Skip to main content
Scientific Reports logoLink to Scientific Reports
. 2018 Jun 27;8:9767. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-27751-0

Cross-Cultural Evidence for Apparent Racial Outgroup Advantage: Congruence between Perceived Facial Aggressiveness and Fighting Success

Vít Třebický 1,2,✉,#, S Adil Saribay 3,#, Karel Kleisner 1,2, Robert Mbe Akoko 4, Tomáš Kočnar 1, Jaroslava Varella Valentova 5, Marco Antonio Correa Varella 5, Jan Havlíček 1,2
PMCID: PMC6021408  PMID: 29950579

Abstract

Research into face processing consistently shows an outgroup disadvantage in areas such as recognition memory and emotional identification. Potential ingroup advantage with respect to inferences regarding personality and behavioural outcomes, on the other hand, has not yet been studied. In the present study, we used the faces of male professional mixed martial arts (MMA) fighters of apparent African, European, or mixed-race origin as targets and males from four distant populations that vary in ethnic composition as perceivers. We compared the perceivers’ inferences about targets’ aggressiveness with the fighters’ actual performance in professional MMA championships. Surprisingly, across three distant populations used in the study (Cameroon, Czech Republic, and Turkey), perceivers’ inferences based on face rating were more congruent with real-world performance for targets belonging to an apparent racial outgroup (as opposed to ingroup). In an ethnically mixed population (Brazil), perceivers showed the lowest congruence for apparently mixed-race targets. It thus seems that the outgroup disadvantage observed in other face processing domains does not carry over to inferences about aggressive behavioural outcomes. In fact, it seems that this relationship is, if anything, reversed.

Introduction

Each human face presents a unique constellation of morphological features. Based on a face, perceivers make inferences about the individual identity, social categories (age, gender, ethnic origin), affective expressions, and personality characteristics of its owner1. Interestingly, it turns out that perceivers’ inferences regarding behavioural tendencies and personality traits are to some degree congruent with the face owner’s self-reports2 and certain objective standards3.

Face processing is, however, also affected by various biases. The most commonly described bias is the tendency to recognise faces belonging to people from one’s apparent racial ingroup better than faces of persons from other groups4, referred to as the cross-race effect (CRE). This effect may be in part due to expertise, as indicated by the fact that increasing familiarity and contact with outgroups leads to a decrease in the CRE5,6, and even categorical inferences about targets (e.g., their sexual orientation) become more accurate7. Efficiency with which minimal8 and cultural ingroup faces are processed9,10 may also be due to greater motivation to attend to ingroup faces. Various accounts of the CRE emphasise these factors (such as expertise and motivation) and commonly assume that they lead to more efficient and careful encoding of ingroup faces, which, in turn, results in the CRE11.

Research into other aspects of face processing also consistently indicates an outgroup disadvantage. While outgroup faces may be categorised more rapidly as such12, this effect is closely linked to task features and may be part of a ‘quick and dirty’ approach that helps to disengage attention from outgroup faces as soon as possible13. Indeed, unless there is a specific reason to attend to outgroups, perceivers tend to preferentially direct their attention toward ingroup members’ faces14 (see Discussion). This attention bias affects especially the eye area, the most social-cognitively significant part of human face15, and it seems to be one of the reasons underlying the CRE14.

Likewise, inference of emotions from facial expressions is also more accurate for minimal and cultural ingroup, as opposed to outgroup, targets16,17. In the sole exception to this well-established pattern, Young18 quite unexpectedly found that perceivers were both more accurate and faster in distinguishing fake from genuine smiles in minimal outgroup targets. Hwang and Matsumoto19 have observed that American perceivers of European origin were more accurate in detecting deceitful communication of Chinese (as opposed to American of European origin) targets from interview videos. The design of this study, however, involved other factors and the pattern was observed only under specific conditions and not as an overall effect.

