Skip to main content
Journal of Community Genetics logoLink to Journal of Community Genetics
. 2017 Nov 13;9(3):217–225. doi: 10.1007/s12687-017-0339-z

Predictors of adverse psychological experiences surrounding genome-wide profiling for disease risk

K M Broady 1, K E Ormond 1,2, E J Topol 3, N J Schork 4, Cinnamon S Bloss 3,5,6,
PMCID: PMC6002310  PMID: 29130150

Abstract

This study aimed to identify predictors of adverse psychological experiences among direct-to-consumer (DTC) genomic test consumers. We performed a secondary analysis on data from the Scripps Genomic Health Initiative (SGHI), which studied 2037 individuals tested with commercially available tests yielding personalized risk estimates for 23 common, genetically complex diseases. As part of the original study, the participants completed baseline and follow-up survey measures assessing demographics, personal and family health history, attitudes toward genetic testing, anxiety (State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI)), test-related distress (Impact of Event Scale—Revised (IES-R)), and reactions to receipt of results. To further describe the participants who had an adverse psychological outcome, this secondary analysis defined two different variables (“distress response” and “psychologically sensitive participants”) and examined their relationship to various demographic variables and other survey responses. One hundred thirty participants (6.4%) were defined as having a “distress response” to receipt of results based on changes in STAI and/or IES. Four hundred thirty-one participants (21.2%) were defined as being “psychologically sensitive” based on high STAI scores both pre- and post-receipt of results. For psychologically sensitive subjects, younger age emerged as a predictor (p < 0.0005). Family history and personal history were only significant predictors for Alzheimer’s disease in the psychologically sensitive participants (p = .03) and restless leg syndrome in those with a distress response (p = .03). Psychologically sensitive participants were more likely to indicate a number of pre-test concerns than were controls, but neither group of participants were any more likely to follow up with their physician or a free genetic counseling service after the return of results.

Keywords: Anxiety, Depression, Direct-to-consumer, Genetic testing, Genomic risk assessment, Impact of events, Personalized medicine, Precision medicine, Psychological distress

Introduction

Ever since direct-to-consumer (DTC) genotyping for genomic risk assessment of common, genetically complex conditions first became available in 2007, there has been research, professional statements, and ongoing commentary expressing concerns regarding its provision without the involvement of a healthcare intermediary and the potential psychological effects on consumers (American College of Medical Genetics 2016; McGuire and Burke 2008; Annes et al. 2010; McBride et al. 2010; Stack et al. 2011; Dohany et al. 2012; Gordon et al. 2013; Skirton et al. 2012; Kaufman et al. 2012; Francke et al. 2013; Roberts and Ostergren 2013). Despite these worries, the limited literature shows that a relatively small percentage of consumers exhibit negative psychological effects related to such testing (Francke et al. 2013; Roberts and Ostergren 2013; Bloss et al. 2011; Egglestone et al. 2013).

Here we present a novel secondary analysis of the Scripps Genomic Health Initiative (SGHI) (Bloss et al. 2011), which was originally performed by several of this paper’s co-authors (ET, NS, CB). The SGHI collected data between 2008 and 2009 on 2037 consumers of DTC genomic risk assessment. As was previously published (Bloss et al. 2011), the original study found that less than 4% of the population had a psychological impact from the testing and return of results process. Specifically, using the Impact of Event Scale—Revised (IES-R), only 2.8% of all subjects reported clinically meaningful test-related distress after receiving personalized genomics results and 3.8% of all subjects experienced a clinically reliable change in anxiety as assessed by the Spielberger State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI). This is similar to that found by (Egglestone et al. 2013), who found that just 2.7% (5/183) of respondents having undergone DTC genomic risk assessment reported an increase in their anxiety. In contrast, a study by 23andMe of 32 individuals having received positive BRCA1/2 founder mutation results found 12.5% reported having been moderately upset and 28.1% reported transient anxiety after receiving their results (Francke et al. 2013). This suggests that higher penetrance genomic results could pose higher risks of adverse psychological responses, particularly in a DTC context, than an elevated risk for a common, complex condition. While much has been made of the small percentages of individuals who experience adverse psychological outcomes after DTC genomic risk assessment in this and other data sets, little is understood about these individuals.

Better characterizing those with adverse psychological experiences surrounding receipt of genomic risk results, including whether they may be prospectively identified as at risk for these experiences, may guide healthcare and industry providers in provision of clinical services as patients undergo genomic risk assessment and learn their results in both a DTC and primary care setting. Thus, the specific aim of this project was to perform a secondary analysis of the SGHI data to specifically characterize the individuals who have an adverse psychological response to DTC genomic testing. We entered our study with several testable hypotheses: (1) demographic factors could be identified that predicted which participants had an adverse psychosocial impact; (2) family history or personal history of a condition for which one was found to be at increased risk would predict an adverse psychosocial impact, (3) persons who were found to be at a higher risk for more conditions, measured through the total number of conditions for which a subject was at elevated genomic risk, or their average estimated lifetime risk value for each condition, would predict a higher risk of adverse psychosocial impact, and (4) participants with an adverse psychological response may communicate differently or express concerns, both pre and post-results, with healthcare providers.

