Abstract
Background
The treatment of cognitive deficits is challenging in pediatric onset multiple sclerosis (POMS) and in patients with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). We performed a pilot double-blind RCT to evaluate the efficacy of a home-based computerized-program for retraining attention in two cohorts of POMS and ADHD patients.
Methods
POMS and ADHD patients failing in at least 2/4 attention tests on a neuropsychological battery were randomized to specific or nonspecific computerized training (ST, nST), performed in one-hour sessions, twice/week for 3 months. The primary outcome was the effect of the training on global neuropsychological performances measured by the cognitive impairment index (CII).
The efficacy of the intervention was evaluated in each disease group by using repeated measures ANOVA.
Results
Sixteen POMS (9 females, age 15.75 ± 1.74 years) and 20 ADHD (2 females, age 11.19 ± 2.49 years) patients were enrolled. In POMS patients the ST exposure was associated to a significantly more pronounced improvement of the CII (p < 0.0001) and on cognitive test exploring attention, concentration, planning strategies and visuo-spatial memory performances in comparison to nST exposure. In ADHD patients the difference between the ST and nST on the CII was not statistical significant (p = 0.06), but a greater effect of the ST was found only on cognitive test exploring attention and delayed recall of visuo-spatial memory performances.
Conclusions
Our data suggest that a cognitive rehabilitation program that targets attention is a suitable tool for improving global cognitive functioning in POMS patients, whereas it has a less pronounced transfer effect in ADHD patients.
Trial Registration
ClinicalTrials.gov; NCT03190902; registration date: June 15, 2017; retrospectively registered.
Keywords: Multiple sclerosis, Attention deficit, Rehabilitation, ADHD
Background
The presence of cognitive deficits of varied intensity is a characteristic of psychiatric disorders of childhood and adolescence such as Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD), but also of neurological pathologies such as pediatric onset multiple sclerosis (POMS).
ADHD is one of the most common neurodevelopmental disorders characterized by pervasive patterns of inattention and/or impulsivity/hyperactivity and a range of cognitive dysfunctions that often persist into adulthood [1, 2].
POMS represent 5–10% of total MS population [3]. Cognitive dysfunction is one of the most remarkable features of MS and particularly in POMS. The percentage of patients with POMS with at least a mild cognitive deficit ranges from 30 to 50% [4–9]. The most affected cognitive domains in POMS are complex attention, information processing speed, executive functions, verbal and visual memory, reasoning and problem solving [4, 9]. Longitudinal studies from US and Canada groups showed a cognitive stability in POMS patients over time [10, 11]. The US study reported an overall percent of patients with cognitive impairment, defined as having one-third or more test scores in the impaired range, of 37.3% at baseline and 32.3% after a mean follow-up of 1.64 year [10]. A Canadian study, examining patients and controls over a 1-year period found that controls generally showed greater improvement than patients, and 25% of patients showed clinically significant decline [11].
In contrast, an Italian longitudinal study demonstrated that cognitive impairment in POMS tend to worsen after a mean period of 2 years since baseline evaluation [12]. At follow-up 75% of the cases were classified as having a deteriorating cognitive performance. Changes were prominent in tests of verbal memory, complex attention, verbal fluency, and receptive language. However the same group reversed their initial negative findings showing that after 5 years from baseline there was more stability than decline [13].
Interestingly, a variety of psychiatric symptoms can occur in POMS. Approximately one-third of children suffer from depressive symptoms and one-fourth of POMS report fatigue [12, 14]. Moreover, one study has reported ADHD as one of the most frequent comorbid psychiatric disorder in POMS [15]. The functional consequences of cognitive impairment can be particularly striking in children and adolescents, since they occur during their formative years, therefore it may affect their academic and social activities. Cognitive training during the developmental age when brain plasticity is at the highest expression can induce a strengthening of the key brain networks implicated in POMS and ADHD.
In adult onset MS patients several studies examined the efficacy of cognitive rehabilitation programs related to attention and additional cognitive domains [16, 17]. Some of them provided class I evidence of beneficial effects of these training programs through randomized clinical trials (RCTS) [18–20].
To date the efficacy of specific cognitive rehabilitation interventions has never been evaluated by a RCT in POMS. Whereas, the fMRI effect of a working memory training has been reported in a small case series of 5 juvenile MS patients [21]. Conversely, in the last years, several RCTs [22–25] assessed the efficacy of cognitive training as a potential non-drug alternative treatment for ADHD disorder [26]. Most of the cognitive trainings focused on the working memory or attention dysfunctions. Preliminary evidence suggests that cognitive remediation might be at least partially effective in the ADHD treatment [27–32]. It is argued that cognitive training can potentially reduce ADHD symptoms and might improve functioning by targeting neuropsychological deficits thought to mediate ADHD pathophysiology [33–37].
In this exploratory pilot study, we assessed by a double-blind RCT the efficacy of a home-based computerized program for retraining attention dysfunction in two cohorts of POMS and ADHD patients. The results in POMS were compared to those obtained in ADHD patients.
Methods
Standard protocol approval, patient consent and recruitment
The study was conducted with approval of the institutional review board (Comitato Etico Indipendente Azienda Ospedaliero-Universitaria Consorziale Policlinico - Approval Number: 0070059/CE). Parents of the participants signed an informed consent.
Recruitment (September–December 2015, predetermined) and assessments were performed at the MS Centers and at the Child Neuropsychiatry Unit both of the University of Bari. Due to the monocentric nature of the study and due to the absence of external funding resources the recruitment has been limited only to referrals to the these two centers. Clinical trial registration information: ClinicalTrials.gov number NCT03190902. Clinical trial registration date: June 15, 2017.