Finally, in research examining categorisation accuracy, where differences are present, perceivers tend to favour the ingroup. For instance, both Czechs and U.S. Americans are more accurate in differentiating homosexuals, based on thin slices of behaviour, among their respective co-nationals20. Judgments on sexual orientation based on facial images did not, however, reveal either ingroup or outgroup advantage21.

The results of existing research on accuracy of facial inferences about personality or behavioural outcomes which examined the effects of targets’ and perceivers’ group membership form a rather inconsistent pattern. Some studies focused on impressions (but not accuracy) found that perceivers from various cultures (e.g., U.S. Americans and Koreans) converge on facial impressions (such as dominance or attractiveness) owing partly to commonly held appearance-related stereotypes22. Even with such stereotypes in place, however, within-culture agreement on judgments regarding faces from racial outgroups is low, which indicates difficulty of processing other-race faces23. Some findings hint at a greater inferential accuracy for ingroup targets24, others for outgroup25 targets. Nonetheless, a particularly relevant study by Short et al.26 found that Chinese and Caucasian child and adult perceivers did not differ in their tendency to rely on facial features in judging ethnic ingroup and outgroup targets’ propensity for aggression, even though accuracy was not assessed.

The Present Study

Research showing an ingroup advantage/outgroup disadvantage in face processing (recognition, attention, categorisation, and identification of emotions) strongly suggests that congruence between facial inferences and behavioural outcomes should, if anything, be higher for apparent racial ingroup than outgroup faces. The present study tested this hypothesis with male targets and perceivers in four disparate cultures. As targets, we used male professional Mixed Martial Arts (MMA) fighters of different apparent races (European, African, and Mixed), because this allowed us to use both standardised facial images and an objective standard for congruence between inference and real-world outcome, namely actual fighting performance. We predicted a greater congruence between these inferences and fighting performance when African (Cameroonian) raters rated African-looking faces and Europeans (Czechs and Turks) rated European-looking faces. Given the ethnic diversity and prevalence of people of mixed-race (European and African) descent in Brazil, neither African- nor European-looking faces clearly represent ingroup or outgroup in that culture. We have therefore expected that among Brazilian perceivers, there would be no differences in accuracy with respect to perceived race.

Methods

The data collection procedure was approved by the Institutional Review Board of the Charles University, Faculty of Science (Ref. num. 2013/14). All methods were performed in accordance with the relevant guidelines and regulations.

Material

Stimuli set consisted of 54 male facial photographs selected from available portraits of Ultimate Fighting Championship (UFC) MMA fighters (images freely accessible on the official website of the UFC; www.ufc.com; downloaded in July 2012). Portraits were selected based on criteria, such as facing directly into the camera, absence of beard or moustache, and a history of at least 2 fights in the UFC (as per Třebický et al.3). Images were then sorted by two independent raters into 3 categories based on apparent race, which resulted in 18 images of men of apparent African origin, 18 images of men of apparent European origin, and 18 images of apparent mixed-race (Mixed) origin. Each category contained the same number of fighters within a weight class (9 in bantamweight, 6 in featherweight, 12 in lightweight, 6 in welterweight, 6 in middleweight, 9 in light heavyweight, and 6 in heavyweight). For each fighter, we obtained data about the number of fights he participated in (Mean = 8.6, SD = 6.83, Range = 2–27) and fights he won (Mean = 5.68, SD = 5.19, Range = 0–22) in the UFC. The images were subsequently standardised with respect to the position of the face within the frame (e.g. same position on a vertical axis), and the background colour was set to grey (RGB 128, 128, 128). To account for differences in the number of fights between fighters, we computed fighting success as a proportion of wins relative to the total number of fights (Mean = 0.59, SD = 0.25). We did not include female fighters in our study because of a low number of fighters and insufficient data regarding the number of fights at the time of data collection.

