Skip to main content
PLOS ONE logoLink to PLOS ONE
. 2018 Jun 13;13(6):e0197394. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0197394

An investigation of the determinants of quality of life in adolescents and young adults with Down syndrome

Fatma Haddad 1, Jenny Bourke 2, Kingsley Wong 2, Helen Leonard 1,2,*
Editor: Stefano Federici3
PMCID: PMC5999114  PMID: 29897903

Abstract

Background

Young people with Down syndrome experience varying abilities in activities of daily living, cognitive functioning, behaviour and social skills. The aim of this research was to investigate, from a carer’s perspective, the factors that influenced the quality of life of these young people.

Methods

Families of young people with Down syndrome (n = 197), aged 16–31 years, living in Western Australia, took part in a questionnaire study regarding young person daily functioning, family characteristics, medical background and quality of life measured by the Kidscreen 27-item scale. Kidscreen-10 total score was used as an outcome in the investigation of determinants with higher scores indicating better quality of life.

Results

After adjustment for confounders including carer’s mental health measured by the Depression and Anxiety Scale (DASS), global impact of illness as well as impact of mental health and bowel conditions were all negatively associated with the young person’s quality of life. Young people who had three or more friends had better quality of life than those with no friends. Scores were lower (reflecting poor quality of life) in individuals who had more behavioural problems but these relationships were attenuated after adjustment for confounders and DASS.

Conclusions

Overall, our findings revealed that quality of life of young people with Down syndrome was most negatively associated with burden of medical conditions, but also with lack of friendships. We were somewhat surprised to find the effect of medical problems on quality of life persisting into adolescence and adulthood where in general the burden of medical comorbidities is much less than in childhood.

Introduction

The concept of quality of life (QOL) is one that broadly encompasses aspects of health and wellbeing. It can be considered as a “complete state of physical, mental, and social wellbeing and not merely the absence of infirmity”, as defined by the WHO [1], or simply as the subjective feelings of satisfaction and wellbeing which occur in relation to life experiences [2]. Generally, quality of life can be a difficult entity to measure, as each person attains unique perceptions of their overall quality of life, which is influenced by cultural contexts, previous experiences, personal values, and aspirations[3].

Down syndrome, also known as trisomy 21, is the commonest known medical cause of intellectual disability [4,5], occurring at a rate of approximately 1 in 1000 live births [6]. Individuals with Down syndrome have varying degrees of impairment, with many experiencing difficulties in communication and understanding [7]. They may also experience emotional and behavioural challenges [810] which may be associated with activity limitations in the areas of social interactions and community skills [11]. As a consequence of communication and cognitive limitations, assessing QOL can be difficult in this population. Previous research involving self-report by young people with intellectual disability has been subject to bias with concern about study reliability and validity [12,13]. However, within a human rights framework, these individuals have the right to attain personal goals and to experience a QOL equivalent to those who do not have a disability and to be able to access suitable, evidence-based services that support them in these goals[14]. Therefore it is important not only to be able to better measure QOL in this population but also to investigate the factors and relationships which impact on it.

Young people with Down syndrome often experience a number of health-related issues, which may affect overall wellbeing. These issues can transcend multiple physical systems and include cardiovascular, gastrointestinal, respiratory, and endocrine abnormalities [15,16]. Like others with intellectual disability but to a lesser extent [17], people with Down syndrome are more likely than their typical peers to experience emotional and behavioural disturbance [18] which can adversely impact on opportunities for social engagement and community participation. Those with higher levels of everyday functioning are more likely to be in employment as are those with fewer behavioural problems [19]. This population may also be at particular risk of developing depressive symptomatology into adult life [20]. The characteristic facial features associated with Down syndrome make it a visible disability that can both impair social acceptance and promote isolation [21]. Despite this, the “Down syndrome Advantage” has been proposed, suggesting that families with a child with Down syndrome experience less stress and greater rewards than families with a child with intellectual disability of another cause [22].

While there is a considerable body of literature focussing on the wellbeing of their family, including mothers and siblings [2327], there has been comparatively less research on the wellbeing of the young people with Down syndrome themselves. This highlights the importance of investigating this construct and the underlying influences in this population. Thus, the aim of the research was to investigate determinants of health-related quality of life of young people with Down syndrome, with a focus on the identification of risk and protective factors pertaining not only to the individuals themselves but also to their family environment.

Methods

Recruitment for this study was from the Western Australian population-based Down syndrome database. The database was first established in 1997 with families of school-aged children identified through the state-wide Disability Services Commission and later the population-based Intellectual Disability Exploring Answers (IDEA) database [28]. In 2004, families of young people with Down syndrome, aged 0–25 years, were invited to participate in an expanded study[25]. In 2009 and 2011 families of those born prior to 1994 were invited to complete further questionnaires in a study which focussed on issues related to transition into adulthood. The current study used data collected in 2011 from caregivers of the young people, with the majority of informants being the young persons’ biological mothers.

The carer-report questionnaire was developed with both consumer and stakeholder input and included two components. The first section was about the individual with Down syndrome including health, functioning, daily occupations, social relationships, quality of life and behaviour, and the second section was about the quality of life and health of the family. Ethical approval was obtained from the Ethics Committee of the Women’s and Children’s Health Services in Western Australia.

Measures

The outcome variable for this study was health-related quality of life of young people with Down syndrome as estimated by caregiver completion of the Kidscreen-27 Parent Proxy Report questionnaire [25] allowing calculation of the five individual domain scores. Individual domain sub-scores were only calculated if there were no missing items. In order to obtain a composite score the 10 items forming the Kidscreen-10 measure were used with a maximum of one missing value allowed and imputed. A Rasch-scaled [29] single sum score was then transformed into T-values with a mean of 50 and a standard deviation of approximately 10, according to the manual.

Personal sociodemographic information included gender and age (16–18, 19–22, 23–31 years). Parental sociodemographic factors included maternal age (38–45, 46–55, 56 and older years) and paternal age (42–55, 56–65, 66 and older years), maternal and paternal highest level of education (primary school, high school, advanced diploma, Bachelor or higher degree), and maternal and paternal work status (full time work, part-time work, not working).

Environmental or family factors such as living region (major city, inner regional, and outer regional and remote), annual family income (AU$31,199 or less, AU$31,200—AU$51,999, AU$52,000–77,999, and AU$78,000 or higher), savings and carer’s mental health were measured. Savings was categorised as: 1) No savings, includes: a. we are spending money we haven’t got; b. we have just enough money to get us through to the next pay day; c. there’s some money left over each week but we just spend it, 2) We can save a bit every now and again, and 3) We can save a lot. Carer mental health was measured using the Depression Anxiety and Stress Scales (DASS 21), a screening tool with three sub-scales that assess symptoms in depression, anxiety, and stress and with validity in both clinical and community settings [30,31].