Study population
POMS
We recruited POMS outpatients consecutively referred to the MS Centers at the University of Bari who met the inclusion/exclusion criteria during the study period. Inclusion criteria were: POMS diagnosed according to the most recent diagnostic criteria [38], aged < 18 years, with an Expanded Disability Status Scale (EDSS) score ≤ 5.5, impairment on at least 2/4 attention tests (see below) defined as scores < 1.5 standard deviation (SD) of normative values [9, 12, 13]. To facilitate recruitment we excluded only patients with important impairments on other cognitive tasks, defined as performance ≤2.0 SD of normative values, still including subjects with milder degrees of impairment (e.g. cognitive scores between 1.51 and 1.99 SD below the normative values). Exclusion criteria were: severe visual loss (unable to read Times New Roman font 16 with the best correction), major psychiatric illness (any severe disabling psychiatric disorders [i.e. major depression, obsessive-compulsive disorder, psychotic disorder] which could interfere with: the understanding of the protocol and of the informed consent; the overall engagement in the study and in particular with the adherence with the treatment regimen and the compliance with the study visits and procedures), alcohol or substance abuse, education < 5 years, previous cognitive rehabilitation training, ongoing relapse or steroid treatment during the 30 days preceding enrollment. Disease-modifying treatments (DMTs) and symptomatic treatments were maintained unchanged during the study.
ADHD
We enrolled ADHD outpatients consecutively referred to the Child Neuropsychiatry Unit at the University of Bari. We included only ADHD patients with the subtype inattention, not previously exposed or not treated with any psychotropic drug. ADHD diagnosis was performed according the DSM-5 criteria and the NIMH Collaborative Multisite Multimodal Treatment Study of Children With Attention- Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (MTA) - Swanson, Nolan, and Pelham IV Rating Scale (MTA-SNAP-IV), Conner’s Parent Rating Scale Revised (CPRS-R), Conner’s Teacher Rating Scale Revised (CTRS-R), Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL), Kiddie Schedule for Affective Disorder and Schizophrenia (K-SADS). The same inclusion/exclusion criteria referring the cognitive performances were applied to the ADHD patients.
Study procedures
We applied the same study procedures already reported in a previous study which evaluate the effect of the cognitive rehabilitation program in adult-onset MS patients [18]. Patients were randomized to receive a specific computer training (ST) or to receive a nonspecific computer training (n-ST) with a 1:1 ratio. Randomization was performed by an independent researcher on the basis of a computerized list of random numbers. A psychologist, blind to the study, was responsible for administering and evaluating the neuropsychological tests, whereas an independent researcher, who was not blind to the study, was responsible for setting up the ST and n-ST programs, explaining the training procedure and supervising the training program.
As defined by the study protocol, during the study period, in case of a suspected MS relapse, the patient underwent a neurological examination within 48 h and a standard steroid treatment was prescribed if needed. A confirmed MS relapse was a reason for study discontinuation.
Study design
This is a single-centre, parallel group double blind-RCT.
Assessment and outcome measures
At the baseline visit, a neurologist and a child/adolescent neuropsychiatrist collected demographic and clinical information of POMS and ADHD (familiar background, any past medical history) patients.
The handedness was evaluated in both groups by the Edinburgh Inventory [39].
The neuropsychological assessment was performed by a psychologist. Two alternate versions of the tests were used at different assessment points.
The primary outcome was to evaluate the effect of the cognitive training on neuropsychological performances.
The neuropsychological test battery assessed the following cognitive areas:
Verbal learning and delayed recall: Selective Reminding Test (SRT) and Selective Reminding Test–Delayed (SRT-D) from the Rao Brief Repeatable Battery (BRB) [40];
Visuo-spatial learning and delayed recall: Spatial Recall Test (SPART) and Spatial Recall Test–Delayed (SPART-D) from the BRB [40];
Concentration, attention, processing speed, working memory and cognitive flexibility: Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT) from the BRB and the Trail Making Tests (TMT) A and B [41];
Expressive language: Semantic Verbal Fluency Test (SVFT), in which the subject is asked to produce as many words as possible belonging to a semantic category (colors, animals, fruits, cities) within 120 s. The score is the average of correct words [42].
Planning: Tower of London Test (TOL). The initial and target configuration of the TOL were presented under the form of 2 identical kits made of a wooden base (22 16 1 2 cm) with 3 rods of 12, 8, and 4.5 cm, and 3 balls (yellow, red, and blue) of 3 cm in diameter. The subject was required to obtain the target configuration in a minimum number of moves, according to the following rules: move only 1 ball at a time; place at most 1 ball on the shortest peg and 2 balls on the middle one; move each ball only from one peg to another. There were no time limits. The execution time and the number of moves were recorded by the examiner. The results (target configuration attained or not, abandoned) and any rule violations were noted [43].
Depression: self-assessed by patients through the Children’s Depression Inventory (CDI) with the assistance of the psychologist [44].
Kiddie Schedule for Affective Disorder and Schizophrenia (K-SADS).
The psychologist administered the above battery, using alternative versions of the tests, at baseline, and within 1 week following the end of the training program. At the same time intervals, self-assessed measures were also repeated.
Intervention
The ST was based on the Attention Processing Training program (APT) [45]. This program targets focused, sustained, selective, alternating and divided attention and consists of a group of hierarchically organized tasks that exercise different components of attention, proceeding from sustained to selective, alternating and finally divided attention exercises. The sequence of the exercises places increasing demands on complex attention control and working memory systems (e.g. identification of target numbers or letters in the presence of distracter images and noises).
The n-ST consisted of a series of nonspecific exercises including the following: text reading and comprehension: e.g. reading brief text extracted from novels, journals, newspaper (without requesting a feedback nor verbal nor written); give feedback on proverbs comprehension; description of pictures: e.g. try to provide a brief verbal description of simple pictures; enumerating words classified in different categories; trying to provide at least 3 synonyms for a given list of words; etc.
Before starting the training at home, a psychologist who was not blind to the patient assignment group conducted a training session for each patient at the MS Clinic, in the presence of the caregiver/parent, in which instructions and procedures for the use of the ST and n-ST were provided (Patients and their parents were not aware about the treatment arm assigned). Each patient was treated at home twice a week for three consecutive months. Each training session lasted 1 h. Each patient applied the training on his/her own under the supervision of the caregiver/parent, who also collected and stored forms reporting patient performance during each training session. During the study period, the psychologist called patients every week and met patients and their caregiver/parent every month to check patient compliance and possible difficulties in the use of the training program.