Participants

In total, 136 male students (mean age = 25.82ys, SD = 4.78; range = 18–49) from the University of São Paulo (São Paulo, Brazil) were invited to participate in the research online. Ninety participants from two different sites in Cameroon: 47 men (mean age = 23.43ys; SD = 5.81; range 18–41) from a rural community of Big Babanki (located in the Northwest Region) were recruited by a local research assistant (Ernest Vunan and JH), while 43 male students (mean age = 22.07ys; SD = 2.14; range 18–30) from the University of Buea, Buea, were recruited with the help of a local researcher (RMA and KK, TK, VT). Both Cameroonian samples participated in our research offline, on site. The two data collection sites did not differ in their ratings, which is why we merged them into one Cameroonian sample (N = 90) (for details, see Supplementary materials). The sample from the Czech Republic (N = 80) consisted of two data collections carried out during 2015 and 2017. An earlier Czech sample collected in 2013 showed effects incongruent with other cultures (see Table S4 in Supplementary materials). We found this suspicious and collected two other Czech samples (reported below), which both behaved differently from the initial Czech sample. This convinced us to treat the earlier as an exception. The 2015 sample consisted of 46 male students (mean age = 24.1ys; SD = 3.86; range 18–33) from the Charles University, Prague. The 2017 sample consisted of 34 male students (mean age = 25.88ys; SD = 7.68; range 18–49), also from the Charles University, Prague. Czech participants were invited to take part in the research online. The two data collection samples did not differ in their ratings, which is why we merged them into one Czech sample (N = 80) (for details, see Supplementary materials). In Turkey, 143 male students (mean age = 20.9ys; SD = 1.84; range 18–29) from Boğaziçi University, Istanbul were invited to participate in the research online. All participants provided their informed consent by clicking on the ‘I agree’ button to consent with their participation in the study.

Procedure

In Brazil, Czech Republic, and Turkey, rating was carried out using an online survey platform (Qualtrics, Provo, UT), while in Cameroon, where access to internet was limited, we used an offline rating software. Participants completed a short demographic questionnaire. Then they were presented with the whole set of images in a random order and asked to rate the aggressiveness of each face3 using a 7-point verbally-anchored scale (ranging from 1 – not aggressive at all, to 7 – very aggressive). A mean perceived aggressiveness rating was calculated separately for each category of stimuli (African, European, and Mixed), resulting in 3 scores for each participant. These scores were subsequently converted into z-scores to account for differences in scale use between participants within each population. We used aggressiveness rating as a perceptual proxy for inference regarding fighting success because our previous study had shown that perceived aggressiveness does predict actual fighting success in UFC and closely correlates with perceived fighting ability3.

Statistical analyses

All analyses were performed using IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, version 23 (IBM Corp., Armonk, N.Y., USA). Effect sizes for two-way mixed ANOVA are reported in partial η2. Aggressiveness ratings of all fighters were correlated with their fighting performance, and the resulting correlation coefficients were used as congruence scores in the corresponding analyses.

Figures were generated in BoxPlotR and post-produced in Adobe Photoshop CC 2018.

Data availability

Datasets generated and analysed during the current study are available in the Open Science Framework, https://osf.io/wec83/.

Results

Results indicating inter-rater reliability of aggressiveness rating based on raw scores from each country are shown in Table 1. In the Brazilian sample, UFC and the MMA in general are widely popular and fighters are well-known persons. For Brazilian participants, rating procedure therefore included an option where raters could indicate recognition for each face. Where a rater indicated recognition, his rating for that face was skipped. For Cronbach’s Alpha analyses, the resulting missing values were thus substituted by the mean ratings from the Brazilian sample for the image in question.

Table 1.

Inter-rater reliability (Cronbach’s alpha) of aggressiveness ratings.

Country Stimuli
African European Mixed
Brazil (N = 136) 0.986 0.987 0.989
Cameroon, Babanki (N = 47) 0.870 0.809 0.762
Cameroon, Buea (N = 43) 0.955 0.944 0.934
Czech Republic, 2015 (N = 46) 0.970 0.965 0.963
Czech Republic, 2017 (N = 34) 0.928 0.921 0.947
Turkey (N = 143) 0.989 0.981 0.988

Two-way mixed ANOVA with congruence scores as a dependent variable and stimuli category (within-subjects) and rater’s country (between-subjects) as independent variables was carried out (the same analysis was conducted using aggressiveness assessments as a dependent variable, see Supplementary materials).