Personal social factors included number of friends, meeting of needs, day occupation, functioning, and behaviour. The number of friends was categorised into 3 groups; (no friends, 1–2 friends, and 3 or more friends). Needs being met was a binary measure (yes/no) in response to the question “Do you feel that your son’s/daughter’s needs are being met under the current day activity arrangements?” Day occupation was categorised in a descending hierarchy as: 1) in open work environment (must not be in school), 2) in a sheltered service environment (must not be in open work environment and must not be in school), and 3) post secondary classes, alternatives to employment, and not working (must not be in a sheltered service environment, in open work environment, or in school). The young person’s functioning in activities of daily living (ADL) was measured using the Index of Social Competence (ISC)[32]. The ISC has 3 subscales, community, self-care and communication skills. Each subdomain is examined individually and a total score of all three subscales (i.e. functioning total) was also assessed for this research. Behaviour was measured using the Developmental Behaviour Checklist Adult version (DBC-A) [9,33]. The DBC, which has been found to be a valid and reliable tool in measuring psychopathology, was developed specifically for people with intellectual and developmental disability [9,33]. It is a 107-item checklist, with each behavioural description scored on a 3-point Likert scale; with 0 being “not true as far as you know”, 1 being “somewhat or sometimes true”, and 2 being “very true or often true”. The DBC provides measures of overall behavioural and emotional disturbance (Total Behaviour Problems Score [TBPS]) and 5 subscale scores derived from factor analysis [9]. The TBPS (referred to as “behaviour” throughout the study) has been found, in instrument validation studies, to be strongly associated with child psychiatrists’ ratings of severity of psychopathology, i.e. the higher the score the higher the likelihood of significantly marked emotional and behavioural disturbance [34]. Behaviour (i.e. TBPS score), initially a continuous variable, was treated as a binary variable representing scores of <46 and > = 46, as a score of 46 and over is consistent with the presence of a psychiatric disorder [9].

Personal medically related factors included body mass index (BMI) categorised as healthy weight (<25), overweight (25–30) or obese (>30), weekly physical activity (0–2 days, 3–4 days, and 5 or more days), 12 monthly GP or psychologist visit for mental health reasons (yes, no), and medical impact. Medical impact was assessed using 13 questions asking about the impact of a range of conditions including heart, bowel, skin, and mental health. Each medical condition was scored based on caregiver’s rating of impact on daily life (0 = none, 1 = minor, 2 = moderate, or 3 = major). For each individual, a total score was calculated by summing the individual item scores. Global impact was then categorised into 4 subgroups (0, 1–4, 5–8, 9 and above). The higher the global impact, the higher the burden of disease. Furthermore, the impact of several specific medical conditions, including mental health, bowel conditions, skin conditions, and hearing loss, was also calculated. Results for these specific conditions were divided into two categories based on impact: 1) none to minor impact, and 2) moderate to major impact.

Statistical analysis

Descriptive statistics were used to summarise the characteristics of the cohort, including those who did or did not meet the inclusion criteria of Kidscreen completion and the overall population of the 2011 study (i.e. 197 participants). Each Kidscreen-27 domain sub-score was only calculated if it had no missing items and Kidscreen-10 total score allowed a maximum of one missing item. Simple linear regression was used to examine the univariate relationship between Kidscreen-10 and the sociodemographic, family, social and medically related factors. Multiple linear regression was used to examine the association between Kidscreen-10 and living region, savings, child occupation, friendships, behaviours, physical activity, and medically related variables allowing adjustment for confounders including young person’s age and gender, parental factors (including maternal and paternal age, maternal and paternal education, and maternal and paternal occupation), and young person’s functioning. When adjusting for confounders, we examined the relationship once with the DASS and once without the DASS, in order to observe any change that may be affected by carer’s mental health and to explore any relationships that the variable may impact. Adjusted estimates and their 95% confidence intervals, and the P-value were reported. Estimated means of Kidscreen-10 were predicted by using Stata’s post-estimation command “margins” after multiple regression models were fitted. STATA 14.2 was used for all analyses.

The relative importance of factors affecting QOL, both positively and negatively after adjustment for DASS, have been graphically displayed.

Results

Questionnaires were administered to 232 families of adolescents and young people aged 16 years and above and responses were received from 197 (84.9%). Of those, 175 (88.8%) met the criteria for Kidscreen-10 completion. A summary of those included in the study, those not included, and total number of participants is presented in Table 1.

Table 1. Characteristics of 197 young people with Down syndrome, by Kidscreen-10 completion status.

Kidscreen-10
Completed
(N = 175)
Did not complete
(N = 22)
Personal sociodemographic factors
Gender, n (%)
    Male 97 (55.4) 13 (59.1)
    Female 78 (44.6) 9 (40.9)
Age group, n (%)
    16–18 years 25 (14.3) 5 (22.7)
    19–22 years 54 (30.9) 4 (18.2)
    23–31 years 96 (54.9) 13 (59.1)
Parent sociodemographic factors
Mother’s age, n (%)
    38–45 13 (7.4) 2 (9.1)
    46–55 68 (38.9) 6 (27.3)
    56 and older 90 (51.4) 14 (63.6)
    Missing 4 (2.3) 0 (0)
Father’s age, n (%)
    42–55 69 (39.4) 7 (31.8)
    56–65 63 (36.0) 8 (36.4)
    66 and older 25 (14.3) 4 (18.2)
    Missing 18 (10.3) 3 (13.6)
Mother’s occupation, n (%)
    Full-time work 36 (20.6) 7 (31.8)
    Part-time work 65 (37.1) 6 (27.3)
    Not working 68 (38.9) 9 (40.9)
    Missing 6 (3.4) 0
Father’s occupation, n (%)
    Full-time work 85 (48.6) 7 (31.8)
    Part-time work 16 (9.1) 1 (4.6)
    Not working 36 (20.6) 7 (31.8)
    Missing 38 (21.7) 7 (31.8)
Mother’s educationa, n (%)
     Primary school 40 (22.8) 7 (31.8)
    Completed high school 38 (21.7) 3 (13.6)
    Advanced diploma 32 (18.3) 3 (13.6)
    Bachelor degree 60 (34.3) 8 (36.4)
    Missing 5 (2.9) 1 (4.6)
Father’s educationa, n (%)
    Primary school 24 (13.7) 2 (9.1)
    Completed high school 25 (14.3) 1 (4.6)
    Advanced diploma 48 (27.4) 7 (31.8)
    Bachelor degree 58 (33.1) 6 (27.3)
    Missing 20 (11.4) 6 (27.3)
Family factors
Living region, n (%)
    Major cities of Australia 128 (73.1) 16 (72.7)
    Inner regional Australia 20 (11.4) 4 (18.2)
    Outer regional and remote 26 (14.9) 2 (9.1)
    Missing 1 (0.6) 0 (0)
Annual family income, n (%)
    Less than AU$31,199 30 (17.1) 6 (27.3)
    Between AU$31,200 and AU$51,999 28 (16.0) 2 (9.1)
    Between AU$52,000 and AU$71,999 23 (13.1) 0 (0)
    Above AU$72,000 77 (44.0) 5 (22.7)
    Missing 17 (9.7) 9 (40.9)
Savings, n (%)
    No savings 44 (25.1) 8 (36.4)
    We can save a bit 92 (52.6) 6 (27.3)
    We can save a lot 35 (20.0) 2 (9.1)
    Missing 4 (2.3) 6 (27.3)
Carer’s emotional state (DASS), n mean (SD) range
    Depression of all mothers 139 5.8 (7.0) 0–34 11 6.7 (6.6) 0–20
    Depression of all carers 162 5.7 (6.8) 0–34 12 6.3 (6.5) 0–20
    Anxiety of all mothers 139 3.4 (5.7) 0–28 11 4 (6.7) 0–20
    Anxiety of all carers 162 3.5 (5.5) 0–28 12 4 (6.4) 0–20
    Stress of all mothers 139 8.4 (8.0) 0–40 11 8.4 (6.1) 0–20
    Stress of all carers 162 8.3 (7.7) 0–40 12 8.8 (6.0) 0–20
Personal social factors
Day occupationb, n (%)
    Open work environment 36 (20.6) 3 (13.6)
    In a sheltered workshop 76 (43.4) 6 (27.3)
    Post-secondary classes 45 (25.7) 7 (31.8)
    Missing 18 (10.3) 6 (27.3)
Number of friends, n (%)
    No friends 35 (20.0) 5 (22.7)
    1–2 friends 77 (44.0) 7 (31.8)
    3 or more friends 63 (36.0) 4 (18.2)
    Missing 0 (0) 6 (27.3)
Needs are metc, n (%)
    Yes 114 (65.1) 11 (50.0)
    No 47 (26.9) 5 (22.7)
    Missing 14 (8.0) 6 (27.3)
Behaviour, n (%)
    <46 137 (78.3) 12 (54.6)
    46 and higher 36 (20.6) 2 (9.1)
    Missing 2 (1.1) 8 (36.4)
Functioning in activities of daily living, n mean (SD) range
    Self care 175 20.2 (4.0) 7–26 21 17.5 (5.0) 7–24
    Community skills 175 11.4 (4.2) 4–19 21 8.4 (4.1) 4–16
    Communication skills 175 5.8 (1.5) 2–8 21 4.7 (1.7) 2–8
    Total 175 37.4 (8.6) 13–52 21 30.6 (9.6) 13–47
Personal medically related factors
BMI, n (%)
    Healthy weight (<25) 37 (21.1) 5 (22.7)
    Overweight (25–30) 38 (21.7) 1 (4.6)
    Obese (>30) 56 (32.0) 5 (22.7)
    Missing 44 (25.1) 0 (0)
Physical activity per week, n (%)
    0–2 days 56 (32.0) 5 (22.7)
    3–4 days 56 (32.0) 1 (4.6)
    5 days or more 57 (32.6) 8 (36.4)
    Missing 6 (3.4) 8 (36.4)
Mental health visits, n (%)
    No 147 (84.0) 12 (54.6)
    Yes 26 (14.9) 5 (22.7)
    Missing 2 (1.1) 5 (22.7)
Global impact of illness, n (%)
    0 27 (15.4) 7 (31.8)
     1–4 58 (33.1) 4 (18.2)
    5–8 39 (22.3) 4 (18.2)
    9 and above 51 (29.1) 7 (31.8)
Impact of mental health illnesses, n (%)
    None to minor 149 (85.1) 20 (90.9)
    Moderate to major 26 (14.9) 2 (9.1)
Impact of skin conditions, n (%)
    None to minor 144 (82.3) 21 (95.5)
    Moderate to major 31 (17.7) 1 (4.6)
Impact of bowel conditions, n (%)
    None to minor 151 (86.3) 19 (86.4)
    Moderate to major 324 (13.7) 3 (13.6)
Impact of hearing loss, n (%)
    None to minor 145 (82.9) 19 (86.4)
    Moderate to major 30 (17.1) 3 (13.6)