Methods

Existing data set

The current study is a secondary analysis of data from the SGHI, which is a longitudinal cohort study originally designed to assess psychological and behavioral impacts experienced by consumers of DTC genomic testing. Participants in the SGHI underwent DTC testing between 2008 and 2009 with the Navigenics Health Compass (NHC), which generated risk assessments based on genotyping of approximately 500,000 bases in the individual’s DNA. The NHC included personalized risk estimates for 23 common, genetically complex conditions. Details describing the SGHI and NHC have been previously reported (Bloss et al. 2010, 2011). Of relevance to this study, in the personalized risk estimates returned to participants, a condition was flagged as “orange” (implying “elevated risk”) if the subject’s estimated lifetime risk was more than 20% above average lifetime risk in the general population, or if the overall lifetime risk was more than 25%. Alternatively, a condition was labeled “gray” if it did not meet these criteria.

Participants in the SGHI completed baseline (prior to testing) and follow-up (offered 3 months after testing, with an average completion time of 5.6 months after testing) survey measures assessing variables such as demographics, personal and family health history, attitudes toward genetic testing, “state” (situational) anxiety (measured with the 20-item State Anxiety subscale of the Spielberger State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI)), test-related distress (measured with the Avoidance and Intrusion subscales of the 22-item Impact of Event Scale—Revised (IES-R)), and reactions to receipt of results.

Current study

Within these existing data, we conducted a novel case-control study, comparing individuals who were considered to either have strong negative psychological responses (anxiety and distress) on follow-up survey measures or were considered to be psychologically sensitive with elevated anxiety throughout the process (“cases”) to corresponding control groups comprised of individuals who did not show such characteristics. Since there is no single agreed-upon criteria for adverse or negative psychological responses and this was an exploratory study to determine if predictors of anxiety and/or distress could be identified, we defined inclusion criteria for the first set of “cases” in two ways using scores related to the STAI- and IES-R-validated measures. An increase of 12 points or more in STAI state score from baseline to follow-up survey is considered to indicate a reliably and clinically meaningful increase in anxiety (Spielberger et al. 1983; Jacobson and Truax 1991). A score of more than 23 points on the Avoidance and Intrusion subscales of the IES-R is also considered to indicate clinically significant distress (Bloss et al. 2011; Weiss and Marmar 1997; Creamer et al. 2003). As such, the first case group is termed “distress response” (N = 130) and is defined as those with clinically meaningful anxiety and/or distress by meeting one or both of the following criteria: (1) individuals with STAI scores with an increase of 12 points or more between the baseline and follow-up surveys (N = 77), and (2) individuals with an IES-R score greater than 23 points (N = 57). There were only 4 individuals who met both criteria and their responses were not significantly different from the rest of the cases. The second case group is termed “psychologically sensitive” (N = 431) and is defined as those who had a score of ≥ 40 points on both the baseline and follow-up STAI measures; based on the definition by (Spielberger et al. 1983), a score of 40 points or higher as indicating clinically relevant state anxiety. This group was thought of as having pre-existing psychological sensitivity (versus an adverse psychological response to the genomic risk results, given that their anxiety was elevated prior to receiving any genomic test results).

Initial statistical analyses were conducted with the use of the statistical software package SPSS 22.0. Pearson’s chi-squared, Fisher’s exact tests, independent samples t tests, and Mann-Whitney U tests were used to compare those with and without clinically significant anxiety and/or distress with respect to demographic variables, reported concerns prior to testing, self-report of feelings after receipt of results, and estimated lifetime risk reports from the NHC for the 23 diseases. Analysis of the extent to which demographic characteristics were associated with clinically meaningful anxiety and/or distress was performed via logistic regression. Power analyses of comparison between subjects’ reported life changes and estimated lifetime risk reports were conducted with the use of the statistical power analysis tool G*Power (Faul et al. 2009). Because we had limited power, which stemmed from a small number of “cases,” we have presented p values < 0.10.

Results

Demographics

A total of 2037 participants in the SGHI completed the follow-up survey measure and were thus included in this analysis. As has been previously published (Bloss et al. 2011), the SGHI population is a largely Caucasian (84%), well-educated population with high socio-economic status; approximately 25% of the participant population were Scripps employees, assumedly with a higher scientific and/or medical literacy and interest (Boeldt et al. 2015).

Two case/control groups, “distress response” and “psychologically sensitive,” were established for analysis based upon STAI and IES-R scores. The “distress response” group consists of 130 individuals while the corresponding control group consists of 1907 individuals. The “psychologically sensitive” group consists of 431 individuals while the corresponding control group consists of 1606 individuals.

Descriptive statistics for demographics and selected outcome variables are presented for the two study populations in Table 1. Individuals in the psychosocially sensitive group were more likely to be younger (p < 0.0005, t = −7.17, df = 749) and not have children (p < 0.0005, χ 2 = 13.33, df = 1). Both case groups had statistically significant differences in income distribution (“distress response”: p = 0.001, Z = −3.22, “psychologically sensitive”: p = 0.010, Z = −2.58); however, both groups had a median income range of $100,000–$149,999 for both cases and controls.

