Slovenia | 6282 | 18-95 | Problematic Internet Use Questionnaire-Short Form | 3.1 |
Durkee, 2012 | 11 primarily European countries*** | 12395 | Mean 15 | Young Diagnostic Questionnaire | 4.4 |
Li, 2014 | China | 24013 | 7-15 | Young Diagnostic Questionnaire | 6.3 |
Wang, 2013 | China | 95322 | 13-24 | Diagnostic Questionnaire for Internet Addiction | 7.5 |
Mihara, 2016 | Japan | 100050 | 12-19 | Young Diagnostic Questionnaire | 7.9 |
Cao, 2011 | China | 17599 | 10-24 | Young Internet Addiction Scale | 8.1 |
Lin, 2011 | Taiwan | 3616 | College | Chen Internet Addiction Scale – Revised | 15.3 |
Ahmadi, 2014 | Iran | 4342 | Mean 16 | Young Internet Addiction Scale | 22.2 |
Countries were: Spain, Romania, Poland, Greece, The Netherlands, Germany, and Iceland.
Countries were: China, Hong Kong, Japan, South Korea, Malaysia, and Philippines.
Countries were: Austria, Estonia, France, Germany, Hungary, Ireland, Israel, Italy, Romania, Slovenia, and Spain.
3. Comorbidities
Carli et al. (2013) identified 20 studies evaluating comorbidities between IA and other mental disorders. Although not all 20 studies assessed each of these other conditions, 100% of those that evaluated IA along with symptoms of ADHD found a significant association between the two conditions. For depression, 75% of studies found a relationship, 60% for obsessive-compulsive symptoms, and 57% for anxiety. This review did not include substance use disorders, but links between IA and substance use problems are reported as well (Ko et al. 2008, Yen et al. 2009).
4. Risk factors
Men and women appear equally likely to exhibit general IA symptoms (Durkee et al. 2012), but studies specific to problems with social network sites reveal women have more difficulties with these applications (Andreassen & Pallesen 2014). Young age is also generally associated with greater difficulties (Bakken et al. 2009; Kim et al. 2016; Macur et al. 2016; Rumpf et al. 2014). Three studies of IA using parallel procedures in multiple countries (Durkee et al. 2012; Mak et al., 2014; Tsitsika et al., 2014) reveal some, but not marked, differences in prevalence rates across European and Asian countries.
Several longitudinal studies of IA reveal that difficulties related to gaming in particular predict continued IA problems over time (Hokby et al. 2016, Ko et al. 2007, Stavropoulos et al. 2017). In addition, prospective studies find emotional problems, such as depression, ADHD, social phobia, and hostility, are associated with developing IA problems up to two years later (Ko et al. 2009, Strittmatter et al. 2016). Family factors, including parental conflict and lack of regulating Internet use, predicted IA in adolescents as well (Ko et al. 2015). Systematic evaluation of IA, family functioning and other mental disorders is necessary to confirm these initial findings to ascertain whether IA is a unique mental disorder or merely an expression of other mental disorders, and which factors precipitate or stem from IA.
5. Neurobiology and genetics
Similarly to substance use populations, persons with IA have lower fractional anisotropy, an index of white matter abnormalities (Lin et al. 2012) and decreased grey matter volume in the left anterior and posterior cingulate cortex, left insula, and left lingual gyrus (Zhou et al. 2011). Studies of fMRI resting state scans in those with IA find increased regional homogeneity in frontal and anterior cingulate cortex regions (Liu et al. 2010), which may reflect greater sensitivity to reward. Studies using similar regional homogeneity assessment methods, however, have only been applied preliminarily to substance use disorder populations. Replications of these neurobiological findings are needed to determine specificity to different primary contexts of IA as well as generalization to other established addictions.
In terms of genetics, one study reports serotonin transporter genes may play a role in IA (Lee et al. 2008), and others (Li et al. 2014, Vink et al. 2016) show that genetic factors explain between 48% and 66% of the variance in IA. Without a consistent and valid method of classifying IA, however, genetic studies may be premature.
6. Treatments
Only two randomized trials of treatment for IA with more than 25 patients per condition are published. Zhong et al. (2011) reported 3 month post-treatment improvements with a group-based family intervention relative to treatment as usual, but no differences during treatment. Zhu et al. (2012) noted that electroacupuncture reduced IA symptoms, and CBT added to this effect. However, due to concerns about classifications and differences in intensities of the interventions evaluated within studies, results are preliminary.
7. Summary
The literature related to IA, not surprising given its greater heterogeneity, is more mixed than that of IGD. Many assessment instruments for IA do not distinguish gaming from other Internet applications, and because gaming is one of the most common applications with which people develop problems, it is unclear the extent to which some of this research reflects gaming problems and not the broader construct of IA. Although many, and some very large, nationally representative studies of IA exist, prevalence rate vary dramatically, from 0.6% to over 22%. Before this field can advance, consensus must be achieved on its defining features, and reliable and valid instruments to assess problems and harms are needed. If there are few differences between gaming and other Internet applications, then they can and should be considered a similar condition. On the other hand, if data demonstrate unique and/or more pronounced and persistent harms with gaming (or other specific applications), then combining conditions will obscure an understanding of them.
III. Other putative behavioral addictions
The literature on other manifestations of potential behavioral addictions, including sex, shopping, exercise, tanning, and eating, suffers from many of the concerns noted for IGD and IA (see Petry 2013, Starcevic & Khazaal 2017, for review). With the exception of shopping and perhaps eating addiction, which have somewhat accepted criteria and instruments, others have a myriad of methods of assessment, few of which have been subjected to extensive psychometric testing. Limited information is available with respect to rates of these conditions in the general population, and most often prevalence data, when available, are obtained from convenience or high risk samples. Little data exist on these conditions with respect to overlap with other mental health disorders, longitudinal course, or neurobiology. In the case of sex addiction, many of the symptoms appear more aligned with sexual disorders than substance use or behavioral addictions. Likewise, eating addiction may have more similarities with eating disorders than other addictions, and shopping addiction may be more closely associated with hoarding disorder than behavioral addictions. Although APA Workgroups considered these conditions for the DSM-5, insufficient evidence was found to support their inclusion in general, or in relation to addictions in particular.
