Skip to main content
The Journal of Physiology logoLink to The Journal of Physiology
. 2018 May 17;596(12):2447–2461. doi: 10.1113/JP275799

Synaptic depression induced by postsynaptic cAMP production in the Drosophila mushroom body calyx

Shoma Sato 1, Kohei Ueno 2, Minoru Saitoe 2, Takaomi Sakai 1,
PMCID: PMC6002214  PMID: 29659025

Abstract

Key points

  • Synaptic potentiation in Drosophila is observed at cholinergic synapses between antennal lobe (AL) and mushroom body (MB) neurons in the adult brain; however, depression at the AL–MB synapses has not yet been identified.

  • By ex vivo Ca2+ imaging in an isolated cultured Drosophila brain, we found novel activity‐dependent depression at the AL–MB synapses.

  • The degree of Ca2+ responses after repetitive AL stimulation is significantly reduced in the dendritic region of MB neurons (calyx) compared with those before AL stimulation, and this reduction of Ca2+ responses remains for at least 30 min.

  • The expression of rutabaga, which encodes Ca2+/calmodulin‐dependent adenylyl cyclase, is essential in the MB neurons for the reduction of Ca2+ responses in the calyx.

  • Our study reveals that elevation of cAMP production in the calyx during repetitive AL stimulation induces the depression at the AL–MB synapses.

Abstract

Synaptic plasticity has been studied to reveal the molecular and cellular mechanisms of associative and non‐associative learning. The fruit fly Drosophila melanogaster can be used to identify the molecular mechanisms of synaptic plasticity because vast genetic information or tools are available. Here, by ex vivo Ca2+ imaging of an isolated cultured Drosophila brain, we examined the novel activity‐dependent synaptic depression between the projection neurons of the antennal lobe (AL) and mushroom body (MB). Ex vivo Ca2+ imaging analysis revealed that electrical stimulation of AL elicits Ca2+ responses in the dendritic (calyx) and axonal (α lobe) regions of MB neurons, and the responses are reduced after repetitive AL stimulation. Since the cAMP signalling pathway plays an important role in synaptic plasticity in invertebrates and vertebrates, we examined whether the reduction of Ca2+ responses is also regulated by the cAMP signalling pathway. The expression of rutabaga (rut), which encodes Ca2+/calmodulin‐dependent adenylyl cyclase, was essential for the reduction of Ca2+ responses in the calyx and α lobe. Furthermore, imaging analysis using a fluorescence resonance energy transfer‐based cAMP indicator revealed that the cAMP level increased in the wild‐type calyx during repetitive AL stimulation, whereas it decreased in rut1mutant flies with a loss‐of‐function mutation of rut. Thus, our study suggests that an increase in postsynaptic cAMP level during repetitive AL stimulation contributes to the attenuation of inputs at AL–MB synapses.

Keywords: synaptic depression, cAMP, Drosophila, mushroom body

Key points

  • Synaptic potentiation in Drosophila is observed at cholinergic synapses between antennal lobe (AL) and mushroom body (MB) neurons in the adult brain; however, depression at the AL–MB synapses has not yet been identified.

  • By ex vivo Ca2+ imaging in an isolated cultured Drosophila brain, we found novel activity‐dependent depression at the AL–MB synapses.

  • The degree of Ca2+ responses after repetitive AL stimulation is significantly reduced in the dendritic region of MB neurons (calyx) compared with those before AL stimulation, and this reduction of Ca2+ responses remains for at least 30 min.

  • The expression of rutabaga, which encodes Ca2+/calmodulin‐dependent adenylyl cyclase, is essential in the MB neurons for the reduction of Ca2+ responses in the calyx.

  • Our study reveals that elevation of cAMP production in the calyx during repetitive AL stimulation induces the depression at the AL–MB synapses.

Introduction

In invertebrates and vertebrates, synaptic plasticity has been extensively studied to reveal the possible molecular and cellular mechanisms of memory formation (Kandel, 2001; Ho et al. 2011). The repetitive stimulation of single‐target neurons or the paired associative stimulation of multiple‐target neurons can enhance or weaken synaptic efficacy leading to long‐term potentiation (LTP) and long‐term depression (LTD) (Collingridge et al. 2010; Nicoll, 2017). It has been considered that LTP and LTD underlie some forms of memory in Aplysia or mice (Hawkins et al. 2006; Glanzman, 2009; Korte & Schmitz, 2016). The cAMP signalling pathway plays an important role in memory formation and synaptic plasticity, in invertebrates and vertebrates (Abel & Nguyen, 2008; Kandel, 2012). Thus, the significance of cAMP signalling in behavioural and synaptic plasticities may be evolutionarily conserved across species.

The fruit fly Drosophila melanogaster has been used as a model to identify the molecular mechanisms of synaptic plasticity because vast genetic information or tools are available. The Drosophila mushroom bodies (MBs) are critical brain structures for associative and non‐associative olfactory memory (Heisenberg, 2003; Cho et al. 2004). The dendritic region of MB neurons (calyx) receives olfactory information through cholinergic inputs from the projection neurons (PNs) of the antennal lobe (AL) (Vosshall & Stocker, 2007), where each MB neuron extends to three types of axonal lobe (α/β, α′/β′ and γ lobes) (Crittenden et al. 1998), and MB axons innervate individual MB output neurons (MBONs) (Aso et al. 2014). Some genes associated with the cAMP signalling pathway are expressed in MB neurons (e.g. rutabaga (rut), encoding an adenylyl cyclase (AC), and dunce (dnc), encoding a phosphodiesterase) (Davis, 2005). Thus, it is possible that the cAMP signalling pathway is also involved in the regulation of the synaptic plasticity of MB neurons.

In Drosophila, in vivo Ca2+ imaging of brain activity revealed that odour‐evoked Ca2+ responses in MB lobes were enhanced after association between the odour and an electrical shock (Davis, 2011). Furthermore, ex vivo Ca2+ imaging using an isolated cultured Drosophila brain is also a powerful tool to identify physiological properties related to synaptic plasticity in specific brain neurons (Wang et al. 2008; Ueno et al. 2013; Cohn et al. 2015). Similar to mammalian LTP, synaptic potentiation in Drosophila, also known as long‐term enhancement (LTE), is observed in synapses between AL and MB neurons. Ca2+ responses in the MB neurons induced by AL stimulation are enhanced after the simultaneous associative stimulation of the AL and the ascending fibres of the ventral nerve cord (AFV) in an isolated cultured Drosophila brain (Ueno et al. 2013). In contrast to LTE in the fly brain, at the Drosophila larval neuromuscular junction, synaptic depression can be induced by specific patterns of tetanic stimulation in a cAMP‐independent manner (Guo & Zhong, 2006). In addition, in the fly brain, pairing an odour with the activation of specific dopaminergic neurons induces the synaptic depression of MBON inputs (Hige et al. 2015), although the molecular mechanisms regulating this synaptic depression remain unclarified.

