Abstract
Whether well-being and depressive symptoms can be considered as two sides of the same coin is widely debated. The aim of this study was to gain insight into the etiology of the association between well-being and depressive symptoms across the lifespan. In a large twin-design, including data from 43,427 twins between age 7 and 99, we estimated the association between well-being and depressive symptoms throughout the lifespan and assessed genetic and environmental contributions to the observed overlap. For both well-being (range 31–47%) and depressive symptoms (range 49–61%), genetic factors explained a substantial part of the phenotypic variance across the lifespan. Phenotypic correlations between well-being and depressive symptoms across ages ranged from −0.34 in childhood to −0.49 in adulthood. In children, genetic effects explained 49% of the phenotypic correlation while in adolescents and young adults, genetic effects explained 60–77% of the phenotypic correlations. Moderate to high genetic correlations (ranging from −0.59 to −0.66) were observed in adolescence and adulthood, while in childhood environmental correlations were substantial but genetic correlations small. Our results suggest that in childhood genetic and environmental effects are about equally important in explaining the relationship between well-being and depressive symptoms. From adolescence onwards, the role of genetic effects increases compared to environmental effects. These results provided more insights into the etiological underpinnings of well-being and depressive symptoms, possibly allowing to articulate better strategies for health promotion and resource allocation in the future.
Keywords: well-being, depressive symptoms, heritability, childhood, adolescence, adulthood
Introduction
Well-being plays an important role in scientific disciplines such as psychology, medicine, and public health policy (1–3). Also, well-being is a topic of great interest in disciplines such as economics with increasing numbers of studies exploring the link between economic factors and well-being [e.g., “whether money buys happiness”, see (4, 5)]. In contemporary sciences, well-being is often defined by a continuous spectrum of positive feelings and subjective life assessments, which is in line with the description of Diener et al. (6), who explained well-being as a broad category of phenomena that includes people's emotional responses, and global judgments of life satisfaction. A considerable number of studies show that well-being is positively associated with physical health (7), success (8), and longevity (3, 9). Additionally, well-being is associated with less mental illness, notably mood disorders such as depressive symptoms (10–12). To this end, the world health organization (WHO) has recommended that national (mental)-health policies should actively promote well-being, rather than focusing exclusively on the prevention of (mental)-health disorders (13–15). Crucial, however, to the possible use of well-being promotion to target depressive symptom is knowledge on the nature of the association between well-being and depressive symptoms throughout the lifespan.
Traditionally, well-being and depressive symptoms have been considered as opposite ends of a continuum: that low scores on depressive symptoms are considered to be indicative of high levels of well-being and vice versa (16). However, measures of well-being and depressive symptoms are only moderately correlated (between −0.40 and −0.55 in the general population (10–12). This suggests that well-being and depressive symptoms belong to separable but correlated dimensions (16, 17). For example, it is possible to score low on psychiatric problems but not necessarily score high on well-being, or to score high on psychiatric problems and exhibit high levels of well-being (11, 17, 18). Whether well-being and depressive symptoms are two sides of the same coin is still widely debated, and more research is needed to understand the commonalities and specificities underlying this association.
Twin-family studies are important in unraveling phenotypic associations. More specifically, twin studies can be used to examine the role of shared genetic and environmental influences in the relationship between traits (19), and have demonstrated that genetic factors play a substantial role in explaining the observed phenotypic correlation between well-being and depressive symptoms (10, 20–23). For example, genetic influences explain between 33 and 66% of the phenotypic association between well-being and depressive symptoms in adolescents (10, 20). Twin studies on the association between well-being and depressive symptoms in adult populations yield similar bivariate heritability estimates ranging between 40 and 74% (21–23). Additionally, these studies report genetic correlations (a quantification of the extent to which two traits are influenced by the same genes) between well-being and depression in the range of −0.55 to −0.79, which is consistent with a recent large scale genome-wide association study that reported a genetic correlation of −0.75 (24).
While these findings extend our knowledge on the association between well-being and depressive symptoms, some limitations exist. First, the current literature is limited to adolescent (10, 20) and adult samples (17, 21–23). Given the growing interest in well-being promotion across the lifespan (14, 15), and the interest of policy makers in early-years interventions to reduce childhood risks (25), it is important to extend these studies to younger ages. Second, studies thus far focused on a specific age group, namely either adolescence or adulthood. As a result, the current literature lacks a perspective on the well-being—depressive symptom relationship throughout the lifespan. Given the complex development of depressive symptoms from childhood into adulthood (26), the changing genetic, and environmental architecture of depressive symptoms over age (27), and the genetic stability of well-being (28, 29), it is possible that contributions of genetic or environmental factors to the relationship between well-being and depressive symptoms vary over the lifespan. Therefore, a genetically informed study from childhood to adulthood is required to provide the necessary insights into the sources of phenotypic overlap between well-being and depression and thereby detect possible vulnerable but also malleable periods.
In sum, the aim of the present study is to evaluate the contributions of genetic and environmental factors to the association between well-being and depressive symptoms over the lifespan using an informative twin-design. Doing so, we provide better insight into the etiological underpinnings of the association, possibly articulating more targeted models of well-being promotion.
Methods
Sample
Participants were registered with the Netherlands Twin Registry (NTR), which consists of the Young NTR (YNTR) (30) and the Adult NTR (ANTR) (31). Subject recruitment is ongoing and is based on voluntary basis, for example, through the website of the register (https://www.tweelingenregister.org/) and through the “Dutch association for parents of multiples,” NVOM (https://www.nvom.nl/). The YNTR twins were registered with the NTR as newborns and were followed throughout childhood and adolescence. Parents completed questionnaires concerning their children when the children were approximately 1, 2, 3, 5, 7, 9/10, and 12 years old. The parents were asked for consent to send their children self-report surveys from age 14 onwards. Given parental consent, twins and their non-twin-siblings received an online or a paper self-report survey when they were 14, 16, or 18 years old. When young twins reached the age of 18, they were enrolled in the ANTR. The ANTR includes adolescence and adults, who were recruited through city councils and by other means (31), and who receive self-report questionnaires every 2–3 years. Participants are allowed to unsubscribe at any moment.