a Grouped and categorised as primary school and some high school, completed high school Year 12 or equivalent, advanced diploma, bachelor degree or graduate diploma or certificate and postgraduate degree (Masters or PHD).

b Open work environment: must not be attending school. Post secondary classes: alternatives to employment, and not working.

c Needs are met under the current day activity arrangements

Among families with complete Kidscreen data, more young people were male (55.4%), aged at or above 23 years (54.9%) and living in the metropolitan area (73.1%). They were most likely to work primarily in a sheltered workshop (43.4%) and fewer participated in post secondary education (25.7%) or worked in an open environment (20.6%). The majority (80.0%) had at least one friend, a similar proportion (78.3%) did not meet the DBC clinical cut-off for a behaviour disorder and caregivers of nearly two thirds (65.1%) felt that their needs were met. The mean total score of functioning in activities of daily living was 37.4 (SD 8.6). In terms of health, close to a third of young people were obese (32.0%) and most (84.0%) had never visited their health professionals for mental health reasons. For just over a half (51.4%) medical problems impacted on their daily life, as reflected by the global and specific measures. Regarding the families of young people, close to half (48.6%) of the fathers worked full-time and most (60.5%) had post secondary school education, whilst the mothers were either part-time workers (37.1%) or not working (38.9%) despite more than half (52.6%) having a trade qualification or bachelor degree. Most of the families earned more than AU$52,000 per year (57.1%) and were able to save a bit (52.6%) or a lot (20.0%). The carer’s emotional state subscale scores for depression, anxiety and stress of all carers were 5.7 (SD 6.8), 3.5 (SD 5.5) and 8.3 (SD 7.7) respectively.

The mean Kidscreen-10 score was 42.0 (SD 8.4, range 26.1–87.9). The mean Kidscreen-27 scores for the five domains were Physical Well-being (38.1 (SD 8.1)), Psychological Well-being (45.6 (SD 9.4)), Autonomy and Parent Relations (50.6 (SD 11)), Social Support and Peers (35.8 (SD 12.7)) and School Environment (48.0 (SD 8.3)) (Table 2).

Table 2. Kidscreen-27 Domain T- scores of 197 young people with Down syndrome, by Kidscreen-10 completion status.

Kidscreen-10
Kidscreen-27 Domain Completed
(N = 175)
Did not complete
(N = 22)
n mean (SD) range n mean (SD) range
Physical well-being 170 38.1 (8.1)
23.1–71.2
7 36.0 (7.5)
26.3–49.5
Psychological well-being (Mood) 163 45.6 (9.4)
23.3–76.4
7 45.0 (13.9)
23.3.-63.1
Autonomy and parent relation (Family) 153 50.6 (11.0)
27.2–79.1
2 45.5 (11.3)
37.5–53.4
Social support and peers (Friends) 159 35.8 (12.7)
9.3–70.3
5 36.3 (8.3)
27.5–49.1
School environment (Daily activity) 170 48.0 (8.3)
26.1–70.7
8 48.4 (4.8)
41.2–55.4

SD, standard deviation

Univariate relationships between Kidscreen-10 and young person sociodemographic, social and medically related factors are presented in Table 3.

Table 3. Univariate relationships between Kidscreen-10 and sociodemographic, social and medically related factors in 175 young people with Down syndrome.