Statistical analysis
Given the exploratory nature of this pilot trial, no sample size analysis was performed. Continuous variables were described as mean and standard deviation (SD), categorical variables as frequency and percentage. Group comparison has been performed using the Student’s t test, the Mann-Whitney U test and the Fisher’s exact test when appropriate.
A global score, defined Cognitive Impairment Index (CII), allowing the evaluation of changes in cognitive performances independently by the number of cognitive tests failed at the neuropsychological evaluation, was obtained using the mean and SD from the normative values for each test [13, 46–48].
For each patient, a grading system was applied to individual cognitive tests, based on the number of SDs below the control mean (i.e. grade 0 was given if the patient scored at or above the control mean, 1 if he/she scored below the control mean, but at or above 1 SD below the control mean, and so on until all patient scores were accommodated) [13, 46–48]. Finally, all the patient’s scores were summed to give one overall measure of cognitive function.
The efficacy of ST on the global cognitive functioning, measured by the CII, and on performance of each cognitive test was evaluated in each disease group by using a 2 (Group–ST and n-ST) × 2 (Time–baseline, 3 months) mixed factorial design, with repeated measures on the second factor.
Finally, the comparative efficacy of the ST on the global neuropsychological performances between the 2 disease groups was assessed by using a generalized linear model. In this model the estimated mean difference between performances recorded after training and those recorded before training on the CII for each disease group was compared. Age, sex and school education were included as covariates in this model.
Statistical analysis was performed by using SPSS software (SPSS, version 22.0; SPSS, Chicago, Ill).
Results
Twenty-four POMS and 33 ADHD patients were assessed for eligibility during the recruitment period. In the POMS group, 2 patients refused to participate, 6 patients did not meet the inclusion criteria regarding the presence of deficits on at least 2/4 attention performance tests. In the ADHD group, 3 patients refused to participate, 10 patients were excluded because they were affected by a combined ADHD. Finally, 16 POMS and 20 ADHD patients were enrolled. All the patients enrolled completed the study procedures and assessments. No patients with POMS reported relapses during the study period. There were no drop-out (Fig. 1).
Fig. 1.
CONSORT 2010 Flow Diagram
The comparison of the baseline cognitive performances between the 2 disease conditions is reported in Table 1. ADHD patients showed significantly worse attention, concentration, processing speed, working memory and cognitive flexibility performances at baseline as measured by the SDMT, TMT-A and TMT-B (p < 0.05) in comparison to POMS patients.
Table 1.
Comparison of baseline neuropsychological performances in POMS and ADHD
Cognitive test | POMS | ADHD | P value |
---|---|---|---|
SRT- LTS | 27.3 (10.1); 24 (9–52) | 30.9 (6.8); 28.5 (20–44) | 0.1 |
SRT – CLTR | 21.3 (9.1); 21.5 (2–40) | 21.9 (8.6); 21 (7–44) | 1.0 |
SPART | 21.0 (3.8); 22 (15–25) | 19.8 (4.6); 20 (13–30) | 0.2 |
SDMT | 22.5 (4.5); 23 (16–29) | 17.3 (8.5); 14.5 (8–43) | 0.002 |
Trail Making Test Aa | 37.0 (10.6); 37.5 (21–58) | 54.0 (23.1); 57 (8–100) | 0.02 |
Trail Making Test Ba | 108.1 (68.6); 97.5 (0–233) | 182.7 (64.9); 188 (96–333) | 0.004 |
SRT-D | 6.0 (2.2); 6 (0–9) | 6.7 (2.0); 7 (3–11) | 0.5 |
SPART-D | 6.9 (1.2); 7 (5–10) | 6.5 (2.3); 5.5 (4–10) | 0.2 |
Tower of Londona | 15.7 (5.8); 14 (10–31) | 14.7 (10.1); 14 (0–33) | 0.7 |
Cognitive Impairment Index | 22.4 (3.1); 22 (19–29) | 23.6 (4.2); 24 (16–30) | 0.3 |
CDI | 12.9 (6.9); 15 (0–25) | 13.4 (7.0); 14.5 (2–25) | 1.0 |
Data are reported as mean (SD), Median (min - max)
Abbreviations: POMS pediatric onset multiple sclerosis, ADHD Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder, ST specific training, nST non specific training, SRT-LTS Selective Reminding Test Long Term Storage, SRT-CTLR Selective Reminding Test – Consistent Long-Term Retrieval, SRT-D Selective Reminding Test–Delayed, SPART Spatial Recall Test, SPART-D Spatial Recall Test–Delayed, SDMT Symbol Digit Modalities Test, CDI Children’s Depression Inventory
aUnit of measure: time in seconds
Comparisons of baseline demographic, clinical characteristics, neuropsychological performances (NP) of POMS, and ADHD subgroups underwent ST and n-ST are reported in Tables 2 and 3, respectively. At baseline, no differences were found between the 2 treatment arms regarding sex, age, and in terms of NP performances. The mean ± SD, median (min-max) age, disease duration and EDSS score of the entire POMS cohort were: 15.8 ± 1.7 years, 16.4 (12.4–17.9); 3.4 ± 3.0 years, 2.4 (0.1–10.5); 2.3 ± 0.9 EDSS score, 2.25 (1–3.5), respectively. All POMS patients were under stable (at least 6 months) first line DMTs. The mean ± SD, median (min-max) age of the entire ADHD cohort were: 11.2 ± 2.5 years, 11.7 (7.4–17.6). No ADHD patients received psychoactive medications during the study period.
Table 2.