Results, including post-hoc tests, are summarised in Tables 2 and 3. The analysis showed a significant main effect of the stimuli category (F1.965,870.591 = 8.886, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.02) on congruence scores. The category of apparent African origin was rated with significantly higher congruence scores (i.e., agreement between perceived aggressiveness and actual fighting success), than either the apparently European or the apparently Mixed origins, which did not significantly differ from one another (Table 2). This main effect was qualified by a statistically significant interaction between stimuli category and rater’s country on congruence scores (F5.896,870.591 = 12.462, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.078) (Fig. 1). To understand the nature of the interaction, we ran pairwise comparisons with Bonferroni correction. In the Brazilian sample, congruence scores were higher for African than for Mixed faces (p < 0.001) and there were no other significant differences. In Cameroon, apparently African faces received significantly lower congruence scores than either apparently European or Mixed faces (ps < 0.002), which did not differ from each other. In the Czech Republic, apparently European and Mixed faces received significantly lower congruence scores than apparently African faces (p < 0.001 and p = 0.022, respectively), but they did not differ from each other. In Turkey, apparently European faces received significantly lower congruence scores than apparently African or Mixed faces (ps < 0.001), but the latter did not differ from one another (Table 3).

Table 2.

Differences in congruence scores - Stimulus category descriptive statistics and Post Hoc comparison.

Stimuli Congruence Score 95% Confidence intervals
Mean SE Lower limit Upper limit
Stimulus category descriptives African 0.197 0.010 0.178 0.216
European 0.136 0.010 0.116 0.156
Mixed 0.169 0.010 0.148 0.189
Stimuli Bonferroni Post Hoc comparison p
Mean difference SE 95% Confidence intervals
Lower limit Upper limit
Pairwise comparisons African - European 0.060 0.014 0.028 0.093 <0.001
African - Mixed 0.028 0.014 −0.006 0.062 0.145
European - Mixed −0.032 0.015 −0.069 0.004 0.102

Table 3.

Differences in congruence scores - Country × Stimulus category descriptive statistics and Post Hoc comparison.

Congruence Score
Country Stimuli Mean SE 95% Confidence intervals
Lower limit Upper limit
Country × stimulus category descriptives Brazil African 0.231 0.017 0.198 0.265
European 0.196 0.018 0.161 0.232
Mixed 0.143 0.018 0.107 0.179
Cameroon African 0.066 0.021 0.025 0.107
European 0.166 0.022 0.123 0.209
Mixed 0.170 0.022 0.126 0.214
Czech Rep. African 0.252 0.022 0.208 0.295
European 0.124 0.023 0.078 0.170
Mixed 0.164 0.024 0.117 0.211
Turkey African 0.238 0.017 0.205 0.270
European 0.059 0.018 0.024 0.093
Mixed 0.198 0.018 0.162 0.233
Bonferroni Post Hoc comparison
Country Stimuli Mean difference SE 95% Confidence intervals p
Lower limit Upper limit
Pairwise comparisons Brazil African - European 0.035 0.024 −0.023 0.093 0.432
African - Mixed 0.089 0.025 0.028 0.149 0.001
European - Mixed 0.053 0.027 −0.011 0.118 0.143
Cameroon African - European −0.100 0.029 −0.171 −0.029 0.002
African - Mixed −0.104 0.031 −0.177 −0.030 0.002
European - Mixed −0.004 0.033 −0.083 0.075 1.000
Czech Rep. African - European 0.128 0.031 0.053 0.203 <0.001
African - Mixed 0.087 0.033 0.009 0.165 0.022
European - Mixed −0.041 0.035 −0.125 0.043 0.739
Turkey African - European 0.179 0.023 0.122 0.235 <0.001
African - Mixed 0.040 0.024 −0.019 0.099 0.303
European - Mixed −0.139 0.026 −0.202 −0.076 <0.001

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Violin and boxplots for differences in congruence scores. Note: Violin plots represent congruence scores. Box limits indicate the 25th and 75th percentiles, whiskers extend 1.5 times the interquartile range from the 25th and 75th percentiles. Asterisks stand for significance levels; *** for p < 0.001, ** for p < 0.01, * for p < 0.05.

