Abstract
Objective
Measures of rapid automatized naming (RAN) have been used for over 50 years to capture vision-based aspects of cognition. The Mobile Universal Lexicon Evaluation System (MULES) is a test of rapid picture naming under investigation for detection of concussion and other neurological disorders. MULES was designed as a series of 54 grouped color photographs (fruits, random objects, animals) that integrates saccades, color perception and contextual object identification. Recent changes to the MULES test have been made to improve ease of use on the athletic sidelines. Originally an 11 × 17-inch single-sided paper, the test has been reduced to a laminated 8.5 × 11-inch double-sided version. We identified performance changes associated with transition to the new, MULES, now sized for the sidelines, and examined MULES on the sideline for sports-related concussion.
Methods
We administered the new laminated MULES to a group of adult office volunteers as well as youth and collegiate athletes during pre-season baseline testing. Athletes with concussion underwent sideline testing after injury. Time scores for the new laminated MULES were compared to those for the larger version (big MULES). Results: Among 501 athletes and office volunteers (age 16 ± 7 years, range 6–59, 29% female), average test times at baseline were 44.4 ± 14.4 s for the new laminated MULES (n = 196) and 46.5 ± 16.3 s for big MULES (n = 248). Both versions were completed by 57 participants, with excellent agreement (p < 0.001, linear regression, accounting for age). Age was a predictor of test times for both MULES versions, with longer times noted for younger participants (p < 0.001). Among 6 athletes with concussion thus far during the fall sports season (median age 15 years, range 11–21) all showed worsening of MULES scores from pre-season baseline (median 4.0 s, range 2.1–16.4).
Conclusion
The MULES test has been converted to an 11 × 8.5-inch laminated version, with excellent agreement between versions across age groups. Feasibly administered at pre-season and in an office setting, the MULES test shows preliminary evidence of capacity to identify athletes with sports-related concussion.
Keywords: Concussion, Sports, Picture naming, Mobile Universal Lexicon Evaluation System (MULES), Saccades, Vision
1. Introduction
There are an estimated 1.6 to 3.8 million sports and activity-related concussions every year across all age groups [1]. Sports-related concussion has received increased attention because of neurological dysfunction that may impair cognition, vision and balance [2,3]. Despite the improved recognition of concussion and the potential neurological consequences, many concussions continue to go undiagnosed or unreported [4,5]. For this reason, rapid and effective sideline performance testing is necessary to detect concussion in order to adequately protect the injured athlete [6]. Rapid sideline tests, including those that assess vision, balance, and cognition, may help confirm the presence of a concussion and prevent the risk of recurrent injury [7–12].
The King-Devick (K-D) test, a sensitive sideline concussion-screening test, uses rapid number naming to capture vergence, saccades and other eye movements as well as attention and language function [7–10]. By testing these functions, widely-distributed and interconnected areas of the cortex and brainstem may be evaluated [13–15]. The Mobile Universal Lexicon Evaluation System (MULES) is a new sideline test of rapid naming of photographic images in context, aims to examine the brain’s visual pathways and neural networks by testing color perception, object identification, conceptual representation, phonology and articulation [16,17].
Measures of rapid automatized naming (RAN), including rapid picture naming, have been used for over 50 years to capture vision-based aspects of cognition and language [18–26]. Such tests that included rapid naming of color photos were described in the 1940’s by Reusch and Wells [19] in a guide entitled Mental Examiner’s Handbook. In the late 1960’s, Geschwind used a version of a picture naming test that included 50 color photographs to be identified by the subject as quickly as possible [20,21]. In 1974, the term rapid automatized naming (RAN) was coined in the context of a study of rapid color, letter and number naming by normal children [22]. Since that time, the literature on RAN has demonstrated that individuals both with and without reading deficits (dyslexia) require more time to name objects than symbols [20–26]. In our group’s initial publication on the MULES [16], it was noted that healthy participants named color photographs in an average of 0.72 s, and named numbers on the King-Devick Test in an average of 0.36 s. Given these important differences in the brains of healthy persons and historically in published reports of those with dyslexia [37–43], the study of picture naming among those with concussion and other neurological disorders will be critical to understanding such injuries.