In this study, by non‐associative repetitive stimulation, we examined the novel synaptic depression in an isolated cultured Drosophila brain. We identified that Ca2+ responses in the MB calyx and α lobe induced by AL stimulation are reduced after repetitive AL stimulation. The reduction of Ca2+ responses remained for at least 30 min after the repetitive AL stimulation. In addition, we examined whether the presynaptic or postsynaptic cAMP signalling modulates the reduction of Ca2+ responses after the repetitive AL stimulation.

Methods

Fly stocks

Fly stocks used for this study are as follows: rut1, dnc1, GH146 (Bloomington Drosophila Stock Center (BS), Bloomington, IN, USA, no. 30026), MB‐GAL4 lines (MB247, c305a, c772, R13F02‐GAL4 (BS, no. 48571) and 30Y), MB‐LexA (II), MB‐LexA (III) (Ueno et al. 2013), UAS‐GCaMP3 (BS, no. 32236), UAS‐GCaMP6m (BS, no. 42748), UAS‐Epac1‐camps (BS, no. 25408), UAS‐rut (BS, no. 9405), LexAop‐GCaMP6f (BS, no. 44277) and LexAop‐GCaMP6m (BS, no. 44275). Flies were raised on glucose–yeast–cornmeal medium at 25.0 ± 0.5°C in a 12 h light:12 h dark cycle. We used 1‐ to 2‐day‐old males in all experiments.

Brain preparation

Brains were prepared for imaging analysis as previously described with minor modifications (Ueno et al. 2013, 2017; Naganos et al. 2016). Briefly, brains were dissected in 0 mm Ca2+ HL3 medium (in mm, NaCl, 70; sucrose, 115; KCl, 5; MgCl2, 20; NaHCO3, 10; trehalose, 5; Hepes, 5; pH 7.3) (Stewart et al. 1994). As previously described, the isolated brains were immobilized by placing their optic lobes between two nylon fibre bundles attached to a platinum grid, and were placed in a recording chamber filled with normal HL3 medium (in mm: NaCl, 70; sucrose, 115; KCl, 5; MgCl2, 20; CaCl2, 1.8; NaHCO3, 10; trehalose, 5; Hepes, 5; pH 7.3). During the experiments, fresh HL3 medium was infused into the chamber using a peristaltic pump (1–2 ml min−1, AC‐2110, ATTO Corporation, Tokyo, Japan) or a Pasteur pipette every 5–10 min.

Electrical stimulation

The AL was electrically stimulated using glass microelectrodes with a stimulator (SEN‐8203 and SEN‐7103, Nihon Kohden, Tokyo, Japan) and an isolator (SS‐203J and SS‐202J, Nihon Kohden). To record Ca2+ responses in the PNs or the MB calyx or lobes, we stimulated the AL with three trains of 30 pulses (100 Hz, 1.0 ms pulse duration, intensity 1–2 times threshold current (0.08–0.12 mA)) at intertrain intervals of 10 s. To apply repetitive AL stimulation, we stimulated the AL with 10–100 trains of 30 pulses (100 Hz, 1.0 ms pulse duration) at intertrain intervals of 1.0 s. The AL was stimulated sequentially before and after repetitive AL stimulation every 3 or 5 min.

Ca2+ imaging

For Ca2+ imaging using GCaMP3 or GCaMP6m, fluorescence images were captured at 30–32 Hz using a fluorescence microscope (ECLIPSE FN1, Nikon Corp., Tokyo, Japan) equipped with a 20× water‐immersion objective (NA, 0.5; Nikon Corp.), and all recordings were captured using an electron multiplier CCD camera (QuantEM:512SC, Photometrics, Tucson, AZ, USA). GCaMP3 or GCaMP6m were excited at 455–485 nm and detected using a 500–545 nm band‐pass filter. The intensity of fluorescence (F) values were calculated for all pixels in the region of interest using NIS‐Elements AR software (Nikon Corp.). We calculated the initial F value (F 0) by averaging the F values recorded from 10 sequential frames before stimulation. To obtain ΔF/F 0 (%) as Ca2+ responses, we calculated (– F 0)/F 0 × 100 in each time point. To measure AL stimulation‐induced Ca2+ responses (ΔF/F 0 ave), we calculated the average ΔF/F 0 change from three AL stimulations. To calculate relative Ca2+ responses, ΔF/F 0 ave in each time point was divided by ΔF/F 0 ave at the first time point.

cAMP imaging

Epac1‐camps was used for cAMP imaging. For the measurement of cAMP responses during repetitive AL stimulation, fluorescence images were captured at 1 Hz using a confocal microscope system (A1R, Nikon Corp.) equipped with a 20× water‐immersion objective (NA, 0.5; Nikon Corp.). Cyan fluorescent protein (CFP) was excited at 458 nm and detected using a 482 ± 17.5 nm band‐pass filter, and yellow fluorescent protein (YFP) was detected simultaneously using a 540 ± 15 nm band‐pass filter. We calculated fluorescence resonance energy transfer (FRET) changes as R using the following formula: R = F of CFP/F of YFP. We calculated the initial R (R 0) by averaging the R values recorded from 10 sequential frames before stimulation. To obtain ΔR/R 0 (%) as cAMP responses, we calculated (– R 0)/R 0 × 100 in each time point.

Pharmacology

To block the GABAA receptor or dopamine receptor, dissected brains were incubated in HL3 medium containing the blocker of each receptor (picrotoxin or butaclamol) or DMSO for 10 min before the start of recording. During the experiments, fresh HL3 medium containing the drug was infused every 5–10 min. To stimulate AC during repetitive AL stimulation, brains were incubated in HL3 medium containing 10 μm forskolin or DMSO for 10 min before repetitive AL stimulation. After the end of repetitive AL stimulation, 10 μm forskolin or DMSO was washed out.

Chemicals

Picrotoxin (cat. no. P1675), butaclamol ((+)‐butaclamol hydrochloride; cat. no. D033) and forskolin (cat. no. F6886) were purchased from Sigma‐Aldrich (St Louis, MO, USA). Picrotoxin, butaclamol and forskolin were dissolved in DMSO.

Statistical analysis

The Kolmogorov–Smirnov test was used to estimate whether the data were normally distributed. When the data were not distributed normally, we carried out the log transformation of the data. When the basic data or transformed data were normally distributed, Student's t test was used for pairwise comparison, and one‐way ANOVA was carried out for multiple pairwise comparisons. When the transformed data were not distributed normally, the Mann–Whitney U test was used for pairwise comparisons, and non‐parametric ANOVA (Kruskal–Wallis test) was used for multiple comparisons. The computer software IBM SPSS statistics 22 (IBM Japan, Ltd, Tokyo, Japan) or BellCurve for Excel (Social Survey Research Information Co., Ltd, Tokyo, Japan) was used for these tests.

