Skip to main content
Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences logoLink to Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences
. 2018 Jun 4;373(1751):20170255. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2017.0255

Neuronal and non-neuronal signals regulate Caernorhabditis elegans avoidance of contaminated food

Alexandra Anderson 1,, Rachel McMullan 2,
PMCID: PMC6000145  PMID: 29866922

Abstract

One way in which animals minimize the risk of infection is to reduce their contact with contaminated food. Here, we establish a model of pathogen-contaminated food avoidance using the nematode worm Caernorhabditis elegans. We find that avoidance of pathogen-contaminated food protects C. elegans from the deleterious effects of infection and, using genetic approaches, demonstrate that multiple sensory neurons are required for this avoidance behaviour. In addition, our results reveal that the avoidance of contaminated food requires bacterial adherence to non-neuronal cells in the tail of C. elegans that are also required for the cellular immune response. Previous studies in C. elegans have contributed significantly to our understanding of molecular and cellular basis of host–pathogen interactions and our model provides a unique opportunity to gain basic insights into how animals avoid contaminated food.

This article is part of the Theo Murphy meeting issue ‘Evolution of pathogen and parasite avoidance behaviours’.

Keywords: Caenorhabditis elegans, avoidance behaviour, host–pathogen interaction

1. Introduction

Almost all animals are at risk from infection by pathogens and parasites in their environment. Consequently, animals have evolved tolerance, resistance and avoidance mechanisms to manage this constant threat. While we know a great deal about the immunology, biochemistry and genetics of infections, we know little about the pathogen avoidance behaviours, which prevent or reduce contact with pathogens.

As highlighted in this issue, pathogen and parasite avoidance behaviours have been observed experimentally in a diverse range of species including worms, ants, flies, bees, marine crustaceans, birds, marsupials, rodents and non-human primates. Of these, invertebrate model organisms such as the fruit fly Drosophila melanogaster and the nematode worm Caenorhabditis elegans provide a unique opportunity to use genetics in order to identify the evolutionarily conserved genes involved in pathogen avoidance. Furthermore, the ability to experimentally manipulate pathogen avoidance in these models allows us to examine the impact of this behaviour on infection.

The nematode worm C. elegans can be easily infected with a wide range of bacterial pathogens by providing them as a food source [1,2] and has evolved behavioural and cellular defences against a number of pathogens [3,4], providing a convenient model for the genetic dissection of pathogen avoidance. With only 302 neurons and 7000 synapses, the simple, well-described nervous system of C. elegans is frequently used to study complex behaviours at the level of genes, neurons and neural circuits [5]. Despite its simplicity, almost all gene families involved in mammalian neuronal function are conserved and behavioural changes, evoked by cues in the environment, can be easily quantified.

C. elegans uses multiple behavioural processes including innate and learned olfactory preference [6,7], mechanosensation [8] and aerotaxis [9] to avoid pathogenic strains of several bacteria, including Serratia marcescens [10,11], Bacillus thuringiensis [12], Streptomyces spp. [13], Pseudomonas aeruginosa [9] and Microbacterium nematophilum [14,15]. In the case of P. aeruginosa, this pathogen avoidance behaviour has been demonstrated to protect C. elegans from infection because mutant C. elegans that are unable to avoid pathogenic lawns are more susceptible to infections [9,16] while manipulating experimental conditions such that wild-type animals are unable to avoid pathogenic lawns also results in increased infection susceptibility [16].

Using genetic and biochemical approaches, some of the microbial cues, neurons and signalling pathways that mediate the avoidance of these pathogens have been identified [4]. G-protein signalling in the chemosensory neuron AWB [11] and signalling via the single C. elegans Toll-like receptor tol-1 [10] are required to mediate the avoidance of pathogenic S. marcesecens with AWB being required to mediate an innate aversion to the bacterial surfactant Serrawettin W2 [11]. In the case of B. thuringiensis, the toxin Cry6A2a promotes the avoidance of bacterial lawns [17], and both neuropeptide [18] and insulin-like signalling [12] have been implicated in the regulation of this avoidance behaviour although the neuronal basis of this response remains to be identified. By contrast, several chemosensory neurons have been identified as required for the avoidance of P. aeruginosa and Streptomyces spp. [6,13]. In the case of Streptomyces, the G-protein-coupled receptor srb-6 is required in chemosensory neurons ASH, ADL, ADF or AWA to detect dodecanoic acid produced by Streptomyces and promote avoidance [13]. A neuronal circuit involving AWC and AWB mediates the innate olfactory preference for P. aeruginosa resulting in initial attraction to P. aeruginosa lawns; however, in animals trained with pathogenic P. aeruginosa, serotonin signalling in ADF sensory neurons alters the downstream output of this circuit to promote pathogen avoidance [6]. Additionally, changes in aerotaxis and mechanosensation have been associated with pathogen avoidance behaviour [8,9]. Bacterial secondary metabolites activate G-protein signalling in the sensory neuron ASJ to mediate neuroendocrine signalling to alter aerotaxis behaviour and promote pathogen avoidance [19] while ablation of the mechanosensory neuron OLL results in enhanced avoidance of P. aeruginosa, suggesting that mechanosensory detection of bacteria may contribute to pathogen avoidance [8]. Lastly, C. elegans avoids the nematode-specific pathogen M. nematophilum [14,15]. The cyclic nucleotide-gated channel encoded by tax-2 and tax-4 is required for the avoidance of M. nematophilum because tax-2(p671) and tax-4(e2861) mutants are defective in this avoidance response and are unable to distinguish between Escherichia coli and M. nematophilum in a choice assay [14]. The tax-2 and tax-4 channel is essential for the function of many sensory neurons implicating chemosensation in this avoidance response, although the identity of the neurons required for this avoidance response remains unknown.

These previous studies have largely used either C. elegans propagated monoaxenically on pathogenic bacteria or bacterial choice assays in which animals chose between monoaxenic lawns of pathogenic and non-pathogenic bacteria. Here, we extend our understanding of the molecular and cellular basis of pathogen avoidance by investigating the response of C. elegans to food contaminated with pathogenic bacteria. Using lawns of C. elegans laboratory food source, E. coli OP50 contaminated with M. nematophilum, we show that C. elegans is also able to detect and avoid low levels of pathogenic bacteria in the presence of attractive cues from food. This avoidance behaviour decreases pathogen load and protects animals from the growth delay caused by this pathogen contamination. We further show that avoidance of contaminated bacterial lawns requires signalling in multiple sensory neurons as well as pathogen attachment to the non-neuronal cells associated with the cellular immune response to M. nematophilum. Our results establish an additional model of pathogen avoidance in C. elegans, which allows the avoidance of pathogen-contaminated food to be studied under controlled experimental conditions. Further investigation of this model will reveal additional insights into the genetics of pathogen avoidance and its role in protecting host species from infection.

2. Material and methods

(a). Strains

C. elegans strains used in this study are detailed in electronic supplementary material, table S1. All strains were cultivated at 20°C on nematode-growth media (NGM) plates seeded with E. coli OP50, unless otherwise stated, and maintained as described previously [20].