Factors n Mean (SD) Range Coefficient (95% CI) P-value
Gender
    Male 97 42.1 (8.2) 26.1–70.4 Reference
    Female 78 41.8 (8.6) 29.8–87.9 -0.2 (-2.8,2.3) 0.85
Age group (yrs)
    16–18 25 40.1 (8.0) 26.1–68.0 Reference
    19–22 54 41.7 (9.6) 29.8–87.9 1.6 (-2.4,5.6) 0.43
    23–31 96 42.6 (7.7) 29.8–70.4 2.4 (-1.3,6.2) 0.20
Day occupationa (n = 157)
    Open work environment 36 43.0 (7.5) 29.8–59.9 Reference
    In a sheltered service environment 76 42.7 (8.4) 31.0–87.9 -0.4 (-3.7,2.9) 0.82
    Post secondary classes 45 39.9 (8.4) 29.8–70.4 -3.2 (-6.8,0.5) 0.09
Needs metb (n = 161)
    Yes 114 42.7 (8.4) 26.1–87.9 Reference
    No 47 39.9 (7.9) 29.8–66.2 -2.8 (-5.6,0.1) 0.06
Number of friends
    No friends 35 37.1 (5.7) 26.1–50.6 Reference
    1–2 close friends 77 41.6 (7.4) 29.8–70.4 4.5 (1.3,7.6) 0.01
    3 or more friends 63 45.1 (9.5) 32.2–87.9 8.0 (4.7,11.3) <0.01
Behaviour (n = 173)
    <46 137 43.3 (8.5) 29.8–87.9 Reference
    46 and higher 36 37.0 (6.1) 26.1–57.3 -6.2 (-9.2,-3.3) <0.01
Functioning in activities in daily living
    Self care 175 20.2 (4.0)c 7–26c 0.7 (0.4,1.0) <0.01
    Community skills 175 11.4 (4.2)c 4–19c 0.5 (0.2,0.8) <0.01
    Communication skills 175 5.8 (1.5)c 2–8c 1.5 (0.7,2.3) <0.01
    Total 175 37.4 (8.6)c 13–52c 0.3 (0.2,0.5) <0.01
BMI (n = 131)
    Healthy weight (<25) 37 42.3 (7.5) 29.8–59.9 Reference
    Overweight (25–30) 38 43.0 (8.1) 31.0–68.0 0.7 (-2.9,4.4) 0.70
    Obese (>30) 56 41.1 (8.4) 26.1–70.4 -1.2 (-4.6,2.2) 0.48
Physical activity per week (n = 169)
    0–2 days 56 40.2 (8.1) 26.1–66.2 Reference -
    3–4 days 56 44.3 (9.7) 29.8–87.9 4.0 (0.9,7.1) 0.01
    5 or more days 57 42.0 (6.9) 32.2–68.0 1.8 (-1.3,4.9) 0.25
Mental Health visitsd (n = 173)
    No 147 42.8 (8.7) 29.8–87.9 Reference
    Yes 26 37.7 (5.3) 26.1–46.7 -5.1 (-8.6,-1.6) <0.01
Global impact of illness
    0 27 48.1 (11.5) 33.5–87.9 Reference
    1–4 58 42.4 (7.5) 29.8–66.2 -5.6 (-9.2,-2.0) <0.01
    5–8 39 42.0 (8.0) 31.0–70.4 -6.1 (-9.9,-2.2) <0.01
    9 and above 51 38.2 (5.5) 26.1–50.6 -9.8 (-13.5,-6.1) <0.01
Impact of mental health conditionse
    None to minor 149 42.9 (8.4) 29.8–87.9 Reference
    Moderate to major 26 36.9 (6.1) 26.1–50.6 -5.9 (-9.3,-2.5) <0.01
Impact of skin conditionsf
    None to minor 144 42.8 (8.6) 29.8–87.9 Reference
    Moderate to major 31 38.3 (6.3) 26.1–55.8 -4.5 (-7.7,-1.3) <0.01
Impact of bowel conditionsg
    None to minor 151 42.5 (8.5) 29.8–87.9 Reference -
    Moderate to major 24 38.6 (6.5) 26.1–57.3 -3.9 (-7.5,-0.3) 0.03
Impact of hearing loss
    None to minor 145 42.7 (8.5) 29.8–87.9 Reference -
    Moderate to major 30 38.3 (6.6) 26.1–59.9 -4.5 (-7.8,-1.2) <0.01

a Open work environment: must not be attending school). Post secondary classes: alternatives to employment, and not working

b Needs are met under current day activity arrangements

c Functioning in activities of daily living score

d Visits to either psychologist or doctor

eDepression, anxiety, and other mental health

f Psoriasis and eczema

g Constipation, coeliac, and other bowel conditions

CI, confidence interval; SD, standard deviation

Univariate relationships between Kidscreen-10 and parental and family factors are presented in Table 4.

Table 4. Univariate relationships between Kidscreen-10 T-score and parental sociodemographic and family factors in 175 young people with Down syndrome.

Factors n Mean (SD) Range Coefficient (95% CI) P-value
Maternal age (years) (n = 171)
    38–45 13 38.3 (3.1) 33.0–45.0 Reference
    46–55 68 41.7 (7.3) 26.1–59.9 3.4 (-1.5,8.3) 0.18
    56 and older 90 42.5 (9.3) 29.8–87.9 4.2 (-0.6,9.0) 0.08
Paternal age (years) (n = 157)
    42–55 69 40.8 (6.6) 26.1–57.3 Reference
    56–65 63 41.2 (7.1) 29.8–59.9 0.4 (-2.4,3.2) 0.80
    66 and older 25 45.8 (13.1) 31.0–87.9 5.0 (1.2,8.7) 0.01
Maternal work status (n = 169)
    Full time work 36 42.3 (10.8) 29.8–87.9 Reference
    Part-time work 65 41.6 (6.5) 26.1–59.9 -0.7 (-4.1,2.7) 0.69
    Not working 68 42.0 (8.3) 29.8–70.4 -0.2 (-3.6,3.2) 0.89
Paternal work status (n = 137)
    Full time work 85 42.4 (8.4) 29.8–87.9 Reference
    Part-time work 16 41.8 (8.3) 29.8–66.2 -0.5 (-4.9,3.9) 0.81
    Not working 36 40.8 (7.5) 29.8–57.3 -1.5 (-4.7,1.7) 0.35
Maternal education (n = 170)
    Primary school 40 42.0 (7.7) 29.8–66.2 Reference
    High school 38 42.0 (6.8) 29.8–57.3 0.0 (-3.7,3.7) 0.99
    Advanced diploma 32 41.7 (10.8) 32.2–87.9 -0.3 (-4.2,3.6) 0.87
    Bachelor degree or higher 60 41.8 (8.1) 26.1–70.4 -0.2 (-3.5,3.2) 0.92
Paternal education (n = 155)
    Primary school 24 40.4 (7.5) 26.1–57.3 Reference
    High school 25 40.5 (6.8) 29.8–57.3 0.1 (-4.4,4.7) 0.95
    Advanced diploma 48 42.1 (9.2) 29.8–87.9 1.7 (-2.3,5.7) 0.41
    Bachelor degree or higher 58 42.1 (7.9) 31.0–70.4 1.7 (-2.2,5.6) 0.39
Living region (n = 174)
    Major cities of Australia 128 42.5 (8.7) 29.8–87.9 Reference
    Inner regional Australia 20 42.6 (8.9) 26.1–68.0 0.1 (-3.9,4.1) 0.96
    Outer regional and remote Australia 26 39.3 (6.1) 29.8–57.3 -3.2 (-6.8,0.4) 0.08
Annual Family income (n = 158)
    Less than $31,199 30 41.8 (9.9) 26.1–68.0 Reference
    Between $31,200 and 51,999 28 38.8 (7.7) 29.8–59.9 -3.1 (-7.0,0.9) 0.13
    Between 52,000 and 77,999 23 39.7 (4.6) 29.8–48.6 -2.2 (-6.4,2.0) 0.30
    Above 78,000 77 43.0 (7.3) 31.0–70.4 1.1 (-2.1,4.4) 0.49
Savings (n = 171)
    No savings 44 37.8 (7.2) 26.1–68.0 Reference
    We can save a bit every now and again 92 43.2 (8.6) 29.8–87.9 5.3 (2.4,8.3) <0.01
    We can save a lot 35 44.0 (8.1) 31.0–70.4 6.2 (2.5,9.8) <0.01
DASS
     Anxiety of all mother 139 3.4 (5.7)a 0–28a -0.3 (-0.5,-0.1) <0.01
    Anxiety of all caregivers 162 3.5 (5.5)a 0–28a -0.3 (-0.5,-0.1) <0.01
    Depression of mother 139 5.8 (7.0)a 0–34a -0.4 (-0.6,-0.2) <0.01
    Depression of all caregivers 162 5.7 (6.8)a 0–34a -0.4 (-0.6,-0.3) <0.01
    Stress of mother 139 8.4 (8.0)a 0–40a -0.3 (-0.5,-0.2) <0.01
    Stress of all caregivers 162 8.3 (7.7)a 0–40a -0.4 (-0.5,-0.2) <0.01

a DASS score

CI, confidence interval; SD, standard deviation

Adjusted assocations between Kidscreen-10 and relevant factors with or without adjusting for DASS are shown in Table 5.