Baseline demographic and clinical characteristics of POMS and ADHD subgroups underwent specific and non specific training
POMS | |||
Variable | Specific training (n = 8) | Non specific training (n = 8) | p - value (t, U, or Fisher’s exact test) |
Sex (F/M) | 5/3 | 4/4 | 1.0 |
Age, years | 15.8 (2.0) | 15.7 (1.5) | 1.0 |
Disease Duration, years | 3.5 (3.5) | 3.3 (2.6) | 0.96 |
Handedness, n. right-handed (%) | 7 (87.5) | 8 (100) | 0.97 |
Disease modifying therapy, n | |||
Nothing | 2 | 2 | 0.67 |
Interferon beta | 6 | 4 | |
Glatiramer Acetate | 0 | 1 | |
Natalizumab | 0 | 1 | |
Annualized Relapse Rate | 0.4 (0.5) | 0.3 (0.5) | 0.72 |
EDSS, median (min - max) | 2.0 (1.0–3.5) | 3.0 (1.0–3.5) | 0.28 |
ADHD | |||
Variable | Specific training (n = 10) | Non specific training (n = 10) | |
Sex (F/M) | 0/10 | 2/8 | 0.47 |
Age, years | 11.5 (3.0) | 11.3 (2.0) | 0.58 |
Handedness, n. right-handed (%) | 9 (90) | 9 (90) | 1.0 |
Abbreviations: POMS pediatric onset multiple sclerosis, EDSS Expanded Disability Status Scale, ADHD Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder
Table 3.
Baseline neuropsychological performances in POMS and ADHD subgroups underwent specific (ST) and non specific (nST) trainings
POMS | |||
Cognitive test | ST | nST | P value |
SRT- LTS | 29.9 (12.6) | 24.6 (6.5) | 0.2 |
SRT - CLTR | 22.1 (11.0) | 20.4 (7.5) | 0.6 |
SPART | 19.3 (4.4) | 22.8 (2.0) | 0.1 |
SDMT | 24.5 (4.6) | 20.5 (3.6) | 0.1 |
Trail Making Test A | 39.4 (11.5) | 34.6 (9.8) | 0.5 |
Trail Making Test B | 108.4 (61.4) | 107.9 (79.4) | 1.0 |
SRT-D | 6.3 (2.8) | 5.8 (1.5) | 0.2 |
SPART-D | 6.8 (1.0) | 7.0 (1.4) | 1.0 |
Tower of London | 15.8 (5.4) | 15.6 (6.6) | 0.8 |
Cognitive Impairment Index | 22.5 (3.9) | 22.3 (2.4) | 0.9 |
CDI | 14.5 (7.6) | 11.4 (6.3) | 0.4 |
ADHD | |||
Cognitive test | ST | nST | P |
SRT- LTS | 32.2 (6.9) | 29.6 (6.7) | 0.4 |
SRT - CLTR | 25.5 (8.2) | 18.3 (7.8) | 0.1 |
SPART | 18.3 (4.9) | 21.2 (4.2) | 0.1 |
SDMT | 17.1 (5.5) | 17.4 (11.1) | 0.4 |
Trail Making Test A | 61.9 (17.3) | 46.1 (26.2) | 0.1 |
Trail Making Test B | 160.8 (45.4) | 204.6 (75.9) | 0.3 |
SRT-D | 7.1 (2.1) | 6.2 (1.8) | 0.6 |
SPART-D | 6.0 (2.2) | 6.9 (2.3) | 0.2 |
Tower of London | 15.9 (9.1) | 13.5 (11.3) | 0.5 |
Cognitive Impairment Index | 23.8 (4.7) | 23.4 (4.0) | 0.8 |
CDI | 13.5 (7.6) | 13.2 (6.7) | 0.9 |
Abbreviations: POMS pediatric onset multiple sclerosis, ADHD Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder, ST specific training, nST non specific training, SRT-LTS Selective Reminding Test Long Term Storage, SRT-CTLR Selective Reminding Test – Consistent Long-Term Retrieval, SRT-D Selective Reminding Test–Delayed, SPART Spatial Recall Test, SPART-D Spatial Recall Test–Delayed, SDMT Symbol Digit Modalities Test, CDI Children’s Depression Inventory
Cognitive performances in POMS
The NP before and after the cognitive training are reported in Table 4.
Table 4.
Impact of 3-month ST and nST training on cognitive performances in POMS and ADHD groups
ST | nST | p-value | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Cognitive Test | Baseline | Post - Treatment | Baseline | Post - Treatment | Effect for group × time |
POMS | |||||
SRT- LTS | 29.9 (12.6) | 35.4 (5.0) | 24.6 (6.5) | 28.9 (4.2) | 0.72 |
SRT - CLTR | 22.1 (11.0) | 27.3 (5.2) | 20.4 (7.5) | 22.6 (6.1)* | 0.30 |
SPART | 19.3 (4.4) | 25.5 (1.7)* | 22.8 (2.0) | 23.1 (1.9) | 0.004 |
SDMT | 24.5 (4.6) | 46.3 (6.7)* | 20.5 (3.6) | 20.8 (4.1) | < 0.0001 |
Trail Making Test A | 39.4 (11.5) | 31.8 (6.6) | 34.6 (9.8) | 43.8 (10.2) | 0.01 |
Trail Making Test B | 108.4 (61.4) | 70.5 (32.1) | 107.9 (79.4) | 64.0 (61.3) | 0.87 |
SRT-D | 6.3 (2.8) | 8.1 (0.8) | 5.8 (1.5) | 6.4 (0.7) | 0.28 |
SPART-D | 6.8 (1.0) | 8.1 (0.8)* | 7.0 (1.4) | 6.1 (0.8) | 0.004 |
Tower of London | 15.8 (5.4) | 30.4 (2.5)* | 15.6 (6.6) | 13.8 (2.0) | < 0.0001 |
Cognitive Impairment Index | 22.5 (3.9) | 10.4 (3.3)* | 22.3 (2.4) | 19.3 (2.2)* | < 0.0001 |
ADHD | |||||
SRT- LTS | 32.2 (6.9) | 34.9 (7.3) | 29.6 (6.7) | 30.5 (6.8) | 0.50 |
SRT - CLTR | 25.5 (8.2) | 27.3 (5.0) | 18.3 (7.8) | 22.6 (4.4) | 0.51 |
SPART | 18.3 (4.9) | 22.0 (4.2)* | 21.2 (4.2) | 21.2 (3.7) | 0.06 |
SDMT | 17.1 (5.5) | 35.8 (7.2)* | 17.4 (11.1) | 22.1 (11.4) | 0.004 |
Trail Making Test A | 61.9 (17.3) | 38.4 (22.2)* | 46.1 (26.2) | 31.5 (16.9) | 0.51 |
Trail Making Test B | 160.8 (45.4) | 108.7 (61.0)* | 204.6 (75.9) | 178.0 (131.11) | 0.51 |
SRT-D | 7.1 (2.1) | 6.8 (2.0) | 6.2 (1.8) | 6.6 (1.0) | 0.41 |
SPART-D | 6.0 (2.2) | 7.7 (2.0)* | 6.9 (2.3) | 6.6 (1.2) | 0.04 |
Tower of London | 15.9 (9.1) | 30.5 (11.0)* | 13.5 (11.3) | 17.9 (12.6) | 0.27 |
Cognitive Impairment Index | 23.8 (4.7) | 15.3 (5.6)* | 23.4 (4.0) | 19.3 (3.6)* | 0.06 |
Abbreviations: POMS pediatric onset multiple sclerosis, ADHD Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder, ST specific training, nST non specific training, SRT-LTS Selective Reminding Test Long Term Storage, SRT-CTLR Selective Reminding Test Consistent Long-Term Retrieval, SRT-D Selective Reminding Test–Delayed, SPART Spatial Recall Test, SPART-D Spatial Recall Test–Delayed, SDMT Symbol Digit Modalities Test, CDI Children’s Depression Inventory
*Indicate significant Effect for time (Baseline vs Post – Treatment comparison); p < 0.05
After the 3-month cognitive training, the ST exposure was associated to a significantly more pronounced reduction of the CII in comparison to the nST exposure (p < 0.0001) (Table 4 and Fig. 2a). POMS patients treated with ST had a significant higher improvement in their performances on SDMT (p < 0.0001), TOL (p < 0.0001), TMT-A (p = 0.01), SPART (p = 0.004) and SPART-D (p = 0.004) in comparison to those treated with nST (Table 4 and Fig. 3).
