Abstract
Crowding is the substantial interference of neighboring items on target identification. Crowding with letter stimuli has been studied primarily in the visual periphery, with conflicting results for foveal stimuli. While a cortical locus for peripheral crowding is well established (with a large spatial extent up to half of the target eccentricity), disentangling the contributing factors in the fovea is more challenging due to optical limitations. Here, we used adaptive optics (AO) to overcome ocular aberrations and employed high-resolution stimuli to precisely characterize foveal lateral interactions with high-contrast letters flanked by letters. Crowding was present, with a maximal edge-to-edge interference zone of 0.75-1.3 minutes at typical unflanked performance levels. In agreement with earlier foveal contour interaction studies, performance was non-monotonic, revealing a recovery effect with proximal flankers. Modeling revealed that the deleterious effects of flankers can be described by a single function across stimulus sizes when the degradation is expressed as a reduction in sensitivity (expressed in Z-score units). The recovery, however, did not follow this pattern, likely reflecting a separate mechanism. Additional analysis reconciles multiple results from the literature, including the observed scale invariance of center-to-center spacing, as well as the size independence of edge-to-edge spacing.
Introduction
The fovea (the center of gaze) represents the most exquisite region of the visual field for high-acuity spatial and color vision. It has been well established that visual perception in this area is limited by factors located early in the visual processing stream, such as the physical spacing of retinal cells and the fidelity of the optical signal passing through the anatomical structures in the eye. Elsewhere in the visual field, however, the fundamental limit to visual perception is the enigmatic phenomenon known as “crowding”–the adverse effect of nearby flankers on identification of a target. Whether crowding in the fovea exists, and its relationship to peripheral crowding, has been hotly debated for several decades, and resolving this debate crucially informs models of cortical processing which often rely on the homogeneity of visual cortex across the visual field. Specifically, given the systematic progression of retinal tissue per degree of visual angle of eccentricity1,2, it would be most parsimonious if the fovea were merely quantitatively different from the periphery, with effects scaling due to the cortical magnification factor3. Alternatively, foveal crowding could be qualitatively different: it could be “absent,” as some have claimed, or could possibly be an optical effect, rather than a neural effect.
Undoubtedly the most dramatic demonstrations of crowding occur in the peripheral visual field, where the phenomenon is spatially extensive and dominates other performance-limiting factors, such as retinal sampling. Several comprehensive reviews of the effect exist4–6. The extent of peripheral crowding has been shown to increase roughly in proportion to the target eccentricity, a relation now known as Bouma’s “Law”7. The initial estimate was a peripheral interaction zone of about “half the target eccentricity,” though the exact formulation depends on the stimuli used, and mathematical descriptions are undergoing continual refinement6,8. Notably, all formulations of crowding zone extent as a function of eccentricity require precise measurement of the foveal crowding zone with appropriate stimuli.
Some of the earliest investigations of crowding were performed in the fovea of normal and amblyopic observers by Flom and colleagues9. They found that the maximal extent of interference from bar flankers on identification of a Landolt C was correlated with the size of the minimum resolvable letter for each subject. An important subsequent finding established that crowding was post-retinal, using an experimental condition where the flankers were presented to one eye and the target to the other, finding equivalent crowding versus non-dichoptic presentations10. In a later review, Flom made a precise distinction between the term “contour interaction,” which he used to refer to low-level spatial interactions between nearby contours, and the more general “crowding effect,” which included eye movements and attentional factors11. In Flom’s view, the more local and low-level contour interaction can be isolated with a target surrounded by bars, while the more general effect is measured using complex displays comprising multiple letters. More recently, usage of the term “crowding” has been expanded to refer to many interference effects from adjacent flankers–basic low-level spatial interactions, as well as those that likely involve much higher-level influences such as attention. Following modern usage, we use the term “crowding” throughout, even in cases that Flom may have distinguished as “contour integration,” such as our main effect.
While interaction effects from nearby flankers in foveal vision have been previously shown to modulate hyperacuity thresholds in tasks such as vernier discrimination3,12,13, results using traditional acuity stimuli such as letter optotypes remain controversial. Foveal crowding with the latter has received some support9,14–17 and has been pursued more recently18–21. It has been argued that in some of these conditions target and flankers may physically overlap6. The deleterious effects in the fovea have also been attributed to simple lateral contrast masking, rather than the characteristic neural spatial disorder that defines crowding22,23. Similarly, it has also been proposed that foveal crowding is due to physical properties of the stimuli themselves (i.e., explainable from the spatial frequency content of the targets and neighboring clutter), and independent of neural properties of the visual system, unlike peripheral crowding24, though this proposal has been challenged14,25.
Importantly, to observe foveal crowding requires procedures to significantly reduce identification performance. The most common method has been to present small, high-contrast stimuli at far distances (physically or optically). To permit testing of larger letter sizes in the fovea, researchers have maintained equivalent performance levels by directly reducing stimulus luminance18 or contrast21, with contrast reduction implicitly as part of contrast threshold measurement23, or by introducing blur26. Shortened presentation times (<100 ms) have also been used to observe foveal crowding19,27. More recently, Pelli et al.20 measured interaction effects using a novel font featuring optotypes with an extreme vertical elongation. See Table 1 for a summary of the previous foveal crowding studies. Some studies, using bar or letter flankers with moderate or infinite durations have observed interactions effects, generally until the flankers are on the order of 3–5 arcminutes from the target. On the other hand, crowding in the fovea with has also been reported as “absent” or “weak”6,28, particularly with low contrast letters29,30. One explanation that has been offered for the previous negative findings concerns the tiny spatial extent of foveal crowding. When previous studies have presented large, low contrast targets and flankers, the target/flanker spacing has typically been specified as a fraction of the target size (at least a bar-width), which in most cases will far exceed the critical spacing. See Siderov et al.21 for this argument, and a resolution using absolute, rather than nominal, letter spacing. Our goal in this study was to unambiguously identify crowding effects using standard, highly discriminable optotypes presented at the fovea.