Results

Repetitive AL stimulation reduces Ca2+ responses in MB neurons

To measure Ca2+ responses in adult MB neurons, we used the green fluorescent protein‐based calcium indicator GCaMP3 (Tian et al. 2009). The GAL4/UAS binary gene expression system (Brand & Perrimon, 1993) was used for the targeted expression of GCaMP3 in the MB neurons (Fig. 1 A). In an isolated cultured fly brain, electrical AL stimulation elicits robust Ca2+ responses in the tip of the MB α lobe via synaptic transmissions between PNs and MB neurons (Ueno et al. 2013). First, we measured AL‐stimulation‐induced Ca2+ responses in the α lobe tip after several stimulus trains, and examined whether repetitive AL stimulation induces synaptic plasticity (Fig. 1 A). When the repetitive AL stimulation was less than 30 stimulus trains, the relative Ca2+ responses did not change or return to the baseline level immediately after the AL stimulation (Fig. 1 B). However, when repetitive AL stimulation was more than 40 stimulus trains, the relative Ca2+ responses after the AL stimulation were significantly reduced in comparison with those before the AL stimulation, and the degree of the reduction of Ca2+ responses was dependent on the number of stimulus trains (Fig. 1 B). In addition, the significant reduction of Ca2+ responses was consistently maintained for at least 30 min after AL stimulation with 40 stimulus trains (Fig. 1 C). Since the AL stimulation‐induced Ca2+ responses in horizontal lobes (β, β′ and γ) were extremely weak and infrequent, as previously reported (Ueno et al. 2013), we next examined whether the reduction of Ca2+ responses is detected in the α′ lobe. In contrast to the α lobe tip, we did not detect significant changes in the α′ lobe tip after repetitive AL stimulation with 40 trains (Fig. 1 D). Thus, in all subsequent experiments, we applied AL stimulation with 40 stimulus trains to induce the reduction of MB Ca2+ responses.

Figure 1. Repetitive AL stimulation induces the reduction of Ca2+ responses in the MB α lobe.

Figure 1

A–C and E, +; MB247 UAS‐GCaMP3/+ males were used in the experiments. A, the left image shows a brain preparation for experiments. A schematic diagram of the stimulation protocol is shown on the right. A glass microelectrode was attached onto the AL. AL was stimulated at 3 min intervals. B, relative Ca2+ responses before and after repetitive AL stimulation. n = 6–8 brains in each train. The upper panels show typical Ca2+ responses in the α lobe tip 3 min before (Pre), during and 9 min after (Test) repetitive AL stimulation. Number of trains in repetitive AL stimulation were 30 (left panels) and 40 (right panels). Pseudo‐color represents the intensity of GCaMP3 fluorescence. The area indicated by dotted lines is the α lobe tip. C, Ca2+ responses before and after application of 40 stimulus trains in the α lobe tip. The upper panel shows a schematic diagram of the stimulation protocol. AL stimulation was applied sequentially every 3 min before the application of 40 stimulus trains. Subsequently, AL stimulation was applied every 5 min after the application of 40 stimulus trains. The middle panel shows typical Ca2+ responses in the α lobe tip 3 min before (Pre), during and 10 min after the application of 40 stimulus trains. Asterisks indicate the statistical significance of difference at each time point between before and after the application of 40 stimulus trains. The Mann–Whitney U test was used for statistical analysis. Error bars represent SEM. ** P < 0.01; n = 7. D, c305a/+; UAS‐GCaMP3/+ males were used. Ca2+ responses before and after application of 40 stimulus trains in the α′ lobe tip. The upper panel shows a schematic diagram of the stimulation protocol. The middle panel shows typical Ca2+ responses in the α′ lobe tip 3 min before (Pre), during and 10 min after the application of 40 stimulus trains. No significant difference was detected at each time point between before and after the application of 40 stimulus trains. Student's t test or the Mann–Whitney U test was used for statistical analysis. Error bars represent SEM. NS, not significant; n = 7. E, GABAA receptor inhibition does not impair the reduction of Ca2+ responses. The upper panel shows a schematic diagram of the stimulation protocol. The middle panel shows typical Ca2+ responses in the α lobe tip 3 min before (Pre), during and 10 min after the application of 40 stimulus trains. The reduction of Ca2+ responses was enhanced by the treatment of the GABAA receptor blocker picrotoxin (blue line; n = 6) compared with that of DMSO as the control (black line; n = 6). Asterisks indicate the statistical significance of the difference between before and after the application of 40 stimulus trains. Hash symbols indicate the statistical significance of difference at each time point between picrotoxin treatment and DMSO control. Student's t test or the Mann–Whitney U test was used for statistical analysis. Error bars represent SEM. ** P < 0.01; *** P < 0.001; # P < 0.05; ## P < 0.01; ### P < 0.001. [Color figure can be viewed at http://wileyonlinelibrary.com]

Resistant to dieldrin, which encodes a GABAA receptor, is expressed in MB neurons (Harrison et al. 1996). When PNs are activated, MB neurons receive cholinergic inputs from PNs and GABAergic inputs from anterior paired lateral neurons (Lin et al. 2014). Thus, we next determined whether the AL stimulation‐induced reduction of Ca2+ responses results from the enhancement of inhibitory transmission via GABAA receptors on MB neurons. The GABAA receptor blocker picrotoxin was used in the experiments (Su & O'Dowd, 2003). It has been reported that picrotoxin treatment acutely enhances the AL stimulation‐induced Ca2+ responses (Ueno et al. 2013). However, far from stopping the reduction of Ca2+ responses in the MB neurons, the reduction of Ca2+ responses was promoted during picrotoxin treatment (Fig. 1 E). Thus, it is unlikely that the reduction of Ca2+ responses in MB neurons simply results from the enhancement of GABA transmission after AL stimulation. In a study by ex vivo Ca2+ imaging analysis, picrotoxin treatment enhances MB Ca2+ responses induced by AL stimulation (Ueno et al. 2013). Thus, the treatment increases Ca2+ influx into the MB neurons during repetitive AL stimulation. Although rigorous physiological mechanisms of the reduction of Ca2+ responses enhanced by picrotoxin are still unclarified, picrotoxin‐induced elevation of [Ca2+]i in the MB neurons during repetitive AL stimulation may cause the enhanced reduction of Ca2+ responses.

Rut‐AC in the MB, but not in the PNs, is essential for the reduction of Ca2+ responses in MB neurons

To identify molecular components regulating the reduction of MB Ca2+ responses induced by AL stimulation, we focused on cAMP signalling. Drosophila rut, which is mainly expressed in MB neurons, encodes Ca2+/calmodulin‐responsive AC (Levin et al. 1992). rut1 mutant flies with a loss‐of‐function mutation of rut were used in the experiments. rut1 showed almost no reduction of Ca2+ responses in the MB neurons (Fig. 2 A and B). We next examined whether the induction of wild‐type rut expression in a rut mutant background rescues the reduction of MB Ca2+ responses. In Fig. 2 D and E, the GAL4/UAS system was used for wild‐type rut transgene expression in the MB neurons of rut1, and the LexA/LexAop system (Lai & Lee, 2006) was used for the targeted expression of the Ca2+ indicator GCaMP6m (Chen et al. 2013) in the MB neurons. Two MB‐GAL4 lines (c772 and R13F02) were used in the experiments. Although GAL4 and UAS control flies with rut1 mutation exhibited the rut1 mutant phenotype (Fig. 2 C, D and E, red lines), wild‐type rut transgene expression in the MB neurons of rut1 rescued the rut1 mutant phenotype (Fig. 2 D and E, black lines). In contrast to rut expression in the MB neurons, the targeted expression of the wild‐type rut transgene in PNs could not rescue the rut1 mutant phenotype (Fig. 2 F, black line), suggesting that Rut‐AC‐dependent cAMP production in the MB neurons, but not in the PNs, is critically involved in the reduction of MB Ca2+ responses after repetitive AL stimulation.