IV. Recommendations
Overall, it is clear that some persons develop a constellation of symptoms from excessive participation in non-substance behaviors. For the case of gambling disorder, data are clear that it is a unique mental disorder. Criteria are established and function well in distinguishing persons with clinically significant impairment related to gambling from those without. Of course, it is possible that other criteria may better or more accurately classify this condition than those in the DSM-5, and future versions may alter criteria or cut points; indeed, this was the process that led to its changes in the DSM-5 relative to DSM-IV (Petry et al. 2013). Interview and self-report scales, respectively, diagnose and screen for gambling disorder, and although not all have undergone thorough psychometric testing, the items across instruments are more similar than dissimilar in terms of content validity for specific criteria. General population prevalence surveys that employ the DSM criteria yield relatively similar rates, regardless of the instrument applied. Although many people do go in and out of periods of gambling disorder over the course of their lifetimes, about half persist with problems (Petry et al. 2005). Gambling disorder is unique from other conditions with which it commonly occurs, such as depression and substance use. Given overlap in neurobiological and genetic findings, it appears better aligned with substance use than with other mental disorders. The neurobiology of gambling disorder is still not well understood, but this is the case for most mental disorders. Studies should attempt to isolate unique neurobiological dysfunction leading to and stemming from gambling disorder, as well as their commonalities with other addictions.
Although many randomized treatment trials of fairly large size exist for gambling, the field lacks standardized outcome measures, with some focusing only on quantity/frequency of gambling and others on clinically significant harms, and only a few employing collateral reports to confirm patient self-reports. As more trials are conducted, investigators should consider existing interventions. Rather than developing new versions of CBT, motivational interviewing or feedback, investigating mechanisms of action of promising existing interventions will probably advance the field more rapidly than testing modest adaptations of similar approaches. Considering the literature in psychotherapies generally, trials comparing two active interventions, group versus individual therapy, or longer and shorter interventions of a similar type rarely yield large effect sizes. Randomized trials should be adequately powered and employ long-term follow-ups, as gambling disorder is clearly a condition that waxes and wanes, and few interventions to date have yielded persistent benefits.
In the case of IGD, the proposed DSM-5 criteria require additional study to determine if each adds to classification of a unique condition that results in distress or disability, and whether there are any additional criteria that improve accuracy. Consistent operationalization of the criteria is also important. Even since publication of the DSM-5, different instruments are assessing the same criteria sometimes in markedly different manners (Petry et al. 2014b). Evaluation of the criteria should be conducted, at least in part, with clinical samples. However, because persons who seek treatment tend to have more severe forms of disorders, evaluation in non-treatment seeking individuals is also important. Much of the psychometric work on IGD has focused on non-clinical samples using self-report methods. These samples and methods of assessment add information, but any screening instrument needs to be tested with clinical samples before it can be considered validated.
Once criteria and instruments with good psychometric properties are established, prevalence rates can be determined. Longitudinal studies are needed to ascertain the natural course of the condition and its long term consequences, as well as its association with other mental disorders. Neurobiology and genetic studies ultimately may uncover that IGD is similar– or distinct– from other behavioral addictions or substance use disorders.
Randomized treatment trials for IGD have primarily focused on pharmacotherapies, but it is likely that psychotherapy will also play an important role in treating this condition. Again, assessment issues are paramount, as trials need to employ reliable and valid instruments to detect changes in gaming and IGD symptoms, using an objective indicator if possible. Treatment trials of psychotherapies should include therapy manuals and employ methods to assess delivery of interventions, and they should evaluate long-term effects. They also need to be adequately powered to detect between group differences.
In terms of IA, a better understanding of what it constitutes is needed, including specific adverse effects. Impairment must go beyond transient cognitions (e.g., feeling like one is “missing out” when not checking devices or sites) to rise to a clinically significant level. Likewise, forgoing household chores or sleep to spend time on the Internet is not a clinically significant manifestation of a mental disorder unless it leads to substantial distress and impairment. Many people check their smartphones multiple times an hour, but this does not mean they have a mental disorder. Lots of people would rather watch television than go to bed, and television viewing is not a mental disorder, even if one loses sleep by doing so.
Related, for activities that are problematic and done on the Internet exclusively or primarily, are the precursors– and consequences– more different or similar across applications? It is likely that excessive pornography viewing on the Internet is distinct from excessive use of social media sites, excessive shopping on the Internet, excessive gambling on the Internet, and excessive gaming, on or off the Internet. In the data available, IA– in at least some of its applications– appears more aligned with other disorders than with substance use disorders.
Although the Internet may present activities in manners that are more likely to lead to problems than traditional formats, does this mean that the Internet is the addiction any more than a needle and a syringe are a vector to obtain more powerful effects from drugs of abuse? Most would argue that a person addicted to intravenous heroin has opioid use disorder while one addicted to cocaine has cocaine use disorder, whether he administers the drug intravenously, intranasally or orally. And, while cocaine and opioid use disorders have many important similarities, they also have some important distinctions biologically. They have unique withdrawal profiles and different efficacious treatment approaches, such as specific pharmacotherapies for opioid use disorders. Our understanding of them would be greatly hindered were they not distinguished nosologically.
Likewise, manifestations of IA, whether they relate to gaming, social networking, information gathering, pornography viewing or other activities, should be studied initially on their own. If substantial overlap exists, then and only then should parallel criteria be applied. Even if similar criteria constitute multiple forms of addictions, prevalence rates, longitudinal courses, comorbidities with other conditions, neurobiology and other risk factors, and treatments may still differ markedly. A primary purpose of classification systems in psychiatry is to carefully and systematically define conditions in a reliable and valid manner so they can be better understood and treated. Balance must be achieved between too restrictive a system that hinders generalizability and too open a system, in which marked heterogeneity obscures knowledge.