Table 1.
Study | Maximal spatial extent of crowding: critical spacing (CS) | Maximal crowding | Target stimulus and task | Target size | Flankers | Dur-ation (ms) | Notes |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Flom et al. 1963 | ~1.9–3.2′ E-E | 0.9–1.3′ | Landolt C | 2.5′–3.3′ | bars | ∞ | Viewing distance set to yield ~80% unflanked. |
Danilova and Bondarko 2006 (Experiment 1) | 1.5–4′ E-E | 1–2′ | Landolt C | 2.1–4.7′ | bars | 500 | Viewing distance: 1.7–8.1 m. Critical spacing for asymptotic performance estimated by eye from their Fig. 2. |
Danilova and Bondarko 2006 (Experiment 3) | 2.5–5′ E-E | 0′ | Tumbling E | 3.5′ and 4.4′ | Tumbling Es | 500 | Viewing distance: 1.7–8.1 m. Critical spacing for asymptotic performance estimated by eye from their Fig. 6. |
Strasburger 1991 | X | X | Numbers | 3.6′ | Numbers | 100 | 3.6′ (0.06 deg) was the smallest size tested, with flankers 7.2′ center-center distance. No effect of flankers on threshold contrast. |
Wolford and Chambers 1984 (foveal condition) | >6′ E-E | 2.4′ (2 of 4 subs.) | Square with gap | 12′ | lines | 17–33 | Short duration adjusted to reduce performance. |
Siderov, Waugh, Bedell 2013 | 4′ E-E | 1′ | Sloan Letters | ~4′ | bars | ∞ | high-contrast condition |
Toet & Levi 1992 | 3.6′ C-C | — | T orientation | ~1.8–4′ | Ts | 150 | CS: spacing with 75% correct. Target size estimated as (1.2–2.6′) *1.5 |
Lev et al. 2014 | >2.8′ E-E | — | E (left or right) | 7.2′ | E | 30–250 | Extent not measured, but crowding found at 0.4 character widths |
Pelli et al. 2016 | 3.9′ C-C | — | 10 numbers, novel font | 1.2′ (width) | numbers | ∞ | Novel font with tall, thin letters. 2–10 m distance. |
Present results | 0.75–1.3′ E-E | 0.5′ | Tumbling E | 1.85′–4′ | Tumbling Es | ∞ | Conditions for 80–95% performance. |
A further consequence of flanking items on target recognition was identified by Flom et al.9, but has received little direct experimental investigation. Specifically, for bars at the very nearest spacings (within approximately one arc-minute), performance begins to recover, relative to the point of worst performance, which occurred when flankers were about ~40% of the target size from the target11. In some cases performance with abutting bars was equivalent or better than performance without bars. This was reminiscent of the earlier results of Takahashi31, who found a similar facilitation effect of nearby bars on a meticulous gap detection task in the fovea. Flom11 proposed that this effect was due to optical spreading of the image on the retina. Subsequent investigations of foveal contour interaction or crowding have had mixed results revealing this aspect of flanker interactions. For many studies, this effect is not explicitly mentioned, but can be inferred from raw data plots. See Table 1 for a summary; a non-zero entry in the ‘maximal crowding’ column indicates a recovery region. It is ubiquitous with basic stimuli such as Landolt-Cs flanked by bars9,14, but has also been observed with Sloan letters flanked by bars18,21, exhibiting a dependence on the spatial characteristics of targets and flankers.
The major obstacle to definitive results concerning foveal spatial vision is the presence of optical aberrations. Irregularities in the cornea and lens of the eye blur the light falling on the retina and constitute pre-neural limits to fine spatial vision. Accordingly, these characteristics determine the smallest stimuli that can be delivered to the retina and resolved in isolation. Therefore, any measure of spatial vision at the fovea necessarily confounds optical and neural factors. Adaptive optics (AO) offers an opportunity to bypass the effect of ocular imperfections and deliver near diffraction-limited stimuli to the eye unhindered by monochromatic aberrations. While its primary application has been to improve the spatial resolution of retinal images, AO has also been used to measure the visual benefit of correcting optical aberration on a variety of psychophysical tasks – acuity and contrast sensitivity in normal observers32,33, real-life tasks34, color vision35, as well as visual performance after correction of highly aberrated keratoconic eyes36. For a detailed review on the use of AO for studying visual function, see the review by Roorda37. Concomitant imaging and stimulus delivery via AO correction has allowed relating the neural architecture of the retina to visual perception38–42. Here, we used a custom AO-based vision testing setup to measure the limits of crowding at the fovea. Under these conditions, tailored to reveal minute spatial interactions, we sought to identify whether crowding exists in the fovea and, if so, to quantify its extent on a precise scale.
Results
Representative data and model
Proportion correct was fit individually for each subject, optotype size, and optical condition, yielding a set of performance-vs.-flanker spacing curves. Representative data are shown in Fig. 1 for observer S2, with adaptive optics correction and a Tumbling-E letter with a bars subtending 0.47 arcminutes. The solid data points indicate average performance at each spacing, with error bars representing 95% confidence intervals from maximum likelihood estimation based on the raw data and binomial statistics. The line shows a non-parametric smoothed curve through the datapoints. The curve can be divided into three regions, depicted with the colored segments in Fig. 1. For sufficiently large spacings (rightmost section of curve, in blue), performance is asymptotic at a high performance level, which corresponds to the unflanked proportion correct. With flankers located closer to the target (middle section of curve, in red), flankers reduce performance, in typical fashion for crowding experiments. The farthest flanker spacing at which performance worsens below the asymptotic level is known as the critical spacing, here approximately 0.75 arcminutes. The magnitude of reduction that defines the transition is not agreed upon in the literature. Some researchers have estimated the maximum spacing causing interference by eye9,18,21, others have utilized a criteria of statistical significance14, while others have interpolated the critical spacing from a parametric fit, with an amplitude reduction by a factor of 1/e43, or, alternatively, by an amount of 10%44. Here, to identify the largest flanker spacing that could exhibit interference, we adopt a conservative criteria of a reduction of 0.025 from the fitted asymptotic performance level. In accordance with previous literature, performance was modeled with a cumulative Gaussian function, with free parameters for the mean (horizontal position, or point of subjective equality) and standard deviation (“slope” or width). The curve was corrected for the 25% guess rate and multiplicatively scaled with an amplitude term corresponding to the asymptotic performance level.