Discussion

Contrary to our hypothesis, congruence between facial inferences and real-world performance in three distant cultures (Cameroon, Czech Republic, and Turkey) indicates an outgroup advantage. In an ethnically heterogenous and mixed culture (Brazil), congruence was the lowest for the apparently mixed-race targets. To the best of our knowledge, only three sets of earlier studies have examined the accuracy of face-based inferences regarding personality or behavioural outcome in perceivers from different cultures and both ingroup and outgroup targets24,25,27. Interestingly, Na et al.25 have observed a weak outgroup advantage in accuracy. In their study, competence judgments of American politicians’ faces by Korean (vs. American) perceivers and competence judgments of Korean politicians’ faces by American (vs. Korean) perceivers were stronger predictors for respective election results. Young18 had also observed an outgroup advantage: he performed two experiments focused on identifying fake smiles using minimal groups. It thus seems that at least in some areas related to competition and cooperation, there may be an outgroup advantage in inferential accuracy.

Our results contrast with research that demonstrated an ingroup advantage in other areas of face processing. Decades of research on social cognition28 have established that ingroup members are more likely to be individuated (i.e., perceivers are especially attentive to facial features which distinguish ingroup faces from one another), whereas outgroup members are more likely to be processed categorically (perceivers are attentive to facial features common across outgroup faces). As a consequence, people tend to have more detailed impressions of apparent racial ingroup (as opposed to outgroup) faces29. Such individuation should logically result in a more accurate prediction of a target’s dispositions than categorisation.

Did our perceivers individuate the outgroup targets more than the ingroup ones? It has been shown that perceivers tend to be motivated to paying special attention to outgroup faces that may present a potential threat30,31. This effect is further amplified by activation of a self-protection goal32 and may well be exclusive to male faces33. Given that in our study, perceivers were instructed to focus on aggressiveness in male faces, it is thus possible that they were motivated to process outgroup faces more carefully (i.e., to focus on individuation) in order to determine each target’s threat potential. This would amount to a self-protective response. Our results may therefore represent an extension of motivated face processing directed at outgroup targets, investigated in earlier face memory research4, to the domain of inferential accuracy. Asking perceivers to judge aggressiveness may have primed a certain level of threat and guardedness (especially since the targets were male fighters), just as Young’s18 experiments where perceivers were asked to detect fake smiles may have activated mistrust and guardedness (especially toward outgroup targets). Prima facie, one could expect that such priming might lead to an indiscriminately heightened attribution of aggressiveness to an outgroup. Interestingly, our results show no differences in perceived aggressiveness level attributed to any particular group of faces. It is yet to be seen, however, whether our findings could be generalised to contexts other than aggression or physical competition and to female faces and female raters. This remains a task for future research.

Another way to interpret our findings is to frame them as an ingroup disadvantage and to investigate why perceivers may find it more difficult to process ingroup faces. The greater propensity to individuating ingroup faces may have somewhat paradoxically detracted from the accuracy of inferences by introducing additional cognitive processing29, which diluted the immediate impact of valid facial cues on perception. Similar detrimental effects on deliberation have been reported in other domains such as decision making34.

As mentioned above, the initial Czech sample collected in 2013 showed effects inconsistent with other cultures. We have therefore collected two other Czech samples which show patterns consistent with the other datasets. We have as yet no rational explanation for why the initial Czech sample was difficult to reconcile with the rest of the data. One might speculate whether it could have been related to the media attention given to the so-called ‘immigration crisis’ which escalated in Europe in 2015. Nevertheless, robustness of our current findings should be reassessed in the future studies.