Table 5. Multiple regression analysis for Kidscreen-10.

Without adjusting for DASS With adjusting for DASS
Adjusted coefficient (95% CI) P-value Estimated meana (95% CI) Adjusted coefficient (95% CI) P-value Estimated meana (95% CI)
Personal sociodemographic and social factors
Friends n = 121 n = 118
    No friends Reference 38.4 (35.1,41.7) Reference 38.1 (34.9,41.4)
    1–2 friends 2.6 (-1.5,6.6) 0.21 41.0 (38.9,43.2) 3.2 (-0.7,7.2) 0.11 41.4 (39.2,43.5)
    3 or more friends 6.0 (1.6,10.4) 0.01 44.4 (41.9,46.9) 6.1 (1.9,10.4) <0.01 44.3 (41.9,46.7)
Day occupationb n = 108 n = 105
    Open work environment Reference 40.3 (36.7,43.9) Reference 39.9 (36.3,43.5)
    In a sheltered service environment 3.2 (-1.3,7.6) 0.16 43.5 (41.2,45.8) 4.0 (-0.4,8.4) 0.08 43.9 (41.7,46.1)
    Post secondary classes -0.9 (-6.2,4.4) 0.75 39.5 (36.2,42.7) -0.7 (-5.8,4.5) 0.79 39.2 (36.1,42.4)
Behaviour n = 121 n = 118
    <46 Reference 42.5 (40.8,44.2) Reference 42.1 (40.4,43.9)
    46 and higher -3.9 (-8.0,0.2) 0.07 38.7 (35.2,42.1) -1.7 (-6.4,3.0) 0.47 40.4 (36.6,44.3)
Personal medically related factors
Physical activity n = 117 n = 114
    0–2 days Reference 41.0 (38.4,43.6) Reference 41.2 (38.7,43.8)
    3–4 days 3.4 (-0.4,7.2) 0.08 44.4 (41.7,47.1) 2.8 (-1.0,6.7) 0.15 44.0 (41.3,46.8)
    5 or more days -0.9 (-4.7,2.8) 0.62 40.0 (37.3,42.8) -0.8 (-4.6,3.0) 0.67 40.4 (37.6,43.2)
Global impact of illness n = 121 n = 118
    0 Reference 48.5 (44.5,52.4) Reference 48.7 (44.7,52.8)
    1–4 -6.0 (-10.7,-1.4) 0.01 42.5 (40.0,44.9) -6.7 (-11.4,-2.1) 0.01 42.0 (39.6,44.4)
    5–8 -7.0 (-12.0,-1.9) <0.01 41.5 (38.5,44.5) -6.6 (-11.7,-1.5) 0.01 42.1 (39.1,45.1)
    9 and above -10.2 (-15.1,-5.3) <0.01 38.2 (35.6,40.9) -10.1 (-15.1,-5.1) <0.01 38.6 (36.0,41.3)
Impact of mental health conditionsc n = 121 n = 118
    None to minor Reference 42.7 (41.1,44.2) Reference 42.4 (40.8,44.0)
    Moderate to major -5.5 (-9.6,-1.5) <0.01 37.1 (33.5,40.7) -3.7 (-8.0,0.5) 0.09 38.7 (34.9,42.4)
Bowel conditionsd n = 121 n = 118
    None to minor Reference 42.3 (40.7,43.8) Reference 42.3 (40.8,43.8)
    Moderate to major -4.1 (-8.4,0.1) 0.06 38.1 (34.3,42.0) -3.9 (-8.1,0.4) 0.07 38.4 (34.6,42.3)
Skin conditionse n = 121 n = 118
    None to minor Reference 42.0 (40.3,43.6) Reference 42.2 (40.6,43.8)
    Moderate to major -1.7 (-5.8,2.5) 0.42 40.2 (36.6,44.0) -2.5 (-6.7,1.6) 0.23 39.6 (36.0,43.3)
Hearing loss n = 121 n = 118
    None to minor Reference 42.2 (40.7,43.8) Reference 42.1 (40.6,43.7)
    Moderate to major -3.2 (-7.2,0.7) 0.11 39.0 (35.5,42.5) -2.1 (-6.2,1.9) 0.30 40.0 (36.4,43.6)
Parental sociodemographic and family factors
Savings n = 119 n = 117
    No savings Reference 38.3 (35.2,41.6) Reference 39.1 (35.8,42.4)
    We save a bit every now and again 4.2 (0.3,8.0) 0.03 42.5 (40.6,44.5) 3.2 (-0.8,7.2) 0.11 42.3 (40.3,44.3)
    We save a lot 4.5 (-0.5,9.5) 0.08 42.9 (39.3,46.4) 4.0 (-1.0,8.9) 0.12 43.0 (39.6,46.5)
Living region n = 121 n = 118
    Major cities of Australia Reference 42.3 (40.7,44.0) Reference 42.6 (40.9,44.2)
    Inner regional Australia -2.3 (-7.8,3.2) 0.41 40.1 (35.0,45.1) -3.0 (-8.3,2.3) 0.27 39.6 (34.7,44.5)
    Outer regional and remote Australia -3.4 (-7.7,1.0) 0.13 39.0 (35.0,42.9) -3.8 (-8.0,0.5) 0.09 38.8 (34.9,42.7)

adjusted for young person’s age and gender, parental sociodemographic factors (including maternal and paternal age, maternal and paternal education, and maternal and paternal occupation), and young person’s functioning of activities of daily living total score

a estimated mean of Kidscreen-10 T-value predicted from the multiple linear regression models

b Open work environment: must not be attending school). Post secondary classes: alternatives to employment, and not working