With flankers very close to the target, in many cases performance was improved relative to the location of maximal crowding (approximately 0.35 arcminutes for this example). This part of the curve is called the recovery region (green section), and was modeled as a linear relationship of performance versus flanker spacing, with a negative slope. In total, performance vs. flanker spacing curves are defined as the numerical maximum of the sigmoidal (crowded plus asymptotic) and linear recovery portions.
The relations are stated formally as Equations 1a, 1b, and 1c below. The recovery portion (Equation 1b) is simply the straight line parameterized as mx + b, with edge-to-edge spacing “x” as the abscissa. The sigmoidal portion (Equation 1c) consists of a cumulative Gaussian with the standard width and location parameters σ and μ, respectively. Parameter A scales the cumulative Gaussian and denotes the asymptotic level, with the sigmoidal curve corrected for chance based on the guess rate gamma (fixed at 0.25 for the Tumbling-E task). Each curve was fit individually with this five-parameter model using Bayesian estimation to determine the most likely parameters and their resultant 95% confidence intervals. Fitting was performed with Python and the NumPy/SciPy tools45, using the PyMC3 library46 for Markov chain Monte Carlo estimation. Weak uniform prior distributions were used for all parameters as follows: A (0:1.0), σ (0:5), μ (−1:1.0), m (−1:0), and b (0:1.0).
1a |
1b |
1c |
To construct a general model that might account for variability across observer, stimulus size, and optical condition, we first normalized each psychometric function to their asymptotic level, yielding a scale reflecting the performance reduction relative to unflanked targets. Furthermore, for results measured on a proportion correct scale, identical changes in performance are not equivalent across the psychometric function, which is nonlinear. For example, a drop in identification accuracy from 95% to 90% represents a more significant effect than a change from 65% to 60%. To compensate for this nonlinearity, Z-scores (or “probit units”), the inverse transformation of the cumulative Gaussian distribution, were used, resulting in a unit akin to d-prime or sensitivity. To fit the data using this scheme, the ordinate was converted to a relative scale indicating the change in Z-score units from the fitted asymptotic performance level. For example, an ordinate of 0 on this scale results from spacings achieving asymptotic performance levels, and negative numbers indicate a drop from this level (in Z-score units). For the examples above, a reduction from 95% to 90% corresponds to a change in (1.645–1.282) = 0.363 Z-units, whereas the reduction from 65% to 60% corresponds to (0.385–0.253) = 0.132 Z-units. Proportions were corrected for guessing prior to Z-score computation.
Figure 2 shows an example of this transformation, with all stimulus sizes for subject S3 with AO, shown as raw proportion correct (left panel) and reduction from the uncrowded level in terms of Z-scores (right panel). Lines indicate best-fitting model and 95% confidence intervals for the parametric fits. Remarkably, this normalization aligns the crowding-related segments of the curves, across all sizes for a given observer and condition, suggesting that the width and location parameters of the sigmoidal portion may be invariant across stimulus size. The recovery portion differs between sizes, however, evident by the non-alignment of the diagonal colored limbs at the closest spacings—they are approximately parallel, but stacked vertically. To determine which model parameters should be fixed or free across conditions and observers, we performed Bayesian model comparison on the set of possible models, described below.
Fitted parameters and model comparison
First, the asymptote parameter A identifies the unflanked performance level at each size and condition for each observer. These are plotted in Fig. 3. The lines show cumulative Gaussian functions fitted to the estimated asymptotes. The performance improvement facilitated by adaptive optics for each subject is evident as the leftward shift, equivalent to MAR improvements of 0.2, 0.225, and 0.025 arcminutes for each subject (respectively). Table 2 indicates MAR at a 75% performance level.
Table 2.
Subject | Habitual spherical correction | Sizes tested with AO (arcmin. of one stroke; 1/5 of letter size) | Estimated 75% Acuity w/AO | Sizes tested w/o AO (arcmin. of one stroke) | Estimated 75% Acuity w/o AO |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
S1 | 4.5D | 0.61, 0.73 | 20/11 (0.55′ MAR) | 0.85, 0.97 | 20/15 (0.75′ MAR) |
S2 | — | 0.37, 0.42, 0.47, 0.53, 0.58 | 20/8 (0.4′ MAR) | 0.63, 0.69, 0.79 | 20/12.5 (0.625′ MAR) |
S3 | 1.5D | 0.53, 0.58, 0.63, 0.69 | 20/12 (0.6′ MAR) | 0.63, 0.69, 0.74, 0.79 | 20/12.5 (0.625′ MAR) |
S2 is an emmetrope.
Fitted critical spacings are plotted in Fig. 4. Within a subject and optical condition, the critical spacing is largest with the smallest letters, and decreases as letters increase in size. There is no systematic pattern across the entire dataset, indicating that individual differences play a role in determining the critical spacing, rather than the zone being a fixed function of bar size. Conditions with unflanked performance at 80–95% are indicated with stars, for comparison to standard practice in the literature. For these conditions, critical spacings were 0.75–1.3 minutes.
Strictly speaking, only two parameters were required to change with each stimulus size: the asymptote (A) and the recovery asymptote (b). The remaining three parameters (σ, μ, and m) could be constrained in several possible ways. First, each parameter could vary independently with observer and size, yielding the full/saturated model with many free parameters. Alternatively, parameters could be fixed across sizes, as suggested by the observed alignment of the crowding portion. Furthermore, parameters could be either fixed within an observer for both optical conditions (AO and non-AO) together, or independently for the two conditions. Finally, parameters could be completely yoked together for all observers and conditions.