The current study included all twins between the ages 7 and 99 years, with data on either or both well-being and depressive symptoms. Specifically, we included twins from the YNTR at age 7, 9/10, 12, 14, and 16 years old. Data from several age groups are collapsed, because of the relatively recent addition of well-being questions to the survey studies of the NTR. ANTR participants were divided in young adults (age-range 18–27 years) and adults (>27 years). We decided on the age bracket of 27 years for the following reasons. First, this cut-off is in line with earlier twin studies involving comparable traits (27). Second, in the Dutch population, the late twenties are characterized by new “live events” such as fulltime working live, considering marriage and having children (32, 33). Third, in order to obtain reliable estimates a minimum sample size is essential. With a cut-off at age 27 reasonable sample sizes are obtained in each age group. The total dataset comprised 42,427 twins, including 16,089 monozygotic (MZ) and 26,338 same-sex and opposite-sex dizygotic (DZ) twins. The majority (54.6%) participated in more than one NTR survey study; with 16.9% taking part 3 times or more. Participants came from all regions of the Netherlands, both rural and urban areas, and were primarily Caucasian. For a detailed overview of included participants in the different age bins for well-being and depressive symptoms respectively see Supplementary Table 1 and Supplementary Figure 1.
Measures
Maternal and self-report ratings based on the Cantril ladder (34) were analyzed for children and (young) adults. The ladder has 10 steps where step 10 indicates the best possible life, and step 1 indicates the worst possible life. Participants were asked to indicate well-being by choosing the step which corresponded to the evaluation of their general well-being (self-ratings, age 14 >) or the general well-being of their child (maternal ratings, age 7–12). In our previous work (35), we report on moderate to strong positive correlations between the Cantril ladder and other measures of well-being [see also (2, 36)]. This measure is frequently used [e.g., see (4)]. Test-retest analyses showed test-retest correlations between 0.66 and 0.70; (37) as well as a substantial degree of concurrent validity with multi-item well-being scales [correlation between 0.62 and 0.64; (38)].
Depressive symptoms were assessed by the “Anxious/Depressed” subscale of the age-appropriate survey of the Achenbach's System of Empirical Based Assessment (ASEBA). At ages 7, 9/10, and 12, maternal reports on the Child Behavior Checklist [CBCL/4-18; (39)] were collected. Participants in the age range 14–16 years, completed the Youth Self Report [YSR; (40)] and adults completed the Adult Self Report [ASR; (41)]. The instruments were designed to measure comparable constructs over the ages. All instruments collect symptom information on a 3-point scale, “Not true,” “somewhat true or sometimes true,” “very or often true.” Reliability and validity tests of the “Anxious/Depressed” subscale revealed test-retest correlations in the range of 0.74–0.82 with a Cronbach's alpha of 0.84 (39, 41).
Strategy of analyses
Descriptive statistics and phenotypic correlations
Descriptive statistics and phenotypic correlations between well-being and depressive symptoms were calculated in R (42). Furthermore, we tested for main effects of sex and age on the two phenotypes.
Bivariate genetic modeling
We applied structural equation modeling to the twin data to estimate contributions of genetic and environmental factors to the phenotypic variance of well-being and depressive symptoms and to their phenotypic covariance. The classical twin design exploits the fact that monozygotic (MZ) twins are genetically identical and dizygotic twins (DZ) share on average 50% of their genetic material to estimate genetic, shared, and unshared environmental variance components. We can estimate additive genetic (A), shared environmental (C), non-shared environmental (E), and dominance genetic (D) components of variance and covariance. As C and D both increase the DZ correlation relative to the AE model they cannot be identified simultaneous in the presence of A and E and will therefore be modeled separately (43).
The depressive symptoms scores were strongly skewed (L-shaped distribution). Such non-normality may bias estimates of environmental influences on the phenotype (44). Thus, we categorized the depressive symptoms data into three (low, middle, high) groups, and analyzed it as an ordinal variable, assuming an underlying liability with a normal distribution and with two thresholds (45). The variance in the liability is subject to the decomposition into genetic and environmental components. The two-threshold model determines the prevalence of the low, middle, and high depressive symptoms scores. The well-being measure was modestly skewed to the right, but largely characterized by a bell-shaped curve and was analyzed as a continuous variable.
The bivariate genetic analyses were performed in OpenMx (46). Within each age-group we estimated the summary statistics separately in the MZmales (MZM), DZmales (DZM), MZfemales (MZF), DZfemales (DZF), and Dizyogitic Opposite Sex (DOS) twin pairs. We estimated the thresholds of the ordinal variables separately in males and females. Sex differences in well-being mean scores and prevalence in depressive symptoms were analyzed by testing whether the means (well-being) or thresholds (depressive symptoms) of males and females could be constrained to be equal.
We used the log-likelihood ratio test to evaluate the significance of parameter estimates. This involves fitting the model with and without the constraints of interest, and basing the test statistic on difference in minus twice the differences in loglikelihood of the models, i.e., the log likelihood ratio (LLR). If the constraints of interest are tenable, this statistic follows a central χ2 distribution with degrees of freedom (df) equal to difference in number of free parameters in the two models. The more parsimonious model (i.e., including the constraints of interest) is rejected if the LLR statistic exceeds the value of p < 0.005 (47). If this is not the case, the more parsimonious model is retained.
We estimated genetic and environmental contributions to the bivariate phenotypic covariance matrix by decomposing the phenotypic covariance matrix into (2 × 2) A, C, and E covariance matrices, or (2 × 2) A, D, and E covariance matrices. Bivariate heritability is a function of the heritability of the two traits and the genetic correlation (see Figure 1). Bivariate heritability tell us what the contribution is of genetic factors to the phenotypic association of well-being and depressive symptoms. The genetic correlation quantifies the extent to which two traits are influenced by the same genes regardless of the magnitude of the contribution of genes (the bivariate heritability) to the phenotypic variance of the traits. We parameterized the covariance matrices using a bivariate Cholesky decomposition (46). We first considered the full ACE or ADE bivariate models, and then fitted reduced models, in which we tested various parameters (e.g., the variance components due to shared environmental or dominance effects). Having established the best fitting bivariate models based on log likelihood tests, we calculated 95% confident intervals of all free parameters in the model of choice.