Table 1.

Demographic and outcome variable comparisons between subjects in “distress response” and “psychologically sensitive” groups*

Variable “Distress response” “Psychologically sensitive”
Case (N = 130) Control (N = 1907) p value Case (N = 431) Control (N = 1606) p value
Gender (% female) 63.1 54.8 0.066 (χ 2 = 3.38, df = 1) 58.7 54.4 0.113 (χ 2 = 2.52, df = 1)
Age (year) Mean 46.3 ± 11.3 47.7 ± 12.1 0.727 (t =−0.35, df = 2035) 43.3 ± 10.8 47.6 ± 12.2 < 0.0005 (t = −7.17, df = 749)
Range 23–72 19–85 20–75 19–85
Have children (% yes) 55.4 62.6 0.103 (χ 2 = 2.66, df = 1) 54.5 64.1 < 0.0005 (χ 2 = 13.33, df = 1)
Subject of prior GT (% yes) 6.2 5.8 0.875 (χ 2 = 0.03, df = 1) 6.7 5.6 0.377 (χ 2 = 0.78, df = 1)
STAI score at baseline Mean 36.5 ± 9.7 35.1 ± 9.6 0.114 (χ 2 = 1.58, df = 2035) 48.4 ± 6.0 31.7 ± 7.0 < 0.0005 (χ 2 = 49.25, df = 767)
Range 21–64 20–72 40–72 20–60
STAI score at follow-up Mean 45.0 ± 11.2 34.0 ± 9.5 < 0.0005 (t = 11.01, df = 142) 48.6 ± 6.5 31.0 ± 6.9 < 0.0005 (t = 49.19, df = 710)
Range 21–79 20–73 40–79 20–64
IES-R score Mean 17.0 ± 17.9 2.3 ± 4.2 < 0.0005 (t = 9.32, df = 130) 5.0 ± 8.4 2.8 ± 6.6 < 0.0005 (t = 5.0; df = 580)
Range 0–60 0–23 0–45 0–60
Income ($) Mean 100,000–149,999 100,000–149,999 0.001 (Z=−3.22) 100,000–149,999 100,000–149,999 0.010 (Z = −2.58)
Modal 50,000–99,999 100,000–149,999 100,000–149,999 100,000–149,999
Graduation from 4-year college (%) 74.6 80.5 0.101 (χ 2 = 2.69, df = 1) 80.6 78.7 0.377 (χ 2 = 0.79, df = 1)
Ethnicity (% white) 83.8 84.2 0.911 (χ 2 = 0.01, df = 1) 82.4 84.7 0.242 (χ 2 = 1.37, df=1)
Recommend to (% yes)Δ Family 77.1 82.1 0.191 (χ 2 = 1.71, df = 1) 81.7 82 0.901 (χ 2 = 0.02, df = 1)
Others 79.8 83.7 0.294 (χ 2 = 1.10, df = 1) 84.2 83.2 0.651 (χ 2 = 0.21, df = 1)
Discuss results with (% yes) PCP 27.7 26.4 0.752 (χ 2 = 0.10, df = 1) 23.7 27.3 0.132 (χ 2= 2.27, df = 1)
GC 13.8 10.1 0.177 (χ 2 = 1.82, df = 1) 7.9 11 0.058 (χ 2 = 3.60, df = 1)
Mean follow-up interval (months) 5.7 5.6 0.742 (t = 0.33, df = 2035) 5.8 5.6 0.198 (t = 1.29, df = 639)

*Plus-minus values are means ± SD. p values for comparisons of gender, have children, subject of prior GT, education, ethnicity, recommend to family/others, and discuss results with PCP/GC were calculated by means of Pearson chi-square; those for age, scores on STAI and IES, and mean follow-up interval by means of independent samples t test; and those for income and education by means of the Mann-Whitney U test

†Ethnicity was self-reported

Δ Based on survey question: would you recommend that (your family members/other people you know) undergo genetic risk assessment?

‡Based upon survey questions: Did you share the results of your genetic test with your physician or healthcare provider? Did you speak with a Navigenics certified genetic counselor about your genetic test results?

In both analyses, logistic regression analysis of demographic characteristics (gender, age, having biological children, ethnicity (Caucasian or not), income category, highest level of education achieved) was not found to yield an effective predictive model. For psychologically sensitive subjects, however, younger age emerged as a predictor (p < 0.0005).

Pre-test concerns about participating in study

In the baseline survey, subjects were asked whether they had any concerns about participating in the study. Specifically, they could select tickboxes for concerns related to learning about their disease risk, to not knowing how they would feel about their results, to the quality and reliability of the testing lab and results, and to potential privacy issues about their data. A prior publication reported (Bloss et al. 2010) that approximately half of all overall participants indicated the statement that they had no concerns related to participation. In the current study, we found that the psychologically sensitive cases were significantly more likely to indicate concerns related to learning about their disease risk (p < 0.0005, χ 2 = 21.62, df = 1), not knowing how they would feel about their results (p < 0.0005, χ 2 = 16.32, df = 1), the quality and reliability of the testing lab and results (p = 0.063, χ 2 = 3.46, df = 1), and potential privacy issues about their data (p = 0.003, χ 2 = 8.55, df = 1) (Table 2). The subjects who demonstrated a distress response to receipt of their results were not significantly more likely to indicate any of the listed pre-test concerns.

