Financial incentives (FI)—in which money is given to an individual in return for a certain action—have been highly effective in motivating a variety of health-related behaviors and HIV interventions, including HIV prevention [1, 2], testing [3–6], linkage to care and retention [7, 8], and treatment adherence [9–11]. Despite their increasing use in research and programs, there are ethical concerns, including the potential for coercion and unintended social harm [12, 13]. Using FI to motivate pediatric HIV testing raises special ethical concerns in this vulnerable population, particularly because the incentives are provided to caregivers who take children for testing.
Background and Rationale
Pediatric HIV testing and treatment maximizes survival and reduces morbidity [14–16]. However, there are programmatic gaps in the testing cascade that result in late diagnosis and preventable deaths [17]. Index-case testing strategies targeting HIV-infected parents receiving care increase uptake of pediatric HIV testing [18–20]; however, the majority still opt not take their child for testing, due fear, misconception of risk, and financial barriers [18, 21]. FI may be an effective intervention to incentivize HIV-infected parents to test their children.
The Financial Incentives to Increase Pediatric HIV Testing (FIT) Pilot Study (NCT02931422) tested the effect of small FI (USD 5–15) to motivate HIV-infected caregivers to test their children. Eligible caregivers were randomized to one of three FI values at recruitment, which would be given to the caregiver upon return for child testing (manuscript under review). The one adverse event recorded during the study was categorized as a social harm; an 11-year-old girl ran away from home temporarily to avoid being tested for HIV. In addition to social harms, the study team attempted to assess the potential for coercion and unintended harm using a structured questionnaire. Analyses revealed discordance between questions assessing coercion or a lack of voluntariness (“Did the offer of money make you feel like you had no choice, you had to test your child?”) and intention to test (“When would you have tested your children without the incentive?”). Specifically, parents who stated they would have tested their children for HIV even without FI also indicated that they felt the incentive gave them no choice but to test, potentially suggesting FI were ethically problematic because they were coercive. A literature review of other FI studies revealed no agreement how best to measure or report coercion [1, 22]. The study team thus engaged the Treuman Katz Center for Pediatric Bioethics at Seattle Children’s Hospital to provide guidance in how to assess coercion and other ethical considerations related to FI.
Ethical analysis
Concerns about the use of FI are often framed by researchers, ethicists, and ethics review committees in terms of 1) coercion, 2) undue inducement, and 3) lack of voluntariness. However, these concepts are often used without precision and clarity. Additionally, most existing scholarship focuses on the ethics of FI for research participation, rather than the use of FI to promote behavior change to improve health outcomes. We will argue that, in the context of FI use for behavior change, researchers should not focus on misplaced concerns about coercion or undue inducement, but try to measure the impact of FI on voluntariness or the potential for social harms.
Coercion
Although most IRBs consider incentives coercive if a person feels they have no choice but to perform the incentivized behavior, this is based on widespread confusion about the concept of coercion [12]. The influential Belmont Report states that “coercion occurs when an overt threat of harm is intentionally presented by one person to another to obtain compliance,” [23] suggesting that there are two important features of coercion: (1) it is morally wrong, and (2) it requires an intentional threat of harm. Scholars have extended this definition to note that threats to violate someone’s rights if they do not comply also count as coercive [24].
Some argue that financial incentives offered to people living in poverty are coercive if individuals have no reasonable alternative but to do what is necessary to be paid. However, Wertheimer and Miller describe the case of a woman who is diagnosed with breast cancer and is offered urgent, life-saving surgery; there is nothing morally wrong about the offer, despite the fact that the patient has no reasonable alternative. They argue that “genuine offers cannot coerce” [24].
For pediatric HIV testing, however, it is possible that parents interested in financial incentives will force their children to be tested or impose consequences if they refuse. Parents generally have the authority to require their children to engage in activities that are intended to benefit their children, ranging from brushing their teeth to staying in school. This suggests that, in general, there is not necessarily anything wrong with parents requiring their children to take an HIV test, provided it does not go beyond the limits of acceptable treatment of children (such as resorting to abuse or threats of violence). Incentives at such low levels as offered in the FIT study seem insufficient to motivate parents to mistreat their children. Nevertheless, measuring whether and how often social harms occur is important to inform future implementation.