c Depression, anxiety, and other mental health

d Constipation, coeliac, and other bowel conditions

e Psoriasis and eczema

CI, confidence interval

Association between Kidscreen-10 and young person and family factors

Personal sociodemographic and social factors

The mean Kidscreen-10 scores were similar between males and females (coefficient [β] -0.2; 95% confidence interval [CI] -2.8,2.3; P = 0.85). Compared to the 16–18 years age group, individuals in the older age group (e.g. 23–31 years) had better QOL (23–31 years: β 2.4; 95% CI -1.3,6.2; P = 0.20) (Table 3). Similarly, the mean score of individuals who had 3 or more friends was higher than that of young people with no friends (β 8.0; 95% CI 4.7,11.3; P<0.01) (Table 3), and the effect remained after accounting for confounders without DASS (adjusted β 6.0; 95% CI 1.6,10.4; P = 0.01) or with DASS (adjusted β 6.1; 95% CI 1.9,10.4; P<0.01) (Table 5). Better functioning in activities in daily living was also associated with an improvement in QOL (β 0.3; 95% CI 0.2,0.5; P<0.01) (Table 3). Compared to their respective reference groups, lower mean Kidscreen-10 scores were detected in individuals who participated in post secondary education (β -3.2; 95% CI -6.8,0.5; P = 0.09), had unmet need (β -2.8; 95% CI -5.6,0.1; P = 0.06) or were considered to have more behavioural problems (β -6.2; 95% CI -9.2,-3.3; P = <0.01) (Table 3). After adjusting for confounders, there was minimal difference in mean Kidscreen-10 score between the post secondary education and open work environment group (without DASS: adjusted β -0.9; 95% CI -6.2,4.4; P = 0.75, with DASS: adjusted β -0.7; 95% CI -5.8,4.5; P = 0.79). However, the decline in QOL related to more behavioural problems and differed depending on adjustment for DASS (without DASS: adjusted β -3.9; 95% CI -8.0,0.2; P = 0.07, with DASS: adjusted β -1.7; 95% CI -6.4,3.0; P = 0.47) (Table 5).

Personal medically related factors

Young people with Down syndrome who were deemed obese appeared to have poorer QOL when compared with those with healthy weight (β -1.2; 95% CI -4.6,2.2; P = 0.48). On the other hand, compared to those who were inactive (0–2 days per week), individuals with physical activity of 3 or more days per week had a higher mean Kidscreen-10 score (3–4 days: β 4.0; 95% CI 0.9,7.1; P = 0.01, 5 or more days: β 1.8; 95% CI -1.3,4.9; P = 0.25). Improvement in QOL remained for the 3–4 days group, but not for the 5 or more days group, after adjusting for confounders with or without DASS. Mental health visits, global impact of illness as well as impact of specific medical conditions were all negatively associated with young people’s quality of life (Mental health visit: β -5.1; 95% CI -8.6,1.6; P<0.01, 9 or above global impact of illness: β -9.8; 95% CI -13.5,-6.1; P<0.01) (Table 3). Adjusting for confounders with or without DASS did not affect the negative relationships (Table 5).

Parental sociodemographic and family factors

The mean Kidscreen-10 scores were higher in the oldest parental age group than in the youngest age group (maternal 56 years and older: β 4.2; 95% CI -0.6,9.0; P = 0.08, paternal 66 years and older: β 5.0; 95% CI 1.2,8.7; P = 0.01) (Table 4). Offspring’s QOL was not affected by parental work status or education level. Poorer QOL was observed for those living in outer regional and remote regions of Western Australia (β -3.2; 95% CI -6.8,0.4; P = 0.08), and the association extended to families in inner regional areas after accounting for confounders without DASS (β -2.3; 95% CI -7.8,3.2; P = 0.41) or with DASS (β -3.0; 95% CI -8.3,2.3; P = 0.27). Compared to families in the lowest annual income (AU $31,199 or less) group, QOL in the offspring of those earning AU$78,000 or higher was minimally improved (β 1.1; 95% CI -2.1,4.4; P = 0.49). Similarly QOL scores for those whose families were able to save, either a bit or a lot, were higher than those who had no savings (saved a bit: β 5.3; 95% CI 2.4,8.3; P<0.01, saved a lot: β 6.2; 95% CI 2.5,9.8; P<0.01) (Table 4), but the magnitude of improvement slightly diminished after adjusting for confounders with or without DASS (Table 5). Either mother’s or carer’s mental health status was negatively associated with their children’s QOL (Table 4).

The relative importance of factors affecting QOL, both positively and negatively after adjustment for DASS, have been graphically displayed in Fig 1.

Fig 1. Relative importance of various factors affecting quality of life, both positively and negatively.

Fig 1

Discussion

This study sought to evaluate quality of life in young people with Down syndrome and to identify its determinants including those related to the young person themselves as well as those related to their home environment. We found an overall mean Kidscreen-10 score of 42.0, considerably lower than population norms. Although, univariately, we found associations with a large number of individual factors, once adjusted and particularly when adjusted for mother’s mental state, we found that many of these associations were lost. What stood out however was the negative effect of high illness burden as well as the protective effect on QOL of having three or more friends.

A considerable strength of this study is that the data were collected from a population-based source and that there was a high response fraction to the administration of the questionnaire, thus ensuring representativeness of the underlying population. The comprehensive set of questions and instruments, including psychometrically validated tools such as the DBC, meant that there was a wide range of variables available for this investigation. Given that our study was based on parental report and that there has been previous concern that reporting on child impairment and quality of life can be mediated by maternal depression [35], we also took the important step of adjusting for carer mental health using the DASS in our multivariate analysis.

A limitation of the study relates to the fact that the assessment of QOL relied on data collected by proxy from parental reports, and that we obtained only one data source per participant. Data collection directly from young people with intellectual disability about their health, including mental health status presents numerous challenges and would not be amenable to a study design such as ours [12,13]. That said, a qualitative component exploring the personal definitions of what is considered good quality of life would bridge a gap in literature, and would empower young people with Down syndrome to express their own feelings.

To date there have only been two studies, both published very recently, examining QOL in Down syndrome in young people of a similar age range to ours [36,37]. Using Kidscreen-27, the earlier of the two compared individual domain scores with European normative data for the younger (12–18 year olds) component of their population while the second US study primarily involved a secondary analysis of PedsQL data collected in a study of body composition and metabolic risk in youth with Down syndrome [37]. In the European study the results for the young adult (aged 18–30 years) component of the population, were extremely consistent with our own findings, with lowest scores in the domains of physical wellbeing and social support while those for psychological wellbeing and school (possibly more appropriately categorised as work) environment were similar to population norms and those for autonomy and parent relations slightly higher. In contrast for the US study the scores for each of the domains apart from emotional functioning were lower than for a non-Down syndrome control group. In the first study data were collected on 90 individuals aged between 12 and 30 years from seven different countries and in the second US study from caregivers of 150 young people with Down syndrome, aged from 10–20 years. However, unlike our study neither study was derived from a population-based source and little detail was provided on recruitment or representativeness. In the US study using an Impact of Weight on Quality of Life measure lower scores were reported for obese compared with non-obese individuals irrespective of Down syndrome status. However in both studies there was an absence of data on other variables which would have permitted, as we have done, a comprehensive investigation of determinants of quality of life. The lower domain scores for physical wellbeing and social support seen in both studies were also consistent with the negative effects of disease burden and lack of friendships that we also identified. In another study, adults with Down syndrome aged 18–61 years completed an oral interview in which they provided responses to the Quality Metric Short Form-12 version 2 (SF-12v2) instrument [38]. Compared with a normative sample from the general population, these 60 adults with Down syndrome had higher than average quality of life scores, although the authors acknowledged that their sample was potentially healthier and not representative of the underlying population. Study representativeness is an essential element required for generalisation of findings. However, the only study we could find which considered this issue was a Dutch study restricted to 8 year old children [39], which had a response fraction of 63%, equivalent to around 50% of this birth cohort. Using the TNO-AZL Children’s Quality of Life (TACQOL) measure, an instrument developed in the Netherlands, children with Down syndrome were shown to have considerably poorer quality of life scores compared with population norms, particularly in the areas of gross motor skills, autonomy, social functioning, and cognitive functioning, although the latter might be expected, given that these children do have an intellectual disability. Although there is a growing body of research about the health-related quality of life of their parents and families, we could not find evidence of any further studies assessing quality of life in young people with Down syndrome and, specifically, none comparable with the age group in our study that considered the issue of representativeness.