Results
Descriptives and phenotypic correlations
Means, standard deviations, and thresholds of males and females in all age-groups are provided in Table 1. The means of both maternal and self-reported ratings of depressive symptoms were significantly higher in females (p < 0.005), with largest effect size observed at age 16 (Cohen's d = −0.62). Sex differences in well-being scores were observed and strongest in adolescence at age 14. At this age, females reported lower levels of well-being (p < 0.005, Cohen's d = 0.19), but the effect was smaller compared to sex differences in depressive symptoms scores. In general, depressive symptoms scores tended to increase with age, whereas well-being scores were more stable, but it shows a decrease from adolescence onwards in both sexes (Supplementary Table 1).
Table 1.
Age 7 Mean (sd) | Age 10 Mean (sd) | Age 12 Mean (sd) | Age 14 Mean (sd) | Age 16 Mean (sd) | Age 18–27 Mean (sd) | Age 27–99 Mean (sd) | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
MALES | |||||||
Mean | |||||||
Well-being | 8.39 (0.98) | 8.27 (1.05) | 8.22 (1.12) | 8.06 (1.03) | 7.82 (1.03) | 7.57 (1.10) | 7.76 (1.01) |
Depressive symptoms | 2.19 (2.58) | 2.26 (2.79) | 1.99 (2.69) | 2.57 (2.83) | 2.63 (2.80) | 3.54 (3.81) | 2.77 (3.16) |
CATEGORIES | |||||||
Depressive symptoms | |||||||
Threshold 1 | 0.05 | 0.07 | −0.31 | 0.31 | 0.26 | −0.46 | −0.20 |
Threshold 2 | 0.65 | 0.69 | 0.42 | 0.90 | 0.63 | 0.38 | 0.42 |
Low | 52.0% | 52.9% | 37.7% | 62.3% | 60.1% | 32.3% | 42.0% |
Middle | 25.8% | 24.4% | 33.7% | 18.3% | 26.6% | 35.3% | 33.6% |
High | 22.2% | 22.7% | 28.5% | 19.4% | 13.3% | 32.4% | 24.4% |
FEMALES | |||||||
Mean | |||||||
Well-being | 8,42 (0.95) | 8.37 (0.98) | 8.27 (1.16) | 7.85 (1.16) | 7.63 (1.13) | 7.51 (1.09) | 7.67 (1.12) |
Depressive symptoms | 2.36 (2.61) | 2.44 (2.90) | 2.21 (2.76) | 4.51 (3.90) | 4.91 (4.08) | 5.25 (4.55) | 4.28 (3.86) |
CATEGORIES | |||||||
Depressive symptoms | |||||||
Threshold 1 | −0.04 | −0.01 | −0.45 | −0.34 | −0.46 | −0.92 | −0.71 |
Threshold 2 | 0.59 | 0.66 | 0.38 | 0.75 | 0.46 | 0.47 | 0.4 |
Low | 48.2% | 49.6% | 32.7% | 36.6% | 32.3% | 17.7% | 23.6% |
Middle | 27.8% | 25.6% | 35.3% | 22.5% | 32.2% | 32.0% | 34.6% |
High | 24.0% | 24.7% | 32.0% | 40.9% | 35.5% | 50.3% | 41.8% |
Well-being and depressive symptoms are significantly correlated and the correlation increases with age, ranging from −0.34 during childhood to −0.49 in adulthood (see Table 2).
Table 2.
Phenotypic | MZ | DZ | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
WB | WB | DS | WB | DS | |
WB y7 | 1 | 0.85 (0.83, 0.87) | 0.66 (0.62, 0.69) | ||
DS y7 | −0.34 (−0.38 to −0.28) | −0.30 (−0.37, −0.22) | 0.71 (0.68, 0.73) | −0.36 (−0.41, −0,30) | 0.46 (0.44, 0.48) |
WB y10 | 1 | 0.79 (0.76, 0.81) | 0.62 (0.59, 0.65) | ||
DS y10 | −0.41 (−0.45 to −0.35) | −0.43 (−0.48, −0.35) | 0.71 (0.68, 0.73) | −0.45 (−0.50, −0.41) | 0.45 (0.42, 0.48) |
WB y12 | 1 | 0.83 (0.81, 0.85) | 0.63 (0.60, 0.65) | ||
DS y12 | −0.39 (−0.43 to −0.34) | −0.34 (−0.40, −0.28) | 0.70 (0.67, 0.72) | −0.39 (−0.43, −0.35) | 0.46 (0.43, 0.48) |
WB y14 | 1 | 0.46 (0.42, 0.50) | 0.25 (0.21, 0.29) | ||
DS y14 | −0.44 (−0.48 to −0.38) | −0.38 (−0.43, −0.33) | 0.60 (0.55, 0.64) | −0.38 (−0.42, −0.34) | 0.28 (0.22, 0.33) |
WB y16 | 1 | 0.47 (0.42, 0.52) | 0.21 (0.16, 0.26) | ||
DS y16 | −0.47 (−0.50 to −0.40) | −0.44 (−0.50, −0.39) | 0.52 (0.46, 0.57) | −0.40 (−0.44, −0.35) | 0.23 (0.16, 0.29) |
WB y18–27 | 1 | 0.42 (0.37, 0.48) | 0.16 (0.11, 0.22) | ||
DS y18–27 | −0.57 (−0.59 to −0.50) | −0.51 (−0.56, −0.45) | 0.56 (0.50, 0.62) | −0.53 (−0.58, −0.49) | 0.28 (0.21, 0.35) |
WB >27y | 1 | 0.30 (0.25, 0.35) | 0.11 (0.04, 0.19) | ||
DS > 27y | −0.49 (−0.54 to −0.45) | −0.50 (−0.55, −0.45) | 0.49 (0.43, 0.54) | −0.56 (−0.60, −0.50) | 0.15 (0.06, 0.23) |
Twin correlations
MZ and DZ twin correlations and cross-twin-cross trait correlations for each age-group are displayed in Table 2. In childhood (i.e., age < 14 years), the MZ correlations of both phenotypes were lower than twice the DZ correlations indicating the contribution of additive genetic (A) shared environment (C) and unique environmental (E) effects to the phenotypes. In adolescence and adulthood, both MZ and DZ correlations decreased, resulting in MZ correlations being larger than twice the DZ correlations, which suggests a role for dominant genetic effects (D) besides additive genetic effects. In these age-groups, an ADE model was fitted to the data to establish the presence of dominance genetic influences. Twin correlations and cross-twin cross trait correlations for each of the five zygosity and age-groups are summarized in Supplementary Table 2. The MZ correlations were always higher than the DZ correlations, indicating that genetic effects play a role in explaining individual differences in well-being and depressive symptoms. Sex differences were investigated by constraining the correlations of MZ males to MZ females and DZ males to DZ females. We observed sex differences in the correlation for well-being at ages 7, 14, and 18–27, and differences for depressive symptoms at age 14. The largest difference in twin correlations was observed at age 7 for well-being between MZ males (r = 0.82) and MZ females (r = 0.89). However, since the differences were relatively rare and small (largest Cohen's d is 0.3; Supplementary Table 3), we decided not to model sex specific effects in the variance decomposition. However, we did retain sex differences in the means and thresholds, allowing for a main effect of sex.