As these fields progress, investigators and clinicians should consider other areas of psychiatry (Stein et al. 2010) and closely evaluate the research that predated other conditions added to the DSM-5. Issues that impact consideration of behavioral addictions as a class, or as specific forms of mental disorders, are similar to issues arising in the context of other mental disorders. Hoarding disorder, for example, suffered from some analogous criticisms as behavioral addictions now do, yet its evidence base developed sufficiently to justify inclusion in the DSM-5. Research has not yet established whether IGD or IA (in any of its potential manifestations) have diagnostic validity or clinical utility on their own, or in regards to differentiation from one another or other mental disorders. Until such data exist, official recognition of these and other forms of behavioral addictions as mental disorders is premature. Indiscriminately lumping together multiple forms of problematic behaviors will likely cloud an understanding of each, and behaviors addictions as a whole. In contrast, reliable and valid data demonstrating similarities in clinically significant symptoms, patterns and outcomes across multiple contexts ultimately may provide convincing evidence of a common underlying behavioral addiction diagnosis.
Table 6.
Author year | Treatments | N | Weeks | Sessions | Internet use outcomes | Significant effects relative to control or other conditions |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Zhong, 2011 | Group-based treatment as usual | 29 | n.r. | n.r | Online Cognition Scale | No between-group statistical test at post-treatment. Benefit of family intervention at 3 months post-treatment. |
Group-based family intervention | 28 | 14 | 14 | |||
Zhu, 2012 | Electroacupuncture (EA) | 39 | n.r. | 20 | Young Internet Addiction Test | Benefit of CBT+EA compared to both other groups. Benefit of EA alone compared to CBT alone. |
Cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT) | 36 | n.r. | 10 | |||
CBT + EA | 37 | n.r. | 30 |
Summary Points.
Substantial debate exists about the existence of behavioral addictions and the recognition of such problems as mental disorders. Currently, the 5th edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual (DSM-5) includes gambling disorder in the Substance-Related and Addictive Disorders section and Internet gaming disorder in the research appendix as a condition requiring further study.
Gambling disorder has been studied more than any other behavioral addiction, has well-defined criteria, and can be distinguished from other similar mental disorders. Some research has identified potentially efficacious treatments for gambling disorder but none that reach rigorous empirical standards for evidence-based treatments.
Inclusion of Internet gaming disorder (IGD) in the DSM-5 has provided a set of guidelines for defining and studying this condition. Major weaknesses in the research literature on IGD are the lack of psychometrically sound instruments for screening and diagnosis and a lack of sufficient research establishing IGD as a mental disorder unique from other psychiatric conditions.
Researchers have not used consistent definitions of Internet addiction, resulting in substantial heterogeneity in definitions and assessment across studies. More research is needed to distinguish Internet addiction from other potential mental disorders, particularly Internet gaming disorder, and to establish a consensus on its criteria and assessment.
Other putative behavioral addictions, including sex, shopping, exercise, tanning, and eating, currently lack empirical evidence related to assessment and overlap with established mental disorders. The American Psychiatric Association’s workgroups considered each of these potential disorders for inclusion in the DSM-5 but none had sufficient evidence to support their inclusion.
FUTURE ISSUES.
Determining the neurobiological dysfunctions leading to and stemming from gambling disorder or any of the putative behavioral addictions. Establishing whether these dysfunctions are unique to the specific behavioral addiction or are common to other addictions and mental disorders.
Development and validation of measures for behavioral addictions, particularly Internet gaming disorder.
For Internet addiction and other putative behavioral addictions, understanding the criteria for each potential disorder and whether associated impairments are clinically significant. Differentiating each of the behavioral disorders from one another and from other already established mental disorders. Evaluating their longitudinal course.
Evaluation of mechanisms of action for existing treatments for gambling disorder, using trials that are adequately powered and include long term follow-ups.
Development and evaluation of psychotherapy approaches for the treatment of Internet gaming disorder. Such treatment trials should include reliable and valid instruments, approaches to ensure treatment fidelity, adequate sample sizes, and evaluation of long-term effects. Development of treatments for other putative behavioral addictions may be warranted if their diagnostic validity can be demonstrated.
Acknowledgments
Preparation of this manuscript was supported, in part, by grants P50-DA09241, P60-AA03510, and K23-DA034879 from the National Institutes of Health (NIH). The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the NIH.
Footnotes
DISCLOSURE STATEMENT
Dr. Petry was a member of the American Psychiatric Association’s Substance Use and Related Disorders Workgroup for the DSM-5. The authors are not aware of any other affiliations, memberships, funding, or financial holdings that might be perceived as affecting the objectivity of this review.
Contributor Information
Nancy M. Petry, University of Connecticut School of Medicine, USA
Kristyn Zajac, University of Connecticut School of Medicine, USA.