Table 2.

Concerns about participating in study reported at baseline*†

Variable “Distress response” “Psychologically sensitive”
Case (N = 130) Control (N = 1907) p value Case (N = 431) Control (N = 1606) p value
I do not have concerns 45.4 49.7 0.365 (χ 2 = 0.89, df = 1) 39.4 52.1 < 0.0005 (χ 2 = 21.62, df = 1)
Concerns related to learning about disease risk 13.1 12.4 0.828 (χ 2 = 0.05, df = 1) 19.7 10.5 < 0.0005 (χ 2 = 26.34, df = 1)
Concerns related to not knowing how I will feel about my results 15.4 16.4 0.759 (χ 2 = 0.09, df = 1) 22.7 14.6 < 0.0005 (χ 2 = 16.32, df = 1)
Concerns related to the quality and reliability of the testing lab and results 14.6 15 0.893 (χ 2 = 0.02, df = 1) 17.9 14.3 0.063 (χ 2 = 3.46, df = 1)
Concerns related to potential privacy issues about my data 40.8 36.9 0.373 (χ 2 = 0.80, df = 1) 43.2 35.5 0.003 (χ 2 = 8.55, df = 1)

*p values were calculated by means of Pearson chi-square

†Based on survey question: Do you have concerns about participating in this initiative? Please check all that apply

Genomic risk results

There were no significant differences detected in either the total number of conditions for which each subject was found to be at “orange” elevated genomic risk or the total or average estimated lifetime risk value given for each of the conditions between cases and controls for either comparison group. The “distress response” case group trended towards a slightly higher total (p = 0.061, t = 1.88, df = 2035) and average (p = 0.123, t = 1.54, df = 2035) estimated lifetime risk values, though there was limited power in these analyses (0.456 and 0.414, respectively; Table 3).

Table 3.

Comparisons of mean (± standard deviation, minimum-maximum) values for numbers of “orange” conditions, total estimated lifetime risks (ELTR), and average estimated lifetime risks (ALTR) for the set of conditions*

Variable “Distress response” “Psychologically sensitive”
Case (N = 130) Control (N = 1907) p value Case (N = 431) Control (N = 1606) p value
Orange 7.4 ± 2.5 (3–17) 7.4 ± 2.3 (0–16) 0.823 (t = −0.22, df = 2035) 7.4 ± 2.1 (0–16) 7.4 ± 2.3 (2–17) 0.858 (t = 0.18, df = 2035)
Total ELTR† 229.7 ± 28.7 (100–308) 225.0 ± 27.7 (3–346) 0.061 (t = 1.88, df = 2035) 225.5 ± 25.8 (3–346) 225.3 ± 28.3 (113–341) 0.882 (t = 0.15, df = 2035)
Average ELTR‡ 10.6 ± 1.2 (8–14) 10.3 ± 1.6 (3–53) 0.123 (t = 1.54, df = 2035) 10.4 ± 1.1 (3–53) 10.4 ± 1.6 (113–341) 0.857 (t = −0.18, df = 2035)

*p values were calculated by means of independent samples t test

†The total estimated lifetime risk (ELTR) is a total of the risk estimates (percentage) for all of the conditions

‡The average estimated lifetime risk (ELTR) is an average of the risk estimates (percentage) for all of the conditions

We assessed various combinations of personal and/or family history for conditions as they may relate to psychological experience and significant trends emerged for two conditions. For Alzheimer’s disease, “psychologically sensitive” cases were significantly more likely to have a positive personal or family history than controls (28.1% of cases, 23% of controls; p = 0.029, χ 2 = 4.78, df = 1) (Table 4). Additionally, “psychologically sensitive” cases were significantly more likely to have a positive family history plus personal “orange” elevated risk than controls (28.1% of cases, 23.1% of controls; p = 0.032, χ 2 = 4.59, df = 1) (Table 5). For restless leg syndrome, both the “distress response” and “psychologically sensitive” case groups were more likely to have a personal or family history of the syndrome (“distress response”: 17.7% of cases, 11.4% of controls; p = 0.033, χ 2 = 4.55, df = 1; “psychologically sensitive”: 14.4% of cases, 11.2% of controls; p = 0.063, χ 2 = 3.47, df = 1) (Tables 4 and 5).

Table 4.