Figure 2. rut expression in MB neurons is required for the reduction of Ca2+ responses.

Figure 2

A, schematic diagram of the stimulation protocol. B–F, the upper panels show typical Ca2+ responses in the α lobe tip 3 min before (Pre), during and 10 min after the application of 40 stimulus trains. Asterisks indicate the statistical significance of the difference between before and after the application of 40 stimulus trains. Student's t test or the Mann–Whitney U test was used for statistical analysis. Error bars represent SEM. NS, not significant; * P < 0.05; ** P < 0.01; *** P < 0.001. B, relative Ca2+ responses in the wild‐type (+; MB247 UAS‐GCaMP3/+; n = 6; black line) and rut1 (rut1; MB247 UAS‐GCaMP3/+; n = 6, red line). Hash symbols indicate the statistical significance of difference at each time point between two genotypes. Student's t test was used for statistical analysis. # P < 0.05; ## P < 0.01. C, relative Ca2+ responses in the wild‐type (+; +; MB‐LexA LexAop‐GCaMP6m/+; n = 5; black line) and rut1 (rut1; +/UAS‐rut; MB‐LexA LexAop‐GCaMP6m/+; n = 7; red line). Hash symbols indicate the statistical significance of difference at each time point between two genotypes. Student's t test was used for statistical analysis. ## P < 0.01; ### P < 0.001. D, relative Ca2+ responses in rut1; c772/UAS‐rut; MB‐LexA LexAop‐GCaMP6m/+ (black line; n = 7) and rut1; c772/+; MB‐LexA LexAop‐GCaMP6m/+ (red line; n = 6). Hash symbols indicate the statistical significance of difference at each time point between two genotypes. Student's t test or the Mann–Whitney U test was used for statistical analysis. ## P < 0.01. E, relative Ca2+ responses in rut1; MB‐LexA LexAop‐GCaMP6f/UAS‐rut; R13F02‐GAL4 /+ (black line; n = 6) and rut1; MB‐LexA LexAop‐GCaMP6f/+; R13F02‐GAL4 /+ (red line; n = 6). Hash symbols indicate the statistical significance of difference at each time point between two genotypes. Student's t test or the Mann–Whitney U test was used for statistical analysis. # P < 0.05. F, relative Ca2+ responses in rut1; GH146/UAS‐rut; MB‐LexA LexAop‐GCaMP6m/+ (black line; n = 6), and rut1; GH146/+; MB‐LexA LexAop‐GCaMP6m/+ (red line; n = 6). No significant difference was detected at each point between two genotypes. Student's t test was used for statistical analysis. [Color figure can be viewed at http://wileyonlinelibrary.com]

Forskolin treatment or dnc1 mutation promotes the reduction of Ca2+ responses after repetitive AL stimulation

In MB neurons, dnc, which encodes a phosphodiesterase, is also expressed (Davis, 2005). It is considered that cAMP level increases in dnc1 mutant flies with a hypomorphic mutation of dnc compared with wild‐type flies (Byers et al. 1981). In contrast to rut1 mutation, dnc1 mutation enhanced the reduction of Ca2+ responses compared with the wild‐type flies (Fig. 3 A). Furthermore, although the repetitive AL stimulation with 30 stimulus trains was not sufficient to reduce the MB Ca2+ responses in wild‐type flies (Fig. 1 B), it induced the reduction of MB Ca2+ responses in dnc1 (Fig. 3 B). Next, to increase cAMP level transiently, we performed pharmacological treatment using an AC activator, forskolin (Boto et al. 2014). As was observed in dnc1, forskolin treatment during repetitive AL stimulation also enhanced the reduction of Ca2+ responses in MB neurons (Fig. 3 C). Thus, these findings show that the increase in cAMP level promotes the reduction of MB Ca2+ responses.

Figure 3. Increasing cAMP production enhances the reduction of Ca2+ responses.

Figure 3

The upper panels show schematic diagrams of the stimulation protocol. Middle panels show typical Ca2+ responses in the α lobe tip 3 or 10 min before (Pre), during and 10 min after the application of 30 or 40 stimulus trains. Asterisks indicate the statistical significance of the difference between before and after the application of 30 or 40 stimulus trains. Student's t test or the Mann–Whitney U test was used for statistical analysis. Error bars represent SEM. NS, not significant; * P < 0.05; ** P < 0.01; *** P < 0.001. A, the reduction of Ca2+ responses was enhanced in the dnc1 mutant (dnc1; MB247 UAS‐GCaMP3/+; n = 4; blue line) compared with the wild‐type (+; MB247 UAS‐GCaMP3/+; n = 6; black line). Hash symbols indicate the statistical significance of difference at each time point between the wild‐type and dnc1. The Mann–Whitney U test was used for statistical analysis. # P < 0.05. B, the reduction of Ca2+ responses was induced in the dnc1 mutant (dnc1; MB247 UAS‐GCaMP3/+; n = 6; blue line) by the application of 30 stimulus trains, while it was not induced in the wild‐type (+; MB247 UAS‐GCaMP3/+; n = 6; black line). Hash symbols indicate the statistical significance of difference at each time point between the wild‐type and dnc1. Student's t test or the Mann–Whitney U test was used for statistical analysis. ## P < 0.01. C, the reduction of Ca2+ responses was enhanced by the treatment of the AC activator forskolin (blue line; n = 7) compared with that of DMSO as the control (black line; n = 6) in the wild‐type (+; MB247 UAS‐GCaMP3/+). The black rectangle indicates the time of forskolin or DMSO treatment. Hash symbols indicate the statistical significance of difference at each time point between forskolin treatment and DMSO treatment. Student's t test was used for statistical analysis. # P < 0.05; ## P < 0.01. D, the reduction of Ca2+ responses was not suppressed by the treatment of the dopamine receptor blocker butaclamol (red line; n = 6) compared with that of DMSO as the control (black line; n = 6) in the wild‐type (+; MB247 UAS‐GCaMP3/+). The hash symbol indicates the statistical significance of the difference between butaclamol treatment and DMSO treatment. Student's t test was used for statistical analysis. # P < 0.05. [Color figure can be viewed at http://wileyonlinelibrary.com]

In Drosophila, four genes that encode dopamine receptors are identified (Dop1R1, Dop1R2, Dop2R and DopEcR), and these receptors are expressed in MB neurons (Crocker et al. 2016). Thus, cAMP could be produced by repetitive AL stimulation through the dopamine signalling pathway. To evaluate this possibility, we used a non‐selective dopamine receptor blocker, butaclamol (Sugamori et al. 1995). However, butaclamol treatment hardly affected the reduction of Ca2+ responses in MB neurons (Fig. 3 D). Thus, the reduction of MB Ca2+ responses may be independent of dopaminergic inputs.