(b). M. nematophilum infection

Infection with M. nematophilum was performed as described previously [21]. NGM plates were seeded with 200 µl of 10% M. nematophilum (CBX102) or non-pathogenic M. nematophilum (UV336) diluted in E. coli OP50. Where indicated, CBX102 and UV336 were heat-inactivated by incubating at 65°C for 30 min. To test the effectiveness of heat-inactivation bacteria were streaked onto NGM plates. After 6 days at 37°C, colonies were observed in plates streaked with untreated CBX102 and UV336; however, no colonies were observed on plates streaked with heat-inactivated bacteria. For assays in which lawn avoidance was prevented, bacterial lawns were prepared as described above, but the culture was spread to cover the whole plate.

Unless otherwise stated, adult animals were transferred from E. coli OP50 plates to infection plates and F1 progeny were scored for lawn avoidance (as described below) and the Dar phenotype. Experiments were performed in triplicate and repeated at least three times.

Pathogen load and pathogen clearance were assessed by labelling M. nematophilum using SYTO13. SYTO13 staining was performed as described previously [22]. Following incubation with SYTO13, animals were either transferred to unseeded plates, for clearance assays as described previously [15], or mounted for imaging and processed as described in Anderson et al. [15].

(c). Lawn avoidance assays

Lawn avoidance assay plates were prepared by seeding standard NGM plates with 200 µl of 10% M. nematophilum CBX102 or UV336 diluted in E. coli OP50. Control plates were seeded with 200 µl of E. coli OP50. Plates were allowed to dry at room temperature overnight. Three adult animals were transferred from E. coli OP50 plates to lawn avoidance plates and allowed to lay eggs for between 4 and 8 h. Adult animals were removed and F1 progeny were allowed to develop at 20°C. Lawn avoidance (percentage of animals not on the bacterial lawn) was scored when animals reached adulthood. Avoidance index was calculated by dividing the percentage of animals avoiding CBX102-contaminated lawns by the percentage of animals avoiding E. coli OP50 lawns. Experiments were performed in triplicate and repeated at least three times.

(d). Lawn-leaving assays

Lawn-leaving assays were adapted from the male-leaving assay described by Lipton et al. [23]. Approximately 20 µl of bacterial culture (OD600 = 0.6) was seeded in the centre of 90 mm plates containing 13 ml of NGM. Plates were allowed to dry overnight at room temperature and individual 1-day-old adults were transferred to the centre of the bacterial lawn. Ten plates were prepared for each condition being tested. Plates were kept at 20°C and scored for leaving every hour. Animals were scored as leavers if they, or their tracks, could be seen 1 cm or less from the edge of the plate. Animals scored as leavers were excluded from further analysis. Experiments were repeated at least three times.

(e). Growth rate assays

Bacterial lawns were prepared as described above. Eggs were obtained by bleaching [24] and allowed to hatch overnight in M9 at 20°C to obtain synchronized populations of L1 animals. Animals were transferred to infection plates as L1s. Approximately 50 L1s were transferred to each plate. After 48 h, the age of animals was determined by categorizing animals into developmental stages (L1–L4 and adult). The age of L4 animals was further categorized based on the development of the vulval as described in MacNeil et al. [25]. Experiments were performed in triplicate and repeated at least three times.

(f). BUS-1 transgenes

pMGN16 (bus-1p∷BUS-1cDNA∷GFP) (a gift from Jonathan Hodgkin, University of Oxford) was injected into bus-1(e2678) at 10 ng µl−1. impEx58 contains an extrachromosomal version of pMGN16. To express the BUS-1 cDNA specifically in rectal epithelial cells, the BUS-1 cDNA was subcloned into a vector containing a 1.3 Kb fragment of the egl-5 promoter that drives GFP expression in B, K, F, U, P12.pa and three posterior body wall muscles [26] (pRJM205). pRJM205 was injected into bus-1(e2678) at 10 ng µl−1 and impEx70 contains an extrachromosomal version of this plasmid.

(g). TAX-2 transgenes

Plasmids expressing the TAX-2 cDNA under the control of flp-6, gcy-32, gcy-8 or flp-17 promoters to drive TAX-2 expression in ASE, AQR, PQR and URX, ADF or BAG, respectively, were a gift from Mario de Bono (MRC Laboratory for Molecular Biology). These plasmids were injected into tax-2(p694) at 20 ng µl−1. impEx71, impEx72 and impEx73 contain extrachromosomal versions of flp-6p∷TAX-2cDNA;gcy-32p∷TAX-2cDNA.

(h). S. marcescens infection and survival and lawn avoidance assay

S. marcescens infections, survival and lawn avoidance assays were performed as described previously [11]. For survival assays, live animals were transferred to new NGM plates seeded with S. marcescens Db10 daily. Experiments were performed in triplicate and repeated at least three times.

(i). Statistical analysis

In all cases, statistical analysis was performed using Prism 6 (GraphPad Software). Normality was determined using a D'Agostino-Pearson omnibus normality test. Pathogen load was compared using a Mann–Whitney test. Dar data were compared using a Mann–Whitney test or one-way ANOVA followed by Tukey's HSP post hoc multiple comparison test. The avoidance index was calculated by dividing the percentage of animals avoiding CBX102-contaminated lawns by the percentage of animals avoiding E. coli OP50 lawns, and data were compared using a one-way ANOVA followed by Tukey's HSP post hoc multiple comparison test (for data with a Gaussian distribution) or the Kruskal–Wallis test followed by Dunn's multiple comparison test (for non-Gaussian data). Lawn avoidance, leaving assay, S. marcescens avoidance, growth rate and pathogen clearance data were compared using a two-way ANOVA followed by Tukey's HSP post hoc multiple comparison test (for lawn avoidance, leaving assay, S. marcescens avoidance and growth rate) or Sidak's multiple comparison test (for pathogen clearance data). Significance levels are indicated on figures and in the text as follows: *p ≤ 0.05, **p ≤ 0.01, ***p ≤ 0.001, ****p ≤ 0.0001, n.s. p > 0.05.

3. Results

(a). C. elegans avoids lawns contaminated with pathogenic M. nematophilum

We and others have previously observed that wild-type C. elegans avoids lawns of its bacterial food E. coli OP50 contaminated with M. nematophilum [14]. To quantify this effect, we scored lawn avoidance for wild-type animals grown for one generation on contaminated bacterial lawns (figure 1a). Approximately 51% of wild-type animals avoided lawns composed of the standard C. elegans bacterial food (E. coli, OP50) contaminated with 10% M. nematophilum. By contrast, only 17% animals avoided uncontaminated E. coli bacterial lawns (figure 1a). This lawn avoidance behaviour required live bacteria because this increase in lawn avoidance was not observed when E. coli lawns were contaminated with heat-killed M. nematophilum (figure 1a).

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

C. elegans avoids pathogen-contaminated lawns. (a) The percentage of wild-type animals avoiding bacterial lawns. Contamination of bacterial lawns with M. nematophilum increases lawn avoidance to 50.8% when compared with control E. coli lawns (16.8%). By contrast, lawn avoidance was not significantly increased when bacterial lawns were contaminated with either non-pathogenic or heat-killed M. nematophilum. (b) Lawn-leaving assays [23] were used to determine whether monoaxenic lawns of E. coli OP50, M. nematophilum or non-pathogenic M. nematophilum alter C. elegans behaviour. Wild-type animals remain on lawns of E. coli for the duration of the assay. By contrast, 28.1% of wild-type animals leave pure non-pathogenic M. nematophilum lawns. The percentage of animals leaving bacterial lawns is further increased to 56.4% when leaving assays are performed using pure M. nematophilum lawns. * indicates significance relative to E. coli. # indicates significance relative to non-pathogenic M. nematophilum (*p ≤ 0.05, ***p ≤ 0.001, ****p ≤ 0.0001. See Material and methods for details of statistical analysis).