Percentage of subjects with a positive personal or family history of the listed condition*

Variable “Distress response” “Psychologically sensitive”
Case (N = 130) Control (N = 1907) p value Case (N = 431) Control (N = 1606) p value
Heart attack 53.8 58.8 0.264 (χ 2 = 1.25, df = 1) 57.8 58.7 0.724 (χ 2 = 0.13, df = 1)
Abnormal heart rhythm 30.8 31.2 0.918 (χ 2 = 0.01, df = 1) 30.2 31.4 0.610 (χ 2 = 0.26, df = 1)
Aortic aneurysm 6.2 6.8 0.771 (χ 2 = 0.09, df = 1) 7.4 6.6 0.545 (χ 2 = 0.37, df = 1)
Prostate cancer 23.4 18.9 0.209 (χ 2 = 1.58, df = 1) 17.9 19.6 0.427 (χ 2 = 0.63, df = 1)
Breast cancer 27.9 29.1 0.769 (χ 2 = 0.09, df = 1) 27 29.6 0.288 (χ 2 = 1.13, df = 1)
Lung cancer 19.2 24 0.219 (χ 2 = 1.51, df = 1) 21.6 24.2 0.251 (χ 2 = 1.32, df = 1)
Colon cancer 25.4 20.2 0.155 (χ 2 = 2.02, df = 1) 19.5 20.8 0.554 (χ 2 = 0.35, df = 1)
Melanoma 12.3 15.6 0.312 (χ 2 = 1.02, df = 1) 15.7 15.3 0.873 (χ 2 = 0.03, df = 1)
Alzheimer’s disease 20.8 24.3 0.362 (χ 2 = 0.83, df = 1) 28.1 23 0.029 (χ 2 = 4.78, df = 1)
Restless leg syndrome 17.7 11.4 0.033 (χ 2 = 4.55, df = 1) 14.4 11.2 0.063 (χ 2 = 3.47, df = 1)
Multiple sclerosis 6.2 3.6 0.140 (χ 2 = 2.18, df = 1) 3.7 4.2 0.626 (χ 2 = 0.24, df = 1)
Crohn’s disease 3.8 2.9 0.530 (χ 2 = 0.39, df = 1) 3.5 2.8 0.460 (χ 2 = 0.55, df = 1)
Lupus 3.1 3.6 0.770 (χ 2 = 0.09, df = 1) 2.6 3.8 0.214 (χ 2 = 1.55, df = 1)
Thyroid disease 23.8 27.4 0.382 (χ 2 = 0.77, df = 1) 29.9 26.4 0.143 (χ 2 = 2.14, df = 1)
Glaucoma 23.8 22 0.628 (χ 2 = 0.23, df = 1) 23.4 21.8 0.466 (χ 2 = 0.53, df = 1)
Diabetes, type 2 43.8 49.6 0.208 (χ 2 = 1.59, df = 1) 51.7 48.5 0.233 (χ 2 = 1.42, df = 1)

*p values calculated by means of Pearson chi-square. Sixteen (as opposed to 24) conditions were assessed here because personal and family history of all of the NHC conditions were not assessed

Table 5.

Percentage of subjects with a positive family history and “orange” (elevated) estimated lifetime risk of the listed condition*

Variable “Distress response” “Psychologically sensitive”
Case (N = 130) Control (N = 1907) p value Case (N = 431) Control (N = 1606) p value
Heart attack 34.6 40.8 0.165 (χ 2 = 1.93, df = 1) 39.4 40.7 0.648 (χ 2 = 0.21, df = 1)
Abnormal heart rhythm 25.4 26 0.885 (χ 2 = 0.02, df = 1) 23.9 26.5 0.281 (χ 2 = 1.17, df = 1)
Aortic aneurysm 5.4 6.8 0.528 (χ 2 = 0.40, df = 1) 7.2 6.6 0.663 (χ 2 = 0.19, df = 1)
Prostate cancer 22.3 18.6 0.290 (χ 2 = 1.12, df = 1) 17.9 19.1 0.575 (χ 2 = 0.31, df = 1)
Breast cancer 26.9 28.1 0.771 (χ 2 = 0.09, df = 1) 26.2 28.5 0.345 (χ 2 = 0.90, df = 1)
Lung cancer 19.2 23.9 0.229 (χ 2 = 1.45, df = 1) 21.6 24.1 0.274 (χ 2 = 1.20, df = 1)
Colon cancer 25.4 19.9 0.134 (χ 2 = 2.24, df = 1) 19.5 20.5 0.648 (χ 2 = 0.21, df = 1)
Alzheimer’s disease 20.8 24.4 0.352 (χ 2 = 0.87, df = 1) 28.1 23.1 0.032 (χ 2 = 4.59, df = 1)
Restless leg syndrome 9.2 8.1 0.641 (χ2 = 0.22, df = 1) 9 7.9 0.442 (χ 2 = 0.59, df = 1)
Multiple sclerosis 4.6 3.4 0.446 (χ 2 = 0.58, df = 1) 3.7 3.4 0.723 (χ 2 = 0.13, df = 1)
Crohn’s disease 2.3 2.7 0.801 (χ 2 = 0.06, df = 1) 3.5 2.4 0.227 (χ 2 = 1.46, df = 1)
Lupus 3.1 3.2 0.939 (χ 2 = 0.01, df = 1) 2.1 3.1 0.142 (χ 2 = 2.15, df = 1)
Glaucoma 22.3 21.3 0.795 (χ 2 = 0.07, df = 1) 23.2 20.9 0.305 (χ 2 = 1.05, df = 1)
Diabetes, type 2 43.8 49 0.257 (χ 2 = 1.28, df = 1) 51.3 47.9 0.219 (χ 2 = 1.51, df = 1)