Since the initial publication on the MULES [16], changes have been made to the design to improve the ease of use of this investigational test on the sidelines. Originally an 11″ × 17″ single-sided paper, the MULES test has been reduced in size to a laminated 8.5″ × 11″ double-sided version. The purpose of this study was to identify performance changes associated with the transition from the larger MULES to the newer, MULES that is better sized for the sidelines according to athletic trainer and parent feedback [16]. Total time scores to complete the test, average time to name each MULES stimulus image, and associations of scores with age and other characteristics of the MULES test were analyzed in a group of athletes undergoing pre-season testing and in healthy volunteers at a single baseline testing session.
2. Subjects and methods
2.1. Study participants
A convenience sample of adult office volunteers as well as athletes from regional collegiate and youth athletic organizations underwent pre-season baseline (pre-injury) testing for concussion, which included the MULES. Athletes with concussion underwent sideline testing after injury. Time scores for the new laminated MULES were compared to those for the larger version (big MULES). Participants with a history of ocular or neurologic disease were excluded from the study. Written informed consent was obtained from each participant; the Institutional Review Board (IRB) at New York University School of Medicine approved all study protocols.
2.2. Two formats of the MULES and testing procedures
Each format of the test consists of 54 original photographs of fruits, objects and animals. The original, “big” MULES test is printed on a single side of a 11 × 17-in. sheet of paper. The modified “laminated” MULES includes the same 54 photographs distributed on two sides of a laminated 8.5 × 11-inch paper (Fig. 1). Participants are asked to name the pictures orally from left to right and top to bottom as rapidly as possible without making errors. The score is the time in seconds required to name all pictures on both sides of the laminated sheet of paper (8.5 × 11-inch paper is flipped by the participant during testing with the timer running). In this study, participants completed two trials of the MULES and the trial with the better score (shorter time) was recorded as the subject’s baseline. This practice of two trials is standard for performance measures used in concussion and in other neurological disorders, such as multiple sclerosis (MS Functional Composite) [27,28]. Testing was administered by trained study personnel at all sites for pre-season baseline; athletic trainers or designated parent testers performed evaluations for concussed athletes on the sidelines. Concussed athletes completed a single trial of the MULES, while pre-season testing involved two trials, recording the best score as the baseline.
2.3. Statistical analyses
Data were analyzed using Stata SE 15.1 (StataCorp, College Station, TX). Within-participant differences between MULES test trials (learning effects) and MULES test versions in terms of time scores were analyzed using paired t-tests. The capacity for MULES test time scores and for inter-trial differences (degrees of learning effect) to predict MULES test version (big vs. laminated MULES) was examined using logistic regression models, accounting simultaneously for age. The relation of big MULES vs. laminated MULES test version scores was determined using Pearson linear correlations. Linear regression models were used to examine the same associations, accounting simultaneously for participant age. Similar analyses were used to examine the relation of age to MULES test time scores for both versions. To further assess agreement between the two versions of the MULES test, the intra-class correlation coefficient (ICC) was calculated. The ICC indicates the proportion of the variability in a dataset that is attributable to between-participant differences. For analyses of the 6 athletes who developed concussion so far during the fall sports season, non-parametric tests (Wilcoxon signed-rank) were used to compare pre-season baseline vs. post-injury laminated MULES scores within participants.
3. Results
Among 501 total participants (athletes and office volunteers, age 16 ± 7 years, range 6–59, 29% female), average test times at baseline (best of two trials) were 44.4 ± 14.4 s for the new laminated MULES (n = 196) and 46.5 ± 16.3 s for big MULES (n = 248; Table 1). Accounting for participant age, MULES version (big vs. laminated) was not a significant predictor of baseline score (p = 0.52, logistic regression; Fig. 2).
Table 1.