Food abundance and food quality have also been shown to regulate lawn occupancy and animals leave bacterial lawns when food levels are low or of poor quality [27,28]. Therefore, the avoidance phenotype that we observed could be due to changes in C. elegans behaviour caused by either decreased availability/quality in contaminated bacterial lawns or the pathogenic nature of M. nematophilum. To distinguish between these two possibilities, we made use of a non-pathogenic form of M. nematophilum [29], which fails to trigger the C. elegans immune response [30]. We first compared the behaviour of C. elegans exposed to monoaxenic lawns of E. coli OP50, M. nematophilum or the non-pathogenic form of M. nematophilum using a lawn-leaving assay. Individual adult animals were transferred to small bacterial lawns seeded in the centre of 90 mm plates and lawn leaving was assessed over a 24 h period. Animals were scored as having left the lawn if they, or their tracks, could be seen within 1 cm of the edge of the plate. Animals placed on E. coli lawns remained feeding on these lawns for the duration of the experiment (figure 1b); however, animals exposed to M. nematophilum left the lawns with only 43.6% of animals remaining on the lawns after 24 h (figure 1b). Using lawns of non-pathogenic M. nematophilum, we observed some animals leaving lawns (figure 1b) although significantly more animals remained on these non-pathogenic lawns after 24 h than on pathogenic M. nematophilum lawns (figure 1b, compare 43.6% remaining on M. nematophilum lawns at 24 h with 71.9% remaining on non-pathogenic M. nematophilum lawns). These data suggest that, although the pathogenic nature of M. nematophilum accounts for the majority of the lawn-leaving phenotype, differences in food quality between E. coli OP50 and M. nematophilum are sufficient to alter C. elegans behaviour.

To determine whether the change in behaviour caused by differences in food quality between M. nematophilum and E. coli had a significant effect on the avoidance of contaminated lawns, we performed lawn avoidance assays using non-pathogenic M. nematophilum. We did not observe any significant differences in lawn avoidance between E. coli OP50 and lawns contaminated with non-pathogenic M. nematophilum (figure 1a), indicating that the pathogenicity of M. nematophilum, and not differences in food quality or abundance on contaminated lawns, was responsible for the lawn avoidance behaviour that we observed on M. nematophilum-contaminated lawns.

(b). Pathogen avoidance decreases pathogen load and increases pathogen clearance rates

What role does pathogen avoidance play in protecting C. elegans from infection by M. nematophilum? To address this question, we modified our experimental conditions to prevent pathogen avoidance by spreading contaminated bacterial lawns over the entire agar surface (full plates) and assessing host responses to infection with M. nematophilum by scoring the Dar phenotype [31], pathogen load [15], pathogen clearance rate [15] and growth rate [25,31].

Preventing pathogen avoidance increased pathogen load, as determined by measuring levels of SYTO-13 labelled M. nematophilum in the rectal opening (figure 2a), and decreased the rate of clearance of this labelled pathogen from the rectal opening (figure 2b) when compared with animals infected under standard avoidance conditions (figure 2a,b). Taken together, these data indicate that animals that were unable to avoid contaminated bacterial lawns were more severely infected than control animals.

Figure 2.

Figure 2.

Avoidance of contaminated bacterial lawns protects C. elegans from infection by reducing pathogen load and increasing pathogen clearance. (a) Pathogen load under avoidance (standard plate) and non-avoidance (full plate) conditions. Pathogen load was assessed by measuring the fluorescence of SYTO13-labelled M. nematophilum in the rectal opening of wild-type animals. SYTO13 fluorescence was increased from 3023 arbitrary units on standard plates to 4634 arbitrary units on full plates. (b) The clearance of SYTO13-labelled pathogen from the rectal opening of animals infected under avoidance (standard plate) and non-avoidance (full plate) conditions. Labelled pathogen is cleared more quickly under avoidance conditions than non-avoidance conditions, with 52.3% of animals on standard plates and 71.4% of animals on full plates retaining labelled pathogen at 90 min. (c) The Dar phenotype of animals grown on lawns contaminated with 0.05% M. nematophilum under avoidance (standard plate) and non-avoidance (full plate) conditions. No significant difference was observed. (d) Developmental stage of animals grown on contaminated bacterial lawns under avoidance (standard plate) and non-avoidance (full plate) conditions. Animals were transferred to plates as L1s and the developmental stage was assessed 48 h after transfer. Infection with M. nematophilum decreases the proportion of animals reaching L4/adult stages; however, the proportion of animals classified as pre-L4 was not significantly altered by manipulating avoidance conditions. (e) L4 animals were further classified as early, mid, mid–late or late L4 according to their vulval development [25]. L4 development was delayed in animals grown on M. nematophilum-contaminated bacterial lawns and this delay was enhanced when animals were prevented from avoiding contaminated bacterial lawns. * indicates significance relative to standard plates (*p ≤ 0.05, ***p ≤ 0.001, ****p ≤ 0.0001. See Material and methods for details of statistical analysis).

When infected with M. nematophilum, animals mount a protective response to infection which includes the deformed anal region, or Dar, phenotype. To determine whether the increased pathogen avoidance altered this protective response, we scored the Dar phenotype in animals grown under standard avoidance conditions and full plate conditions. Under our standard infection conditions of 10% M. nematophilum, almost all animals (greater than 97%) become Dar on standard plates making it difficult to determine whether inability to avoid contaminated bacteria could alter the Dar phenotype (A. Anderson, data not shown). Therefore, we modified our infection conditions, so that E. coli OP50 lawns were contaminated with 0.05% M. nematophilum [15]. Under standard avoidance conditions, 68% of animals had the Dar phenotype on these lawns and we observed no difference when animals were infected under full plate conditions (figure 2c). Therefore, failure to avoid contaminated bacterial lawn does not have an observable effect on this protective response.

Animals infected with M. nematophilum grow more slowly than those grown on E. coli OP50 [31]; therefore, we asked whether preventing pathogen avoidance and increasing pathogen load would exacerbate this growth delay. To do this, we scored the developmental stage of animals grown from L1 stage on standard and full plates. After 48 h on standard E. coli lawns or lawns contaminated with non-pathogenic M. nematophilum, almost all animals had reached L4 or adult stages and this was not significantly altered by preventing lawn avoidance (figure 2d). However, after 48 h, only 26% of animals grown on standard lawns contaminated with M. nematophilum had reached the L4 stage (figure 2d). The percentage of animals reaching the L4 stage was not significantly altered by preventing pathogen avoidance (figure 2d). These L4 animals were further classified according to whether they had reached the early, mid- or late L4 stage using vulval development [25]. We found that 38% of animals on standard plates were at the mid–late L4 stage (figure 2e) after 48 h on standard M. nematophilum-contaminated lawns. By contrast, when animals were unable to avoid M. nematophilum-contaminated lawns, the percentage of animals at the mid–late L4 stage was decreased to 12% while the percentage of animals at the early L4 was increased from 7% on standard plates to 31% on full plates (figure 2e). Taken together, these results demonstrate that pathogen avoidance protects animals from the detrimental effects of infection on growth by decreasing pathogen load.