*p values calculated by means of Pearson chi-square

Reactions to receipt of genomic risk results

There were no differences with regard to communicating about results with healthcare providers (26.5% of the total population shared results with a primary healthcare provider and 10.4% spoke with a Navigenics certified genetic counselor) between case and control groups for either the distress response or psychologically sensitive group (Table 1). Table 6 shows how subjects in each group responded to the question of whether they experienced any changes in their lives as a result of receiving their genomic risk results. Subjects with a psychological distress response reported significant changes in the way they thought of themselves as a result of receiving their risk assessment compared to controls (p = 0.032), and both subjects with a psychological distress response and those who were psychologically sensitive reported significant changes in their emotions (p < 0.0005 and p = 0.049, respectively). Of specific relevance, both case groups reported being significantly more concerned about their health at follow-up than their respective control groups (p = 0.011, Z = −2.59 and p = 0.001, Z = −3.46, respectively) (Table 7).

Table 6.

Percent of subjects’ who reported life changes since receipt of test results*†

Variable “Distress response” “Psychologically sensitive”
Case (N = 130) Control (N = 1907) p value Case (N = 431) Control (N = 1606) p value
Body image 4.6 8.3 0.181 10.2 7.5 0.074
Childbearing decisions 0.8 0.6 0.577 0.9 0.6 0.492
Emotions 11.5 3.4 < 0.0005 5.6 3.4 0.049
The way I think of myself 22.3 14.9 0.032 18.1 14.7 0.084
Relationship with partner 5.4 2.6 0.086 3.9 2.4 0.097
Relationship with children 2.3 1.7 0.486 1.9 1.7 0.835
Relationship with other relatives 3.1 1.8 0.301 2.6 1.7 0.232
Employment 2.3 0.7 0.077 1.6 0.6 0.057
Insurance 3.8 1.5 0.055 2.8 1.3 0.049
Other 10.8 9.1 0.529 10 9 0.512
None 58.5 69 0.015 61.7 79 0.001

*p values were calculated by means of Fisher’s exact test

†Based on survey question: What changes, if any, have occurred in your life as a result of receiving your genetic test results? Each variable listed was represented as an independent box to check or leave unchecked. Unfortunately due to the limitation of existing survey data, qualitative data elaborating on what specific changes (or direction of changes) occurred is not available

Table 7.

Comparisons between subjects reported concern about their health after receipt of test results*

Variable “Distress response” “Psychologically sensitive”
Case (N = 130) Control (N = 1907) p value Case (N = 431) Control (N = 1606) p value
Significantly or somewhat less concerned about my health (%) 18.9 20.8 0.011 (Z = −2.59) 16.5 21.8 0.001 (Z = −3.46)
As concerned about my health (%) 38.5 50.8 47.9 50.6
Significantly or somewhat more concerned about my health (%) 42.6 28.4 35.6 27.5

*p values were calculated by means of Mann-Whitney U test. Those who selected the answer “Less concerned about my general health, but more concerned about one disease” were omitted from this analysis as this response did not clearly fit into a category

Discussion

This study performed a secondary analysis to characterize individuals with adverse psychological experiences surrounding genomic risk assessment for a set of 23 common, complex conditions and identify predictors of such reactions by a secondary analysis of data from the Scripps Genomic Health Initiative (SGHI). Of the 2037 individuals studied, 130 (6.4%) experienced adverse psychological reactions to receipt of their test results using one of two definitions. This relatively small percentage of subjects with adverse psychosocial response is consistent with other similar reports in the literature around the receipt of low-penetrance genomic data (Francke et al. 2013; Egglestone et al. 2013). Our study was unable to find any demographic factors that predicted who would have an adverse psychological response aside from those participants who self-indicated pre-participation concerns. Further, the only medical conditions that were predictive of clinically significant psychological distress or psychological sensitivity were having a personal or family history of Alzheimer’s disease or restless leg syndrome, and it remains unclear if this is a clinically meaningful discovery.

Though we were unable to identify many significant predictors of either clinically meaningful anxiety and/or distress responses to receipt of results or heightened anxiety both before and after receipt of results indicating psychological sensitivity, our analyses have provided some insights into our study population. First, we were able to differentiate two types of patients who experience distress after genomic testing—one population who experiences a change in distress and one that experiences distress both before and after testing, who we refer to as the psychologically sensitive patients. These psychologically sensitive patients indicated a significantly higher frequency of pre-test concerns about participating in the SGHI than their corresponding control group in all but concerns related to quality and reliability. In contrast, individuals with a distress response indicated fewer pre-test concerns. It is possible that the distress response individuals may have been less effective at pre-test identification of which aspects of the SGHI may have ultimately lead them to experience a distress response.