Bivariate genetic analyses
The proportions of phenotypic variance of well-being and depressive symptoms attributable to genetic variance, the heritability (h2), and environmental variance effects (i.e., c2 and e2) are displayed in Table 3 and Supplementary Figure 2. For both well-being and depressive symptoms, a substantial amount of the phenotypic variances was due to additive genetic effects. For well-being, genetic effects explained 31–47% of the phenotypic variation, while for depressive symptoms estimates were between 49 and 61%. Supplementary Table 4 shows the result of the Cholesky decompositions, illustrating the full model and sub-models that were tested. Models in bold are judged to provide the best model fit.
Table 3.
A | C | E | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
WB | DS | WB | DS | WB | DS | |
WB y7 | 0.43 (0.37–0.49) | 0.43 (0.37–0.49) | 0.13 (0.12–0.16) | |||
DS y7 | 0.49 (0.29–0.70) | 0.49 (0.29–0.70) | 0.29 (0.10–0.48) | 0.20 (0.15–0.25) | 0.21 (0.16–0.27) | 0.29 (0.27–0.31) |
WB y10 | 0.40 (0.34–0.47) | 0.41 (0.35–0.46) | 0.20 (0.17–0.21) | |||
DS y10 | 0.41 (0.26–0.57) | 0.53 (0.46–0.60) | 0.30 (0.17–0.44) | 0.18 (0.12–0.23) | 0.28 (0.23 −0.34) | 0.28 (0.27–0.31) |
WB y12 | 0.36 (0.31–0.41) | 0.46 (0.41–0.50) | 0.18 (0.17–0.20) | |||
DS y12 | 0.49 (0.32–0.66) | 0.50 (0.43–0.58) | 0.23 (0.08–0.38) | 0.19 (0.14–0.26) | 0.28 (0.22–0.34) | 0.30 (0.27–0.32) |
WB y14 | 0.47 (0.43–0.50) | – | 0.53 (0.50–0.57) | |||
DS y14 | 0.77 (0.70–0.84) | 0.60 (0.57–0.65) | – | – | 0.23 (0.16–0.30) | 0.39 (0.35–0.43) |
WB y16 | 0.45 (0.32–0.50) | – | 0.55 (0.51–0.59) | |||
DS y16 | 0.68 (0.47–0.78) | 0.53 (0.42–0.58) | – | – | 0.32 (0.25–0.40) | 0.47 (0.42–0.52) |
WB y18–27 | 0.42 (0.37–0.47) | – | 0.58 (0.53–0.63) | |||
DS y18–27 | 0.60 (0.52–0.67) | 0.57 (0.52–0.62) | – | – | 0.40 (0.33–0.48) | 0.43 (0.38–0.48) |
WB > 27y | 0.31 (0.18–0.36) | – | 0.69 (0.64–0.75) | |||
DS > 27y | 0.46 (0.36–0.58) | 0.50 (0.42–0.55) | – | – | 0.54 (0.46–0.62) | 0.50 (0.45–0.55) |
The bivariate Cholesky decomposition provide the decomposition of the variance of the two phenotypes and the decomposition of their covariance into genetic and environmental components (Supplementary Table 4). The results are presented in Figure 2 and Table 3. In childhood, additive genetic and shared environmental effects contribute significantly to the phenotypic correlations. The bivariate heritability ranged from 41 to 49% and bivariate shared environmental effects ranged from 23 to 30%. In adolescence and young adults, additive genetic and non-shared environmental factors contribute largely to the phenotypic correlation, with genetic effects explaining a slightly larger proportion of the phenotypic correlation (range 60–77%). In adults over 27 years, non-shared environmental effects explained 54% of the phenotypic correlation, with the rest explained by additive genetic effects.
Figure 3 and Table 4 shows the genetic correlations (rG) and environmental (rC/rE) correlations between well-being and depressive symptoms for all age-groups. In childhood, we observe moderate genetic and environmental correlations, indicating that, while part of the genetic (correlations ranged between −0.36 and −0.39) and environmental (rC ranged from −0.27 to −0.47 and rE ranging from −0.35 to −0.5) susceptibility to well-being and depressive symptoms overlap, there are substantial trait specific genetic and environmental influences. With increasing age, from adolescence onwards, genetic correlations seems to become more important (range −0.59 to −0.66), while the environmental correlations decrease and become limited to non-shared environmental overlap (range −0.20 to −0.48).
Table 4.
Age | rg | rc | re |
---|---|---|---|
7 | −0.36 (−0.49 to −0.20) | −0.33 (−0.54 to −0.11) | −0.35 (−0.44 to −0.27) |
10 | −0.37 (−0.50 to −0.24) | −0.47 (−0.67 to −0.27) | −0.50 (−0.57 to −0.42) |
12 | −0.39 (−0.51 to −0.26) | −0.27 (−0.42 to −0.09) | −0.41 (−0.48 to −0.33) |
14 | −0.59 (−0.70 to −0.46) | – | −0.20 (−0.25 to −0.14) |
16 | −0.60 (−0.66 to −0.53) | – | −0.28 (−0.32 to −0.21) |
18-17 | −0.66 (−0.75 to −0.55) | – | −0.44 (−0.49 to −0.36) |
>27 | −0.60 (−0.68 to −0.52) | – | −0.48 (−0.53 to −0.42) |
Discussion
The aim of this study was to gain insight into the etiology of the association between well-being and depressive symptoms across the lifespan. Phenotypic correlations between well-being and depressive symptoms ranged from −0.34 in childhood, to −0.49 in adulthood, with the highest correlations in young adults (−0.57). Bivariate twin models revealed that shared environmental factors play an important role in explaining the relationship between well-being and depressive symptoms in childhood, while in adolescence and adulthood genetic factors become increasingly important.