An important limitation of the current study was its failure to assess inter-ethnic contact among the respondents. Based on societal demographics, we assumed that Czechs and Turks have relatively little exposure to faces of African appearance, while Cameroonians are the opposite, and Brazilians have approximately equal exposure to faces of European and African appearance. It has, however, been established that attenuation of the CRE can be the result of inter-ethnic contact measured at an individual level35. Future research should thus examine whether contact plays a similar role for accuracy of facial inferences with respect to behavioural outcomes. If accuracy decreases with increased contact (as the cross-cultural differences seem to suggest), this would present an interesting explanatory challenge.

Data regarding the ethnic origin of the individual fighters were not available on UFC website. Moreover, we gathered no data on how raters perceive the depicted faces in terms of their ethnic similarity. Nevertheless, given a generally high contrast between faces of European and African appearance, we believe that the assumption about perceived ethnic similarity is justified.

Because social-motivational factors can significantly affect face processing17, it is possible that perceivers’ own ethnic identity (and their categorisation of target faces into ethnic ingroup and outgroup with subsequent biases triggered by such a categorisation) may have influenced the current findings9. Given that the extent to which perceivers identify with their ingroup is known to moderate face processing14,36, future research should test the influence of perceivers’ ethnic identity on the accuracy of inferences made from ingroup and outgroup faces.

Finally, facial morphology differs between groups of people, such as heterosexuals and homosexuals37,38, and is related to behaviour39. Future research should thus investigate the facial cues our perceivers used to judge aggressiveness and determine which of these cues actually increased the accuracy of perceivers’ inferences. To wit, it is possible that biased processing of faces led to increased accuracy merely accidentally. It is known that, for instance, perceivers focus more attention on the eye area of ingroup target faces than outgroup faces15. If our perceivers were more likely to attend to other areas of outgroup faces than the eye region, and if those areas happened to contain cues that aided accurate inferences (e.g., relative facial width), their accuracy for outgroup faces may have been higher not despite but because of biased outgroup face processing.

Within the still little body of research that examined face processing across different groups of both targets and perceivers and applied some real-world accuracy criterion, reports of an outgroup advantage are beginning to emerge18,25, countering evidence for an outgroup disadvantage in other face-processing domains. Our findings suggest that these recent reports of an outgroup advantage have captured a real and replicable phenomenon that may have as yet escaped the researchers’ attention. Future research should try to uncover the mechanisms and moderators of this outgroup advantage in inferential accuracy.

Electronic supplementary material

Supplementary materials (243.6KB, docx)

Acknowledgements

This research was supported by Czech Science Foundation GAČR P407/16/03899S, and by the Ministry of Education, Youth, and Sports NPU I program (No. LO1611). S. Adil Saribay was supported by a grant from The Scientific and Technological Research Council of Turkey (TÜBITAK 2219, no: 1059B191600901). We thank Jaroslav Flegr, Rachel Ripardo, Kamila Janaina Pereira, and Ernest Vunan for their help with data collection, Anna Pilátová for English proofreading and to all volunteers for their participation.

Author Contributions

V.T. and J.H. developed the study concept. K.K. contributed to the study design. Data collection was performed by J.V.V. and M.A.C.V. in Brazil, by R.M.A., K.K., V.T., T.K. and J.H. in Cameroon, by V.T., K.K. and J.H. in the Czech Republic, and by S.A.S. in Turkey. V.T. performed the data analysis and interpretation under the supervision of S.A.S., J.H. and K.K. S.A.S. and V.T. drafted the manuscript, and K.K., J.H., J.V.V. and M.A.C.V. provided critical revisions. All authors approved the final version of the manuscript for submission.

Competing Interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Footnotes

Vít Třebický and S. Adil Saribay contributed equally to this work.

Electronic supplementary material

Supplementary information accompanies this paper at 10.1038/s41598-018-27751-0.