One especially interesting aspect of these data is the fact that only 15% (306/2037) of the total study population indicated concerns related to quality and reliability of the DTC lab and their results. This is in contrast to the concerns that have been documented in the literature (Ng et al. 2009; Mathews et al. 2012; Annas and Elias 2014). Given that the study population had a high level of education and may have greater-than-average scientific acumen compared to the general population (Roberts and Ostergren 2013), the general population may be less able to understand potential limitations of this type of data and potentially more likely to overestimate the meaning this information has to their lives. This may be one point in support of the argument that discussion of these types of results with a genetics specialist who is equipped to provide context for interpretation could serve to increase the likelihood of appropriate understanding of results. Overall, there are limited data on how people incorporate this type of moderate and low predictive genomic information into their lives and whether or not it is likely to present clinically relevant psychological risks to them in the long term.

Limitations

This study is limited by the use of an existing sample for the study population. These participants are largely Caucasian, highly-educated, and of high socioeconomic status, with likely high medical and/or scientific literacy and comfort. Thus, caution should be taken in generalizing this analysis to the broader population of those who may undergo genomic testing for common, complex conditions in the future. These features may both explain the relatively low rate of adverse psychosocial outcomes in the original SGHI study and our difficulty in identifying predictors towards it. Additionally, 44% of subjects who completed the baseline survey and underwent genomic testing did not complete the follow-up survey and thus were not analyzed. As a result, it is possible a segment of individuals with a distress response or high anxiety surrounding the time of testing dropped out of follow-up, which would lead to an underestimate of the individuals with distress responses.

Critical to interpretation of DTC testing data is that these studies most likely include a biased population of early-adopter individuals who are self-selecting to receive such data. As has been noted in predictive testing for Huntington disease (Codori et al. 1994) and other Mendelian highly penetrant conditions, early adopters are likely self-selected individuals who may be better prepared to both understand and emotionally incorporate such results into their self-concept, and thus, these studies may potentially underestimate the psychosocial impact that may be noted if such genomic testing becomes more widespread in the future.

Finally, as noted in the methods and results section, the sample size, particularly for the “distress response” group, was too small to have enough statistical power to detect the ideal effect sizes at the recommended 0.80 level (Cohen 1992). Thus, it is possible that a larger sample size would demonstrate statistically significant differences and thus, further study of larger samples would be useful.

Conclusions

DTC genomic risk assessment has been a subject of much discussion and study over the past several years, and it is still to be determined how much of personalized genomics will primarily utilize the DTC format (for example through “apps”) as compared to a healthcare system delivery model as the primary manner in which genomic information is provided to patients and consumers in the future (Annas and Elias 2014; Allison 2012). There are also changing trends with regard to the role of pre-test genetic counseling, with ClinGen’s Consent and Disclosure of Results (CADRe) workgroup developing a rubric to assess which genomic tests warrant a more traditional genetic counseling pre-test approach as compared to a targeted discussion or even static educational materials (personal communication). Although we identified only a small number of individuals with a psychological distress response, our inability to identify meaningful predictors of such adverse psychological experiences surrounding genomic risk assessment serves to emphasize the difficulty in easily assessing who may have a post-test distress response (Dohany et al. 2012; Matloff and Caplan 2008). Our study’s key finding suggests that a subset of patients receiving genomic risk information can be described as psychologically sensitive and may be at risk for adverse psychological outcomes after results disclosure. As such, it is important to identify ways to best serve these patients before they receive genetic testing results. It should be acknowledged that there are likely both patient-specific and result-specific risk factors that may help predict psychological distress and be used to identify candidate patients who stand to benefit from special attention. At a minimum, we encourage both healthcare providers and DTC companies developing future approaches to find ways to assess psychological distress and sensitivity before testing and to encourage more extensive consideration and pre-test discussion in those patients identified as being psychologically sensitive (Shiloh et al. 2013; Boeldt et al. 2015). Genetic counselors, psychologists, social workers, and other mental health professionals may be useful in providing such services.

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank Atul Butte for his contributions as a member of the research committee during the first author’s graduate training.

Compliance with ethical standards

Conflict of interest

Kelly Ormond was a paid consultant for Navigenics in 2007–2009, during which time the original SGHI data was obtained, but she was not involved in the creation or oversight of the original Bloss et al. (2011) study or its data in any way.

Footnotes

This project was completed in fulfillment of the first author’s graduate degree requirements and was presented at the EMPAG meeting in 2014.