The results of evaluation of 12 models are given in Table 3. Models ranged from the most constrained model where the three parameters are each yoked together for all observers, sizes, and optical conditions (model #0), to the least constrained/most saturated version of complete independence (model #11), which was used earlier. Goodness of fit was quantified using WAIC47, a fully Bayesian fitness measure like the more well-known chi-squared, AIC/BIC and the newer deviance information criteria (DIC), except that WAIC uses the entire model posterior distribution, resulting in more accurate estimates48. Like other fitness measures, models with more parameters are penalized, in order to combat overfitting. Lower scores reveal better-fitting models. The effective number of parameters (pWAIC) can be fractional, as the estimate is not simply an integral count of free parameters. Fitting was performed using PyMC3, with 1000 estimates generated for each model tested.
Table 3.
Model # | σ | μ | m | WAIC | pWAIC | LogLik |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
5 | O | O | F | 4054.7 | 46.12 | 2085.77 |
6 | O | O | O | 4056.59 | 46.38 | 2090.85 |
1 | F | O | F | 4064 | 48.34 | 2083.9 |
8 | C | C | F | 4064.54 | 53.73 | 2099.41 |
7 | F | C | F | 4066.14 | 52.56 | 2087.52 |
10 | N | O | F | 4071.1 | 55.58 | 2111.92 |
9 | C | C | C | 4075.29 | 67.44 | 2127.68 |
2 | O | F | F | 4088.33 | 45.97 | 2100.38 |
3 | O | F | O | 4090.84 | 47.82 | 2104.61 |
11 | N | N | N | 4091.7 | 73.62 | 2205.4 |
0 | F | F | F | 4100.69 | 50.07 | 2098.27 |
4 | F | F | O | 4101.85 | 53.75 | 2100.92 |
Models are ordered from best-fitting (lowest WAIC) to worst-fitting (highest WAIC). Effective number of parameters are given by pWAIC, and the log-likelihood of each model is given by LogLik. WAIC relates to the likelihood, minus a parameter penalty. Models reflect hierarchical constraints on the three parameters, described in the text.
In Table 3, models are ordered by increasing WAIC. The degrees of freedom accorded to the variables in each model are indicated with several codes: “N” (no constraints: freely varies), “O” (fixed for each observer across sizes), “C” (fixed for each observer across sizes, separately for the two optical conditions), and “F” (fixed across observers). Two models were clearly superior: models #5 and #6, in which the two crowding sigmoid parameters (width and location) were fixed for each observer. The location parameter was most critical; models that attempted to fix this parameter across observers comprised four of the worst five models tested. The recovery slope could be yoked across all three observers (#5), or fit separately for each (#6), with only a slight preference for the yoked model (#5). In general, models with individual sigmoid location parameter for each observer were generally best (#5, #6, #1), and separating the optical conditions (#8 and #9) tended to not improve fitness. Models with the most degrees of freedom (#9, #10, #11) were typically worse, despite fitting the data better (higher log-likelihood).
With the best models (model #5), the Gaussian parameters were fixed across all sizes for each observer (as suggested by Fig. 2), meaning that the results for all sizes of each observer can be captured by a single canonical crowding function. Figure 5 shows the entire data set fit with this model, with critical spacing estimates indicated by the triangle along each function.
Discussion and Conclusions
In summary, we have shown that the fovea is not immune to crowding, in a standard spatial vision paradigm (high-contrast, unlimited duration Tumbling-E optotypes) with clearly non-overlapping flankers, confirming the neural basis of the phenomenon seen with traditional means9,14,18,21. Whereas previous studies have typically reduced the visibility of targets, either by reducing their contrast, luminance or duration in order to observe interference effects, we instead used adaptive optics to counteract the natural blur impeding normal spatial vision in the fovea, permitting the presentation of tiny optotypes and precisely-sized gaps with greater precision than previously possible.
The critical spacings we observed were smaller than those previously found for letter acuity stimuli (see Table 1). In the regime of most crowding experiments, where unflanked levels are held at 80–95%, the critical spacings we observed were 0.75 to 1.3 arcminutes edge-to-edge (or 3.0–6.0 arcminutes center-to-center, depending on the subject and condition). These are much smaller than the 4–5 arcminute spacings that have typically been found in the literature9,14,18,21. Even without adaptive optics correction, we measured critical spacings one quarter to one half of previously-reported extents. There are several possible causes for the difference, including our use of a Maxwellian view adaptive optics setup, with an artificial pupil and a bite bar to control fixation. Fixation stability may have played a role, as Bedell et al.49 found that imprecise eye movements may contribute to increased difficulty correctly locating letters in a string, as previously proposed by Flom11. While Bedell et al.18 found minimal effects of luminance on foveal crowding extent, optical factors may have played a role. Lastly, our use of Tumbling E stimuli may have contributed to smaller critical spacings. However, Danilova and Bondarko14 found critical spacings for Tumbling Es flanked by Tumbling Es of approximately 2.5–5 arcminutes: twice as extensive as our results.
When proportion correct was converted to reduction in Z-score units from the asymptotic (unflanked) performance level, a single canonical crowding curve emerged for each subject that could account for all stimulus sizes, with both AO correction and natural viewing. With this transformation, flankers had a maximum influence zone of 1–1.5 arcminute (edge-to-edge), reducing performance by about 1 Z-score unit for each half arcminute of flanker proximity. As mentioned previously, the standard practice has been to test crowding with specific sizes to avoid floor and ceiling effects. Here we directly model the relationship between target size and foveal crowding extent, capturing the fact that crowding functions flatten (in terms of proportion correct) for stimuli that are more difficult to resolve. The slope is constant when specified in terms of a reduction in Z-score. Hence, flankers multiplicatively affect identification performance in a consistent manner.