The reduction of Ca2+ responses after repetitive AL stimulation is also detected in the MB calyx

Rut‐AC is localized in the axonal lobes and dendritic calyx in the MB (Han et al. 1992), and cAMP signalling regulates Ca2+ responsivity in these regions (Pavot et al. 2015). To examine whether repetitive AL stimulation also induces the reduction of Ca2+ responses in the calyx, we observed AL stimulation‐induced Ca2+ responses in the α lobe and calyx in the same brains (Figs 4 A and 5 A). After AL stimulation with 40 stimulus trains, Ca2+ responses in the calyx of wild‐type flies were reduced, as was observed in the α lobe (Fig. 4 B and C).

Figure 4. Repetitive AL stimulation induces the reduction of Ca2+ responses in the MB calyx.

Figure 4

A, schematic diagrams of AL stimulation protocol. AL stimulation was applied sequentially every 3 min before and after repetitive AL stimulation with 40 trains. Ca2+ responses were recorded in the α lobe and calyx in the same brain. B and C, +; MB247 UAS‐GCaMP3/+ males were used in the experiments. Asterisks indicate the statistical significance of the difference between before and after the application of 40 stimulus trains. The Mann–Whitney U test was used for statistical analysis. Error bars represent SEM. ** P < 0.01; n = 7. B, Ca2+ responses in the calyx after repetitive AL stimulation. The upper panel shows typical Ca2+ responses in the calyx 3 min before (Pre) and 12 min after the application of 40 stimulus trains. Pseudo‐color represents the intensity of GCaMP3 fluorescence. The area enclosed by a dashed circle is the calyx. C, Ca2+ responses in the α lobe after repetitive AL stimulation in the same brain shown in B. The upper panel shows typical Ca2+ responses in the α lobe 6 min before (Pre), during and 9 min after the application of 40 stimulus trains. The area enclosed by dashed lines is the α lobe. [Color figure can be viewed at http://wileyonlinelibrary.com]

Figure 5. rut expression in MB neurons is required for the reduction of Ca2+ responses in the MB calyx.

Figure 5

A, schematic diagram of AL stimulation protocol. B and C, the upper panels show typical Ca2+ responses in the calyx 3 min before (Pre) and 12 min after the application of 40 stimulus trains. E, the upper panels show typical Ca2+ responses in the α lobe 6 min before (Pre), during and 9 min after the application of 40 stimulus trains. B, C and E, asterisks indicate the statistical significance of the difference between before and after the application of 40 stimulus trains. The Mann–Whitney U test was used for statistical analysis. Error bars represent SEM. NS, not significant; * P < 0.05; ** P < 0.01; *** P < 0.001. B, relative Ca2+ responses in the calyx of wild‐type (black line; +; +; MB‐LexA LexAop‐GCaMP6m/+; n = 7) and rut1 (red line; rut1; +/UAS‐rut; MB‐LexA LexAop‐GCaMP6m/+; n = 7). Hash symbols indicate the statistical significance of difference at each time point between two genotypes. Student's t test or the Mann–Whitney U test was used for statistical analysis.# P < 0.05; ## P < 0.01. C, relative Ca2+ responses in the calyx of rut1; c772/UAS‐rut; MB‐LexA LexAop‐GCaMP6m/+ (black line; n = 9) and rut1; c772/+; MB‐LexA LexAop‐GCaMP6m/+ (red line; n = 9). Hash symbols indicate the statistical significance of difference at each time point between two genotypes. Student's t test was used for statistical analysis. # P < 0.05; ## P < 0.01. D, no significant difference was detected at each time point between the wild‐type (black line; +; +; MB‐LexA LexAop‐GCaMP6m/+) and rut1; c772/UAS‐rut (red line; rut1; c772/UAS‐rut; MB‐LexA LexAop‐GCaMP6m/+) in the calyx. The results for the wild‐type are the same as those shown in B, and the results for rut1; c772/UAS‐rut are the same as those shown in C. The Mann–Whitney U test was used for statistical analysis. Error bars represent SEM. NS, not significant. E, in the α lobe, the degree of reduction of Ca2+ responses in rut1; c772/UAS‐rut (red line; rut1; c772/UAS‐rut; MB‐LexA LexAop‐GCaMP6m/+) was lower than that in the wild‐type (black line; +; +; MB‐LexA LexAop‐GCaMP6m/+). Hash symbols indicate the statistical significance of difference at each time point between two genotypes. Student's t test was used for statistical analysis. Error bars represent SEM. # P < 0.05; ### P < 0.001. F and G, Ca2+ responses to AL stimulation with a stimulus train in the calyx (F) and the α lobe (G). +; +; MB‐LexA LexAop‐GCaMP6m/+ (white bar), rut1; +/UAS‐rut; MB‐LexA LexAop‐GCaMP6m/+ (light grey bar), rut1; c772/+; MB‐LexA LexAop‐GCaMP6m/+ (grey bar), and rut1; c772/UAS‐rut; MB‐LexA LexAop‐GCaMP6m/+ (red bar) were used for analysis. One‐way ANOVA or the non‐parametric ANOVA (Kruskal–Wallis test) was used for statistical analysis. Error bars represent SEM. NS, not significant. H, +; GH146 UAS‐GCaMP6m/+; + males were used. Ca2+ responses before and after application of 40 stimulus trains in the terminal of PNs. The upper panel shows a schematic diagram of the stimulation protocol. The middle panel shows typical Ca2+ responses in the terminal of PNs 3 min before (Pre), during and 10 min after the application of 40 stimulus trains. Asterisks indicate the statistical significance of difference at each time point between before and after the application of 40 stimulus trains. Student's t test or the Mann–Whitney U test was used for statistical analysis. Error bars represent SEM. * P < 0.05; ** P < 0.01; *** P < 0.001; n = 6. I, cAMP responses during repetitive AL stimulation in the wild‐type (+; +; 30Y UAS‐Epac1‐camps/+) and rut1 (rut1; +; 30Y UAS‐Epac1‐camps/+). Time course of cAMP responses in the MB calyx (left) and summary of maximum scores from each trace (right). The black line and bar indicate results for the wild‐type (WT; n = 7) and the red line and bar indicate those for rut1 (n = 7). The black rectangle indicates the time of repetitive AL stimulation. Student's t test was used for pairwise comparisons. Error bars represent SEM. * P < 0.05. [Color figure can be viewed at http://wileyonlinelibrary.com]

In contrast to wild‐type flies, rut1 flies showed impaired reduction of Ca2+ responses in the calyx (Fig. 5 B). However, the targeted expression of the wild‐type rut transgene in the MB neurons in rut1 mutant flies (rut1; c772/UAS‐rut) induced the reduction of Ca2+ responses after repetitive AL stimulation in the calyx (Fig. 5 C). Regarding the degree of reduction of Ca2+ responses in the calyx, no significant differences were detected between the wild‐type and rut1; c772/UAS‐rut flies (Fig. 5 D), indicating that rut expression in MB neurons in rut1 completely rescues the rut1 phenotype in the calyx. In the α lobe, the degree of reduction of Ca2+ responses in rut1; c772/UAS‐rut flies was lower than that in wild‐type flies (Fig. 5 E). Thus, these findings indicate that rut expression in rut1 partially rescues the rut1 phenotype in the α lobe.