References

  1. Allison M. Direct-to-consumer genomics reinvents itself. Nat Biotechnol. 2012;30:1027–1029. doi: 10.1038/nbt.2409. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. American College of Medical Genetics Direct-to-consumer genetic testing: a revised position statement of the American College of Medical Genetics and Genomics. Genet Med. 2016;18(2):207–208. doi: 10.1038/gim.2015.190. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Annas GJ, Elias S. 23andMe and the FDA. N Engl J Med. 2014;370:985–988. doi: 10.1056/NEJMp1316367. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Annes JP, Giovanni MA, Murray MF. Risks of presymptomatic direct-to-consumer genetic testing. N Engl J Med. 2010;363:1100–1101. doi: 10.1056/NEJMp1006029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bloss CS, Ornowski L, Silver E, et al. Consumer perceptions of direct-to-consumer personalized genomic risk assessments. Genet Med. 2010;12:556–566. doi: 10.1097/GIM.0b013e3181eb51c6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bloss CS, Schork NJ, Topol EJ. Effect of direct-to-consumer genomewide profiling to assess disease risk. N Engl J Med. 2011;364:524–534. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa1011893. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Boeldt DL, Schork NJ, Topol EJ, Bloss CS. Influence of individual differences in disease perception on consumer response to direct-to-consumer genomic testing. Clin Genet. 2015;87(3):225–232. doi: 10.1111/cge.12419. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Codori AM, Hanson R, Brandt J. Self-selection in predictive testing for Huntington’s disease. Am J Med Genet. 1994;54(3):167–173. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.1320540303. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Cohen J. A power primer. Psychol Bull. 1992;112:155–159. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.112.1.155. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Creamer M, Bell R, Failla S. Psychometric properties of the Impact of Event Scale—Revised. Behav Res Ther. 2003;41:1489–1496. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2003.07.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Dohany L, Gustafson S, Ducaine W, Zakalik D. Psychological distress with direct-to-consumer genetic testing: a case report of an unexpected BRCA positive test result. J Genet Couns. 2012;21:399–401. doi: 10.1007/s10897-011-9475-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Egglestone C, Morris A, O’Brien A. Effect of direct-to-consumer genetic tests on health behavior and anxiety: a survey of consumers and potential consumers. J Genet Couns. 2013;22:565–575. doi: 10.1007/s10897-013-9582-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Faul F, Erdfelder E, Buchner A, Lang AG. Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behav Res Methods. 2009;41:1149–1160. doi: 10.3758/BRM.41.4.1149. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Francke U, Dijamco C, Kiefer AK, et al. Dealing with the unexpected: consumer responses to direct-access BRCA mutation testing. PeerJ. 2013;1:e8. doi: 10.7717/peerj.8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Gordon ES, Griffin G, Wawak L, Pang H, Gollust SE, Bernhardt BA. “It’s not like judgement day”: public understanding of and reactions to personalized genomic risk information. J Genet Couns. 2013;21:423–432. doi: 10.1007/s10897-011-9476-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Jacobson NS, Truax P. Clinical significance: a statistical approach to defining meaningful change in psychotherapy research. J Consult Clin Psychol. 1991;59:12–19. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.59.1.12. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Kaufman DJ, Bollinger JM, Dvoskin RL, Scott JA. Risky business: risk perception and the use of medical services among customers of DTC personal genetic testing. J Genet Couns. 2012;21:413–422. doi: 10.1007/s10897-012-9483-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Mathews R, Hall W, Carter A. Direct-to-consumer genetic testing for addiction susceptibility: a premature commercialisation of doubtful validity and value. Addiction. 2012;107:2069–2074. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2012.03836.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Matloff E, Caplan A. Direct to confusion: lessons learned from marketing BRCA testing. Am J Bioeth. 2008;8:5–8. doi: 10.1080/15265160802248179. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. McBride CM, Wade CH, Kaphingst KA. Consumers’ views of direct-to-consumer genetic information. Annu Rev Genomics Hum Genet. 2010;11:427–446. doi: 10.1146/annurev-genom-082509-141604. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. McGuire AL, Burke W. An unwelcome side effect of direct-to-consumer personal genome testing: raiding the medical commons. JAMA. 2008;300:2669–2671. doi: 10.1001/jama.2008.803. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Ng PC, Murray SS, Levy S, Venter JC. An agenda for personalized medicine. Nature. 2009;8(461):724–726. doi: 10.1038/461724a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Roberts JS, Ostergren J. Direct-to-consumer genetic testing and personal genomics services: a review of recent empirical studies. Curr Genet Med Rep. 2013;1:182–200. doi: 10.1007/s40142-013-0018-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Shiloh S, Wade CH, Roberts JS, Alford SH, Biesecker BB. Associations between risk perceptions and worry about common diseases: a between- and within-subjects examination. Psychol Health. 2013;28(4):434–449. doi: 10.1080/08870446.2012.737464. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Skirton H, Goldsmith L, Jackson L, O’Connor A. Direct to consumer genetic testing: a systematic review of position statements, policies and recommendations. Clin Genet. 2012;82:210–218. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-0004.2012.01863.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Spielberger CD, Gorsuch RL, Lushene PR, Vagg PR, Jacobs AG. Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (form Y) Palo Alto: Consulting Psychologists Press; 1983. [Google Scholar]
  27. Stack CB, Gharani N, Gordon ES, Schmidlen T, Christman MF, Keller MA. Genetic risk estimation in the Coriell Personalized Medicine Collaborative. Genet Med. 2011;13:131–139. doi: 10.1097/GIM.0b013e318201164c. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Weiss DS, Marmar CR. The Impact of Event Scale—Revised. In: Wilson JP, Keane TM, editors. Assessing psychological trauma and PTSD. New York: Guilford Press; 1997. pp. 399–411. [Google Scholar]

Articles from Journal of Community Genetics are provided here courtesy of Springer

RESOURCES