The recovery portion of the curve likely reflects a different mechanism. This effect has been observed previously in the literature9,14,21,31, but as far as we know has not been explicitly characterized. Here this effect is robust, though absent for conditions with very high asymptotic performance levels (largest letter for each subject), and more variable without AO. Targets with abutting flankers were identified with an accuracy approximately 20% lower than unflanked targets. Performance worsened with increasing flankers separation, until overtaken by the typical crowding effect (sigmoidal function of flanker spacing) at spacings of approximately 0.5–0.75 minutes. This transition occurs at a spacing one-half to one-quarter of that seen in previous studies. Previous authors have proposed that the recovery effect results from “optical smearing” of flanking bars with the target11, which implicates spatial cues that would be most useful for bar flankers and simple targets such as Tumbling Es or Landolt Cs. Takahashi31 observed this influence on gap detection even with wide bars, finding that only the nearest edge defined the resultant curve, which included a significant recovery portion. In another study, thinner bars were found to exhibit some recovery, although the effect was most robust with bars matched to the size of the target’s bars14. We believe that in our paradigm, the nearest limbs of the flanking Es behave much like bar flankers. The spatial cues that result from the integration of the flanking bars with the target cause a noticeable widening of the target bars on each side except its opening (to the right for an upright E), a subtle low-level cue that subjects quickly learn. It has been found earlier that subjects can use width cues for spatial judgments, even when two adjacent lines are within the resolution limit50. Though most evident with Tumbling Es or Landolt Cs, a similar recovery pattern has also been observed with high-contrast Sloan letters18,21, again likely resulting from spatial cues inferred by acute observers. Importantly, in our study optical influences were carefully controlled yet the recovery persisted, contrary to Flom’s proposal for a purely optical contribution11. This suggests that neural smearing of the target and flanker features is the probable cause of this component, with a spatial extent much smaller than the interference effect. A possible mechanism is spatial summation, which was recently shown to be post-receptoral using adaptive optics and eye-tracking51.
Our results differ from those found in peripheral vision in several ways. First, critical spacings were parsimoniously captured by edge-to-edge spacing, rather than center-to-center spacing. We believe that this property may be unique to foveal vision22. Second, the robust flanker recovery, while common in foveal contour interaction studies, is generally absent in peripheral experiments, though it was seen in the parafovea (0.54 and 1.05 degree) in the flanked gap-detection task of Takahashi31 and with very brief peripheral presentations (2 or 5 degrees) of a square with a gap on one side, flanked by lines27. Lastly, several studies have reported that the center-to-center critical spacing in the periphery is independent of stimulus size. Importantly, these studies adjusted the contrast of differently-sized stimuli to test at similar performance levels, either by explicit manipulation of stimulus parameters43, or as a result of a contrast threshold procedure52,53. Our finding that critical spacing depends crucially on asymptotic performance agrees with these findings, though with edge-to-edge critical spacing in the fovea–in agreement with Siderov et al.21.
Reconciling the spatial extent of crowding across studies in foveal and peripheral vision has proven to be elusive. One approach is to plot center-to-center critical spacing versus target size, allowing comparisons of critical spacings across varying stimulus conditions22,26,54,55. This analysis has proven useful in showing how peripheral, crowding-limited data follow one pattern and foveal, blur-limited data follow another pattern26,55. Figure 6 extends this analysis more precisely into the fovea, plotting our own data (fitted with model #5) and those from several studies from Table 1. The dotted line is based on foveal results from contrast thresholds of blurred stimuli, revealing a “size-limited” regime, which also matches results from non-strabismic amblyopia26, non-foveal AMD patients55 and S-cone isolating stimuli56. For results on this line, critical spacing is proportional to stimulus size, such that critical spacing = 1.4 × size, a straight line parallel to the identity line on this log-log plot. The peripheral crowding line is not relevant for this discussion, but is shown for reference as the dashed line in the upper part of the plot. Data in the periphery (from 3–10 degrees) has been shown to fall on this line26,55, where the critical spacing rises steeply compared to the threshold size.
Since the ordinate of this plot requires center-to-center spacing, we first converted data into center-to-center measures. For targets flanked by bars, this value is the edge-to-edge spacing plus 0.6 times the target size. For letters flanked by letters, center-to-center spacing is simply the edge-to-edge spacing plus the letter size. We have colored each symbol on the figure according to the corresponding asymptotic performance level, with values determined from published plots14,21 or as stated in the texts9,17,20. As mentioned previously, to allow the same criteria for critical spacings that we used (the abscissa for approximately asymptotic performance), we estimated critical spacings by eye from raw data plots when possible14,21. The two prior studies with the smallest target sizes and critical spacings both utilized Landolt C stimuli flanked by bars9,14. Flom et al.9 controlled the distance such that each observer achieved approximately 80% unflanked, and each observer is shown, with all observers connected with the green dashed line. Danilova and Bondarko14 tested several observers at several asymptotic performance levels. Here we connected each observer’s conditions with a gray dashed line. Two other studies used non-standard stimuli at a single high performance level: Tumbling T’s flanked by T’s17 and a novel elongated number font20. Lastly, Siderov et al.21 tested Sloan letters flanked by bars, at an asymptotic level of approximately 90% correct.
The colorization reveals a remarkable patterns in the results. Individual results at the same performance level form parallel diagonal lines on this plot, parallel to the dotted line. For example, the individual observers from Flom et al.9 and the appropriate conditions of Danilova and Bondarko14 fall approximately on a single line for the ~80% performance level (greenish color), above and to the left of the dotted line. Asymptotic results closer to 90% were used by Toet and Levi17, Siderov et al.21, and would be expected from the adaptive procedure used by Song et al.26 and Pelli et al.20 These are indicated in yellow, on or near the dotted line. Multiple conditions of Siderov et al.21 are all on this same line. Although their conditions correspond to different stimulus sizes, they lowered the contrast to maintain equivalent visibility and asymptotic performance, and are therefore consistent with the pattern we report. Finally, both our data and those of Danilova and Bondarko14 show that center-to-center critical spacing is fairly flat within each observer measured at different target sizes (but with different asymptotic levels), with a slight increase as target sizes increase. The steeper slope for our data may be the result of either our Tumbling-E targets or usage of letter flankers rather than bar flankers.