It is possible that the impaired reduction of Ca2+ responses in the rut1 mutant calyx or α lobe results from reduced Ca2+ responses to AL stimulation. Thus, Ca2+ responses to AL stimulation with one stimulus train, that is, without repetitive AL stimulation, were compared among four genotypes (Fig. 5 F and G). In the calyx and α lobe, no significant differences were detected in Ca2+ responses (Fig. 5 F and G). Thus, it is unlikely that the reduced Ca2+ responses to electrical stimulation of AL in rut1 impair the reduction of Ca2+ responses after repetitive AL stimulation.

Next, we examined whether the activity of PNs is modified after repetitive AL stimulation with 40 stimulus trains. AL stimulation slightly reduced Ca2+ responses in PN axon terminals after repetitive AL stimulation (Fig. 5 H), suggesting that repetitive AL stimulation slightly modifies physiological properties of PN axon terminals. However, the reduction of Ca2+ responses in axon terminals in PNs was smaller in magnitude than that in the MB calyx (Fig. 5 B and H). Furthermore, the impaired reduction of Ca2+ responses in MB neurons in rut1 flies was fully rescued by the expression of the wild‐type rut transgene in the calyx in rut1 (Fig. 5 D). Thus, it is most unlikely that the cAMP‐dependent reduction of Ca2+ responses in the calyx simply results from the physiological properties of PNs modified by the repetitive AL stimulation.

If Rut‐AC‐dependent cAMP production is required for the reduction of Ca2+ responses in the calyx, AL stimulation with 40 stimulus trains should increase the cAMP level in the calyx. We measured the cAMP level in the calyx using a FRET‐based cAMP indicator, Epac1‐camps (Nikolaev et al. 2004). Although the cAMP level increased in the wild‐type calyx during repetitive AL stimulation, cAMP production was inhibited in rut1 mutant flies (Fig. 5 I).

Discussion

In this study, we found novel non‐associative synaptic depression in the MB calyx of an isolated cultured Drosophila brain. Ca2+ responses in the calyx were reduced after repetitive AL stimulation (Fig. 4), and rut expression in the MB neurons was essential for the reduction of Ca2+ responses in the calyx (Fig. 5). Since Rut‐AC is a Ca2+/calmodulin‐dependent AC (Levin et al. 1992), it is possible that Rut‐AC‐dependent cAMP production was induced via the increase in [Ca2+]i. AL–MB synaptic transmissions are cholinergic and nicotinic acetylcholine receptors (nAChRs) are expressed in the calyx (Fayyazuddin et al. 2006). Thus, it is likely that increasing [Ca2+]i through nAChRs activates Rut‐AC. Taken together, the synaptic plasticity in the calyx may be explained as follows. In the wild‐type flies, the cAMP level through nAChRs increases in the calyx during repetitive AL stimulation, and the increased cAMP level induces synaptic depression in the calyx (Fig. 6 A). However, in rut1, Rut‐AC‐dependent cAMP production is inhibited during repetitive AL stimulation; consequently, synaptic depression does not occur in the calyx (Fig. 6 B). In rut1 mutant flies, cAMP production was inhibited, whereas it was slightly detected during repetitive AL stimulation (Fig. 5 I). Since four AC genes apart from rut are highly expressed in the adult brain (Naganos et al. 2016), these ACs in MB neurons may contribute to the slight increase in cAMP level in rut1. However, rut1 did not reduce Ca2+ responses after repetitive AL stimulation (Fig. 5), suggesting that Rut‐AC‐dependent cAMP production in the calyx is sufficient for the reduction of Ca2+ responses after repetitive AL stimulation.

Figure 6. Possible model of the reduction of Ca2+ responses in the MB calyx and α lobe.

Figure 6

A, in the wild‐type, the projection neurons of the antennal lobe (AL‐PN) release acetylcholine (black dots) following repetitive AL stimulation, and nAChRs (blue) are activated in the MB calyx. The increase in [Ca2+]i through nAChRs stimulates Rut‐AC (red), and cAMP is produced. The increase in cAMP level induces synaptic depression in the MB calyx. B, in the rut1 mutant, repetitive AL stimulation induces Ca2+ entry through nAChRs. However, Rut‐AC‐dependent cAMP production does not occur. Thus, synaptic depression also does not occur. C, in the wild‐type, action potentials generated by repetitive AL stimulation may induce Ca2+ influx through voltage‐gated calcium channels (VGCCs; green) in the α lobe. The increase in [Ca2+]i activates Rut‐AC, and cAMP is produced. The increase in cAMP level induces the reduction of Ca2+ responses in the α lobe. D, in the rut1 mutant, repetitive AL stimulation induces Ca2+ influx into the α lobe. However, Rut‐AC‐dependent cAMP production does not occur. Thus, the reduction of Ca2+ responses also does not occur. E, forskolin treatment or dnc1 mutation increases the cAMP level. In this situation, the reduction of Ca2+ responses in the α lobe is promoted. [Color figure can be viewed at http://wileyonlinelibrary.com]

We further found that Ca2+ responses in the α lobe were also reduced after repetitive AL stimulation (Fig. 1). The reduction rate was dependent on the number of stimulus trains (Fig. 1). As was observed in the calyx, rut expression in MB neurons was required for the reduction of Ca2+ responses in the α lobe (Fig. 5). In contrast to the rut1 mutation, the dnc1 mutation or forskolin treatment promoted the reduction of Ca2+ responses after repetitive AL stimulation with 40 stimulus trains (Fig. 3 A and C). In addition, the repetitive AL stimulation with 30 stimulus trains was sufficient to reduce the MB Ca2+ responses in dnc1 (Fig. 3 B). Taking these results together, in wild‐type flies, the increase in Rut‐AC‐dependent cAMP production during repetitive AL stimulation induces the reduction of Ca2+ responses in the α lobe (Fig. 6 C). However, in rut1, the cAMP production is inhibited during repetitive AL stimulation; consequently, the Ca2+ responses are not reduced in the α lobe (Fig. 6 D). In contrast, forskolin treatment or dnc1 mutation further increases the cAMP level during repetitive AL stimulation. Thus, the reduction of Ca2+ responses is also promoted in the α lobe (Fig. 6 E).