Fig. 2.
Cognitive Impairment Index before and after the cognitive specific training (ST) and non specific training (nST) in POMS (a) and ADHD (b)
Fig. 3.
Effect of the cognitive specific training (ST) and non specific training (nST) on SDMT, TMT-A, TOL, SPART, SPART-D performances in POMS patients. P values indicate the effect for group * time interaction
Cognitive performances in ADHD
Unlike in POMS, the difference between the ST and nST subgroups in ADHD patients on the global neuropsychological performances measured by the CII did not reach statistical significance (p = 0.06) (Table 4 and Fig. 2b). A greater effect of the ST in comparison to the nST in ADHD patients was found only for performances on SDMT (p = 0.004), and SPART-D (p = 0.04) (Table 4 and Fig. 4). No differences were found between the two treatment arms in other cognitive domains.
Fig. 4.
Effect of the cognitive specific training (ST) and non specific training (nST) on SDMT and SPART-D performances in ADHD patients
The generalized linear model performed to evaluate the magnitude of the effect of the ST in both the disease groups demonstrated a significant greater reduction of the CII in POMS than that observed in ADHD patients (p = 0.042).
Discussion
This pilot study confirms the presence of a cognitive impairment in children affected by ADHD and POMS, with a more severe deficit of attention, concentration, processing speed, working memory and cognitive flexibility performances in ADHD than POMS.
Most importantly, the results of this controlled, double-blind, randomized study, demonstrate that a home-based computerized training of specific aspects of attention, the APT, has a different effect on cognitive functions in POMS and ADHD patients.
In POMS, APT improves in the short-term the global cognitive functions and individual performances in several cognitive domains. POMS patients improved in SDMT and TMT-A performances, which evaluate concentration, attention, processing speed, working memory and cognitive flexibility.
Furthermore, we observed an improvement also in cognitive domains not specifically trained by the program. Patients with POMS improve their executive functioning, planning strategies, visuo-spatial memory and delayed recall performances, as assessed by TOL, SPART and SPART-D test scores.
These findings indicate that APT induces both a near transfer effect in the domain of the planning strategies and a far transfer effect in the domain of visuo-spatial memory.
Interestingly a previous RCT [18], which assessed the efficacy of APT in adult MS, found a significant improvement exclusively on tasks of sustained attention such as the PASAT and, marginally, the SDMT. No effect was detected on tests tapping other aspects of attention or other cognitive domains. The different effect of APT in POMS and adult MS provides support for the idea that restorative exercises started during the developmental age can induce a greater strengthening of the key brain networks implicated in attention processing. This can be explained by a greater brain plasticity in younger patients. Indeed, recent RCTs focused on attention rehabilitation and assessing fMRI outcomes demonstrated that intensive ST improved some aspects of cognitive functioning and also affected neural plasticity and increased fMRI brain activity in the cerebellum of adult MS patients with impaired attention and/or memory [19, 49].
Therefore, early detection and management of cognitive dysfunction is momentous in POMS.
Longitudinal studies in patients with POMS have provided conflicting results about the evolution of the cognitive deficits. Most of them [10, 11] have shown a stability in cognitive performance in POMS over time suggesting a lack of the expected age-related cognitive development. An Italian study lasting over 5 years demonstrated that cognitive deficits, mainly in visual-spatial learning and expressive language, tend to worsen over time, affecting the patient’s academic and professional achievements [13, 50].
Different studies have provided some information about the predictive factors associated to the longitudinal evolution of the cognitive performances in patients with POMS. Till C and colleagues reported that a longer disease duration was associated with greater deterioration in visuomotor integration and an increased lesion volume was associated with slower psychomotor speed over a 1 year follow-up [11]. Recently, Pastò and colleagues reported that the major predictor of cognitive stability or improvement over a follow-up longer than 4 years in patients with POMS was a higher cognitive reserve [51].
The availability of an easy to use and home-based cognitive training is of paramount importance in this pediatric population. In this light, another recent study investigated the feasibility of a home-based computerized program for working memory training in patients with POMS [52] supporting the use of this kind of approach to treat cognitive dysfunction in this specific population. Moreover, given all these premises, a computerized cognitive training would be recommended for all the patients with POMS as a tool to maintain level of cognitive functioning (i.e. before impairment becomes apparent) and to increase the cognitive reserve.