Undue inducement
Incentives or inducements are widespread and well-accepted; people generally receive compensation for employment, for instance. Undue inducement is problematic and can occur in research when financial incentives are high enough to tempt participants to ignore high risks or make decisions contrary to their core values [25]. Determining whether an incentive is unduly large varies by context and circumstance. Perhaps the most prominent model in the literature for determining whether payments are undue inducements is the ‘Wage-Payment Model’, which concludes that payments that are similar to what an individual would otherwise earn for similar work and the amount of time required are not undue [25]. However, pediatric HIV testing and treatment offers substantial, proven benefits for the children involved, and few risks. Additionally, the incentives being offered ranged between $5–$15, the highest level representing an estimated cost of transportation and two days’ lost wages. Testing a child typically takes a half day of time, requiring a parent to skip a half or full day or work—or only slightly more than the Wage-Payment model would require. In this population, mean monthly income was $83 (median [IQR]: $48 [$15–$100]). Given that these incentives are not unduly large by comparison to what individuals could earn otherwise, it is unlikely that parents would ignore risks or violate their deeply-held values because of these payments [26]. In fact, given the low risks and significant potential for benefit involved, higher incentives could also be ethically appropriate. This is because financial incentives to promote healthy behaviors can be autonomy-enhancing, in that they help people to “bring about a personal change they might already desire, through a means with which they are already familiar” by eliminating financial or other barriers to action [27]. Arguably, an even stronger case can be made that financial incentives to promote healthy behaviors are welfare-enhancing, which is the appropriate ethical and legal focus of parents making decisions on behalf of their children.
Voluntariness
Coercion and undue inducement are two types of interference with a person’s ability to make a voluntary decision, but there are other, more subtle influences on decision-making. For instance, a person might have the perception that they are being coerced, and even if that perception is inaccurate, it may influence their decision nonetheless. Although coercion and undue influence are not significant ethical concerns in this context, ensuring voluntariness is more complicated. First, it is hard to know what counts as sufficiently voluntary, since there are many different influences that can affect day-to-day decision-making [28]. Few, if any, decisions are completely voluntary. Second, measuring voluntariness is challenging. Previous studies have sought to measure voluntariness by determining whether some individuals choose not to participate, whether participants report feeling pressure to participate in a study, and whether participants know they could refuse participation or withdraw; however, each of these approaches has limitations. Some individuals may choose not to participate at the same time that other individuals feel their decision was not their own. In other studies, although some research participants have reported feeling pressure to participate in research, when asked about the source of the pressure, it comes from their child’s illness, not the study team [29]. Finally, some research participants who reported knowing they could withdraw also indicated a belief that the clinic would not let them quit [30]. This suggests that questions seeking to measure voluntariness should be carefully crafted to determine whether pressure felt by study participants could be attributed to a specific and ethically problematic source.
Summary
Coercion and undue inducement are not significant ethical concerns in offering FI for pediatric HIV testing, provided FI are not so large as to exert undue influence and the interventions are beneficial and not excessively risky. The authors are currently working to measure voluntariness in the uptake of FI and social harms that may result from their use, which are challenging issues that merit further attention.
Acknowledgments
Funding: This publication was made possible with support from a Center for AIDS Research (CFAR) International Pilot Award, which is supported through the National Institutes of Health (NIH) award P30AI027757, and by the Collaborative Initiative for Pediatric HIV Education and Research (CIPHER) International AIDS Society (323-NJU-TRIAL). Additional support was provided by the UW Global Center for Integrated Health of Women, Adolescents and Children (Global WACh), the University of Washington CFAR (P30 AI027757), and the University of Washington Institute for Translational Health Sciences (ITHS) UL1 TR002319. This publication was supported in part by the National Institute of Child Health and Development (NICHD) F32HD088204 to ADW and the Fogarty International Center (FIC) R25 TW009345 and D43TW009783 to ADW and INN, respectively.
The Bioethics Consultation Service, part of the Treuman Katz Center for Pediatric Bioethics, is supported by the National Center For Advancing Translational Sciences of the National Institutes of Health (NIH) under award number UL1 TR002319.
We would like to acknowledge the FIT Study Clinic Staff in Kisumu (Pamela Agola, Lukio Agalo), the UW-Kenya operations staff, Kenya Pediatric Research Consortium (KEPRECON) in Nairobi and Collaborative Initiative for Pediatric HIV Education and Research (CIPHER) staff for their support on this project. We thank the Kizazi Working Group and Global Center for Integrated Health of Women, Adolescents and Children (Global WACh) for comments and insights provided during study design and manuscript development. Most of all, we thank the women and children who participated in the study.