Our findings paint a picture of worse QOL with increasing burden of illness. The relationship was observed specifically with bowel conditions and mental health problems as well as with our measure of the global impact of illness. Uniformly, a lower quality of life was reported for those with a higher burden of illness compared to those with no impact, with the effects persisting even after adjustment for mother’s mental health status. We were somewhat surprised to find the effect of medical problems on quality of life persisting into adolescence and adulthood where in general the burden of medical comorbidities is much less than in childhood [40]. In regard to mental illness our previous research has suggested that the presence of depressive symptomatology in adolescence/early adulthood might increase the risk of depressive illness occurring later in adulthood [20]. This highlights the importance of raising awareness among families and service providers and of routine screening for depressive symptoms [20].

Consistent with the findings from the international study where low scores were found in the “Social Support and Peers” domain [36], our other major finding related to the value of friendships and positive social relationships. Compared with those reported as having no friends, those with one to two friends had a higher Kidscreen score and it was much higher again in those with three or more friends. In the multivariate analysis after adjustment for other factors the relationship with three or more friends remained although that with one to two friends was attenuated. We previously found in an earlier study relating to school-aged children with Down syndrome that parents reported as many as a third of their children had no friends [41]. The proportion in this adolescent and young adult group with no friends was surprisingly slightly lower, although one might expect the reverse with friendships dropping off after school. In the previous study those with more friends were more likely to have better functional ability but in this study we still saw the relationship with quality of life after adjusting for functional ability.

We found that for young people with Down syndrome, QOL was reported as poorer the further away from the city (i.e. in outer regional and remote Australia) they lived. This is considered consistent with past research in the general population where those living further away from metropolitan areas have been shown to have poorer health outcomes and health risk factor profiles [42]. In our study, this relationship was observed in the univariate and multivariate analysis and did not attenuate after adjusting for carer mental health.

We had anticipated that young people with Down syndrome who participated in open employment would have better QOL than those in sheltered employment, given that such a relationship was found with family QOL[43] and since a higher level of functioning has been shown to be associated with increased likelihood of participation in open employment or training post-school [11]. However, this was not the case. In contrast we saw that better QOL was reported for those in sheltered employment. It is likely that sheltered employment provides more longterm security and stability for people with Down syndrome and possibly better opportunities for developing peer relationships. Further research into the impact of occupation (including frequency of work hours, type of work, interactions etc), in this group, is needed. This could assist policy makers and service providers with regards to occupational oriented services.

In our study, as might be expected from past research [39] behavioural issues were associated with a significant reduction in QOL in the univariate analysis but the effect was attenuated after adjustment for other factors, particularly caregiver mental health. Although previous research showed that approximately one quarter to one third of the young people with Down syndrome have significant behavior problems [44], we found a lower proportion would meet the clinical cut-off score for psychological caseness. Nevertheless, the development and chronicity of behaviour disorders, including risk factors, have been given less attention in the literature in people with intellectual disability [45]. Children with Down syndrome at risk of behavioural disorder in adult life may be identified in childhood and as a young person. Thus, there is a further need to target this area in future research, and appropriate interventions should be offered to reduce risk [46].

Conclusion

This retrospective cohort study would appear to be the first to investigate a range of factors relating to QOL of young people with Down syndrome. Overall, our findings revealed that QOL of young people with Down syndrome was most negatively associated with burden of medical conditions, but also with lack of friendships. The negative effect of behavioural symptoms on quality of life was much reduced when other factors such as functioning were taken into account but the impact of a mental health condition was still associated with a reduced quality of life. Supporting young people with Down syndrome by educating service providers and advocating for a positive and judgment free environment is important in order to enhance quality of life and reduce stigma.

Acknowledgments

We wish to gratefully thank the families for their participation in the study and the Disability Services Commission of WA for facilitating the distribution of questionnaires.

Data Availability

Data are available with approval from the Ethics Committee of the Women’s and Children’s Health Services in Western Australia (Project#1715/EP) for researchers who meet the criteria for access to confidential data. Further Data Requests may be sent to: Human Research Ethics Committee of the Child and Adolescent Health Services in Western Australia (pmhethics@health.wa.gov.au).

Funding Statement

We acknowledge the initial seed funding from Australian Research Alliance for Children and Youth and the ongoing funding provided by the Australian Research Council #LP0989847 for the project ‘Leaving School: Maximising participation and life outcomes in youth with an intellectual disability transitioning from secondary school to adult life’. We acknowledge NHMRC Fellowship funding for Helen Leonard (#1117105).