There are several conclusions to take away from this analysis. First, the good fit of these data on lines parallel to the dotted line indicate that, in the fovea, the center-to-center crowding zone is proportional to the stimulus size for a wide range of stimulus conditions. That was proposed initially by Flom et al.9, although they based their arguments on the edge-to-edge spacing of several individual observers including amblyopes. Second, as noted by Siderov et al.21, edge-to-edge spacing is consistent across stimuli sizes, but here we clarify that maintaining equivalent performance levels (which they achieved with contrast) is crucial. The equal edge-to-edge spacings mean that center-to-center spacing is proportional, also in agreement with Song et al.26, and Levi et al.23. Finally, for situations with different target sizes at different asymptotic levels (Danilova and Bondarko14 and our study), there are three observations. First, the edge-to-edge spacing of the crowding zone decreases for larger targets (Fig. 4), which is why crowding “disappears” for sufficiently well-resolved foveal targets. On the other hand, the center-to-center extent of the crowding zone shows a slight increase as a function of size, shown by the positive slope of individual observer lines in Fig. 6. Finally, if measured as a normalized Z-score reduction from asymptotic performance (Fig. 2), edge-to-edge critical spacings are exactly the same across sizes, which would result, again, in proportional increases for center-to-center spacing.
As theories attempt to link the crowding zone size to anatomical measures such as the extent of lateral neural connections or receptive field sizes4–6, it is vital to accurately characterize the extent of crowding at the fovea. It is straightforward to increase the size of the crowding zone, by reducing stimulus visibility such as with shortened stimulus duration43,44,57,58 or by decreasing contrast59,60. However, these manipulations introduce possible confounding effects to determination of the actual extent of the crowding zone. Instead, our method allowed testing of standard, high-contrast, long duration acuity targets smaller than those previously possible with traditional methods. Thus, these results provide the most accurate estimate of the spatial extent of crowding at the fovea, free of pre-neural limiting factors that may have obscured previous measures.
Methods
Adaptive optics vision simulator
The apparatus was a custom-built optical system consisting of a deformable mirror (Mirao52D. Imagine Eyes, Orsay, France) and a wavefront sensor as its key components. A superluminescent diode centered at 840 nm served as the wavefront sensing beacon. The light source was collimated and passed through an 800 micron pinhole en route to the eye. This ensured that the effect of aberrations on the beacon during the first pass into the eye was minimal. A 90:10 (Transmission:reflection) pellicle beamsplitter reflected the wavefront sensing beacon into the eye. The light scattered back from the retina was imaged on to the wavefront sensor. Image relay optics were constructed using a train of achromatic lenses to relay the scattered light from the eye’s pupil onto the deformable mirror and the lenslet array of the wavefront sensor. An extra pupil conjugate plane was constructed to allow insertion of trial lenses to compensate for sphere and cylinder. The wavefront sensing arm of the setup was separated from the vision testing arm using a cold mirror. An artificially adjustable iris allowed for selecting the optimum pupil size for vision testing. The size of the field on the retina was 1 deg. Stimuli were displayed on a computer projector (Sharp PG-M20X) placed at an imaging plane and the retinal illuminance through a 6-mm eye’s pupil was measured to be 100 cd/m2. Aberrations were corrected dynamically in a closed-loop fashion over a 6.75 mm eye’s pupil while vision testing was performed over an artificial 6-mm pupil. A continuous AO correction allowed stable optical quality throughout vision testing. Subjects were allowed to blink at their discretion during psychophysics. Prior to vision testing, the subjects were subjectively refracted to compensate for the longitudinal chromatic aberration arising between the wavefront sensing beacon and the stimulus display. A schematic of the system is shown in Fig. 7.
Stimuli and Procedure
The stimuli were high contrast black-on-white Tumbling-E targets, presented in isolation or flanked by four randomly-oriented Tumbling-Es located above, below, and to either size side of the target. Stimuli were displayed until the subject responded. Blocks comprised 300 trials, with several combinations of sizes and spacings interleaved to yield 10 trials per size/spacing condition. Sizes were chosen to yield a range of reasonable performance levels along the psychometric function for each observer, with spacings ranging from 1.6 arc minutes of space between target and flanker, to abutting (no space between target and flanker), and an unflanked condition. Each size and spacing was evaluated in at least two separate sessions. The wavefront measurement and closed-loop correction were continuously monitored, with appropriate adjustments performed as necessary within each trial. Subjects were encouraged to take breaks whenever needed between trials. A condition without AO correction was also run wherein subjects’ sphere and cylinder were minimized using trial lenses.
Participants
The University of Washington institutional review board approved this research and all subjects signed an informed consent before their participation in this study. All procedures involving human subjects were in accordance with the tenets of the Declaration of Helsinki. Three subjects participated in the study. All were authors, with one subject/author (S3) naive to the purpose of the study during the experiment. S2 was emmetropic and an experienced observer in this apparatus, while S1 was an experienced psychophysical observer. S1 and S3 were myopes and their prescription was corrected with trial lenses. Accommodation was paralyzed with 1% tropicamide ophthalmic drops and a bite bar was used to minimize head movements. Table 2 summarizes participant characteristics and basic stimulus parameters.
Data Availability
The datasets generated during and/or analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.