The results of our study go beyond the available literature in several ways. First, the few twin studies carried out so far focused either on adolescents (10, 20) or adults (17, 21–23). Our study is the first to extend these analyses to middle childhood, investigating the association between well-being and depressive symptoms in a cohort-sequential design for age 7, 9/10, and 12, respectively. Results showed that common environmental factors (ranging between 23 and 30%), unique environmental factors (ranging between 21 and 28%), and genetic factors (ranging between 41 and 49%), explain the phenotypic correlation between well-being and depressive symptoms in middle childhood.
Second, instead of focusing on a specific age group, this study examined the association between well-being and depressive symptoms across the lifespan. This study allowed us to shed light on contributions of genetic and environmental factors at different ages. Remarkably, from childhood to adolescence a stark increase was found in the contribution of genetic factors. In adolescence, and young adults, 60–77% of the phenotypic association was explained by genetic factors, with no influence of the shared environment. When looking at the genetic correlations, indicating to what extend the same group of genes influence different traits, moderate to high genetic correlations (between rg = −0.59 and rg = −0.66) were observed in adolescence, while in childhood environmental correlations are substantial, but genetic correlations small (ranging between rg = −0.36 and rg = −0.39). These results show that environmental factors are important in explaining the relationship between well-being and depressive symptoms in childhood, while in adolescence genetic factors play a more substantial role. In adulthood, unique environmental effects showed to be increasingly important, explaining 54% of the phenotypic correlation (with bivariate heritability of 46%). Genetic correlations were high in adulthood, with rg = −0.60, showing overlap in genetic factors influencing both well-being and depressive symptoms. These results were consistent with the results of Kendler et al. (21), who reported similar genetic and phenotypic correlations. However, the proportion of the phenotypic correlation explained by genetic effects was larger in their study 86%, compared to 46% in our study. A possible explanation might be that the heritability of the latent factor mental well-being in Kendler et al. (21) was substantially higher (72%) than the heritability of our measure of well-being (31%). This is attributable to their assessment of well-being which is modeled with a latent factor allowing to correct more explicitly for measurement error. In our design, not modeling well-being as a latent factor, part of the measurement error falls into the E component instead of the additive genetic component explaining the discrepancy in heritability estimates (48).
Overall, the moderate phenotypic correlations between well-being and depressive symptoms in the present study support the notion that well-being and depressive symptoms could belong to distinct, but correlated, dimensions. However, results of the genetic informative twin design shows that shared genetic effects explain a substantial part of this phenotypic correlation, especially from adolescence onwards. This finding raises the question whether different interventions are needed for promoting well-being and treating depressive symptoms, or whether we can use the promotion of well-being to reduce depressive symptoms. On the one hand, a growing body of literature suggests that, based on the unique environmental influences on both well-being and depressive symptoms, interventions targeting well-being may not necessarily have a direct impact on depressive symptoms (17, 20). On the other hand, empirical studies suggest that improved positive emotions enhance coping skills, weaken physiological effects of negative emotions and diminish relapses in depressed individuals (49–53). Additionally, a recent meta-analysis on the effectiveness of positive psychology interventions, including 51 studies and 4,266 individuals, illustrate that, overall, enhancing well-being with positive psychology interventions significantly decrease depressive symptoms (54). These findings, together with our results, suggest that well-being could be used in future studies as an index of mental health complementing other indices that focus on mental illness.
Still, the question remains if these findings hold for the prevention of depressive symptoms by early screening and well-being promotion. Put differently, can we use measures of well-being to inform us about vulnerability to depression? Benefits of this approach include the low stigma associated with the content of well-being questionnaires compared to depressive symptom screening (i.e., people are more willing to answer questions on their quality of life than on their depressive symptoms), and the possibility of screening those at risk in a timely manner. The relatively strong genetic correlation implies that we can identify individuals characterized by low well-being, and offer them suitable interventions to improve their well-being. Even stronger effect may be anticipated if we consider well-being promotion at a population level. Within epidemiology and somatic medicine, it has been proposed that larger benefits to overall public health are to be expected when the bell curve of mental health in the human population is shifted slightly to the healthy side, the so-called population strategy (55, 56). Specifically, a relative slight increase in the level of well-being of the majority of the population may have a larger preventive effect, than targeting the much smaller group of people at high risk or in the early stages of depressive symptoms.
Future studies should, however, focus on the direction of the relationship between well-being and depressive symptoms to use well-being as a possible candidate for novel approaches to reduce depressive symptoms. Recent methodological developments such as Mendelian randomization (MR) designs (57, 58) together with the availability of large scale molecular genetic data provide additional opportunities to address the process underlying the correlation between well-being and depressive symptoms.
Limitations
This study has several strengths and weaknesses. First, well-being is a complex phenotype consisting of two well-recognized constructs: Subjective Well-being (SWB) and Psychological Well-being (PWB), shaped by the philosophical concepts of hedonism and eudaimonism, respectively (59). Hedonic well-being is centered around pleasure, or how good a person feels about his or her life, whereas eudaimonic well-being is centered around living well or doing well and the fulfillment of human capacities (60). We recognize that, by using the Cantril ladder, the present study does not capture the complete construct of well-being, but rather focuses on SWB. We are confident however, that our results are representative for SWB as the different questionnaires measuring SWB used in social and behavioral sciences correlate highly with the Cantril ladder, both phenotypically and genetically (35). Second, it is important to keep in mind that high scores on the CBCL, YSR, and ASR “Anxiety and Depressive symptoms” subscales are good predictors of depressive symptoms (61), but are not equivalent to a clinical diagnosis of depression (62). Third, due to highly skewed scores, we analyzed the depressive symptoms data using a threshold model, resulting in lower statistical power compared to an analysis of continuous data (44). However, the parameter estimates in a threshold model are more accurate than in an analysis of continuous data characterized by large skewness (63). Fourth, in adolescence and adulthood, both MZ and DZ correlations decreased, resulting in MZ correlations being larger than twice the DZ correlations, suggesting a role for dominant genetic effects besides additive genetic effects. We recognize that this might be a methodological artifact as a result of the difference between parent rating (e.g., parents phenotype possibly contribute to similar ratings for both twins resulting in shared environmental influences) and self-rating scores (e.g., a twin's own genetic architecture contributing to their own behavior and therefore self-rating scores on well-being and depressive symptoms) (48, 64–66). However, while differences in parent reports and child reports exist, earlier studies have illustrated sufficient agreement between child and parent reports on children's quality of life (67). Fifth, earlier research (68) has postulated that a U-shaped pattern of well-being mean-scores over time exist. It is important to note that the method applied in this study focuses on variance decomposition, rather than mean comparison. Additional analyses specifically exploring the mean of well-being over time did not yield a U-shaped pattern. Therefore we believe this does not influence the results presented in our paper.