Publisher's note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

References

  • 1.Todorov AT, Olivola CY, Dotsch R, Mende-Siedlecki P. Social attributions from faces: determinants, consequences, accuracy, and functional significance. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2015;66:519–545. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143831. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Borkenau P, Brecke S, Möttig C, Paelecke M. Extraversion is accurately perceived after a 50-ms exposure to a face. J. Res. Pers. 2009;43:703–706. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2009.03.007. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Třebický V, Havlíček J, Roberts SC, Little AC, Kleisner K. Perceived aggressiveness predicts fighting performance in mixed-martial-arts fighters. Psychol. Sci. 2013;24:1664–72. doi: 10.1177/0956797613477117. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Meissner CA, Brigham JC. Thirty years of investigating the own-race bias in memory for faces: A meta-analytic review. Psychol. Public Policy, Law. 2001;7:3–35. doi: 10.1037/1076-8971.7.1.3. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Furl N, Phillips PJ, O’Toole AJ. Face recognition algorithms and the other-race effect: Computational mechanisms for a developmental contact hypothesis. Cogn. Sci. 2002;26:797–815. doi: 10.1207/s15516709cog2606_4. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Sangrigoli S, Pallier C, Argenti A-M, Ventureyra VAG, De Schonen S. Reversibility of the other-race effect in face recognition during childhood. Psychological Science. 2005;16:440–444. doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.01554.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Brambilla M, Riva P, Rule NO. Familiarity increases the accuracy of categorizing male sexual orientation. Pers. Individ. Dif. 2013;55:193–195. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2013.02.023. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Tajfel H, Billig MG, Bundy RP, Flament C. Social categorization and intergroup behaviour. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 1971;1:149–178. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.2420010202. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Hugenberg K, Corneille O. Holistic processing is tuned for in-group faces. Cogn. Sci. 2009;33:1173–1181. doi: 10.1111/j.1551-6709.2009.01048.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Michel C, Rossion B, Han J, Chung CS, Caldara R. Holistic processing is finely tuned for faces of one’s own race. Psychological Science. 2006;17:608–615. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01752.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Young SG, Hugenberg K, Bernstein MJ, Sacco DF. Perception and motivation in face recognition: A critical review of theories of the cross-race effect. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 2012;16:116–142. doi: 10.1177/1088868311418987. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Levin DT. Classifying faces by race: The structure of face categories. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 1996;22:1364–1382. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.22.6.1364. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Lipp OV, et al. Searching for differences in race: Is there evidence for preferential detection of other-race faces? Emotion. 2009;9:350–360. doi: 10.1037/a0015530. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Van Bavel JJ, Cunningham WA. A social identity approach to person memory: Group membership, collective identification, and social role shape attention and memory. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 2012;38:1566–78. doi: 10.1177/0146167212455829. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Kawakami K, et al. An eye for the I: Preferential attention to the eyes of ingroup members. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2014;107:1–20. doi: 10.1037/a0036838. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Elfenbein HA, Ambady N. On the universality and cultural specificity of emotion recognition: A meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 2002;128:203–235. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.128.2.203. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Young SG, Hugenberg K. Mere social categorization modulates identification of facial expressions of emotion. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2010;99:964–977. doi: 10.1037/a0020400. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Young SG. An outgroup advantage in discriminating between genuine and posed smiles. Self Identity. 2017;16:298–312. doi: 10.1080/15298868.2016.1270851. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Hwang HC, Matsumoto D. Sender Ethnicity Differences in Lie Detection Accuracy and Confidence. GSTF J. Law Soc. Sci. 2014;3(1):15–19. [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Valentová J, Rieger G, Havlíček J, Linsenmeier JAW, Bailey JM. Judgments of sexual orientation and masculinity-femininity based on thin slices of behavior: a cross-cultural comparison. Arch. Sex. Behav. 