(c). Pathogen avoidance requires signalling in sensory neurons

Previous studies have shown that the cyclic nucleotide-gated channel encoded by tax-2 and tax-4 is required for the avoidance of S. marcescens [11]. Furthermore, alleles of tax-2 were isolated in a screen for genes required for the host response to M. nematophilum infection and shown to be required for choice between E. coli OP50 and M. nematophilum [14]. To determine whether the TAX-2/TAX-4 channel was required for the avoidance of M. nematophilum-contaminated bacterial lawns, we performed lawn avoidance assays using tax-2 and tax-4 mutants and calculated a pathogen avoidance index for each strain (see Material and methods for details), which allowed us to compare the ability of different strains to initiate avoidance behaviours in response to the presence of pathogenic bacteria. When compared with wild-type animals, we observed no difference in the ability of tax-2(p671) and tax-4(p678) animals to avoid E. coli lawns or lawns contaminated with non-pathogenic M. nematophilum (figure 3b). However, we observed a significant decrease in both the percentage of tax-2(p671) and tax-4(p678) animals avoiding M. nematophilum-contaminated lawns (figure 3b) and the pathogen avoidance index of tax-2(p671) and tax-4(p678) animals (figure 3a), suggesting that neurons expressing TAX-2 and TAX-4 are required for the avoidance of M. nematophilum.

Figure 3.

Figure 3.

Avoidance of pathogen-contaminated lawns requires TAX-2/4. (a) Lawn avoidance index was calculated by dividing the percentage of animals avoiding M. nematophilum-contaminated lawns by the percentage of animals avoiding OP50 E. coli lawns. Pathogen avoidance index of wild-type, tax-2(p671), tax-2(p694) and tax-4(p678) animals. (b) The percentage of tax-2(p671), tax-2(p694) and tax-4(p678) animals avoiding M. nematophilum-contaminated bacterial lawns is decreased when compared with wild-type controls. (**p ≤ 0.01, ***p ≤ 0.001, ****p ≤ 0.0001. See Material and methods for details of statistical analysis).

The TAX-2/TAX-4 channel is essential for the function of at least nine pairs of amphid chemosensory neurons [32,33]. One of these, AWB, is required to stimulate the avoidance of S. marcescens [11]; however, two lines of evidence suggest that AWB is not required for the avoidance of M. nematophilum-contaminated lawns. Firstly, lim-4(ky403) animals, which lack functional AWB neurons, have a wild-type avoidance response to bacterial lawns contaminated with M. nematophilum (figure 4a,b). Secondly, an additional tax-2 allele, tax-2(p694), resulted in a pathogen avoidance defect similar to that observed in tax-2(p671) animals (figure 3a,b). tax-2(p694) mutants carry a deletion in tax-2 that has been demonstrated to affect TAX-2 function only in AQR, PQR, URX, AFD, ASE and BAG chemosensory neurons [32]. The fact that tax-2(p694) animals are defective in avoidance of M. nematophilum-contaminated lawns implicates one or more of these neurons in this pathogen avoidance response.

Figure 4.

Figure 4.

Avoidance of pathogen-contaminated lawns requires signalling in multiple sensory neurons. (a) Pathogen avoidance index of animals lacking functional sensory neurons. The avoidance index was decreased when compared with wild type in animals lacking ASEL and AWC (ceh-36), ASEL alone (ΔASEL), AQR, PQR and URX (ΔAQR, PQR, URX) or AFD alone (ΔAFD). Pathogen avoidance index was increased when compared with wild type in animals lacking ASER alone (ΔASER) or BAG (ΔBAG). (b) Percentage of animals lacking functional sensory neurons avoiding M. nematophilum-contaminated bacterial lawns. The percentage of animals avoiding M. nematophilum-contaminated lawns was decreased when compared with wild type in animals lacking ASEL and AWC (ceh-36), ASEL alone (ΔASEL) or AQR, PQR and URX (ΔAQR, PQR and URX). The percentage of animals avoiding E. coli lawns was increased in animals lacking AFD (ΔAFD). (c) Pathogen avoidance index of tax-2(p694) cell-specific rescuing lines. The decreased pathogen avoidance index of tax-2(p694) was rescued by expression of TAX-2 cDNA in ASE, AQR, PQR, URX, AFD and BAG and partly by expression in ASE, AQR, PQR and URX but not by expression of TAX-2 cDNA in ASE alone, AFD alone or AQR, PQR and URX. (d) Percentage of tax-2(p694) cell-specific rescuing lines avoiding M. nematophilum-contaminated bacterial lawns. The lawn avoidance defect of tax-2(p694) was rescued by expression of TAX-2 cDNA in ASE, AQR, PQR, URX, AFD and BAG or by expression in ASE, AQR, PQR and URX but not by expression of TAX-2 cDNA in ASE alone, AFD alone or AQR, PQR and URX. See electronic supplementary material, table S1, for details of strains used. * indicates significance relative to wild type. # indicates significance relative to tax-2(p694) (**p ≤ 0.01, ***p ≤ 0.001, ****p ≤ 0.0001. See Material and methods for details of statistical analysis).

Which of these neurons is required for the avoidance of M. nematophilum contamination? To address this question, we assessed the consequences of functionally ablating AFD alone, ASE alone, BAG alone or AQR, PQR and URX on avoidance of M. nematophilum-contaminated lawns. Cell-specific expression of the egl-1 cell death gene to ablate BAG neurons or ablation of ASER using cell-specific expression of caspase3 resulted in an increase in the pathogen avoidance index of these strains when compared with wild-type controls (figure 4a); however, this effect may be due, at least in part, to decreased avoidance of E. coli lawns, or increased variability in the avoidance response in these strains because we observed that the avoidance of M. nematophilum-contaminated bacterial lawns by these strains was not significantly different from wild type (figure 4b). Conversely, genetically disrupting ASEL (using ceh-36(ks86) mutants), or cell-specific expression of caspase3 to ablate ASEL alone, AFD alone or AQR, PQR and URX, decreased the pathogen avoidance index (figure 4a). ceh-36(ks86) are defective in the development of both ASEL and AWC; therefore we used animals expressing caspase3 in AWC to confirm that ablation of AWC did not affect lawn avoidance (figure 4a,b). Since the ability of AQR-, PQR- and URX-ablated animals to avoid E. coli lawns is indistinguishable from wild type and the percentage of animals avoiding M. nematophilum-contaminated lawns is significantly decreased when compared with wild type (figure 4b), the decreased pathogen avoidance index we observed in these animals is likely to reflect a specific defect in pathogen avoidance caused by the loss of AQR, PQR or URX. However, the decreased pathogen avoidance index in AFD-ablated animals appears to be caused by a basal avoidance defect because we failed to observed a significant difference between the percentage of animals avoiding M. nematophilum-contaminated lawns in wild-type and AFD-ablated animals and instead observed a significant increase in the percentage of animals avoiding E. coli lawns in this strain when compared with wild-type animals (figure 4b). Ablation of ASEL also resulted in a slight defect in basal avoidance because there was a small but significant increase in the percentage of ASEL-ablated animals avoiding E. coli lawns when compared with wild-type controls (figure 4b). However, ASEL also appears to be defective in pathogen avoidance because the percentage of animals avoiding M. nematophilum­-contaminated lawns was decreased when compared with wild-type controls (figure 4b). Taken together, these results suggest that ASEL and/or AQR, PQR and URX are required for the avoidance of pathogen-contaminated lawns while AFD is required to regulate basal lawn avoidance.