Future recommendations
As the genetic and environmental factors explaining the relation between well-being and depressive symptoms differ between the included age-bins, future studies are needed to study the etiology of the relationship between well-being and depressive symptoms in more depth. Longitudinal twin designs such as the genetic simplex model or common factor model (with or without age specific influences) allow for estimation of the stability of the effects of genetic and environmental factors over time, and show to what extent genetic innovation come into play. Additionally, future studies are recommended to investigate key biological and environmental factors of relevance to well-being and depressive symptoms. For example, Routledge et al. (69) investigated the link between well-being, depression and cognitive functioning (and their genetic and environmental overlap). They illustrated some differentiation, with well-being in some cases related to specific cognitive functions independent of depression while for other cognitive functions they showed an overlap between well-being and depression. Additionally, the first genetic variants associated with both well-being and depressive symptoms are recently identified (24). With the increasing availability of large-scale genetic data, it would be interesting to study whether different genetic variants are associated with well-being over age and whether these variants have a protective effect on the development of depressive symptoms. Finally, further research should not isolate genetic and environmental influences and preferably apply multi-layer designs incorporate both aspects explaining the underlying etiology of well-being and depressive symptoms. Finally, although this study is about variance decomposition with relatively small sex differences we observed larger sex differences in mean scores especially from adolescence onwards. Future studies should focus on the origin of these differences especially in (pre)-clinical settings. Furthermore, larger studies are needed to investigate sex-differences in variance components as the presence or absence is still inconclusive [see also review (28)].
Conclusion
In the present study we dissected the association between well-being and depressive symptoms from childhood to adulthood. We confirmed that well-being and depressive symptoms correlate moderately across the lifespan. Importantly, shared environmental factors play an important role in explaining the relationship between well-being and depressive symptoms in childhood. However, from adolescence onward, we found evidence for the prominence of shared genetic effects, with genetic factors explaining a substantial part of the phenotypic correlation from adolescence onward. Therewith, this study provided more insights into the etiological underpinnings of well-being and depressive symptoms, possibly allowing to articulate better strategies for health promotion and resource allocation in the future.
Data availability
Datasets used in the present studies are available on request to any qualified researcher.
Research involving human participants
All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments and comparable ethical standards.
Ethics statement
Data collection was approved by the Central Ethics Committee on Research Involving Human Subjects of the VU University Medical Centre Amsterdam, An Institutional Review Board certified by the US Office of Human Research Protections (IRB number IRB00002991 under Federal-wide Assurance- FWA00017598; IRB/institute codes, NTR 03-180).
Informed consent
Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.
Author contributions
BB wrote the manuscript, performed the main analyses and designed all (supplementary) figures and tables. YW wrote the manuscript and helped with the analyses. MB supervised the project, curated the data and wrote the manuscript. DB supervised the project and curate the data, CvB, LL, and GW were involved in data collection and data curation. CD helped with the analyses and writing.
Conflict of interest statement
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest. The reviewer EC and handling Editor declared their shared affiliation.
Acknowledgments
Authors thank all NTR participants, who participated in this study.
Footnotes
Funding. This study was funded by the following grants: Spinozapremie (NWO/SPI 56-464-14192); twin-family database for behavior genetics and genomics studies (NWO 480-04-004); Genetics of Mental Illness: European Research Council (ERC-230374); Genetic influences on stability and change in psychopathology from childhood to young adulthood (NWO/ZonMW 91210020); NARSAD Brain & Behavior Research Foundation-18633; Determinants Of Adolescent Exercise Behavior (NIH-1R01DK092127-01). The Netherlands organization for Scientific Research (NWO): Genetic and Family influences on Adolescent psychopathology and Wellness (NWO 463-06-001); A twin-sib study of adolescent wellness (NWO-VENI 451-04-034). MB and DB are supported by the European Union Seventh Framework Program (FP7/2007-2013, grant no. 602768). YW was supported by the research talent fund (NWO 406-15-132). Netherlands Twin Registry repository: researching the interplay between genome and environment (NWO 480-15-001/674). MB and BB are financially supported by the University Research Chair position of MB.
Supplementary material
The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00261/full#supplementary-material
References
- 1.Diener E, Oishi S, Lucas RE. National accounts of subjective well-being. Am Psychol. (2015) 70:234–42. 10.1037/a0038899 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Helliwell J, Layard R, Sachs J. World Happiness Report 2016, Update (Vol. I). (2017) New York, NY: Sustainable Development Solutions Network; (2016). [Google Scholar]
- 3.Steptoe A, Deaton A, Stone A. Subjective wellbeing, health, and ageing. Lancet (2014) 385:640–8. 10.1016/S0140-6736(13)61489-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Kahneman D, Deaton A. High income improves evaluation of life but not emotional well-being. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. (2010) 107:16489–93. 10.1073/pnas.1011492107 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Layard R. Happiness and public policy: a challenge to the profession. Econ J. (2006) 116:24–33. 10.1111/j.1468-0297.2006.01073.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Diener E, Suh EM, Lucas RE, Smith HL. Subjective well-being: three decades of progress. Psychol Bull. (1999) 125:276–302. 10.1037/0033-2909.125.2.276 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Boehm JK, Kubzansky LD. The heart's content: The association between positive psychological well-being and cardiovascular health. Psychol Bull. (2012) 138:655–91. 10.1037/a0027448 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Lyubomirsky S, King L, Diener E. The benefits of frequent positive affect: does happiness lead to success? Psychol Bull. (2005) 131:803–55. 10.1037/0033-2909.131.6.803 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Veenhoven R. Healthy happiness: effects of happiness on physical health and the consequences for preventive health care. J Happiness Stud. (2008) 9:449–69. 10.1007/s10902-006-9042-1 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Bartels M, Cacioppo JT, van Beijsterveldt TC, Boomsma DI. Exploring the association between well-being and psychopathology in adolescents. Behav Genet. (2013) 43:177–90. 10.1007/s10519-013-9589-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Greenspoon PJ, Saklofske DH. Toward an integration of subjective well-being and psychopathology. Soc Indic Res. (2010) 54:81–108. 10.1023/A:1007219227883 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Proctor CL, Linley PA, Maltby J. Youth life satisfaction: a review of the literature. J Happiness Stud. (2009) 10:583–630. 10.1007/s10902-008-9110-9 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Healy T, Côté S, Helliwell J, Field S. The well-being of nations. Oecd Org (2001) 7. [Google Scholar]
- 14.World Health Organization Prevention of Mental Disorders: Effective Interventions and Policy Options. World Health (2004) 68 Available online at: http://www.who.int/entity/mental_health/evidence/en/prevention_of_mental_disorders_sr.pdf
- 15.World Health Organization Mental Health: Strengthening our Response; Fact sheet N°220 (2010).