MULES version Completed at baseline session |
Scores in seconds, mean ± SD (range) | ||
---|---|---|---|
All participants | Group | Totals, gender | Age, mean ± SD |
n = 501 | Office volunteers | N = 32, 81% female | 31 ± 10 years |
Youth athletes | N = 246, 11% female | 11 ± 3 years | |
Collegiate athletes | N = 223, 43% female | 19 ± 1 years | |
Participants completing laminated MULES | Trial 1 | 53.4 ± 18.0 | |
n = 196 | Trial 2 | 44.6 ± 14.6a | |
Best of 2 Trials = Baseline | 44.4 ± 14.4 | ||
Inter-trial difference | 8.9 ± 9.0 | ||
Age | 17 ± 7 years | ||
Gender | 35% female | ||
Participants completing big MULES | Trial 1 | 54.7 ± 19.0 | |
n = 248 | Trial 2 | 47.5 ± 18.6a | |
Best of 2 Trials = Baseline | 46.5 ± 16.3 | ||
Inter-trial difference | 7.2 ± 10.3 | ||
Age | 15 ± 5 years | ||
Gender | 27% female | ||
Participants completing both MULES versions | Laminated MULES | Trial 1 | 54.8 ± 17.1 |
n = 57 | Trial 2 | 49.6 ± 17.9a | |
Best of 2 Trials = Baseline | 48.9 ± 16.4 | ||
Inter-trial difference | 5.2 ± 7.2 | ||
Big MULES | Trial 1 | 63.0 ± 21.5 | |
Trial 2 | 57.5 ± 22.6b | ||
Best of 2 Trials = Baseline | 55.5 ± 18.7 | ||
Inter-trial difference | 5.6 ± 12.6 | ||
Age | 15 ± 10 years | ||
Gender | 18% female |
MULES = Mobile Universal Lexicon Evaluation System.
Comparison of trial 1 vs. trial 2, paired t-test, p < 0.0001.
Comparison of trial 1 vs. trial 2, paired t-test, p = 0.002.
There were significant reductions in time score between trials 1 and 2 for both big and laminated MULES tests, consistent with learning effects that are inherent in performance measures (p < 0.0001 for each MULES version, paired t-test; Fig. 3). MULES version (big vs. laminated), however, was not a significant predictor of inter-trial difference, or degree of learning effect between the two trials (p = 0.31, logistic regression).
Both versions of the MULES were completed by 57 participants, with excellent agreement of best baseline scores between the big and laminated MULES (p < 0.001, linear regression, accounting for age; Fig. 4). These testing sessions were approximately one year apart due to timing of development of the laminated MULES following testing of the big MULES. In terms of the magnitude of association between scores for the two MULES test versions among the same group of participants, the linear correlation was r = 0.89, p < 0.0001). The intra-class correlation coefficient (ICC) between the two MULES versions within this cohort (n = 57) was 0.90, 95% CI [0.66, 0.96], indicating excellent agreement between the big and laminated MULES scores. Average times required to name each MULES picture were similar for both the big MULES (0.89 ± 0.32 s) and laminated MULES (0.84 ± 0.28 s). Accounting for participant age among those who completed both versions of the test (n = 57), the time required to name big MULES pictures was a significant predictor of the same measure for the laminated MULES (p < 0.001, linear regression).
Age was a predictor of test times for both MULES versions, with longer times noted for younger participants (p < 0.001, linear regression). Among 6 athletes with concussion thus far during the fall sports season (age 16 ± 4 years) all showed significant worsening of laminated MULES scores from pre-season baseline to post-injury testing (median 4.0 s, range 2.1–16.4, p = 0.003, Wilcoxon signed-rank test; Table 2).
Table 2.