A number of sensory neurons required for pathogen avoidance have been identified [6,11,19,34]. In some cases, these neurons appear to regulate the avoidance of more than one pathogen; for example AWB is required for the avoidance of both S. marcescens and P. aeruginosa [6,11]. To determine whether ablation of ASEL alone, AFD alone or AQR, PQR and URX altered C. elegans ability to avoid other pathogens, we performed lawn avoidance assays using S. marcescens Db10. Although some avoidance of E. coli OP50 was observed in animals lacking AFD alone (electronic supplementary material, figure S1), confirming our previous observations (figure 4b) that AFD regulates basal avoidance, ablation of these neurons individually did not have any effect on the ability of these animals to avoid S. marcescens (electronic supplementary material, figure S1). These experiments confirm that animals lacking ASEL alone or AQR, PQR and URX retain the ability to initiate avoidance behaviours in the presence of some noxious stimuli and demonstrate that these neurons are not required for the avoidance of all pathogens, raising the possibility that these neurons may be sensing pathogen-specific cues to initiate avoidance behaviours.

To begin to understand how signalling in these neurons regulates the avoidance of contaminated bacterial lawns, we sought to rescue the pathogen avoidance defect of tax-2(p694) mutants by expressing the TAX-2 cDNA from cell-specific promoters. Expression of TAX-2 in ASE, AFD, BAG, AQR, PQR and URX was sufficient to rescue tax-2(p694) (figure 4c,d); however, expression of TAX-2 in ASE alone, AFD alone or AQR, PQR and URX was unable to restore pathogen avoidance in tax-2(p694) animals (figure 4c,d), indicating that TAX-2 is required in multiple neurons to promote pathogen avoidance. To begin to address the combinations of neurons in which TAX-2 is required, we attempted to rescue tax-2(p694) mutants by expressing the TAX-2 cDNA in ASE, AQR, PQR and URX. Expression of TAX-2 in ASE, AQR, PQR and URX increased the pathogen avoidance index of tax-2(p694) animals (figure 4c) and restored the percentage of tax-2(p694) animals avoiding M. nematophilum-contaminated lawns to levels that were indistinguishable from wild type (figure 4d), indicating that expression of TAX-2 in these neurons was sufficient to rescue tax-2(p694). However, the pathogen avoidance index of these rescued animals was significantly lower than in wild-type animals (figure 4c). This difference most likely reflects an increase in the ability of these animals to avoid lawns of E. coli (figure 4d).

Taken together, these data indicate that ASEL, AQR, PQR and URX sensory neurons can suppress pathogen avoidance behaviour. Furthermore, signalling in more than one of these neurons is necessary for wild-type avoidance of M. nematophilum-contaminated lawns (figure 4), but not avoidance of S. marcescens (electronic supplementary material, figure S1).

(d). Pathogen attachment to epithelial cells is also required for efficient pathogen clearance

Several tax mutants are variably Bus (bacterial unswollen) following infection with M. nematophilum, implicating the TAX-2/TAX-4 channel in regulation of the Dar phenotype [14]. The Bus phenotype can be caused by failure to M. nematophilum to adhere to the rectal opening and has been observed in animals with a defective cuticle including srf-2, srf-3 and srf-5 [31]. Cuticle defects were observed in tax-2 and tax-4 mutants, suggesting that the Bus phenotype may be a consequence of impaired bacterial adherence in these animals [31]. Does impaired bacterial adherence also contribute to the pathogen avoidance defect observed in tax-2 and tax-4 mutants?

To test the hypothesis that impaired bacterial adherence could cause pathogen avoidance defects, we sought to determine the lawn avoidance phenotype of a C. elegans Bus mutant defective in M. nematophilum adherence. bus-1 encodes an integral membrane O-acyltransferase which is expressed in C. elegans neurons and rectal epithelial cells and required for M. nematophilum adherence to the rectal opening [35]. M. nematophilum cannot be observed adhering to the rectal opening of bus-1(e2678) mutants, and these animals fail to become Dar following infection [35]. bus-1(e2678) animals failed to avoid bacterial lawns contaminated with M. nematophilum (figure 5a,b). By contrast, we observed a wild-type avoidance response to S. marcescens in bus-1(e2678) animals (electronic supplementary material, figure S2A), suggesting that the role for bus-1 in avoidance is pathogen-specific and that bus-1 does not have a general role in avoiding noxious stimuli. This is consistent with the observation that survival of bus-1(e2678) animals infected with S. marcescens was not significantly different from wild-type survival (electronic supplementary material, figure S2B).

Figure 5.

Figure 5.

Efficient avoidance of pathogen-contaminated lawns requires pathogen attachment to rectal epithelial cells. (a) Pathogen avoidance index is decreased in bus-1(e2678) and lin-45(sy96) animals, but not in mpk-1(ku1) animals. (b) The percentage of bus-1(e2678) animals avoiding M. nematophilum-contaminated bacterial lawns is significantly decreased when compared with wild type. The bus-1(e2678) avoidance defect can be rescued by expression of BUS-1 cDNA in rectal epithelial cells (egl-5p∷BUS-1;bus-1(e2678)) (a and b). (c,d) Expression of BUS-1 cDNA in the rectal epithelial cells of bus-1(e2678) using a fragment of the egl-5 promoter (egl-5p∷BUS-1;bus-1(e2678)) rescues the Bus phenotype of bus-1(e2678) animals. (c) SYTO13-labelled M. nematophilum can be observed attaching to the rectal opening of transgenic (labelled with red asterisk), but not bus-1(e2678) animals. Rectal openings are indicated with black arrowheads. * indicates significance relative to wild type. # indicates significance relative to bus-1(e2678) (*p ≤ 0.05, ****p ≤ 0.0001. See Material and methods for details of statistical analysis).

BUS-1 is expressed in neurons in the head and tail and in rectal epithelial cells [35]. Although cell-specific rescue of bus-1(e2678) has not been reported, the observations that the C. briggsae bus-1 gene is also expressed in the vulva and M. nematophilum can be observed adhering to the vulva of C. briggssae but not C. elegans [35] suggest that BUS-1 expression in the rectal epithelium is required for bacterial adherence. To determine where BUS-1 expression in the rectal epithelium was required to promote pathogen avoidance, we attempted to rescue the pathogen avoidance defect of bus-1(e2678) by expressing the BUS-1 cDNA under the control of a fragment of the egl-5 promoter that drives expression in K, F, U, B and P12.pa rectal epithelial cells as well as three posterior body wall muscles [26]. bus-1(e2678) animals expressing the BUS-1cDNA from the egl-5 promoter were Dar, and SYTO13-labelled M. nematophilum could be observed adhering to the rectal opening (figure 5c,d), confirming that expression in the rectal epithelium was sufficient to rescue bacterial adherence in bus-1(e2678). This rescuing transgene was also sufficient to rescue the bus-1(e2678) lawn avoidance defect (figure 5a,b), indicating that impaired bacterial adherence in these animals contributes to pathogen avoidance.