Meredith K. Ginley, University of Connecticut School of Medicine, USA
LITERATURE CITED
- Agrawal A, Verweij KJ, Gillespie NA, Heath AC, Lessov-Schlaggar CN, et al. The genetics of addiction-a translational perspective. Transl Psychiatry. 2012;2:e140. doi: 10.1038/tp.2012.54. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ahmadi K. Internet addiction among Iranian adolescents: a nationwide study. Acta Med Iran. 2014;52(6):467–72. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Andreassen CS, Pallesen S. Social network site addiction - an overview. Curr Pharm Des. 2014;20(25):4053–61. doi: 10.2174/13816128113199990616. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Andreassen CS, Torsheim T, Brunborg GS, Pallesen S. Development of a Facebook Addiction Scale. Psychol Rep. 2012;110(2):501–17. doi: 10.2466/02.09.18.PR0.110.2.501-517. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bakken IJ, Wenzel HG, Götestam KG, Johansson A, Oren A. Internet addiction among Norwegian adults: A stratified probability sample study. Scand J Psychol. 2009;50(2):121–7. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9450.2008.00685.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Balodis IM, Kober H, Worhunsky PD, Stevens MC, Pearlson GD, Potenza MN. Diminished frontostriatal activity during processing of monetary rewards and losses in pathological gambling. Biol Psychiatry. 2012;71(8):749–57. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.01.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bonke J, Borregaard K. The Prevalence and Heterogeneity of At-risk and Pathological Gamblers: The Danish Case. Social for sknings instituttet; 2006. [Google Scholar]
- Cao H, Sun Y, Wan Y, Hao J, Tao F. Problematic Internet use in Chinese adolescents and its relation to psychosomatic symptoms and life satisfaction. BMC Public Health. 2011;11:802. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-11-802. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carli V, Durkee T, Wasserman D, Hadlaczky G, Despalins R, et al. The association between pathological internet use and comorbid psychopathology: A systematic review. Psychopathology. 2013;46(1):1–13. doi: 10.1159/000337971. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Casey LM, Oei TP, Raylu N, Horrigan K, Day J, Ireland M, Clough BA. Internet-based delivery of Cognitive Behaviour Therapy compared to monitoring, feedback and support for problem gambling: A randomised controlled trial. J Gambl Stud. 2017 doi: 10.1007/s10899-016-9666-y. Epub ahead of print. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chambless DL, Hollon SD. Defining empirically supported therapies. J Consult Clin Psychol Review. 1998;66(1):7–18. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.66.1.7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chan PA, Rabinowitz T. A cross-sectional analysis of video games and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder symptoms in adolescents. Ann Gen Psychiatry. 2006;5:16. doi: 10.1186/1744-859X-5-16. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Choo H, Gentile DA, Sim T, Li D, Khoo A, Liau AK. Pathological video-gaming among Singaporean youth. Ann Acad Med Singapore. 2010;39(11):822–9. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cunningham JA, Hodgins DC, Toneatto T, Murphy M. A randomized controlled trial of a personalized feedback intervention for problem gamblers. PLoS One. 2012;7(2):e31586. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0031586. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Currie SR, Hodgins DC, Casey DM. Validity of the Problem Gambling Severity Index interpretive categories. J Gambl Stud. 2013;29(2):311–27. doi: 10.1007/s10899-012-9300-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Desai RA, Krishnan-Sarin S, Cavallo D, Potenza MN. Video-gaming among high school students: Health correlates, gender differences, and problematic gaming. Pediatrics. 2010;126(6):e1414–24. doi: 10.1542/peds.2009-2706. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Diskin KM, Hodgins DC. A randomized controlled trial of a single session motivational intervention for concerned gamblers. Behav Res Ther. 2009;47(5):382–8. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2009.01.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dong G, DeVito E, Huang J, Du X. Diffusion tensor imaging reveals thalamus and posterior cingulate cortex abnormalities in internet gaming addicts. J Psychiatr Res. 2012a;46(9):1212–6. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2012.05.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dong G, Huang J, Du X. Alterations in regional homogeneity of resting-state brain activity in internet gaming addicts. Behav Brain Funct. 2012b;8:41. doi: 10.1186/1744-9081-8-41. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Durkee T, Kaess M, Carli V, Parzer P, Wasserman C, et al. Prevalence of pathological internet use among adolescents in Europe: Demographic and social factors. Addiction. 2012;107(12):2210–22. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2012.03946.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ferris J, Wynne H. The Canadian problem gambling index. Ottawa, ON: Canadian Centre on Substance Abuse; 2001. [Google Scholar]
- Festl R, Scharkow M, Quandt T. Problematic computer game use among adolescents, younger and older adults. Addiction. 2013;108(3):592–9. doi: 10.1111/add.12016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gentile D. Pathological video-game use among youth ages 8 to 18: A national study. Psychol Sci. 2009;20(5):594–602. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02340.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gentile DA, Choo H, Liau A, Sim T, Li D, Fung D, Khoo A. Pathological video game use among youths: A two-year longitudinal study. Pediatrics. 2011;127(2):e319–29. doi: 10.1542/peds.2010-1353. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gerstein D, Murphy S, Toce M, Hoffmann J, Palmer A, et al. Gambling Impact and Behavior Study: A Report to the National Gambling Impact Study Commission. Chicago: National Opinion Research Center; 1999. [Google Scholar]
- Grant JE, Kim SW, Potenza MN, Blanco C, Ibanez A, et al. Paroxetine treatment of pathological gambling: A multi-centre randomized controlled trial. Int Clin Psychopharmacol. 2003;18(4):243–9. doi: 10.1097/00004850-200307000-00007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grant JE, Odlaug BL, Potenza MN, Hollander E, Kim SW. Nalmefene in the treatment of pathological gambling: Multicentre, double-blind, placebo-controlled study. Br J Psychiatry. 2010;197(4):330–1. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.110.078105. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grant JE, Potenza MN, Hollander E, Cunningham-Williams R, Nurminen T, Smits G, Kallio A. Multicenter investigation of the opioid antagonist nalmefene in the treatment of pathological gambling. Am J Psychiatry. 2006;163(2):303–12. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.163.2.303. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Griffiths MD, van Rooij AJ, Kardefelt-Winther D, Starcevic V, Király O, et al. Working towards an international consensus on criteria for assessing internet gaming disorder: A critical commentary on Petry et al. 2014. Addiction. 2016;111(1):167–75. doi: 10.1111/add.13057. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ha Y, Hwang WJ. Gender differences in Internet addiction associated with psychological health indicators among adolescents using a national web-based survey. Int J Ment Health Addict. 2014;12:660–9. [Google Scholar]