In this study, we identified that repetitive AL stimulation induces the reduction of Ca2+ responses in the calyx and α lobe. The rut expression in MB neurons in rut1 completely rescued the rut1 phenotype in the calyx (Fig. 5 D). However, in the α lobe, the rut expression partially rescued the rut1 phenotype (Fig. 5 E), suggesting that Rut‐AC in brain neurons other than MBs also contributes to the reduction of Ca2+ responses in the α lobe. Thus, although the detailed clarification of the physiological mechanisms regulating the reduction of Ca2+ responses in the α lobe remains elusive, it is possible that the reduction of Ca2+ responses in the α lobe does not simply result from the altered Ca2+ responsivity in the calyx.

We previously reported that AL stimulation with 12 stimulus trains (intertrain intervals of 5 s) alone does not induce plasticity at AL–MB synapses in the dissected brain, whereas simultaneous associative stimulation of the AL (12 stimulus trains) and AFV generates long‐term enhancement (Ueno et al. 2013). In this study, we showed that AL stimulation with more than 40 stimulus trains (intertrain intervals of 1 s) induces synaptic depression, suggesting that AL stimulation with 12 stimulus trains is insufficient for producing cAMP in the MB calyx, which is essential for synaptic depression. In honeybee, MB responses gradually increase during continuous low‐frequency stimulation of AL (0.1 ms pulse duration, interpulse intervals of 1–50 s), indicating that MB neurons represent sensitization through AL–MB synaptic transmission (Oleskevich et al. 1997). Since the stimulation protocol of AL in the previous study is completely different from that in this study, in insects, the generation of non‐associative sensitization or depression at AL–MB synapses may be dependent on the protocol of AL stimulation.

Insects show species‐specific behavioural modification following odour exposure (Anderson et al. 2003; Minoli et al. 2013). Similarly to other insect species, Drosophila also shows odour‐based behavioural plasticity. Prolonged odour exposure leads to reduced behavioural responses, and this behavioural plasticity is considered to be caused by experience‐dependent or adaptive synaptic plasticity in the AL (Sachse et al. 2007; Das et al. 2011; Pech et al. 2015). In addition, MB neurons also play an important role in the regulation of odour‐based behavioural plasticity. The locomotor activity of adult flies rapidly and transiently increases following exposure to ethanol vapour, and this odour‐mediated startle response is attenuated by repetitive exposure to ethanol vapour (Cho et al. 2004). However, dysfunction of MB neurons and rut mutations reduce this non‐associative behavioural plasticity (Cho et al. 2004), suggesting that the cAMP signalling pathway in the MB neurons regulate non‐associative behavioural plasticity induced by olfactory inputs. In this study, we showed by ex vivo imaging analysis that repetitive AL stimulation induces synaptic depression in the MB calyx. Although the role of synaptic depression in vivo still remains unclarified, it is likely that the synaptic depression in the MB calyx also causes odour‐based behavioural plasticity. As was observed in Drosophila, in the honey bee Apis mellifera carnica, AL–MB transmission is also cholinergic (Goldberg et al. 1999), and odour‐evoked Ca2+ responses in the MB calyx are attenuated after repeated odour stimulation (Szyszka et al. 2008). Thus, it is possible that the non‐associative olfactory learning based on synaptic depression in the MB calyx is evolutionarily conserved among insect species. Ex vivo imaging to monitor synaptic depression in the MB calyx will be carried out to identify additional molecules or clarify the physiological property underlying odour‐based behavioural plasticity in insect species.

Additional information

Competing interests

None declared.

Author contributions

S.S. and T.S. contributed to the conception or design of the work. S.S. performed most of the experiments. K.U. and M.S. contributed to the imaging study. T.S. supervised and wrote the manuscript with S.S., K.U. and M.S. All experiments were carried out in TMU and TMIMS. All authors have approved the final version of the manuscript and agree to be accountable for all aspects of the work. All persons designated as authors qualify for authorship, and all those who qualify for authorship are listed.

Funding

This work was supported by grants from JSPS KAKENHI (12J06931 to S.S., JP21700376 to K.U., 16H04816 and JP25650116 to T.S.) and the Ministry of Education, Culture, Sports, Science and Technology (JP25115006 to M.S., JP21115514 and JP23115714 to T.S.)

Acknowledgements

We thank S. Miyata for technical assistance. We also thank T. Miyashita and S. Naganos for helpful discussions.

Biography

Shoma Sato is a PhD candidate in the Department of Biological Sciences at Tokyo Metropolitan University. His research interests are on the molecular and cellular mechanisms of neural plasticity underlying memory formation.

graphic file with name TJP-596-2447-g001.gif

Edited by: Ian Forsythe & Reinhold Penner

This is an Editor's Choice article from the 15 June 2018 issue.