Footnotes
Author contributions: ADW, SS, KMP, INN, JAS conceptualized the article. All authors read the manuscript draft, provided feedback and approved the final submitted manuscript.
The content of this paper is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the NIH. The funding sources were not involved in the analyses or interpretation of data. None of the authors was paid to write this article by a pharmaceutical company or other agency. The authors have no conflicts of interests to declare.
References
- 1.Pettifor A, MacPhail C, Nguyen N, Rosenberg M. Can money prevent the spread of HIV? A review of cash payments for HIV prevention. AIDS Behav. 2012;16:1729–1738. doi: 10.1007/s10461-012-0240-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Baird SJ, Garfein RS, McIntosh CT, Ozler B. Effect of a cash transfer programme for schooling on prevalence of HIV and herpes simplex type 2 in Malawi: a cluster randomised trial. Lancet. 2012;379:1320–1329. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(11)61709-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Nglazi MD, van Schaik N, Kranzer K, Lawn SD, Wood R, Bekker LG. An incentivized HIV counseling and testing program targeting hard-to-reach unemployed men in Cape Town, South Africa. J Acquir Immune Defic Syndr. 2012;59:e28–34. doi: 10.1097/QAI.0b013e31824445f0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Thornton RL. The Demand for, and Impact of, Learning HIV Status. Am Econ Rev. 2008;98:1829–1863. doi: 10.1257/aer.98.5.1829. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.McCoy SI, Shiu K, Martz TE, Smith CD, Mattox L, Gluth DR, et al. Improving the efficiency of HIV testing with peer recruitment, financial incentives, and the involvement of persons living with HIV infection. J Acquir Immune Defic Syndr. 2013;63:e56–63. doi: 10.1097/QAI.0b013e31828a7629. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Haukoos JS, Witt MD, Coil CJ, Lewis RJ. The effect of financial incentives on adherence with outpatient human immunodeficiency virus testing referrals from the emergency department. Acad Emerg Med. 2005;12:617–621. doi: 10.1197/j.aem.2005.02.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Solomon SS, Srikrishnan AK, Vasudevan CK, Anand S, Kumar MS, Balakrishnan P, et al. Voucher incentives improve linkage to and retention in care among HIV-infected drug users in Chennai, India. Clin Infect Dis. 2014;59:589–595. doi: 10.1093/cid/ciu324. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Yotebieng M, Thirumurthy H, Moracco KE, Kawende B, Chalachala JL, Wenzi LK, et al. Conditional cash transfers and uptake of and retention in prevention of mother-to-child HIV transmission care: a randomised controlled trial. Lancet HIV. 2016;3:e85–93. doi: 10.1016/S2352-3018(15)00247-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Linnemayr S, Stecher C, Mukasa B. Behavioral economic incentives to improve adherence to antiretrovirals: early evidence from a randomized controlled trial in Uganda. AIDS. 2017 doi: 10.1097/QAD.0000000000001387. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Javanbakht M, Prosser P, Grimes T, Weinstein M, Farthing C. Efficacy of an individualized adherence support program with contingent reinforcement among nonadherent HIV-positive patients: results from a randomized trial. J Int Assoc Physicians AIDS Care (Chic) 2006;5:143–150. doi: 10.1177/1545109706291706. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Rigsby MO, Rosen MI, Beauvais JE, Cramer JA, Rainey PM, O’Malley SS, et al. Cue-dose training with monetary reinforcement: pilot study of an antiretroviral adherence intervention. J Gen Intern Med. 2000;15:841–847. doi: 10.1046/j.1525-1497.2000.00127.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Largent E, Grady C, Miller FG, Wertheimer A. Misconceptions about coercion and undue influence: reflections on the views of IRB members. Bioethics. 2013;27:500–507. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8519.2012.01972.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Largent EA, Grady C, Miller FG, Wertheimer A. Money, coercion, and undue inducement: attitudes about payments to research participants. IRB. 2012;34:1–8. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Violari A, Cotton MF, Gibb DM, Babiker AG, Steyn J, Madhi SA, et al. Early antiretroviral therapy and mortality among HIV-infected infants. N Engl J Med. 2008;359:2233–2244. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa0800971. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Cotton MF, Violari A, Otwombe K, Panchia R, Dobbels E, Rabie H, et al. Early time-limited antiretroviral therapy versus deferred therapy in South African infants infected with HIV: results from the children with HIV early antiretroviral (CHER) randomised trial. Lancet. 2013;382:1555–1563. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(13)61409-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.MRBL, Drouin O, Bartlett G, Nguyen Q, Low A, Gavriilidis G, et al. Incidence and Prevalence of Opportunistic and Other Infections and the Impact of Antiretroviral Therapy Among HIV-infected Children in Low- and Middle-income Countries: A Systematic Review and Meta-analysis. Clin Infect Dis. 2016;62:1586–1594. doi: 10.1093/cid/ciw139. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Njuguna IN, Wagner ADCL, Otieno VO, Onyango JA, Chebet DJ, Okinyi HM, Benki-Nugent S, Maleche-Obimbo E, Slyker JA, John-Stewart GC, Wamalwa DC. Hospitalized Children Reveal Health Systems Gaps in the Mother–Child HIV Care Cascade in Kenya. AIDS Patient Care and STDs. 2016;30:119–124. doi: 10.1089/apc.2015.0239. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Wagner AD, Wachira CM, Njuguna IN, Maleche-Obimbo E, Sherr K, Inwani IW, et al. Active referral of children of HIV-positive adults reveals high prevalence of undiagnosed HIV. J Acquir Immune Defic Syndr. 2016 doi: 10.1097/QAI.0000000000001184. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Ahmed S, Sabelli RA, Simon K, Rosenberg NE, Kavuta E, Harawa M, et al. Index case finding facilitates identification and linkage to care of children and young persons living with HIV/AIDS in Malawi. Trop Med Int Health. 2017;22:1021–1029. doi: 10.1111/tmi.12900. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Govindasamy D, Ferrand RA, Wilmore SM, Ford N, Ahmed S, Afnan-Holmes H, et al. Uptake and yield of HIV testing and counselling among children and adolescents in sub-Saharan Africa: a systematic review. J Int AIDS Soc. 2015;18:20182. doi: 10.7448/IAS.18.1.20182. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Wagner AOM, Firdawsi G, Mugo O, Njuguna C, Maleche-Obimbo I, Inwani E, Wamalwa I, John-Stewart D, Slyker GJ. HIV testing for older children: A mixed-methods study examining challenges in decision to test, testing process, and coping post-testing. 21st International AIDS Conference; Durban, South Africa. 2016. [Google Scholar]
- 22.Lee R, Cui RR, Muessig KE, Thirumurthy H, Tucker JD. Incentivizing HIV/STI testing: a systematic review of the literature. AIDS Behav. 2014;18:905–912. doi: 10.1007/s10461-013-0588-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Research NCftPoHSoBaB. The Belmont Report. Washington, DC: 1979. [Google Scholar]
- 24.Wertheimer A, Miller FG. Payment for research participation: a coercive offer? J Med Ethics. 2008;34:389–392. doi: 10.1136/jme.2007.021857. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Dickert N, Grady C. What’s the price of a research subject? Approaches to payment for research participation. N Engl J Med. 1999;341:198–203. doi: 10.1056/NEJM199907153410312. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Emanuel EJ. Undue inducement: nonsense on stilts? Am J Bioeth. 2005;5:9–13. doi: 10.1080/15265160500244959. discussion W18-11, W17. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.London AJ, Borasky DA, Jr, Bhan A Ethics Working Group of the HIVPTN. Improving ethical review of research involving incentives for health promotion. PLoS Med. 2012;9:e1001193. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1001193. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Kahneman D. Thinking, Fast and Slow. New York: Farrar, Straus and Giroux; 2011. [Google Scholar]
- 29.Pace C, Talisuna A, Wendler D, Maiso F, Wabwire-Mangen F, Bakyaita N, et al. Quality of parental consent in a Ugandan malaria study. Am J Public Health. 2005;95:1184–1189. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2004.053082. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Abdool Karim Q, Abdool Karim SS, Coovadia HM, Susser M. Informed consent for HIV testing in a South African hospital: is it truly informed and truly voluntary? Am J Public Health. 1998;88:637–640. doi: 10.2105/ajph.88.4.637. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]