- Han DH, Lee YS, Yang KC, Kim EY, Lyoo IK, Renshaw PF. Dopamine genes and reward dependence in adolescents with excessive internet video game play. J Addict Med. 2007;1(3):133–8. doi: 10.1097/ADM.0b013e31811f465f. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Han DH, Renshaw PF. Bupropion in the treatment of problematic online game play in patients with major depressive disorder. J Psychopharmacol. 2012;26(5):689–96. doi: 10.1177/0269881111400647. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hasin DS, O’Brien CP, Auriacombe M, Borges G, Bucholz K, et al. DSM-5 criteria for substance use disorders: Recommendations and rationale. Am J Psychiatry. 2013;170(8):834–51. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2013.12060782. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hodgins DC. Using the NORCDSM Screen for Gambling Problems as an outcome measure for pathological gambling: Psychometric evaluation. Addict Behav. 2004;29(8):1685–90. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2004.03.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hodgins DC, Currie SR, Currie G, Fick GH. Randomized trial of brief motivational treatments for pathological gamblers: More is not necessarily better. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2009;77(5):950–60. doi: 10.1037/a0016318. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hodgins DC, Currie SR, el-Guebaly N. Motivational enhancement and self-help treatments for problem gambling. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2001;69(1):50–7. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.69.1.50. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hökby S, Hadlaczky G, Westerlund J, Wasserman D, Balazs J, et al. Are Mental Health Effects of Internet Use Attributable to the Web-Based Content or Perceived Consequences of Usage? A Longitudinal Study of European Adolescents. JMIR Ment Health. 2016;3(3):e31. doi: 10.2196/mental.5925. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johansson A, Götestam KG. Internet addiction: Characteristics of a questionnaire and prevalence in Norwegian youth (12–18 years) Scand J Psychol. 2004;45(3):223–9. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9450.2004.00398.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Joutsa J, Saunavaara J, Parkkola R, Niemelä S, Kaasinen V. Extensive abnormality of brain white matter integrity in pathological gambling. Psychiatry Res. 2011;194(3):340–6. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2011.08.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kessler RC, Hwang I, LaBrie R, Petukhova M, Sampson NA, Winters KC, Shaffer HJ. DSM-IV pathological gambling in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Psychol Med. 2008;38(9):1351–60. doi: 10.1017/S0033291708002900. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kim BS, Chang SM, Park JE, Seong SJ, Won SH, Cho MJ. Prevalence, correlates, psychiatric comorbidities, and suicidality in a community population with problematic Internet use. Psychiatry Res. 2016;244:249–56. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2016.07.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kim SM, Han DH, Lee YS, Renshaw PF. Combined cognitive behavioral therapy and bupropion for the treatment of problematic on-line game play in adolescents with major depressive disorder. Computers in human behavior. 2012;28(5):1954–9. [Google Scholar]
- Kim PW, Kim SY, Shim M, Im CH, Shon YM. The influence of an educational course on language expression and treatment of gaming addiction for massive multiplayer online role-playing game (MMORPG) players. Comput Educ. 2013;63:208–17. [Google Scholar]
- Ko CH, Liu GC, Yen JY, Chen CY, Yen CF, Chen CS. Brain correlates of craving for online gaming under cue exposure in subjects with Internet gaming addiction and in remitted subjects. Addict Biol. 2013;18(3):559–69. doi: 10.1111/j.1369-1600.2011.00405.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ko CH, Wang PW, Liu TL, Yen CF, Chen CS, Yen JY. Bidirectional associations between family factors and Internet addiction among adolescents in a prospective investigation. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2015;69(4):192–200. doi: 10.1111/pcn.12204. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ko CH, Yen JY, Chen SH, Wang PW, Chen CS, Yen CF. Evaluation of the diagnostic criteria of Internet gaming disorder in the DSM-5 among young adults in Taiwan. J Psychiatr Res. 2014;53:103–10. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2014.02.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ko CH, Yen JY, Chen CS, Yeh YC, Yen CF. Predictive values of psychiatric symptoms for internet addiction in adolescents: A 2-year prospective study. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2009;163(10):937–43. doi: 10.1001/archpediatrics.2009.159. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ko CH, Yen JY, Yen CF, Chen CS, Weng CC, Chen CC. The association between Internet addiction and problematic alcohol use in adolescents: The problem behavior model. Cyberpsychol Behav. 2008;11(5):571–6. doi: 10.1089/cpb.2007.0199. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ko CH, Yen JY, Yen CF, Lin HC, Yang MJ. Factors predictive for incidence and remission of internet addiction in young adolescents: A prospective study. Cyberpsychol Behav. 2007;10(4):545–51. doi: 10.1089/cpb.2007.9992. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koo HJ, Han DH, Park SY, Kwon JH. The structured clinical interview for DSM-5 Internet gaming disorder: Development and validation for diagnosing IGD in adolescents. Psychiatry Investig. 2017;14(1):21–29. doi: 10.4306/pi.2017.14.1.21. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kovanen L, Basnet S, Castrén S, Pankakoski M, Saarikoski ST, et al. A randomised, double-blind, placebo-controlled trial of as-needed Naltrexone in the treatment of pathological gambling. Eur Addict Res. 2016;22(2):70–9. doi: 10.1159/000435876. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kwon M, Lee JY, Won WY, Park JW, Min JA, et al. Development and validation of a smartphone addiction scale (SAS) PLoS One. 2013;8(2):e56936. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0056936. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- LaBrie RA, Peller AJ, Laplante DA, Bernhard B, Harper A, Schrier T, Shaffer HJ. A brief self-help toolkit intervention for gambling problems: A randomized multisite trial. Am J Orthopsychiatry. 2012;82(2):278–89. doi: 10.1111/j.1939-0025.2012.01157.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Laconi S, Rodgers RF, Chabrol H. The measurement of Internet addiction: A critical review of existing scales and their psychometric properties. Computers in Human Behavior. 2014;41:190–202. [Google Scholar]