Participant, Age (years), Gender (M/F) | Sport | Baseline (s) | Post-injury (s) | Difference (post-injury minus baseline time, s) |
---|---|---|---|---|
13 M | Ice Hockey | 43.0 | 45.2 | 2.1 |
13 M | Ice Hockey | 37.9 | 52.3 | 14.4 |
11 M | Football | 45.4 | 47.8 | 2.4 |
21 M | Ice Hockey | 36.2 | 39.7 | 3.5 |
19 F | Soccer | 47.7 | 64.0 | 16.4 |
19 M | Wrestling | 49.3 | 53.8 | 4.5 |
Median (range) | 44.2 (36.2–49.3) | 50.1 (39.7–64.0) | 4.0 (2.1–16.4)a |
Comparison of baseline vs. post-injury test times, Wilcoxon signed-rank test, p = 0.003.
4. Discussion
An investigational vision-based timed performance measure, the MULES test of picture naming has been successfully converted to a more compact laminated version. Our data show excellent agreement between the new laminated MULES and the big MULES in both youth and collegiate athletes and among healthy adult volunteers. Similar to previous studies of the big MULES, time scores for laminated MULES decrease with age among youth athletes, supporting the use of pre-season baseline measurements. Feasibly administered at pre-season and in an office setting, the laminated MULES test shows preliminary evidence of capacity to identify athletes with sports-related concussion.
Sensitive performance measures and other markers for concussion are needed to support what remains primarily a clinical diagnosis [17]. Such measures factor into important sideline decisions in athletes with concussion. The MULES is a rapid picture naming task designed to employ widely distributed afferent and efferent visual networks in the brain. Picture naming also involves other cortical regions responsible for object categorization and language [18–35]. A method to capture vision-based aspects of cognition, rapid automatized naming (RAN) tests involve serial naming aloud of line drawings, pictures or, as in the case of the MULES, color photographic images in context [22].
The MULES test has been recently reformatted, from the “big” single-sided 11 × 17-inch paper format to the “laminated” double sided 8.5 × 11-inch page (Fig. 1). Based on feedback from athletic trainers, clinicians and parents involved in the ongoing research studies, these features make the test more accessible in a sideline and clinical setting. For example, the smaller version fits in a briefcase or athletic bag.
In comparing the two versions of the MULES test, it should be kept in mind that our cohorts that completed the big vs. laminated MULES (15 vs. 17 years; Table 1) differ slightly with respect to mean age. This difference in age is attributable to the timing of our implementation of the newer laminated MULES version in the research studies of youth and collegiate athletes. The new laminated version was introduced just as our collegiate athletics baseline testing program began to expand; thus, the laminated MULES cohort (n = 196, Table 1) includes greater numbers of collegiate athletes and also reflects greater numbers of female participants. As such, our analyses comparing MULES versions with regard to baseline time score and learning effects accounted simultaneously for participant age, with no significant differences found.
As may be expected developmentally for a vision-based performance measure, younger age among youth athlete participants was associated with greater baseline time scores for both versions of the MULES. These findings may be attributable to the ongoing development of frontal lobe executive functions and the temporal-parietal regions responsible for semantic and visual categorization [36–39]. The pre-frontal cortex, which serves many executive functions including temporal integration, preparatory setup for saccades, working memory, and inhibitory control, is among the last structures to develop. Within these structures, white matter volume increases into adulthood, and cortico-cortical tracts reach a full state of myelination in the third decade of life [40–42]. Thus, it may be expected that performance on a rapid-picture naming task, which presumably integrates these functions, will be age-dependent in such a way as to reflect developmental trajectory. This peak in baseline scores in late adolescence interestingly corresponds to performance peaks in processing speed, response suppression, and working memory that were documented in one study of adolescent cognitive development [43].
Correlations of the big vs. laminated MULES scores, along with the intra-class correlation (ICC) and linear regression models, demonstrate excellent agreement between the two test versions in our study cohort. Specifically, the ICC value of 0.90 suggests that a majority of the variation seen among baseline MULES scores is due to differences between participants rather than differences between MULES test versions or test administrators. While the between-participant variability is consistent with the MULES tests capturing individual aspects of visual performance, the reassuring linear correlations and ICC indicate that MULES test versions closely parallel each other in their capacity to measure aspects of vision and cognition. While the observed levels of agreement may have been anticipated based on the similarities of format, spacing and picture content, it is reassuring that data generated from the big MULES testing can be applied to groups now receiving laminated MULES testing.