2011;40:1145–52. doi: 10.1007/s10508-011-9818-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Rule NO. The influence of target and perceiver race in the categorisation of male sexual orientation. Perception. 2011;40:830–839. doi: 10.1068/p7001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Zebrowitz LA, Montepare JM, Lee HK. They don’t all look alike: individuated impressions of other racial groups. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1993;65:85–101. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.65.1.85. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Zebrowitz LA, et al. First Impressions From Faces Among U.S. and Culturally Isolated Tsimane’ People in the Bolivian Rainforest. J. Cross. Cult. Psychol. 2012;43:119–134. doi: 10.1177/0022022111411386. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Rule NO, et al. Polling the face: Prediction and consensus across cultures. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2010;98:1–15. doi: 10.1037/a0017673. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Na J, Kim S, Oh H, Choi I, O’Toole A. Competence judgments based on facial appearance are better predictors of American elections than of Korean elections. Psychol. Sci. 2015;26:1107–1113. doi: 10.1177/0956797615576489. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Short LA, et al. Detection of propensity for aggression based on facial structure irrespective of face race. Evol. Hum. Behav. 2012;33:121–129. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2011.07.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Rule NO, Ishii K, Ambady N. Cross-cultural impressions of leaders’ faces: Consensus and predictive validity. Int. J. Intercult. Relations. 2011;35:833–841. doi: 10.1016/j.ijintrel.2011.06.001. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Brewer, M. In Advances in Social Cognition (eds Srull, T. K. & Wyer, R. S.) vol. 1, 1–36, 10.1521/soco.1993.11.1.150 (Lawrence Erlbaum Associates 1988).
  • 29.Chance JE, Goldstein AG. Depth of processing in response to own- and other-race faces. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 1981;7:475–480. doi: 10.1177/014616728173017. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Trawalter S, Todd AR, Baird AA, Richeson JA. Attending to threat: Race-based patterns of selective attention. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 2008;44:1322–1327. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2008.03.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Ackerman JM, et al. They all look the same to me (unless they’re angry): From out-group homogeneity to out-group heterogeneity. Psychol. Sci. 2006;17:836–840. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01790.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Becker DV, et al. More memory bang for the attentional buck: Self-protection goals enhance encoding efficiency for potentially threatening males. Soc. Psychol. Personal. Sci. 2010;1:182–189. doi: 10.1177/1948550609359202. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Maner JK, Miller S. Adaptive attentional attunement: Perceptions of danger and attention to outgroup men. Soc. Cogn. 2013;31:733–744. doi: 10.1521/soco.2013.31.6.733. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Wilson TD, Schooler JW. Thinking too much: Introspection can reduce the quality of preferences and decisions. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1991;60:181–192. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.60.2.181. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Hancock KJ, Rhodes G. Contact, configural coding and the other-race effect in face recognition. Br. J. Psychol. 2008;99:45–56. doi: 10.1348/000712607X199981. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Stevenson MT, Soto JA, Adams RB. More than meets the eye: The role of self-identity in decoding complex emotional states. Emotion. 2012;12:882–886. doi: 10.1037/a0028628. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Skorska, M. N., Geniole, S. N., Vrysen, B. M., McCormick, C. M. & Bogaert, A. F. Facial structure predicts sexual orientation in both men and women. Arch. Sex. Behav. 44, 1377–1394 (2015). [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 38.Valentová J, Kleisner K, Havlíček J, Neustupa J. Shape differences between the faces of homosexual and heterosexual men. Arch. Sex. Behav. 2014;43:353–61. doi: 10.1007/s10508-013-0194-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Stirrat MR, Perrett DI. Valid facial cues to cooperation and trust: male facial width and trustworthiness. Psychol. Sci. 2010;21:349–54. doi: 10.1177/0956797610362647. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplementary materials (243.6KB, docx)

Data Availability Statement

Datasets generated and analysed during the current study are available in the Open Science Framework, https://osf.io/wec83/.


Articles from Scientific Reports are provided here courtesy of Nature Publishing Group

RESOURCES