Referring to the ADHD population, our results show that APT is effective in improving the targeted cognitive domains which has been specifically trained by the program. In this population, APT determines only a slight improvement of the global cognitive functions, as measured by the reduction of the CII. These findings indicate that there is a less pronounced transfer effect in ADHD patients in comparison to POMS patients after the exposure to a cognitive training with a focus on attention. Although the different effect of the cognitive training in the two disease groups may be due to the more severe baseline cognitive impairment in ADHD patients, these results seem to be consistent with the majority of previous studies on cognitive rehabilitation which trains attention in ADHD patients [22, 28, 30, 53]. In these studies it has been show an improvement of the targeted ability with only a limited transfer to other cognitive performances. As a matter of fact, by considering attention as the key impairment in ADHD, the main selection criteria in such studies were based on the central-deficit assumption [29]. Moreover, the theoretical premise of these studies was that the remediation of attention deficits could reduce ADHD associated cognitive and behavioral difficulties [28, 30, 33, 53]. There is no a specific profile of executive functions impairment in ADHD [54].
Therefore, it is necessary to find out other approaches focusing on multiple neuropsychological processes to optimize the transfer of effect from cognitive deficit to clinical symptoms in ADHD.
It is noteworthy that in our study design we have also included a comparative training based on a nonspecific reinforce of text reading, comprehension and verbal performances that could have had an impact on the global cognitive functions [55, 56]. This could explain why in the nST group we have observed an improvement of the CII, although not statistically significant. These results are in line with previous findings in adult onset MS treated with the same cognitive training [18].
Several limitations of this study deserve discussion. First, the inclusion of two very diverse patient populations (POMS and ADHD), with small sample sizes in each group, may limit the generalizability of our findings. Secondly, the mean EDSS score level of our POMS cohort is slightly higher than that previously reported in other cohorts. This was due to the inclusion criteria which required only the enrollment of patients with a discrete cognitive deficit (with impairment in at least 2/4 attention) and thus with a slightly more severe course. Thirdly, we did not measure effort assessment (e.g., symptom validity measures), and most importantly we did not include measures of “clinically meaningful change” such as (e.g., school performance or parent reported behaviors) in the study protocol. Fourthly, we have applied the same cognitive battery to two different disease groups: POMS and ADHD. It is possible that the outcome assessments performed during the RCT were not sensitive to deficits in ADHD, but we choose the outcomes more recommended in POMS (at least in Italy and Europe) [9, 12, 13] and for a better comparison between the two groups, we used the same outcome assessments also in ADHD patients.
Referring to the statistical analysis, we should mention that we did not perform a multiple comparisons correction, therefore there is a potential risk of false positive results. We did so because we don’t think that the statistical approach of adjusting for multiple testing is necessary or even adequate. Reducing the type I error for null associations increases the type II error for those associations that are not null.
Finally, another limitation of this study should be discussed. The possibility that the positive effect of the ST we have observed is at least in part due to an expectancy effects among participants assigned to the ST.
However, this pilot study was designed for a fast evaluation of the efficacy of a cognitive training (in treated vs untreated patients) in the two different groups of pediatric patients (POMS or ADHD) with attention impairment. The internal validity and the consistency of the results are assured by a randomized controlled design and robust statistical analysis, which in part might mitigate all these limitations.
Conclusions
Our results indicate that a cognitive rehabilitation program that targets attention may be a suitable tool for improving global cognitive functioning in POMS patients, whereas it has a less pronounced transfer effect in ADHD patients. Future RCTs on larger populations of both POMS and ADHD patients, including a healthy subjects control group, clinical and fMRI outcomes, are needed to confirm the efficacy of cognitive rehabilitation programs. Furthermore, future studies aimed to evaluate the efficacy of computerized cognitive rehabilitation program on remediating attention deficits should include a more specific sustained attention task as efficacy measure. Studies aimed at identifying factors that influence transfer effects, as well the mechanism underlying these effects, especially in ADHD patients and in subpopulations of POMS with and without ADHD symptoms are desirable.
Availability of data and materials
The datasets used and analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.
Abbreviations
- ADHD
Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder
- CDI
Children’s Depression Inventory
- nST
Non specific training
- POMS
Pediatric onset multiple sclerosis
- SDMT
Symbol Digit Modalities Test
- SPART
Spatial Recall Test
- SPART-D
Spatial Recall Test–Delayed
- SRT-CTLR
Selective Reminding Test – Consistent Long-Term Retrieval
- SRT-D
Selective Reminding Test–Delayed
- SRT-LTS
Selective Reminding Test Long Term Storage
- ST
Specific training
Authors’ contributions
MS recruited patients and collected, analysed and interpreted the data. RGV performed the neuropsychological evaluations and collected, analysed and interpreted the data. LM performed the study supervision and a critical revision of the manuscript for intellectual content. PI defined the study concept and design and collected, analysed and interpreted the data. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.
Ethics approval and consent to participate
The study was conducted with approval of the institutional review board (Comitato Etico Indipendente Azienda Ospedaliero-Universitaria Consorziale Policlinico - Approval Number: 0070059/CE). Parents of the participants signed an informed consent.
Competing interests
Marta Simone reports no disclosures.
Rosa Gemma Viterbo has received speaker honoraria from Biogen Idec and Teva.
Lucia Margari reports no disclosures.
Pietro Iaffaldano has served on scientific advisory boards for Biogen Idec and Bayer-Shering, and has received funding for travel and/or speaker honoraria from Genzyme, Biogen Idec, Merck-Serono, Teva and Novartis.
Publisher’s Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Footnotes
Marta Simone and Rosa Gemma Viterbo contributed equally to this work.
Contributor Information
Marta Simone, Email: martasimone_ms@libero.it.
Rosa Gemma Viterbo, Email: rosa.viterbo@uniba.it.
Lucia Margari, Email: lucia.margari@uniba.it.
Pietro Iaffaldano, Phone: +39 0805593589, Email: pietro.iaffaldano@uniba.it.