The Dar phenotype requires ERK/MAPK signalling in the rectal epithelium [21,22]. This signalling pathway acts downstream of bacterial adherence because lin-45(sy96) and mpk-1(ku1) animals lacking components of the ERK/MAPK pathway fail to become Dar despite M. nematophilum attaching to the rectal opening [22]. To determine whether bacterial adherence, the Dar phenotype or both was required for wild-type levels of pathogen avoidance, we performed lawn avoidance assays using lin-45(sy96) and mpk-1(ku1). Both lin-45(sy96) and mpk-1(ku1) animals grow poorly on M. nematophilum, making it difficult to score pathogen avoidance in these strains; however, we observed a small decrease in the pathogen avoidance index in the lin-45(sy96) animals when compared with wild type (figure 5a). The pathogen avoidance index decrease observed in lin-45(sy96) animals was significantly weaker than that observed in the bus-1(e2678) animals and is most likely a consequence of increased basal avoidance in this strain (figure 5b), suggesting that although the Dar phenotype may play a small role in regulating pathogen avoidance, bacterial adherence is required for wild-type levels of pathogen avoidance.

4. Discussion

(a). Avoidance of contaminated food protects C. elegans from infection by reducing pathogen load

Understanding the importance of pathogen avoidance in protecting animals from infection in their natural environment is challenging and, consequently, much evidence for the role of pathogen avoidance in disease prevention is anecdotal. To assess the contribution of pathogen avoidance to disease prevention, controlled experimental models in which avoidance behaviour can be manipulated and the effect on infection outcome assessed are required. C. elegans is ideally suited to such investigation because avoidance can be prevented easily by spreading pathogenic bacteria, or bacteria contaminated with pathogens, over the entire experimental arena [9]. The effect of these manipulations can easily be assessed by measuring changes in pathogen load using labelled pathogens [22,36], expression of infection-regulated genes [30] or infection outcomes such as death [9] or delayed growth rate [31].

Here, we have established a model of contaminated food avoidance using C. elegans propagated on M. nematophilum-contaminated bacterial lawns. We find that, in agreement with previous studies [14], C. elegans avoids lawns contaminated with pathogenic M. nematophilum. Using a non-pathogenic form of M. nematophilum, we show that, although C. elegans is less attracted to M. nematophilum than E. coli OP50, avoidance of contaminated bacterial lawns is mediated by pathogenic elements of M. nematophilum and not by decreased food availability or poor food quality.

M. nematophilum establishes a persistent infection in C. elegans, which results in slowed growth [31]. We find that avoidance of M. nematophilum-contaminated bacterial lawns protects C. elegans from the deleterious effects of M. nematophilum infection because we observed that, under conditions where wild-type animals were unable to avoid pathogen-contaminated lawns, their growth was further slowed. This is consistent with previous results obtained using monoaxenic lawns of pathogenic P. aeruginosa where manipulating experimental conditions to prevent avoidance of P. aeruginosa increases C. elegans susceptibility to infection [9].

M. nematophilum specifically attaches to the cuticle around the rectal opening of C. elegans [22,31] and activates signalling in the rectal epithelial cells as part of a protective cellular immune response that includes changes in morphology of the rectal opening known as the Dar phenotype [31]. Our data suggest that avoidance protects animals by reducing pathogen load independently of the cellular immune response because animals that are unable to avoid pathogen-contaminated lawns retain more labelled pathogen, but do not observably alter their Dar phenotype. Previous studies have also demonstrated a separation of immune signalling and avoidance responses following cellular damage [37]. Further investigation to determine the role of other aspects of the cellular immune response, such as the upregulation of antimicrobial peptide expression, in the avoidance of M. nematophilum-contaminated lawns will be required in order to fully exclude a role for cellular immune responses in the regulation of this pathogen avoidance behaviour.

Our results establish C. elegans as a model that can be used to genetically dissect the molecular and cellular basis of behaviours that lead to the avoidance of contaminated food and to examine the impact of these behaviours on infection.

(b). Regulation of multiple sensory neurons is required for the avoidance of contaminated food

The cyclic nucleotide-gated channel encoded by tax-2 and tax-4 is essential for the function of many sensory neurons [32,33] and is required for the avoidance of S. marcescens [11] and M. nematophilum [14]. Here, we show that animals lacking tax-2 or tax-4 are defective in their ability to avoid M. nematophilum­-contaminated bacterial lawns, implicating sensory neuron signalling in the avoidance of contaminated food. We find that ASEL, AQR, PQR and URX sensory neurons act to promote the avoidance of contaminated bacterial lawns. This is in contrast to previous work by Yook & Hodgkin [14] who demonstrated that tax-2(p694) responded in a similar manner to wild-type animals in a bacterial choice assay. Since tax-2(p694) has been demonstrated to only affect TAX-2 function in a subset of TAX-2-expressing neurons, their data suggest that the TAX-2/4 channel is required in AWB, AWC, ASJ, ASI, ASG or ASK to mediate the choice between M. nematophilum and E. coli OP50 [14]. Further work is required to resolve this discrepancy which likely reflects the use of monoaxenic bacterial lawns [14] versus mixed bacterial lawns (this study).

Our results identify a novel role for ASEL in regulating pathogen avoidance; however, URX has previously been shown to regulate aversive olfactory learning of P. aeruginosa [38]. Interestingly, the neurons that we have identified are also required for the avoidance of other unfavourable environmental conditions [27,39]. Under conditions where food is depleted, ASE, AQR, PQR and URX promote food-leaving behaviour via TAX-2/4, while AFD suppresses the response [27]. Additional neurons, including ASI and ADF, are also required for this response [27], and the role of these neurons in the avoidance of contaminated bacterial lawns remains to be determined.

Although ablation of individual neurons is sufficient to supress pathogen avoidance, our rescue data suggest that efficient pathogen avoidance requires the integration of sensory inputs from multiple neurons because expression of the TAX-2 cDNA in multiple neurons is required to rescue tax-2(p694). The requirement for multiple sensory neurons suggests that C. elegans is detecting multiple cues in order to avoid contaminated bacterial lawns. This may include attractive chemosensory cues provided by the E. coli to promote lawn retention and feeding as well as repulsive cues from the pathogenic M. nematophilum. These microbial cues remain to be identified however, based on the requirement for TAX-2/4 in this response, it is likely that at least some of these cues will involve G-protein signalling. Aerotaxis has been implicated in the regulation of pathogen avoidance with chemosensation of P. aeruginosa secondary metabolites by the sensory neuron ASJ altering C. elegans aerotaxis behaviour to promote pathogen avoidance [19]. Further experiments are required to elucidate the role of aerotaxis in our model; however, our current study raises the possibility that aerotaxis has a role in regulating the avoidance of M. nematophilum-contaminated bacterial lawns because AQR, PQR and URX are activated by high ambient O2 [40,41] to promote adaptive food-leaving behaviour [27] while AFD, ASE and BAG act as a CO2 sensors to mediate CO2 avoidance [39].