- Lee YS, Han DH, Yang KC, Daniels MA, Na C, Kee BS, Renshaw PF. Depression like characteristics of 5HTTLPR polymorphism and temperament in excessive internet users. J Affect Disord. 2008;109(1–2):165–9. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2007.10.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li Y, Zhang X, Lu F, Zhang Q, Wang Y. Internet addiction among elementary and middle school students in China: A nationally representative sample study. Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw. 2014;17(2):111–6. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2012.0482. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Limbrick-Oldfield EH, van Holst RJ, Clark L. Fronto-striatal dysregulation in drug addiction and pathological gambling: Consistent inconsistencies. Neuroimage Clin. 2013;2:385–93. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2013.02.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lin X, Dong G, Wang Q, Du X. Abnormal gray matter and white matter volume in ‘Internet gaming addicts’. Addict Behav. 2015;40:137–43. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2014.09.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lin MP, Ko HC, Wu JY. Prevalence and psychosocial risk factors associated with internet addiction in a nationally representative sample of college students in Taiwan. Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw. 2011;14(12):741–6. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2010.0574. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lin F, Zhou Y, Du Y, Qin L, Zhao Z, Xu J, Lei H. Abnormal white matter integrity in adolescents with internet addiction disorder: A tract-based spatial statistics study. PLoS One. 2012;7(1):e30253. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0030253. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lind PA, Zhu G, Montgomery GW, Madden PA, Heath AC, Martin NG, Slutske WS. Genome-wide association study of a quantitative disordered gambling trait. Addict Biol. 2013;18(3):511–22. doi: 10.1111/j.1369-1600.2012.00463.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liu J, Gao XP, Osunde I, Li X, Zhou SK, Zheng HR, Li LJ. Increased regional homogeneity in internet addiction disorder: A resting state functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Chin Med J (Engl) 2010;123(14):1904–8. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lorains FK, Cowlishaw S, Thomas SA. Prevalence of comorbid disorders in problem and pathological gambling: Systematic review and meta-analysis of population surveys. Addiction. 2011;106(3):490–8. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2010.03300.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Luquiens A, Tanguy ML, Lagadec M, Benyamina A, Aubin HJ, Reynaud M. The efficacy of three modalities of internet-based psychotherapy for non-treatment-seeking online problem gamblers: A randomized controlled trial. J Med Internet Res. 2016;18(2):e36. doi: 10.2196/jmir.4752. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Macur M, Király O, Maraz A, Nagygyörgy K, Demetrovics Z. Prevalence of problematic internet use in Slovenia. Zdr Varst. 2016;55(3):202–211. doi: 10.1515/sjph-2016-0026. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mak KK, Lai CM, Watanabe H, Kim DI, Bahar N, et al. Epidemiology of internet behaviors and addiction among adolescents in six Asian countries. Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw. 2014;17(11):720–8. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2014.0139. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martens MP, Arterberry BJ, Takamatsu SK, Masters J, Dude K. The efficacy of a personalized feedback-only intervention for at-risk college gamblers. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2015;83(3):494–9. doi: 10.1037/a0038843. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mihara S, Osaki Y, Nakayama H, Sakuma H, Ikeda M, et al. Internet use and problematic Internet use among adolescents in Japan: A nationwide representative survey. Addict Behav Rep. 2016;4:58–64. doi: 10.1016/j.abrep.2016.10.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moulton EA, Elman I, Becerra LR, Goldstein RZ, Borsook D. The cerebellum and addiction: Insights gained from neuroimaging research. Addict Biol. 2014;19(3):317–31. doi: 10.1111/adb.12101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Müller KW, Dreier M, Duven E, Giralt S, Beutel ME, Wölfling K. Adding clinical validity to the statistical power of large-scale epidemiological surveys on internet addiction in adolescence: A combined approach to investigate psychopathology and development-specific personality traits associated with internet addiction. J Clin Psychiatry. 2017;78(3):e244–e251. doi: 10.4088/JCP.15m10447. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Müller KW, Janikian M, Dreier M, Wölfling K, Beutel ME, et al. Regular gaming behavior and internet gaming disorder in European adolescents: Results from a cross-national representative survey of prevalence, predictors, and psychopathological correlates. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2015;24(5):565–74. doi: 10.1007/s00787-014-0611-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- National Council on Problem Gambling. Report of Survey on Participation in Gambling Activities Among Singapore Residents, 2014. Singapore: Ministry of Social and Family Development; 2015. Retrieved from: https://www.msf.gov.sg/research-and-data/Research-and-Statistics/Pages/Survey-On-Gambling-Participation-Among-Singapore-Residents-2014.aspx. [Google Scholar]
- Neighbors C, Rodriguez LM, Rinker DV, Gonzales RG, Agana M, Tackett JL, Foster DW. Efficacy of personalized normative feedback as a brief intervention for college student gambling: A randomized controlled trial. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2015;83(3):500–11. doi: 10.1037/a0039125. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oei TP, Raylu N, Casey LM. Effectiveness of group and individual formats of a combined motivational interviewing and cognitive behavioral treatment program for problem gambling: A randomized controlled trial. Behav Cogn Psychother. 2010;38(2):233–8. doi: 10.1017/S1352465809990701. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oacute;lason DT, Barudottir SK, Gretarsson SJ. Prevalence of pathological gambling among adults in Iceland; Paper presented at the 6th conference on research in Social Sciences; Reykjavík, Iceland. 2005. [Google Scholar]