References

  1. Abel T & Nguyen PV (2008). Regulation of hippocampus‐dependent memory by cyclic AMP‐dependent protein kinase. Prog Brain Res 169, 97–115. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Anderson P, Sadek MM & Hansson BS (2003). Pre‐exposure modulates attraction to sex pheromone in a moth. Chem Senses 28, 285–291. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Aso Y, Hattori D, Yu Y, Johnston RM, Iyer NA, Ngo TT, Dionne H, Abbott LF, Axel R, Tanimoto H & Rubin GM (2014). The neuronal architecture of the mushroom body provides a logic for associative learning. Elife 3, e04577. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Boto T, Louis T, Jindachomthong K, Jalink K & Tomchik SM (2014). Dopaminergic modulation of cAMP drives nonlinear plasticity across the Drosophila mushroom body lobes. Curr Biol 24, 822–831. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Brand AH & Perrimon N (1993). Targeted gene expression as a means of altering cell fates and generating dominant phenotypes. Development 118, 401–415. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Byers D, Davis RL & Kiger JA Jr (1981). Defect in cyclic AMP phosphodiesterase due to the dunce mutation of learning in Drosophila melanogaster . Nature 289, 79–81. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Chen TW, Wardill TJ, Sun Y, Pulver SR, Renninger SL, Baohan A, Schreiter ER, Kerr RA, Orger MB, Jayaraman V, Looger LL, Svoboda K & Kim DS (2013). Ultrasensitive fluorescent proteins for imaging neuronal activity. Nature 499, 295–300. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Cho W, Heberlein U & Wolf FW (2004). Habituation of an odorant‐induced startle response in Drosophila . Genes Brain Behav 3, 127–137. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Cohn R, Morantte I & Ruta V (2015). Coordinated and compartmentalized neuromodulation shapes sensory processing in Drosophila . Cell 163, 1742–1755. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Collingridge GL, Peineau S, Howland JG & Wang YT (2010). Long‐term depression in the CNS. Nat Rev Neurosci 11, 459–473. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Crittenden JR, Skoulakis EM, Han KA, Kalderon D & Davis RL (1998). Tripartite mushroom body architecture revealed by antigenic markers. Learn Mem 5, 38–51. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Crocker A, Guan XJ, Murphy CT & Murthy M (2016). Cell‐type‐specific transcriptome analysis in the Drosophila mushroom body reveals memory‐related changes in gene expression. Cell Rep 15, 1580–1596. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Das S, Sadanandappa MK, Dervan A, Larkin A, Lee JA, Sudhakaran IP, Priya R, Heidari R, Holohan EE, Pimentel A, Gandhi A, Ito K, Sanyal S, Wang JW, Rodrigues V & Ramaswami M (2011). Plasticity of local GABAergic interneurons drives olfactory habituation. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 108, E646–654. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Davis RL (2005). Olfactory memory formation in Drosophila: from molecular to systems neuroscience. Annu Rev Neurosci 28, 275–302. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Davis RL (2011). Traces of Drosophila memory. Neuron 70, 8–19. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Fayyazuddin A, Zaheer MA, Hiesinger PR & Bellen HJ (2006). The nicotinic acetylcholine receptor Dα7 is required for an escape behavior in Drosophila . PLoS Biol 4, e63. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Glanzman DL (2009). Habituation in Aplysia: the Cheshire cat of neurobiology. Neurobiol Learn Mem 92, 147–154. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Goldberg F, Grunewald B, Rosenboom H & Menzel R (1999). Nicotinic acetylcholine currents of cultured Kenyon cells from the mushroom bodies of the honey bee Apis mellifera . J Physiol 514, 759–768. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Guo HF & Zhong Y (2006). Requirement of Akt to mediate long‐term synaptic depression in Drosophila . J Neurosci 26, 4004–4014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Han PL, Levin LR, Reed RR & Davis RL (1992). Preferential expression of the Drosophila rutabaga gene in mushroom bodies, neural centers for learning in insects. Neuron 9, 619–627. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Harrison JB, Chen HH, Sattelle E, Barker PJ, Huskisson NS, Rauh JJ, Bai D & Sattelle DB (1996). Immunocytochemical mapping of a C‐terminus anti‐peptide antibody to the GABA receptor subunit, RDL in the nervous system in Drosophila melanogaster . Cell Tissue Res 284, 269–278. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Hawkins RD, Kandel ER & Bailey CH (2006). Molecular mechanisms of memory storage in Aplysia . Biol Bull 210, 174–191. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Heisenberg M (2003). Mushroom body memoir: from maps to models. Nat Rev Neurosci 4, 266–275. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Hige T, Aso Y, Modi MN, Rubin GM & Turner GC (2015). Heterosynaptic plasticity underlies aversive olfactory learning in Drosophila . Neuron 88, 985–998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Ho VM, Lee JA & Martin KC (2011). The cell biology of synaptic plasticity. Science 334, 623–628. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Kandel ER (2001). The molecular biology of memory storage: a dialogue between genes and synapses. Science 294, 1030–1038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Kandel ER (2012). The molecular biology of memory: cAMP, PKA, CRE, CREB‐1, CREB‐2, and CPEB. Mol Brain 5, 14. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Korte M & Schmitz D (2016). Cellular and system biology of memory: timing, molecules, and beyond. Physiol Rev 96, 647–693. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Lai SL & Lee T (2006). Genetic mosaic with dual binary transcriptional systems in Drosophila . Nat Neurosci 9, 703–709. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Levin LR, Han PL, Hwang PM, Feinstein PG, Davis RL & Reed RR (1992). The Drosophila learning and memory gene rutabaga encodes a Ca2+/calmodulin‐responsive adenylyl cyclase. Cell 68, 479–489. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Lin AC, Bygrave AM, de Calignon A, Lee T & Miesenbock G (2014). Sparse, decorrelated odor coding in the mushroom body enhances learned odor discrimination. Nat Neurosci 17, 559–568. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Minoli S, Palottini F & Manrique G (2013). The main component of an alarm pheromone of kissing bugs plays multiple roles in the cognitive modulation of the escape response. Front Behav Neurosci 7, 77. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Naganos S, Ueno K, Horiuchi J & Saitoe M (2016). Learning defects in Drosophila growth restricted chico mutants are caused by attenuated adenylyl cyclase activity. Mol Brain 9, 37. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Nicoll RA (2017). A brief history of long‐term potentiation. Neuron 93, 281–290. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Nikolaev VO, Bunemann M, Hein L, Hannawacker A & Lohse MJ (2004). Novel single chain cAMP sensors for receptor‐induced signal propagation. J Biol Chem 279, 37215–37218. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Oleskevich S, Clements JD & Srinivasan MV (1997). Long‐term synaptic plasticity in the honeybee. J Neurophysiol 78, 528–532. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Pavot P, Carbognin E & Martin JR (2015). PKA and cAMP/CNG channels independently regulate the cholinergic Ca2+‐response of Drosophila mushroom body neurons. eNeuro 2, ENEURO.0054‐14.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Pech U, Revelo NH, Seitz KJ, Rizzoli SO & Fiala A (2015). Optical dissection of experience‐dependent pre‐ and postsynaptic plasticity in the Drosophila brain. Cell Rep 10, 2083–2095. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Sachse S, Rueckert E, Keller A, Okada R, Tanaka NK, Ito K & Vosshall LB (2007). Activity‐dependent plasticity in an olfactory circuit. Neuron 56, 838–850. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Stewart BA, Atwood HL, Renger JJ, Wang J & Wu CF (1994). Improved stability of Drosophila larval neuromuscular preparations in haemolymph‐like physiological solutions. J Comp Physiol A 175, 179–191. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Su H & O'Dowd DK (2003). Fast synaptic currents in Drosophila mushroom body Kenyon cells are mediated by α‐bungarotoxin‐sensitive nicotinic acetylcholine receptors and picrotoxin‐sensitive GABA receptors. J Neurosci 23, 9246–9253. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Sugamori KS, Demchyshyn LL, McConkey F, Forte MA & Niznik HB (1995). A primordial dopamine D1‐like adenylyl cyclase‐linked receptor from Drosophila melanogaster displaying poor affinity for benzazepines. FEBS Lett 362, 131–138. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Szyszka P, Galkin A & Menzel R (2008). Associative and non‐associative plasticity in Kenyon cells of the honeybee mushroom body. Front Syst Neurosci 2, 3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Tian L, Hires SA, Mao T, Huber D, Chiappe ME, Chalasani SH, Petreanu L, Akerboom J, McKinney SA, Schreiter ER, Bargmann CI, Jayaraman V, Svoboda K & Looger LL (2009). Imaging neural activity in worms, flies and mice with improved GCaMP calcium indicators. Nat Methods 6, 875–881. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Ueno K, Naganos S, Hirano Y, Horiuchi J & Saitoe M (2013). Long‐term enhancement of synaptic transmission between antennal lobe and mushroom body in cultured Drosophila brain. J Physiol 591, 287–302. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Ueno K, Suzuki E, Naganos S, Ofusa K, Horiuchi J & Saitoe M (2017). Coincident postsynaptic activity gates presynaptic dopamine release to induce plasticity in Drosophila mushroom bodies. Elife 6, e21076. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Vosshall LB & Stocker RF (2007). Molecular architecture of smell and taste in Drosophila . Annu Rev Neurosci 30, 505–533. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Wang Y, Mamiya A, Chiang AS & Zhong Y (2008). Imaging of an early memory trace in the Drosophila mushroom body. J Neurosci 28, 4368–4376. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from The Journal of Physiology are provided here courtesy of The Physiological Society

RESOURCES