(c). Avoidance of contaminated food requires neuronal and non-neuronal signals

Pathogen attachment to the rectal opening requires expression of the integral O-membrane-linked acyltransferase BUS-1 in rectal epithelial cells. In bus-1(e2678) animals, M. nematophilum fails to attach and, as a consequence, these animals do not develop the Dar phenotype [35]. Here, we show that BUS-1 is required in the rectal epithelium for wild-type levels of avoidance, revealing a novel role for the rectal epithelium in regulating behaviour and indicating that pathogen attachment is required for both behavioural and cellular responses to infection.

How does bacterial adherence to non-neuronal cells alter behaviour? Although a link between bacterial adherence and regulation of neuronal activity has not previously been demonstrated, the kinetics of aversive learning in avoidance of P. aeruginosa suggests that ingestion of the bacteria and subsequent cellular damage are required for lawn avoidance [7]. Furthermore, there is evidence that cellular damage to non-neuronal cells, such as that caused by ingestion of pathogenic bacteria, can modify behaviour via neuroendocrine signalling pathways [37]. Using non-pathogenic bacteria expressing RNAi to knockdown essential genes Melo & Ruvkun [37] demonstrated that disrupting core cellular activities in non-neuronal cells could stimulate avoidance behaviours. This lawn avoidance behaviour requires signalling by the stress-activated JNK–MAPK pathway but not signalling via immune response pathways that are activated by infection with P. aeruginosa [37]. Adherence of M. nematophilum to the rectal opening of C. elegans has not yet been reported to cause cellular damage or inactivate essential cellular pathways in the manner described by Melo & Ruvkun [37], and the JNK–MAPK pathway has not been implicated in the host response to M. nematophilum infection. However, consistent with the previous observation that immune response pathways are not required for avoidance [37], the ERK–MAPK pathway, which is required for activation of the cellular immune response to M. nematophilum, does not appear to be required for avoidance of M. nematophilum-contaminated lawns. Further work to elucidate the cellular response to M. nematophilum adherence will be required in order to understand the neuroendocrine signals that link bacterial adherence with changes in behaviour.

Our study also raises the possibility that impaired bacterial adherence contributes to the pathogen avoidance phenotype observed in tax-2 and tax-4 mutants. tax-2(p694) animals have a wild-type Dar response [14]; however, we have shown that these animals are defective in pathogen avoidance, suggesting that impaired bacterial adherence does not contribute to the observed pathogen avoidance defect in this mutant. However, bacterial adherence is impaired in tax-2(p671) and tax-4(p678) animals [14], and the contribution of impaired bacterial adherence to the pathogen avoidance defect in these animals remains to be established.

5. Conclusion

One way in which animals minimize the risk of infection is to reduce their contact with contaminated food. To do this, they must detect the presence of pathogen contamination and integrate this information with attractive food cues and information about their internal nutritional status to elicit an appropriate behavioural response. Here, we have established a model of contaminated food avoidance using the invertebrate model C. elegans. The genetic tractability of this model provides a unique opportunity to dissect these behaviours at molecular and cellular levels in order to gain basic insights into how animals avoid contaminated food. Given the evolutionary importance of avoiding contaminated food, we anticipate that at least some of these mechanisms will be conserved.

Supplementary Material

Supporting Data
rstb20170255supp1.xlsx (82.2KB, xlsx)

Supplementary Material

Supplemental figure 1
rstb20170255supp2.eps (796.5KB, eps)

Supplementary Material

Supplemental figure 2
rstb20170255supp3.eps (1.1MB, eps)

Supplementary Material

Supplemental Table 1
rstb20170255supp4.docx (84.4KB, docx)

Acknowledgements

We thank Arantza Barrios (University College London), Valerie Curtis (London School of Hygiene and Tropical Medicine), Mario de Bono (MRC Laboratory for Molecular Biology, Cambridge) and Jonathan Hodgkin (University of Oxford) for strains, advice and technical support. Some strains were provided by the Caenorhabditis Genetics Center (University of Minnesota), which is funded by NIH Office of Research Infrastructure Programs (P40 OD010440).

Data accessibility

The datasets supporting this article have been uploaded as part of the electronic supplementary material.

Authors' contributions

A.A. and R.M. designed the study, collected and analysed the data, and wrote the manuscript.

Competing interests

We declare we have no competing interests.

Funding

This work was supported by the Wellcome Trust Career Development Fellowship to R.M. (grant no. WT088409AIA).