- Pagani LS, Derevensky JL, Japel C. Predicting gambling behavior in sixth grade from kindergarten impulsivity: A tale of developmental continuity. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2009;163(3):238–43. doi: 10.1001/archpediatrics.2009.7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pápay O, Urbán R, Griffiths MD, Nagygyörgy K, Farkas J, et al. Psychometric properties of the problematic online gaming questionnaire short-form and prevalence of problematic online gaming in a national sample of adolescents. Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw. 2013;16(5):340–8. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2012.0484. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Park JH, Lee YS, Sohn JH, Han DH. Effectiveness of atomoxetine and methylphenidate for problematic online gaming in adolescents with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Hum Psychopharmacol. 2016;31(6):427–432. doi: 10.1002/hup.2559. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Petry NM. Substance abuse, pathological gambling, and impulsiveness. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2001;63(1):29–38. doi: 10.1016/s0376-8716(00)00188-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Petry NM. A comparison of treatment-seeking pathological gamblers based on preferred gambling activity. Addiction. 2003;98(5):645–55. doi: 10.1046/j.1360-0443.2003.00336.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Petry NM. Pathological Gambling: Etiology, Comorbidity and Treatments. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association Press; 2005. [Google Scholar]
- Petry NM. Behavioral Addictions: DSM-5 and Beyond. Oxford University Press; New York: 2016. [Google Scholar]
- Petry NM, Ammerman Y, Bohl J, Doersch A, Gay H, et al. Cognitive-behavioral therapy for pathological gamblers. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2006;74(3):555–67. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.74.3.555. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Petry NM, Blanco C, Auriacombe M, Borges G, Bucholz K, et al. An overview of and rationale for changes proposed for pathological gambling in DSM-5. J Gambl Stud. 2014a;30(2):493–502. doi: 10.1007/s10899-013-9370-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Petry NM, Blanco C, Stinchfield R, Volberg R. An empirical evaluation of proposed changes for gambling diagnosis in the DSM-5. Addiction. 2013;108(3):575–81. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2012.04087.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Petry NM, Rash CJ, Alessi SM. A randomized controlled trial of brief interventions for problem gambling in substance abuse treatment patients. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2016a;84(10):874–86. doi: 10.1037/ccp0000127. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Petry NM, Rehbein F, Gentile DA, Lemmens JS, Rumpf HJ, et al. An international consensus for assessing internet gaming disorder using the new DSM-5 approach. Addiction. 2014b;109(9):1399–406. doi: 10.1111/add.12457. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Petry NM, Rehbein F, Gentile DA, Lemmens JS, Rumpf HJ, et al. Griffiths et al.’s comments on the international consensus statement of internet gaming disorder: furthering consensus or hindering progress? Addiction. 2016b;111(1):175–8. doi: 10.1111/add.13189. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Petry NM, Rehbein F, Ko CH, O’Brien CP. Internet Gaming Disorder in the DSM-5. Curr Psychiatry Rep. 2015;17(9):72. doi: 10.1007/s11920-015-0610-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Petry NM, Stinson FS, Grant BF. Comorbidity of DSM-IV pathological gambling and other psychiatric disorders: Results from the National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions. J Clin Psychiatry. 2005;66(5):564–74. doi: 10.4088/jcp.v66n0504. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Petry NM, Weinstock J, Ledgerwood DM, Morasco B. A randomized trial of brief interventions for problem and pathological gamblers. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2008;76(2):318–28. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.76.2.318. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pontes HM, Király O, Demetrovics Z, Griffiths MD. The conceptualisation and measurement of DSM-5 Internet Gaming Disorder: the development of the IGD-20 Test. PLoS One. 2014;9(10):e110137. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0110137. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rehbein F, Baier D. Family-, media-, and school-related risk factors of video game addiction. J Media Psychol. 2013;25(3):118–128. [Google Scholar]
- Rehbein F, Kliem S, Baier D, Mößle T, Petry NM. Prevalence of Internet gaming disorder in German adolescents: Diagnostic contribution of the nine DSM-5 criteria in a state-wide representative sample. Addiction. 2015;110(5):842–51. doi: 10.1111/add.12849. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rehbein F, Kleimann M, Mössle T. Prevalence and risk factors of video game dependency in adolescence: Results of a German nationwide survey. Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw. 2010;13(3):269–77. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2009.0227. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rumpf HJ, Vermulst AA, Bischof A, Kastirke N, Gürtler D, et al. Occurence of Internet addiction in a general population sample: A latent class analysis. Eur Addict Res. 2014;20(4):159–66. doi: 10.1159/000354321. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rush BR, Bassani DG, Urbanoski KA, Castel S. Influence of co-occurring mental and substance use disorders on the prevalence of problem gambling in Canada. Addiction. 2008;103(11):1847–56. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2008.02338.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Saiz-Ruiz J, Blanco C, Ibáñez A, Masramon X, Gómez MM, Madrigal M, Díez T. Sertraline treatment of pathological gambling: A pilot study. J Clin Psychiatry. 2005;66(1):28–33. doi: 10.4088/jcp.v66n0104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Slutske WS, Ellingson JM, Richmond-Rakerd LS, Zhu G, Martin NG. Shared genetic vulnerability for disordered gambling and alcohol use disorder in men and women: Evidence from a national community-based Australian Twin Study. Twin Res Hum Genet. 2013;16(2):525–34. doi: 10.1017/thg.2013.11. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Slutske WS, Moffitt TE, Poulton R, Caspi A. Undercontrolled temperament at age 3 predicts disordered gambling at age 32: A longitudinal study of a complete birth cohort. Psychol Sci. 2012;23(5):510–6. doi: 10.1177/0956797611429708. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith DP, Battersby MW, Harvey PW, Pols RG, Ladouceur R. Cognitive versus exposure therapy for problem gambling: Randomised controlled trial. Behav Res Ther. 2015;69:100–10. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2015.04.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Song J, Park JH, Han DH, Roh S, Son JH, et al. Comparative study of the effects of bupropion and escitalopram on Internet gaming disorder. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2016;70(11):527–535. doi: 10.1111/pcn.12429. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sproston K, Erens B, Orford J. Gambling behaviour in Britain: results from the British gambling prevalence survey. London: National Centre for Social Research; 2000. p. iv-p. [Google Scholar]