References

  • 1.World Health Organisation. Constitution of the World Health Organisation Geneva, Switzerland: World Health Organization; 1948. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Felce D, Perry J. Quality of life: its definition and measurement. Research in Developmental Disabilities. 1995. Jan-Feb;16(1):51–74. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.White-Koning M, Arnaud C, Bourdet-Loubère S, Bazex H, Colver A, Grandjean H. Subjective quality of life in children with intellectual impairment–how can it be assessed? Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology. 2005;47:281–285. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Bower C, Leonard H, Petterson B. Intellectual disability in Western Australia Journal of Paediatrics and Child Health. 2000. June;36(3):213–5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Bittles AH, Bower C, Hussain R, Glasson EJ. The four ages of Down syndrome. European Journal of Public Health. 2007. April;17(2):221–5. doi: 10.1093/eurpub/ckl103 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Fitzgerald P, Leonard H, Pikora TJ, Bourke J, Hammond G. Hospital admissions in children with Down syndrome: experience of a population-based cohort followed from birth. PLoS One. 2013;8(8):e70401 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0070401 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Leonard S, Msall M, Bower C, Tremont M, Leonard H. Functional status of school‐aged children with Down syndrome. Journal of paediatrics and child health. 2002;38(2):160–165. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.de Winter CF, Jansen AA, Evenhuis HM. Physical conditions and challenging behaviour in people with intellectual disability: a systematic review. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research. 2011. July;55(7):675–98. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2788.2011.01390.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Einfeld SL, Piccinin AM, Mackinnon A, Hofer SM, Taffe J, Gray KM, et al. Psychopathology in young people with intellectual disability. JAMA. 2006. October 25;296(16):1981–9. doi: 10.1001/jama.296.16.1981 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Einfeld SL, Tonge BJ. Population prevalence in children and adolescents with intellectual disabilities: II epidemiological findings. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research. 1996;40:99–109. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Foley KR, Jacoby P, Girdler S, Bourke J, Pikora T, Lennox N, et al. Functioning and post-school transition outcomes for young people with Down syndrome. Child: Care, Health and Development. 2013. November;39(6):789–800. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Heal LW, Sigelman CK. Response biases in interviews of individuals with limited mental ability. J Intellect Disabil Res. 1995. August;39 (Pt 4):331–40. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Finlay WML, Lyons E. Acquiescence in Interviews With People Who Have Mental Retardation. Mental Retardation. 2002;40(1):14–29. doi: 10.1352/0047-6765(2002)040<0014:AIIWPW>2.0.CO;2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Brown I, Brown RI. Choice as an aspect of quality of life for people with intellectual disabilities. Journal of Policy and Practice in Intellectual Disabilities. 2009;6(11–18). [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Thomas K, Girdler S, Bourke J, Deshpande A, Bathgate K, Fehr S, et al. Overview of Health Issues in School- aged Children with Down Syndrome. International Review of Research in Mental Retardation. 2010;39. [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Roizen NJ. Overview of Health Issues among Persons with Down Syndrome. International Review of Research in Mental Retardation. 2010;39. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Mantry D, Cooper SA, Smiley E, Morrison J, Allan L, Williamson A, et al. The prevalence and incidence of mental ill-health in adults with Down syndrome. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research. 2008. February;52(Pt 2):141–55. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2788.2007.00985.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Fidler DJ, Most DE, Booth-LaForce C, Kelly JF. Temperament and behaviour problems in young children with Down syndrome at 12, 30, and 45 months. Downs Syndrome Research and Practice. 2006. July;10(1):23–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Foley KR, Jacoby P, Einfeld S, Girdler S, Bourke J, Riches V, et al. Day occupation is associated with psychopathology for adolescents and young adults with Down syndrome. BMC Psychiatry. 2014. October 3;14(1):266. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Foley KR, Bourke J, Einfeld SL, Tonge BJ, Jacoby P, Leonard H. Patterns of depressive symptoms and social relating behaviors differ over time from other behavioral domains for young people with Down syndrome. Medicine (Baltimore). 2015. May;94(19):e710. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Betancor Rodriguez V, Arino Mateo E, Rodriguez-Perez A, Delgado Rodriguez N. Do they feel the same as us? The infrahumanization of individuals with Down syndrome. Psicothema. 2016. August;28(3):311–7. doi: 10.7334/psicothema2016.10 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Hodapp RM, Ly TM, Fidler DJ, Ricci LA. Less stress, more rewarding: Parenting children with Down syndrome. Parenting: Science and practice. 2001;1(4):317–337. [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Olssen MB, Hwang CP. Depression in mothers and fathers of children with intellectual disability. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research. 2001;45(6):535–543. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Pillay D, Girdler S, Collins M, Leonard H. “It’s not what you were expecting, but it’s still a beautiful journey”: The experience of mother of children with Down syndrome. Disability and Rehabilitation. 2012;34(18):1501–1510. doi: 10.3109/09638288.2011.650313 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Bourke J, Ricciardo B, Bebbington A, Aiberti K, Jacoby P, Dyke P, et al. Physical and mental health in mothers of children with Down syndrome. Journal of Paediatrics. 2008;153(3):320–326. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Mulroy S, Robertson L, Aiberti K, Leonard H, Bower C. The impact of having a sibling with an intellectual disability: parental perspectives in two disorders. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research. 2008;52(3):216–229. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Cuskelly M, & Gunn P. Sibling relationships of children with Down syndrome: Perspectives of mothers, fathers and siblings American Journal on Mental Retardation. 2003;108(4):234–244. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Petterson B, Leonard H, Bourke J, Sanders R, Chalmers R, Jacoby P, et al. IDEA (Intellectual Disability Exploring Answers): a population-based database for intellectual disability in Western Australia. Annals of Human Biology. 2005. Mar-Apr;32(2):237–43. doi: 10.1080/03014460500075035 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Rasch G. An item analysis which takes individual differences into account. British journal of mathematical and statistical psychology. 1966;19(1):49–57. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Brown TA, Chorpita BF, Korotitsch W, Barlow DH. Psychometric properties of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS) in clinical samples. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 1997. January;35(1):79–89. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Ng F, Trauer T, Dodd S, Callaly T, Campbell S, Berk M. The validity of the 21-item version of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales as a routine clinical outcome measure. Acta Neuropsychiatrica journal. 2007;19(5):304–10. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.McConkey R, Walsh J. An index of social competence for use in determining the service needs of mentally handicapped adults. Journal of Mental Deficiency Research. 1982. March;26(Pt 1):47–61. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Einfeld SL, Tonge BJ. The Developmental Behavior Checklist: the development and validation of an instrument to assess behavioral and emotional disturbance in children and adolescents with mental retardation. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 1995. April;25(2):81–104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Clarke AR, Tonge BJ, Einfeld SL, Mackinnon A. Assessment of change with the Developmental Behaviour Checklist. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research. 2003. March;47(Pt 3):210–2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Davis E, Nicolas C, Waters E, Cook K, Gibbs L, Gosch A, et al. Parent-proxy and child self-reported health-related quality of life: using qualitative methods to explain the discordance. Quality of Life Research. 2007. June;16(5):863–71. doi: 10.1007/s11136-007-9187-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Rofail D, Froggatt D, de la Torre R, Edgin J, Kishnani P, Touraine R, et al. Health-Related Quality of Life in Individuals with Down Syndrome: Results from a Non-Interventional Longitudinal Multi-National Study. Advances in Therapy. 2017. August 09. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Xanthopoulos MS, Walega R, Xiao R, Prasad D, Pipan MM, Zemel BS, et al. Caregiver-Reported Quality of Life in Youth with Down Syndrome. The Journal of Pediatrics. 2017. 2017/10/01/;189(Supplement C):98–104.e1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Graves RJ, Graff JC, Esbensen AJ, Hathaway DK, Wan JY, Wicks MN. Measuring Health-Related Quality of Life of Adults With Down Syndrome. American Journal on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities. 2016. July;121(4):312–26. doi: 10.1352/1944-7558-121.4.312 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.van Gameren-Oosterom HB, Fekkes M, Buitendijk SE, Mohangoo AD, Bruil J, Van Wouwe JP. Development, problem behavior, and quality of life in a population based sample of eight-year-old children with Down syndrome. PLoS One. 2011;6(7):e21879 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0021879 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Pikora TJ, Bourke J, Bathgate K, Foley KR, Lennox N, Leonard H. Health conditions and their impact among adolescents and young adults with Down syndrome. PLoS One. 2014. May 12;9(5):e96868 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0096868 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Oates A, Bebbington A, Bourke J, Girdler S, Leonard H. Leisure participation for school-aged children with Down syndrome. Disability and Rehabilitation. 2011;33(19–20):1880–9. doi: 10.3109/09638288.2011.553701 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Phillips A. Health status differentials across rural and remote Australia. Australian Journal of Rural Health. 2009. February;17(1):2–9. doi: 10.1111/j.1440-1584.2008.01029.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Foley KR, Girdler S, Downs J, Jacoby P, Bourke J, Lennox N, et al. Relationship between family quality of life and day occupations of young people with Down syndrome. Social Psychiatry. 2014;49(9):1455–1465. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Dykens EM. Psychiatric and behavioral disorders in persons with Down syndrome. Mental retardation and developmental disabilities research reviews. 2007;13(3):272–8. doi: 10.1002/mrdd.20159 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.McClintock K, Hall S, Oliver C. Risk markers associated with challenging behaviours in people with intellectual disabilities: a meta-analytic study. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research. 2003. September;47(Pt 6):405–16. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.McCarthy J. Behaviour problems and adults with Down syndrome: childhood risk factors. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research. 2008. October;52(10):877–82. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2788.2008.01104.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Data Availability Statement

Data are available with approval from the Ethics Committee of the Women’s and Children’s Health Services in Western Australia (Project#1715/EP) for researchers who meet the criteria for access to confidential data. Further Data Requests may be sent to: Human Research Ethics Committee of the Child and Adolescent Health Services in Western Australia (pmhethics@health.wa.gov.au).


Articles from PLoS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

RESOURCES