References

  • 1.Darby C. 2005. Interactions with microbial pathogens. WormBook 1–16. ( 10.1895/wormbook.1.21.1) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Couillault C, Ewbank JJ. 2002. Diverse bacteria are pathogens of Caenorhabditis elegans. Infect. Immun. 70, 4705–4707. ( 10.1128/IAI.70.8.4705-4707.2002) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Ewbank JJ. 2006. Signaling in the immune response. WormBook 1–12. ( 10.1895/wormbook.1.83.1) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Meisel JD, Kim DH. 2014. Behavioral avoidance of pathogenic bacteria by Caenorhabditis elegans. Trends Immunol. 35, 465–470. ( 10.1016/j.it.2014.08.008) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.de Bono M, Maricq AV. 2005. Neuronal substrates of complex behaviors in C. elegans. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 28, 451–501. ( 10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144259) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Harris G, Shen Y, Ha H, Donato A, Wallis S, Zhang X, Zhang Y. 2014. Dissecting the signaling mechanisms underlying recognition and preference of food odors. J. Neurosci. 34, 9389–9403. ( 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0012-14.2014) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Zhang Y, Lu H, Bargmann CI. 2005. Pathogenic bacteria induce aversive olfactory learning in Caenorhabditis elegans. Nature 438, 179–184. ( 10.1038/nature04216) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Chang HC, Paek J, Kim DH. 2011. Natural polymorphisms in C. elegans HECW-1 E3 ligase affect pathogen avoidance behaviour. Nature 480, 525–529. ( 10.1038/nature10643) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Reddy KC, Andersen EC, Kruglyak L, Kim DH. 2009. A polymorphism in npr-1 is a behavioral determinant of pathogen susceptibility in C. elegans. Science 323, 382–384. ( 10.1126/science.1166527) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Pujol N, et al. 2001. A reverse genetic analysis of components of the Toll signaling pathway in Caenorhabditis elegans. Curr. Biol. 11, 809–821. ( 10.1016/S0960-9822(01)00241-X) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Pradel E, Zhang Y, Pujol N, Matsuyama T, Bargmann CI, Bargmann C, Ewbank JJ, Ewbank J. 2007. Detection and avoidance of a natural product from the pathogenic bacterium Serratia marcescens by Caenorhabditis elegans. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 104, 2295–2300. ( 10.1073/pnas.0610281104) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Hasshoff M, Bohnisch C, Tonn D, Hasert B, Schulenburg H. 2007. The role of Caenorhabditis elegans insulin-like signaling in the behavioral avoidance of pathogenic Bacillus thuringiensis. FASEB J. 21, 1801–1812. ( 10.1096/fj.06-6551com) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Tran A, Tang A, O'Loughlin CT, Balistreri A, Chang E. 2017. C. elegans avoids toxin-producing Streptomyces using a seven transmembrane domain chemosensory receptor. eLife 6, e23770 ( 10.7554/eLife.23770) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Yook K, Hodgkin J. 2007. Mos1 mutagenesis reveals a diversity of mechanisms affecting response of Caenorhabditis elegans to the bacterial pathogen Microbacterium nematophilum. Genetics 175, 681–697. ( 10.1534/genetics.106.060087) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Anderson A, Laurenson-Schafer H, Partridge FA, Hodgkin J, McMullan R. 2013. Serotonergic chemosensory neurons modify the C. elegans immune response by regulating G-protein signaling in epithelial cells. PLoS Pathog. 9, e1003787 ( 10.1371/journal.ppat.1003787) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Shivers RP, Kooistra T, Chu SW, Pagano DJ, Kim DH. 2009. Tissue-specific activities of an immune signaling module regulate physiological responses to pathogenic and nutritional bacteria in C. elegans. Cell Host Microbe 6, 321–330. ( 10.1016/j.chom.2009.09.001) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Luo H, Xiong J, Zhou Q, Xia L, Yu Z. 2013. The effects of Bacillus thuringiensis Cry6A on the survival, growth, reproduction, locomotion, and behavioral response of Caenorhabditis elegans. Appl. Microbiol. Biotechnol. 97, 10 135–10 142. ( 10.1007/s00253-013-5249-3) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Nakad R, et al. 2016. Contrasting invertebrate immune defense behaviors caused by a single gene, the Caenorhabditis elegans neuropeptide receptor gene npr-1. BMC Genomics 17, 280 ( 10.1186/s12864-016-2603-8) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Meisel JD, Panda O, Mahanti P, Schroeder FC, Kim DH. 2014. Chemosensation of bacterial secondary metabolites modulates neuroendocrine signaling and behavior of C. elegans. Cell 159, 267–280. ( 10.1016/j.cell.2014.09.011) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Brenner S. 1974. The genetics of Caenorhabditis elegans. Genetics 77, 71–94. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.McMullan R, Anderson A, Nurrish S. 2012. Behavioral and immune responses to infection require Gαq-RhoA signaling in C. elegans. PLoS Pathog. 8, e1002530 ( 10.1371/journal.ppat.1002530) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Nicholas HR, Hodgkin J. 2004. The ERK MAP kinase cascade mediates tail swelling and a protective response to rectal infection in C. elegans. Curr. Biol. 14, 1256–1261. ( 10.1016/j.cub.2004.07.022) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Lipton J, Kleemann G, Ghosh R, Lints R, Emmons SW. 2004. Mate searching in Caenorhabditis elegans: a genetic model for sex drive in a simple invertebrate. J. Neurosci. 24, 7427–7434. ( 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1746-04.2004) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Stiernagle T. 2006. Maintenance of C. elegans. WormBook 1–11. ( 10.1895/wormbook.1.101.1) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.MacNeil LT, Watson E, Arda HE, Zhu LJ, Walhout AJM. 2013. Diet-induced developmental acceleration independent of TOR and insulin in C. elegans. Cell 153, 240–252. ( 10.1016/j.cell.2013.02.049) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Teng Y, Girard L, Ferreira HB, Sternberg PW, Emmons SW. 2004. Dissection of cis-regulatory elements in the C. elegans Hox gene egl-5 promoter. Dev. Biol. 276, 476–492. ( 10.1016/j.ydbio.2004.09.012) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Milward K, Busch KE, Murphy RJ, de Bono M, Olofsson B. 2011. Neuronal and molecular substrates for optimal foraging in Caenorhabditis elegans. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 108, 20 672–20 677. ( 10.1073/pnas.1106134109) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Shtonda BB, Avery L. 2006. Dietary choice behavior in Caenorhabditis elegans. J. Exp. Biol. 209, 89–102. ( 10.1242/jeb.01955) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Akimkina T, Yook K, Curnock S, Hodgkin J. 2006. Genome characterization, analysis of virulence and transformation of Microbacterium nematophilum, a coryneform pathogen of the nematode Caenorhabditis elegans. FEMS Microbiol. Lett. 264, 145–151. ( 10.1111/j.1574-6968.2006.00469.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.O'Rourke D, Baban D, Demidova M, Mott R, Hodgkin J. 2006. Genomic clusters, putative pathogen recognition molecules, and antimicrobial genes are induced by infection of C. elegans with M. nematophilum. Genome Res. 16, 1005–1016. ( 10.1101/gr.50823006) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Hodgkin J, Kuwabara PE, Corneliussen B. 2000. A novel bacterial pathogen, Microbacterium nematophilum, induces morphological change in the nematode C. elegans. Curr. Biol. 10, 1615–1618. ( 10.1016/S0960-9822(00)00867-8) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Coburn CM, Bargmann CI. 1996. A putative cyclic nucleotide-gated channel is required for sensory development and function in C. elegans. Neuron 17, 695–706. ( 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)80201-9) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Komatsu H, Mori I, Rhee JS, Akaike N, Ohshima Y. 1996. Mutations in a cyclic nucleotide-gated channel lead to abnormal thermosensation and chemosensation in C. elegans. Neuron 17, 707–718. ( 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)80202-0) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Horspool AM, Chang HC. 2017. Superoxide dismutase SOD-1 modulates C. elegans pathogen avoidance behavior. Sci. Rep. 7, 45128 ( 10.1038/srep45128) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Gravato-Nobre MJ, Hodgkin J. 2008. The acyltransferase gene bus-1 exhibits conserved and specific expression in nematode rectal cells and reveals pathogen-induced cell swelling. Dev. Dyn. 237, 3762–3776. ( 10.1002/dvdy.21792) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Aballay A, Yorgey P, Ausubel FM. 2000. Salmonella typhimurium proliferates and establishes a persistent infection in the intestine of Caenorhabditis elegans. Curr. Biol. 10, 1539–1542. ( 10.1016/S0960-9822(00)00830-7) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Melo JA, Ruvkun G. 2012. Inactivation of conserved C. elegans genes engages pathogen- and xenobiotic-associated defenses. Cell 149, 452–466. ( 10.1016/j.cell.2012.02.050) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Chen Z, Hendricks M, Cornils A, Maier W, Alcedo J, Zhang Y. 2013. Two insulin-like peptides antagonistically regulate aversive olfactory learning in C. elegans. Neuron 77, 572–585. ( 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.11.025) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Bretscher AJ, Kodama-Namba E, Busch KE, Murphy RJ, Soltesz Z, Laurent P, de Bono M. 2011. Temperature, oxygen and salt sensing neurons in C. elegans are carbon dioxide sensors that control avoidance behavior. Neuron 69, 15 ( 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.02.023) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Zimmer M, et al. 2009. Neurons detect increases and decreases in oxygen levels using distinct guanylate cyclases. Neuron 61, 865–879. ( 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.02.013) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Persson A, Gross E, Laurent P, Busch KE, Bretes H, de Bono M. 2009. Natural variation in a neural globin tunes oxygen sensing in wild Caenorhabditis elegans. Nature 458, 1030–1033. ( 10.1038/nature07820) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supporting Data
rstb20170255supp1.xlsx (82.2KB, xlsx)
Supplemental figure 1
rstb20170255supp2.eps (796.5KB, eps)
Supplemental figure 2
rstb20170255supp3.eps (1.1MB, eps)
Supplemental Table 1
rstb20170255supp4.docx (84.4KB, docx)

Data Availability Statement

The datasets supporting this article have been uploaded as part of the electronic supplementary material.


Articles from Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences are provided here courtesy of The Royal Society

RESOURCES