- Starcevic V, Khazaal Y. Relationships between behavioural addictions and psychiatric disorders: What is known and what is yet to be learned. Front Psychiatry. 2017;8:53. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2017.00053. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stavropoulos V, Gomez R, Steen E, Beard C, Liew L, Griffiths MD. The longitudinal association between anxiety and Internet addiction in adolescence: The moderating effect of classroom extraversion. J Behav Addict. 2017:1–11. doi: 10.1556/2006.6.2017.026. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stein DJ, Phillips KA, Bolton D, Fulford KW, Sadler JZ, Kendler KS. What is a mental/psychiatric disorder? From DSM-IV to DSM-V. Psychol Med. 2010;40(11):1759–65. doi: 10.1017/S0033291709992261. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stinchfield R. Reliability, validity, and classification accuracy of the South Oaks Gambling Screen (SOGS) Addict Behav. 2002;27(1):1–19. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4603(00)00158-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Strittmatter E, Parzer P, Brunner R, Fischer G, Durkee T, et al. A 2-year longitudinal study of prospective predictors of pathological Internet use in adolescents. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2016;25(7):725–34. doi: 10.1007/s00787-015-0779-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Swing EL, Gentile DA, Anderson CA, Walsh DA. Television and video game exposure and the development of attention problems. Pediatrics. 2010;126(2):214–21. doi: 10.1542/peds.2009-1508. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- TNS EMNID. Spielen mit und um Geld in Deutschland. Bielefeld, Frankfurt: AWI Automaten-Wirtschaftsverbände-Info GmbH; 2011. [Google Scholar]
- Toneatto T, Brands B, Selby P. A randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled trial of naltrexone in the treatment of concurrent alcohol use disorder and pathological gambling. Am J Addict. 2009;18(3):219–25. doi: 10.1080/10550490902787007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tsitsika A, Janikian M, Schoenmakers TM, Tzavela EC, Olafsson K, et al. Internet addictive behavior in adolescence: A cross-sectional study in seven European countries. Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw. 2014;17(8):528–35. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2013.0382. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van Holst RJ, van der Meer JN, McLaren DG, van den Brink W, Veltman DJ, Goudriaan AE. Interactions between affective and cognitive processing systems in problematic gamblers: A functional connectivity study. PLoS One. 2012;7(11):e49923. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0049923. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Rooij AJ, Kuss DJ, Griffiths MD, Shorter GW, Schoenmakers MT, Van De Mheen D. The (co-)occurrence of problematic video gaming, substance use, and psychosocial problems in adolescents. J Behav Addict. 2014;3(3):157–65. doi: 10.1556/JBA.3.2014.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Rooij AJ, Schoenmakers TM, Vermulst AA, Van den Eijnden RJ, Van de Mheen D. Online video game addiction: Identification of addicted adolescent gamers. Addiction. 2011;106(1):205–12. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2010.03104.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vink JM, van Beijsterveldt TC, Huppertz C, Bartels M, Boomsma DI. Heritability of compulsive Internet use in adolescents. Addict Biol. 2016;21(2):460–8. doi: 10.1111/adb.12218. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vitaro F, Arseneault L, Tremblay RE. Impulsivity predicts problem gambling in low SES adolescent males. Addiction. 1999;94(4):565–75. doi: 10.1046/j.1360-0443.1999.94456511.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Walther B, Morgenstern M, Hanewinkel R. Co-occurrence of addictive behaviours: personality factors related to substance use, gambling and computer gaming. Eur Addict Res. 2012;18(4):167–74. doi: 10.1159/000335662. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wardle H, Moody A, Griffiths M, Orford J, Volberg R. Defining the online gambler and patterns of behaviour integration: Evidence from the British Gambling Prevalence Survey 2010. Int Gambl Stud. 2011;11(3):339–56. [Google Scholar]
- Wang L, Luo J, Bai Y, Kong J, Luo J, Gao W, Sun X. Internet addiction of adolescents in China: Prevalence, predictors, and association with well-being. Addict Res Theory. 2013;21:62–9. [Google Scholar]
- Weng CB, Qian RB, Fu XM, Lin B, Han XP, Niu CS, Wang YH. Gray matter and white matter abnormalities in online game addiction. Eur J Radiol. 2013;82(8):1308–12. doi: 10.1016/j.ejrad.2013.01.031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wickwire EM, Jr, Burke RS, Brown SA, Parker JD, May RK. Psychometric evaluation of the National Opinion Research Center DSM-IV Screen for Gambling Problems (NODS) Am J Addict. 2008;17(5):392–5. doi: 10.1080/10550490802268934. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wittek CT, Finserås TR, Pallesen S, Mentzoni RA, Hanss D, Griffiths MD, Molde H. Prevalence and predictors of video game addiction: A study based on a national representative sample of gamers. Int J Ment Health Addict. 2016;14(5):672–86. doi: 10.1007/s11469-015-9592-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yen JY, Ko CH, Yen CF, Chen CS, Chen CC. The association between harmful alcohol use and Internet addiction among college students: Comparison of personality. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2009;63(2):218–24. doi: 10.1111/j.1440-1819.2009.01943.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yoo YS, Cho OH, Cha KS. Associations between overuse of the internet and mental health in adolescents. Nurs Health Sci. 2014;16(2):193–200. doi: 10.1111/nhs.12086. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Young KS. Caught in the Net: How to recognize signs of Internet addiction and a winning strategy for recovery. New York: John Wiley & Sons; 1998. [Google Scholar]
- Yuan K, Cheng P, Dong T, Bi Y, Xing L, et al. Cortical thickness abnormalities in late adolescence with online gaming addiction. PLoS One. 2013;8(1):e53055. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0053055. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhong X, Zu S, Sha S, Tao R, Zhao C, et al. The effect of a family-based intervention model on Internet-addicted Chinese adolescents. Soc Behav and Personal. 2011;39(8):1021–34. [Google Scholar]
- Zhou Y, Lin FC, Du YS, Qin LD, Zhao ZM, Xu JR, Lei H. Gray matter abnormalities in Internet addiction: A voxel-based morphometry study. Eur J Radiol. 2011;79(1):92–5. doi: 10.1016/j.ejrad.2009.10.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhu TM, Li H, Jin RJ, Zheng Z, Luo Y, Ye H, Zhu HM. Effects of electroacupuncture combined psycho-intervention on cognitive function and event-related potentials P300 and mismatch negativity in patients with internet addiction. Chin J Integr Med. 2012;18(2):146–51. doi: 10.1007/s11655-012-0990-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zois E, Kiefer F, Lemenager T, Vollstädt-Klein S, Mann K, Fauth-Bühler M. Frontal cortex gray matter volume alterations in pathological gambling occur independently from substance use disorder. Addict Biol. 2017;22(3):864–72. doi: 10.1111/adb.12368. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]