Acknowledgements
The authors thank Harold Bedell for comments on an earlier draft of this manuscript. This work was supported by grant R01EY020976 from the National Eye Institute to D.M.L., and the following awards to R.S.: an unrestricted grant from the Research to Prevent Blindness, NIH P30 EY001730, M.J. Murdock Charitable Trust, Research to Prevent Blindness Career Development Award, and a Burroughs Wellcome Fund Careers at the Scientific Interfaces Award.
Author Contributions
D.C., D.L., and R.S. were involved in the design of the study and preparation of the manuscript. D.C., P.T., and R.S. collected and analyzed the data. All authors reviewed the manuscript.
Competing Interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher's note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
References
- 1.Hubel DH, Wiesel TN. Uniformity of monkey striate cortex: A parallel relationship between field size, scatter, and magnification factor. J. Comp. Neurol. 1974;158:295–305. doi: 10.1002/cne.901580305. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Dow DBM, Snyder AZ, Vautin RG, Bauer R. Magnification factor and receptive field size in foveal striate cortex of the monkey. Exp. Brain Res. 1981;44:213–228. doi: 10.1007/BF00237343. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Levi DM, Klein SA, Aitsebaomo AP. Vernier acuity, crowding and cortical magnification. Vision Res. 1985;25:963–977. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(85)90207-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Levi DM. Crowding–An essential bottleneck for object recognition: A mini-review. Vision Res. 2008;48:635–654. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2007.12.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Pelli DG, Tillman KA. The uncrowded window of object recognition. Nat. Neurosci. 2008;11:1129–1135. doi: 10.1038/nn.2187. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Strasburger H, Rentschler I, Jüttner M. Peripheral vision and pattern recognition: A review. J. Vis. 2011;11(5):1–82. doi: 10.1167/11.5.13. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Bouma H. Interaction effects in parafoveal letter recognition. Nature. 1970;226:177–178. doi: 10.1038/226177a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Rosen S, Chakravarthi R, Pelli DG. The Bouma law of crowding, revised: Critical spacing is equal across parts, not objects. J. Vis. 2014;14(6):1–15. doi: 10.1167/14.6.10. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Flom MC, Weymouth FW, Kahneman D. Visual resolution and contour interaction. JOSA. 1963;53:1026–1032. doi: 10.1364/JOSA.53.001026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Flom MC, Heath GG, Takahashi E. Contour Interaction and Visual Resolution: Contralateral Effects. Science. 1963;142:979–980. doi: 10.1126/science.142.3594.979. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Flom, M. C. Contour interaction and the crowding effect. Probl. Optom. 237–257 (1991).
- 12.Malania M, Herzog MH, Westheimer G. Grouping of contextual elements that affect vernier thresholds. J. Vis. 2007;7:1–7. doi: 10.1167/7.2.1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Westheimer G, Hauske G. Temporal and spatial interference with vernier acuity. Vision Res. 1975;15:1137–1141. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(75)90012-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Danilova MV, Bondarko VM. Foveal contour interactions and crowding effects at the resolution limit of the visual system. J. Vis. 2007;7:25–25. doi: 10.1167/7.2.25. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Latham K, Whitaker D. Relative roles of resolution and spatial interference in foveal and peripheral vision. Ophthalmic Physiol. Opt. J. Br. Coll. Ophthalmic Opt. Optom. 1996;16:49–57. doi: 10.1016/0275-5408(95)00124-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Loomis JM. Lateral masking in foveal and eccentric vision. Vision Res. 1978;18:335–338. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(78)90168-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Toet A, Levi DM. The two-dimensional shape of spatial interaction zones in the parafovea. Vision Res. 1992;32:1349–1357. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(92)90227-A. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Bedell HE, et al. Contour interaction for foveal acuity targets at different luminances. Vision Res. 2013;89:90–95. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2013.07.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Lev M, Yehezkel O, Polat U. Uncovering foveal crowding? Sci. Rep. 2014;4:4067. doi: 10.1038/srep04067. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Pelli, D. G. et al. A clinical test for visual crowding. F1000Research10.12688/f1000research.7835.1 (2016).
- 21.Siderov J, Waugh SJ, Bedell HE. Foveal contour interaction for low contrast acuity targets. Vision Res. 2013;77:10–13. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2012.11.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Coates DR, Levi DM. Contour interaction in foveal vision: A response to Siderov, Waugh, and Bedell (2013) Vision Res. 2014;96:140–144. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2013.10.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Levi, D. M., Klein, S. A. & Hariharan, S. Suppressive and facilitatory spatial interactions in foveal vision: Foveal crowding is simple contrast masking. J. Vis. 2 (2002). [DOI] [PubMed]
- 24.Hess RF, Dakin SC, Kapoor N. The foveal ‘crowding’effect: physics or physiology? Vision Res. 2000;40:365–370. doi: 10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00193-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Liu L. Can the amplitude difference spectrum peak frequency explain the foveal crowding effect? Vision Res. 2001;41:3693–3704. doi: 10.1016/S0042-6989(01)00221-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Song S, Levi DM, Pelli DG. A double dissociation of the acuity and crowding limits to letter identification, and the promise of improved visual screening. J. Vis. 2014;14(5):1–37. doi: 10.1167/14.5.3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Wolford G, Chambers L. Contour interaction as a function of retinal eccentricity. Percept. Psychophys. 1984;36:457–460. doi: 10.3758/BF03207498. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Strasburger H, Harvey LO, Rentschler I. Contrast thresholds for identification of numeric characters in direct and eccentric view. Percept. Psychophys. 1991;49:495–508. doi: 10.3758/BF03212183. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Giaschi DE, Regan D, Kraft SP, Kothe AC. Crowding and contrast in amblyopia. Optom. Vis. Sci. Off. Publ. Am. Acad. Optom. 1993;70:192–197. doi: 10.1097/00006324-199303000-00003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Simmers AJ, Gray LS, McGraw PV, Winn B. Contour interaction for high and low contrast optotypes in normal and amblyopic observers. Ophthalmic Physiol. Opt. 1999;19:253–260. doi: 10.1016/S0275-5408(98)00056-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Takahashi, E. Effects of flanking contours on visual resolution at foveal and near-foveal loci. ([dissertation]. Berkeley, CA: University of California, Berkeley, 1968).