References
- 1.Frazier TW, Demaree HA, Youngstrom EA. Meta-analysis of intellectual and neuropsychological test performance in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Neuropsychology. 2004;18:543–555. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.18.3.543. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Polanczyk G, de Lima MS, Horta BL, Biederman J, Rohde LA. The worldwide prevalence of ADHD: a systematic review and metaregression analysis. Am J Psychiatry. 2007;164:942–948. doi: 10.1176/ajp.2007.164.6.942. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Iaffaldano P, Simone M, Lucisano G, et al. Prognostic indicators in pediatric clinically isolated syndrome. Ann Neurol. 2017; 10.1002/ana.24938. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 4.Bobholz JA, Rao SM. Cognitive dysfunction in multiple sclerosis: a review of recent developments. Curr Opin Neurol. 2003;16(3):283–288. doi: 10.1097/00019052-200306000-00006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Banwell BL, Anderson PE. The cognitive burden of multiple sclerosis in children. Neurology. 2005;64(5):891–894. doi: 10.1212/01.WNL.0000152896.35341.51. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.MacAllister WS, Belman AL, Milazzo M, et al. Cognitive functioning in children and adolescents with multiple sclerosis. Neurology. 2005;64(8):1422–1425. doi: 10.1212/01.WNL.0000158474.24191.BC. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Julian L, Serafin D, Charvet L, et al. Cognitive impairment occurs in children and adolescents with multiple sclerosis: results from a United States network. J Child Neurol. 2013;28(1):102–107. doi: 10.1177/0883073812464816. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Parrish JB, Farooq O, Weinstock-Guttman B. Cognitive deficits in pediatric-onset multiple sclerosis: what does the future hold? Neurodegener Dis Manag. 2014;4(2):137–146. doi: 10.2217/nmt.14.4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Amato MP, Goretti B, Ghezzi A, et al. Cognitive and psychosocial features of childhood and juvenile MS. Neurology. 2008;70(20):1891–1897. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000312276.23177.fa. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Charvet LE, O’Donnell EH, Belman AL, et al. Longitudinal evaluation of cognitive functioning in pediatric multiple sclerosis (MS): report from the U.S. pediatric multiple sclerosis network. Mult Scler. 2014;20(11):1502–1510. doi: 10.1177/1352458514527862. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Till C, Racine N, Araujo D, et al. Changes in cognitive performance over a 1-year period in children and adolescents with multiple sclerosis. Neuropsychology. 2013;27(2):210–219. doi: 10.1037/a0031665. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Amato MP, Goretti B, Ghezzi A, et al. Cognitive and psychosocial features in childhood and juvenile MS: two-year follow-up. Neurology. 2010;75(13):1134–1140. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181f4d821. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Amato MP, Goretti B, Ghezzi A, et al. Neuropsychological features in childhood and juvenile multiple sclerosis: five-year follow-up. Neurology. 2014;83(16):1432–1438. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000000885. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Parrish JB, Weinstock-Guttman B, Smerbeck A, Benedict RH, Yeh EA. Fatigue and depression in children with demyelinating disorders. J Child Neurol. 2013;28(6):713–718. doi: 10.1177/0883073812450750. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Weisbrot D, Charvet L, Serafin D, et al. Psychiatric diagnoses and cognitive impairment in pediatric multiple sclerosis. Mult Scler. 2014;20(5):588–593. doi: 10.1177/1352458513504249. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Goverover Y, Chiaravalloti ND, O’Brien AR, DeLuca J. Evidenced-based cognitive rehabilitation for persons with multiple sclerosis: an updated review of the literature from 2007 to 2016. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2018;99(2):390–407. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2017.07.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Rosti-Otajärvi EM, Hämäläinen PI. Neuropsychological rehabilitation for multiple sclerosis. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2014;2:CD009131. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD009131.pub3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Amato MP, Goretti B, Viterbo RG, et al. Computer-assisted rehabilitation of attention in patients with multiple sclerosis: results of a randomized, double-blind trial. Mult Scler. 2014;20(1):91–98. doi: 10.1177/1352458513501571. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Cerasa A, Gioia MC, Valentino P, et al. Computer-assisted cognitive rehabilitation of attention deficits for multiple sclerosis: a randomized trial with fMRI correlates. Neurorehabil Neural Repair. 2013;27(4):284–295. doi: 10.1177/1545968312465194. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Chiaravalloti ND, Moore NB, Nikelshpur OM, DeLuca J. An RCT to treat learning impairment in multiple sclerosis: the MEMREHAB trial. Neurology. 2013;81(24):2066–2072. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000437295.97946.a8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Hubacher M, DeLuca J, Weber P, et al. Cognitive rehabilitation of working memory in juvenile multiple sclerosis-effects on cognitive functioning, functional MRI and network related connectivity. Restor Neurol Neurosci. 2015;33(5):713–725. doi: 10.3233/RNN-150497. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Cortese S, Ferrin M, Brandeis D, et al. European ADHD Guidelines Group (EAGG).Cognitive training for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: meta-analysis of clinical and neuropsychological outcomes from randomized controlled trials. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2015;54(3):164–174. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2014.12.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Steeger CM, Gondoli DM, Gibson BS, Morrissey RA. Combined cognitive and parent training interventions for adolescents with ADHD and their mothers: a randomized controlled trial. Child Neuropsychol. 2016;22(4):394–419. doi: 10.1080/09297049.2014.994485. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Lo HH, Wong SY, Wong JY, Wong SW, Yeung JW. The effect of a family-based mindfulness intervention on children with attention deficit and hyperactivity symptoms and their parents: design and rationale for a randomized, controlled clinical trial (Study protocol) BMC Psychiatry. 2016;16:65. doi: 10.1186/s12888-016-0773-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Benzing V, Schmidt M. Cognitively and physically demanding exergaming to improve executive functions of children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: a randomised clinical trial. BMC Pediatr. 2017;17(1):8. doi: 10.1186/s12887-016-0757-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Froehlich TE, Lanphear BP, Epstein JN, et al. Prevalence, recognition, and treatment of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in a national sample of US children. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2007;161(9):857–864. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.161.9.857. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Johnstone SJ, Roodenrys S, Blackman R, et al. Neurocognitive training for children with and without AD/HD. Atten Defic Hyperact Disord. 