- 16.Gatt JM, Burton KLO, Schofield PR, Bryant RA, Williams LM. The heritability of mental health and wellbeing defined using COMPAS-W, a new composite measure of wellbeing. Psychiatry Res. (2014) 219:204–13. 10.1016/j.psychres.2014.04.033 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Routledge KM, Burton KLO, Williams LM, Harris A, Schofield PR, Clark CR, et al. Shared versus distinct genetic contributions of mental wellbeing with depression and anxiety symptoms in healthy twins. Psychiatry Res. (2016) 244:65–70. 10.1016/j.psychres.2016.07.016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Keyes CLM. Mental Illness and/or Mental Health? Investigating Axioms of the Complete State Model of Health. J Consult Clin Psychol. (2005) 73:539–48. 10.1037/0022-006X.73.3.539 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Boomsma D, Busjahn A, Peltonen L. Classical twin studies and beyond. Nat Rev Genet. (2002) 3:872–82. 10.1038/nrg932 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Haworth CMA, Carter K, Eley TC, Plomin R. Understanding the genetic and environmental specificity and overlap between well-being and internalizing symptoms in adolescence. Dev Sci. (2015) 20:e12376. 10.1111/desc.12376 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Kendler KS, Myers JM, Maes HH, Keyes CLM. The relationship between the genetic and environmental influences on common internalizing psychiatric disorders and mental well-being. Behav Genet. (2011) 41:641–50. 10.1007/s10519-011-9466-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Nes RB, Czajkowski N, Roysamb E, Reichborn-Kjennerud T, Tambs K. Well-being and ill-being: shared environments, shared genes? J Posit Psychol. (2008) 3:253–65. 10.1080/17439760802399323 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Nes RB, Czajkowski NO, Roysamb E, Orstavik RE, Tambs K, Reichborn-Kjennerud T. Major depression and life satisfaction: a population-based twin study. J Affect Disord. (2013) 144:51–8. 10.1016/j.jad.2012.05.060 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Okbay A, Baselmans BML, De Neve JE, Turley P, Nivard MG, Fontana MA, et al. Genetic variants associated with subjective well-being, depressive symptoms, and neuroticism identified through genome-wide analyses. Nat Genet. (2016) 48:624–33. 10.1038/ng.3552 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Caspi A, Houts RM, Belsky DW, Harrington H, Hogan S, Ramrakha S, et al. Childhood forecasting of a small segment of the population with large economic burden. Nat Hum Behav. (2016) 1:5. 10.1038/s41562-016-0005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Polanczyk GV, Salum GA, Sugaya LS, Caye A, Rohde LA. Annual research review: a meta-analysis of the worldwide prevalence of mental disorders in children adolescents. J Child Psychol Psychiatry Allied Discip. (2015) 56:345–65. 10.1111/jcpp.12381 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Nivard MG, Dolan CV, Kendler KS, Kan K.-J., Willemsen G, van Beijsterveldt CEM, et al. Stability in symptoms of anxiety and depression as a function of genotype and environment: a longitudinal twin study from ages 3 to 63 years. Psychol Med. (2014) 45:1039–49. 10.1017/S003329171400213X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Bartels M. Genetics of happiness, satisfaction with life, and wellbeing; a review and meta-analysis of heritability studies. Behav Genet. (2015) 45:137–56. 10.1007/s10519-015-9713-y [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Nes RB, Røysamb E, Tambs K, Harris JR, Reichborn-Kjennerud T. Subjective well-being: genetic and environmental contributions to stability and change. Psychol Med. (2006) 36:1033–42. 10.1017/S0033291706007409 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.van Beijsterveldt CEM, Groen-Blokhuis M, Hottenga JJ, Franić S, Hudziak JJ, Lamb D, et al. The Young Netherlands Twin Register (YNTR): longitudinal twin and family studies in over (2013) 70,000 children. Twin Res Hum Genet. 16:252–67. 10.1017/thg.2012.118 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Willemsen G, Vink JM, Abdellaoui A, den Braber A, van Beek JHD, Draisma HHM, et al. The adult netherlands twin register: twenty-five years of survey and biological data collection. Twin Res Hum Genet. (2013) 16:271–81. 10.1017/thg.2012.140 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Souren MHJ. Het Werkende Leven van Twintigers, Centraal Bureau van de Statistiek. (2015).