- 32.Liang J, Williams DR, Miller DT. Supernormal vision and high-resolution retinal imaging through adaptive optics. JOSA A. 1997;14:2884. doi: 10.1364/JOSAA.14.002884. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Yoon G-Y, Williams DR. Visual performance after correcting the monochromatic and chromatic aberrations of the eye. J. Opt. Soc. Am. A. 2002;19:266. doi: 10.1364/JOSAA.19.000266. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Sawides L, Gambra E, Pascual D, Dorronsoro C, Marcos S. Visual performance with real-life tasks under Adaptive-Optics ocular aberration correction. J. Vis. 2010;10:19–19. doi: 10.1167/10.5.19. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Sabesan R, Schmidt BP, Tuten WS, Roorda A. The elementary representation of spatial and color vision in the human retina. Sci. Adv. 2016;2:e1600797. doi: 10.1126/sciadv.1600797. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Sabesan R, Yoon G. Visual performance after correcting higher order aberrations in keratoconic eyes. J. Vis. 2009;9:6–6. doi: 10.1167/9.5.6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Roorda, A. Adaptive optics for studying visual function: a comprehensive review. J. Vis. 11 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 38.Tuten WS, Harmening WM, Sabesan R, Roorda A, Sincich LC. Spatiochromatic Interactions between Individual Cone Photoreceptors in the Human Retina. J. Neurosci. 2017;37:9498–9509. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0529-17.2017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Harmening WM, Tuten WS, Roorda A, Sincich LC. Mapping the Perceptual Grain of the Human Retina. J. Neurosci. 2014;34:5667–5677. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5191-13.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Hofer H, Singer B, Williams DR. Different sensations from cones with the same photopigment. J. Vis. 2005;5:5–5. doi: 10.1167/5.5.5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Makous W, et al. Retinal Microscotomas Revealed with Adaptive-Optics Microflashes. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 2006;47:4160–4167. doi: 10.1167/iovs.05-1195. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Schmidt, B. P., Sabesan, R., Tuten, W. S., Neitz, J. & Roorda, A. Sensations from a single M-cone depend on the activity of surrounding S-cones. bioRxiv 260653 10.1101/260653 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 43.Tripathy SP, Cavanagh P. The extent of crowding in peripheral vision does not scale with target size. Vision Res. 2002;42:2357–2369. doi: 10.1016/S0042-6989(02)00197-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Tripathy SP, Cavanagh P, Bedell HE. Large crowding zones in peripheral vision for briefly presented stimuli. J. Vis. 2014;14(6):1–11. doi: 10.1167/14.6.11. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Jones, E., Oliphant, T. & Peterson, P. SciPy: Open source scientific tools for Python (2001).
- 46.Salvatier J, Wiecki TV, Fonnesbeck C. Probabilistic programming in Python using PyMC3. PeerJ Comput. Sci. 2016;2:e55. doi: 10.7717/peerj-cs.55. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Watanabe S. Asymptotic Equivalence of Bayes Cross Validation and Widely Applicable Information Criterion in Singular Learning Theory. J. Mach. Learn. Res. 2010;11:3571–3594. [Google Scholar]
- 48.Vehtari A, Gelman A, Gabry J. Practical Bayesian model evaluation using leave-one-out cross-validation and WAIC. Stat. Comput. 2017;27:1413–1432. doi: 10.1007/s11222-016-9696-4. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Bedell HE, Siderov J, Formankiewicz MA, Waugh SJ, Aydin S. Evidence for an Eye-Movement Contribution to Normal Foveal Crowding. Optom. Vis. Sci. 2015;92:237–245. doi: 10.1097/OPX.0000000000000480. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Levi DM, Klein SA. Equivalent intrinsic blur in spatial vision. Vision Res. 1990;30:1971–1993. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(90)90016-E. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Tuten, W. S. et al. Spatial summation in the human fovea: the effect of optical aberrations and fixational eye movements. bioRxiv 283119 10.1101/283119 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 52.Levi DM, Hariharan S, Klein SA. Suppressive and facilitatory spatial interactions in peripheral vision: peripheral crowding is neither size invariant nor simple contrast masking. J. Vis. 2002;2(2):167–177. doi: 10.1167/2.2.3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Pelli DG, Palomares M, Majaj NJ. Crowding is unlike ordinary masking: Distinguishing feature integration from detection. J. Vis. 2004;4(12):1136–1169. doi: 10.1167/4.12.12. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Levi, D. M. & Carney, T. The effect of flankers on three tasks in central, peripheral, and amblyopic vision. J. Vis. 11 (2011). [DOI] [PubMed]
- 55.Chung STL. Size or spacing: Which limits letter recognition in people with age-related macular degeneration? Vision Res. 2014;101:167–176. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2014.06.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Coates DR, Chung STL. Crowding in the S-cone pathway. Vision Res. 2016;122:81–92. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2016.03.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Chung STL, Mansfield JS. Contrast polarity differences reduce crowding but do not benefit reading performance in peripheral vision. Vision Res. 2009;49:2782–2789. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2009.08.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Wallace JM, Chiu MK, Nandy AS, Tjan BS. Crowding during restricted and free viewing. Vision Res. 2013;84:50–59. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2013.03.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Coates DR, Chin JM, Chung STL. Factors affecting crowded acuity: eccentricity and contrast. Optom. Vis. Sci. 2013;90:628–638. doi: 10.1097/OPX.0b013e31829908a4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Kooi FL, Toet A, Tripathy SP, Levi DM. The effect of similarity and duration on spatial interaction in peripheral vision. Spat. Vis. 1994;8:255–279. doi: 10.1163/156856894X00350. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
The datasets generated during and/or analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.