2012;4:11–23. doi: 10.1007/s12402-011-0069-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Shalev L, Tsal Y, Mevorach C. Computerized progressive attentional training (CPAT) program: effective direct intervention for children with ADHD. Child Neuropsychol. 2007;13:382–388. doi: 10.1080/09297040600770787. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Klingberg T, Fernell E, Olesen PJ, Johnson M, Gustafsson P, Dahlström K, et al. Computerized training of working memory in children with ADHD–a randomized, controlled trial. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2005;44:177–186. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200502000-00010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Kerns KA, Eso K, Thomson J. Investigation of a direct intervention for improving attention in young children with ADHD. Dev Neuropsychol. 1999;16:273–295. doi: 10.1207/S15326942DN1602_9. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Murray DW, Skinner AT, Malone PS. A randomized trial of two promising computer-based interventions for students with attention difficulties. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2010;38(1):131–142. doi: 10.1007/s10802-009-9353-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Johnston MV. Plasticity in the developing brain: implications for rehabilitation. Dev Disabil Res Rev. 2009;15:94–101. doi: 10.1002/ddrr.64. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Tajik-Parvinchi D, Wright L, Schachar R. Cognitive rehabilitation for Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD): promises and problems. J Can Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2014;23(3):207–217. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Rutledge KJ, van den Bos W, McClure SM, Schweitzer JB. Training cognition in ADHD: current findings, borrowed concepts, and future directions. Neurotherapeutics. 2012;9(3):542–558. doi: 10.1007/s13311-012-0134-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Lévesque J, Beauregard M, Mensour B. Effect of neurofeedback training on the neural substrates of selective attention in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Neurosci Lett. 2006;394(3):216–221. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2005.10.100. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Liu ZX, Lishak V, Tannock R, Woltering S. Effects of working memory training on neural correlates of go/Nogo response control in adults with ADHD: a randomized controlled trial. Neuropsychologia. 2017;95:54–72. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.11.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Johnstone SJ, Roodenrys SJ, Johnson K, Bonfield R, Bennett SJ. Game-based combined cognitive and neurofeedback training using focus pocus reduces symptom severity in children with diagnosed AD/HD and subclinical AD/HD. Int J Psychophysiol. 2017;116:32–44. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2017.02.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Krupp LB, Tardieu M, Amato MP, et al. International Pediatric Multiple Sclerosis Study Group criteria for pediatric multiple sclerosis and immune-mediated central nervous system demyelinating disorders: revisions to the 2007 definitions. Mult Scler J. 2013;19(10):1261–1267. doi: 10.1177/1352458513484547. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Oldfield RC. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia. 1971;9(1):97–113. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Rao SM. A Manual for the Brief Repeatable Battery of Neuropsychological Tests in Multiple Sclerosis. Milwaukee: Medical College of Wisconsin; 1990. [Google Scholar]
- 41.Reitan RM. The relation of the trail making test to organic brain damage. J Consult Psychol. 1955;19:393–394. doi: 10.1037/h0044509. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Spinnler H, Tognoni G. Standardizzazione e taratura Italiana di test neuropsicologici. Ital J Neurol Sci. 1987;6(suppl 8):12–120. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Sannio Fancello G, Vio C, Cianchetti C. Torre di Londra: test di valutazione delle funzioni esecutive (pianificazione e problem solving) Gardolo: Erickson; 2006. [Google Scholar]
- 44.Kovacs M, It A, Camuffo M, Cerutti R, Lucarelli L, Mayer R. C.D.I. Children Depression Inventory: Questionario Di Autovalutazione. Firenze: Organizzazioni Speciali; 1988. [Google Scholar]
- 45.Sohlberg MM, Mateer CA. Effectiveness of an attention training program. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 1987;9:117–130. doi: 10.1080/01688638708405352. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Iaffaldano P, Viterbo RG, Paolicelli D, et al. Impact of Natalizumab on cognitive performances and fatigue in relapsing multiple sclerosis: a prospective, open-label, two years observational study. PLoS One. 2012;7(4):e35843. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0035843. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Iaffaldano P, Ruggieri M, Viterbo RG, Mastrapasqua M, Trojano M. The improvement of cognitive functions is asociated with a decrease of plasma Osteopontin levels in Natalizumab treated relapsing multiple sclerosis. Brain Behav Immun. 2014;35:176–181. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2013.08.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Iaffaldano P, Viterbo RG, Trojano M. Natalizumab discontinuation is associated with a rebound of cognitive impairment in multiple sclerosis patients. J Neurol. 2016;263(8):1620–1625. doi: 10.1007/s00415-016-8177-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Sastre-Garriga J, Alonso J, Renom M, et al. A functional magnetic resonance proof of concept pilot trial of cognitive reha-bilitation in multiple sclerosis. Mult Scler. 2011;17:457–467. doi: 10.1177/1352458510389219. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Banwell BL. Multiple sclerosis in children. Handb Clin Neurol. 2014;122:427–441. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-444-52001-2.00018-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Pastò L, Portaccio E, Goretti B, et al. The cognitive reserve theory in the setting of pediatric-onset multiple sclerosis. Mult Scler. 2016;22(13):1741–1749. doi: 10.1177/1352458516629559. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Till C, Kuni B, De Somma E, Yeh EA, Banwell B. A feasibility study of working memory training for individuals with paediatric-onset multiple sclerosis. Neuropsychol Rehabil. 2017;18:1–16. doi: 10.1080/09602011.2017.1372786. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Slate SE, Meyer TL, Burns WJ, Montgomery DD. Computerized cognitive training for severely emotionally disturbed children with ADHD. Behav Modif. 1998;22:415–437. doi: 10.1177/01454455980223012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Qian Y, Shuai L, Chan RC, Qian QJ, Wang Y. The developmental trajectories of executive function of children and adolescents with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder. Res Dev Disabil. 2013;34(5):1434–1445. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2013.01.033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Hall CB, Lipton RB, Sliwinski M, et al. Cognitive activities delay onset of memory decline in persons who develop dementia. Neurology. 2009;73(5):356–361. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181b04ae3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Pillai JA, Hall CB, Dickson DW, et al. Association of crossword puzzle participation with memory decline in persons who develop dementia. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2011;17(6):1006–1013. doi: 10.1017/S1355617711001111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]