- 33.te Riele SMM, Harmsen CN. Relatievorming van Twintigers, Centraal Bureau van de Statistiek. (2015). [Google Scholar]
- 34.Cantril H. The Pattern of Human Concern. New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press; (1965). [Google Scholar]
- 35.Bartels M, Boomsma DI. Born to be happy? the etiology of Subjective well-being. Behav Genet. (2009) 39:605–15. 10.1007/s10519-009-9294-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Adamson P. (2013). Child Well-being in Rich Countries: A Comparative Overview. Available online at: http://www.unicef-irc.org/publications/683
- 37.Levin KA, Currie C. Reliability and validity of an adapted version of the cantril ladder for use with adolescent samples. Soc Indic Res. (2013) 119:1047–63. 10.1007/s11205-013-0507-4 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Cheung F, Lucas RE. Assessing the validity of single-item life satisfaction measures: results from three large samples. Qual Life Res. (2014) 23:2809–18. 10.1007/s11136-014-0726-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Achenbach TM, Rescorla L. Manual for the ASEBA School -Age Forms & Profiles. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont Research Center for Children, Youth, & Families; (2001). [Google Scholar]
- 40.Achenbach TM. Manual for the Young Adult Self-Report and Young Adult Behavior Checklist. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont, Department of Psychiatry; (1997). [Google Scholar]
- 41.Achenbach TM, Rescorla L. Manual for the ASEBA Adult Forms & Profiles. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont; (2003). [Google Scholar]
- 42.R Core Team R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna (2017). Available online at: https://www.R-project.org/
- 43.Posthuma D, Beem AL, de Geus EJC, van Baal GCM, von Hjelmborg JBV, Iachine I, et al. Theory and practice in quantitative genetics. Twin Res. (2003) 6:361–76. 10.1375/136905203770326367 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Derks EM, Dolan CV, Boomsma DI. Effects of censoring on parameter estimates and power in genetic modeling. Twin Res Off J Int Soc Twin Stud. (2004) 7:659–69. 10.1375/1369052042663832 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Falconer DS, Mackay TFC. Introduction to quantitative genetics, 4th edition. Trends Genet. (1996) 12:280 10.1016/0168-9525(96)81458-2 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Boker S, Neale M, Maes H, Wilde M, Spiegel M, Brick T, et al. OpenMx: an open source extended structural equation modeling framework. Psychometrika (2011) 76:306–17. 10.1007/s11336-010-9200-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Benjamin DJ, Berger JO, Johannesson M, Nosek BA, Wagenmakers EJ, Berk R, et al. Redefine statistical significance. Nat Hum Behav. (2017) 2:6–10. 10.1038/s41562-017-0189-z [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Bartels M, Boomsma DI, Hudziak JJ, van Beijsterveldt TCEM, van den Oord EJCG. Twins and the study of rater (dis)agreement. Psychol Methods (2007) 12:451–66. 10.1037/1082-989X.12.4.451 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Boumparis N, Karyotaki E, Kleiboer A, Hofmann SG, Cuijpers P. The effect of psychotherapeutic interventions on positive and negative affect in depression: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J Affect Disord. (2016) 202:153–62. 10.1016/j.jad.2016.05.019 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Fredrickson BL, Levenson RW. Positive emotions speed recovery from the cardiovascular sequelae of negative emotions. Cogn Emot. (1998) 12:191–220. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Fredrickson BL, Joiner T. Positive emotions trigger upward spirals toward emotional well-being. Psychol Sci. (2002) 13:172–5. 10.1111/1467-9280.00431 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Fava GA, Ruini C. Development and characteristics of a well-being enhancing psychotherapeutic strategy: well-being therapy. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry (2003) 34:45–63. 10.1016/S0005-7916(03)00019-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Tugade MM, Fredrickson BL. Resilient individuals use positive emotions to bounce back from negative emotional experiences. J Pers Soc Psychol. (2004) 86:320–33. 10.1037/0022-3514.86.2.320 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Sin NL, Lyubomirsky S. Enhancing well-being and alleviating depressive symptoms with positive psychology interventions: a practice-friendly meta-analysis. J Clin Psychol. (2009) 65:467–87. 10.1002/jclp.20593 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Rose G. The Strategy of Preventive Medicine. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; (1992). [Google Scholar]
- 56.Rose GA, Khaw KT, Marmot MG. Rose's Strategy of Preventive Medicine: The Complete Original Text. Oxford: Oxford University Press; (2008). [Google Scholar]
- 57.Smith GD, Ebrahim S. “Mendelian randomization”: Can genetic epidemiology contribute to understanding environmental determinants of disease? Int J Epidemiol. (2003) 32:1–22. 10.1093/ije/dyg070 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Smith GD, Ebrahim S. Mendelian randomization: prospects, potentials, and limitations. Int J Epidemiol. (2004) 33:30–42. 10.1093/ije/dyh132 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Deci EL, Ryan RM. Hedonia, eudaimonia, and well-being: an introduction. J Happiness Stud. (2006) 9:1–11. 10.1007/s10902-006-9018-1 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Biswas-Diener R, Kashdan TB, King LA. Two traditions of happiness research, not two distinct types of happiness. J Posit Psychol. (2009) 4:208–11. 10.1080/17439760902844400 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Rescorla LA. Assessment of young children using the Achenbach System of Empirically Based Assessment (ASEBA). Ment Retard Dev Disabil Res Rev. (2005) 11:226–37. 10.1002/mrdd.20071 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Bird HR, Gould MS, Rubio-Stipec M, Staghezza BM, Canino G. Screening for childhood psychopathology in the community using the Child Behavior Checklist. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry (1991) 30:116–23. 10.1097/00004583-199101000-00018 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Posthuma D, Boomsma DI. A note on the statistical power in extended twin designs. Behav Genet. (2000) 30:147–58. 10.1023/A:1001959306025 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Hewitt JK, Silberg JL, Neale MC, Eaves LJ, Erickson M. The analysis of parental ratings of children's behavior using LISREL. Behav Genet. (1992) 22:293–317. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Kan KJ, van Beijsterveldt CEM, Bartels M, Boomsma DI. Assessing genetic influences on behavior: informant and context dependency as illustrated by the analysis of attention problems. Behav Genet. (2014) 44:326–36. 10.1007/s10519-014-9657-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Wesseldijk LW, Fedko IO, Bartels M, Nivard MG, van Beijsterveldt CEM, Boomsma DI, et al. Psychopathology in 7-year-old children: differences in maternal and paternal ratings and the genetic epidemiology. Am J Med Genet Part B Neuropsychiatr Genet. (2017) 174:251–60. 10.1002/ajmg.b.32500 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Theunissen NCM, Vogels TGC, Koopman HM, Verrips GHW, Zwinderman KAH, Verloove-Vanhorick SP, et al. The proxy problem: child report versus parent report in health-related quality of life research. Qual Life Res. (1998) 7:387–97. 10.1023/A:1008801802877 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Cheng TC, Powdthavee N, Oswald AJ. Longitudinal evidence for a midlife nadir in human well-being: results from four data sets. Econ J. (2017) 127:126–42. 10.1111/ecoj.12256 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Routledge KM, Burton KLO, Williams LM, Harris A, Schofield PR, Clark CR, et al. The shared and unique genetic relationship between mental well-being, depression and anxiety symptoms and cognitive function in healthy twins. Cogn Emot. (2017) 31:1464–1479. 10.1080/02699931.2016.1232242 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
Datasets used in the present studies are available on request to any qualified researcher.