The results of our study to date are promising, showing consistent worsening of laminated MULES scores from pre-season baseline among a small group of athletes who have suffered a concussion. Most of the laminated MULES scores worsened by only 2–4 s (Table 2), while two others had a 14–16-second worsening from pre-season baseline. Since the MULES is a vision-based performance measure, learning effects are typically noted between testing sessions. For this reason, any worsening of the time score from pre-season baseline is currently considered consistent with injury. Since vision comprises approximately 50% of the brain pathways, and prior studies have demonstrated a need for composite measures to identify all athletes with concussion, addition of a classic rapid picture naming task to the test of rapid number naming may augment the capacity for sideline vision-based tests to diagnose concussion.
It will be interesting in future studies to examine the relation of MULES time scores to other aspects of neurological function, including cognition, balance and symptom burden. Task-based functional MRI protocols implementing the MULES may identify patterns of network activation during baseline assessments as well as those that become dysfunctional following concussion. With imaging studies employing rapid automatic naming tasks, we may gain insight into the pathophysiology of concussions especially its impact on the visual system. Since the MULES test integrates widely distributed cortical networks, applicability of the MULES as an assessment tool will likely extend to a variety of neurological and neurodegenerative disorders beyond concussion.
Acknowledgments
Financial support
This study was supported in part by the NYU School of Medicine.
Footnotes
Disclosure statements
The authors have no financial interests.
References
- 1.Langlois JA, Rutland-Brown W, Wald MM. The epidemiology and impact of traumatic brain injury: a brief overview. J Head Trauma Rehabil. 2006;21:375–378. doi: 10.1097/00001199-200609000-00001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Plassman BL, Havlik RJ, Steffens DC, et al. Documented head injury in early adult-hood and risk of Alzheimer’s disease and other dementias. Neurology. 2000;55:1158–1166. doi: 10.1212/wnl.55.8.1158. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Gavett BE, Stern RA, McKee AC. Chronic traumatic encephalopathy: a potential late effect of sport-related concussive and subconcussive head trauma. Clin Sports Med. 2011;30:179–188. doi: 10.1016/j.csm.2010.09.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Torres DM, Galetta KM, Phillips HW, et al. Sports-related concussion: anonymous survey of a collegiate cohort. Neurol Clin Pract. 2013;3:279–287. doi: 10.1212/CPJ.0b013e3182a1ba22. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Register-Mihalik JK, Guskiewicz KM, McLeod TC, Linnan LA, Mueller FO, Marshall SW. Knowledge, attitude, and concussion-reporting behaviors among high school athletes: a preliminary study. J Athl Train. 2013;48:645–653. doi: 10.4085/1062-6050-48.3.20. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.McCrea M, Kelly JP, Kluge J, Ackley B, Randolph C. Standardized assessment of concussion in football players. Neurology. 1997;48:586–588. doi: 10.1212/wnl.48.3.586. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Marinides Z, Galetta KM, Andrews CN, et al. Vision testing is additive to the side-line assessment of sports-related concussion. Neurol Clin Pract. 2015;5:25–34. doi: 10.1212/CPJ.0000000000000060. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Galetta KM, Barrett J, Allen M, et al. The King-Devick test as a determinant of head trauma and concussion in boxers and MMA fighters. Neurology. 2011;76:1456–1462. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e31821184c9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Galetta KM, Brandes LE, Maki K, et al. The King-Devick test and sports-related concussion: study of a rapid visual screening tool in a collegiate cohort. J Neurol Sci. 2011;309:34–39. doi: 10.1016/j.jns.2011.07.039. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Galetta KM, Morganroth J, Moehringer N, et al. Adding vision to concussion testing: a prospective study of sideline testing in youth and college athletes. J Neuroophthalmol. 2015;35:235–241. doi: 10.1097/WNO.0000000000000226. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Galetta KM, Liu M, Leong DF, Ventura RE, Galetta SL, Balcer LJ. The King-Devick test of rapid number naming for concussion detection: meta-analysis and systematic review of the literature. Concussion. 2016;2 doi: 10.2217/cnc.15.8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Heitger MH, Anderson TJ, Jones RD. Saccade sequences as markers for cerebral dysfunction following mild closed head injury. Prog Brain Res. 2002;140:433–448. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(02)40067-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Pierrot-Deseilligny C, Muri RM, Ploner CJ, Gaymard B, Demeret S, Rivaud-Pechoux S. Decisional role of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in ocular motor behavior. Brain. 2003;126:1460–1473. doi: 10.1093/brain/awg148. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.White OB, Fielding J. Cognition and eye movements: assessment of cerebral dysfunction. J Neuroophthalmol. 2012;32:266–273. doi: 10.1097/WNO.0b013e3182688230. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Gilbert LC. Functional Motor Efficiency of the Eyes and its Relation to Reading. University of California Publications in Education. 1953;11 [Google Scholar]
- 16.Cobbs L, Hasanaj L, Amorapanth P, et al. Mobile Universal Lexicon Evaluation System (MULES) test: a new measure of rapid picture naming for concussion. J Neurol Sci. 2017;372:393–398. doi: 10.1016/j.jns.2016.10.044. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.McCrory P, Meeuwisse W, Dvorak J, et al. Consensus statement on concussion in sport-the 5th international conference on concussion in sport held in Berlin, October 2016. Br J Sports Med. 2017;51:838–847. doi: 10.1136/bjsports-2017-097699. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Kandel ER. The Age of Insight. Random House; New York: 2012. [Google Scholar]
- 19.Wells FL, Reusch J. Mental Examiner’s Handbook. The Psychological Corporation; New York: 1945. [Google Scholar]
- 20.Geschwind N. The varieties of naming errors. Cortex. 1967;3:97–112. [Google Scholar]
- 21.Geschwind N, Fusillo M. Color-naming defects in association with alexia. Arch Neurol. 1966;15:137–146. doi: 10.1001/archneur.1966.00470140027004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Denckla MB, Rudel R. Rapid “automatized” naming of pictured objects, colors, letters and numbers by normal children. Cortex. 1974;10:185–202. doi: 10.1016/s0010-9452(74)80009-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Denckla MB, Cutting LE. History and significance of rapid automatized naming. Ann Dyslexia. 1999;49:29–42. [Google Scholar]
- 24.Norton ES, Wolf M. Rapid automatized naming (RAN) and reading fluency: implications for understanding and treatment of reading disabilities. Annu Rev Psychol. 2012;63:427–452. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100431. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Denckla MB, Rudel RG. Naming of objects by dyslexic and other learning-disabled children. Brain Lang. 1976;3:1–15. doi: 10.1016/0093-934x(76)90001-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Denckla MB, Rudel RG. Rapid automatized naming (R.A.N): dyslexia differ-entiated from other learning disabilities. Neuropsychologia. 1976;14:471–479. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(76)90075-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.LaRocca NG, Hudson LD, Rudick R, et al. The MSOAC approach to developing performance outcomes to measure and monitor multiple sclerosis disability. Mult Scler J. 2017;1:1–16. doi: 10.1177/1352458517723718. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Balcer LJ, Raynowska J, Nolan R, et al. Validity of low-contrast letter acuity as a visual performance outcome measure for multiple sclerosis. Mult Scler J. 2017;23:734–747. doi: 10.1177/1352458517690822. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Ruesch J. Intellectual impairment in head injuries. Am J Psychiatry. 1944;100:480–496. [Google Scholar]
- 30.Seymour KJ, Williams MA, Rich AN. The representation of color across the human visual cortex: distinguishing chromatic signals contributing to object form versus surface color. Cereb Cortex. 2016;26:1997–2005. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhv021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Kellenbach M, Hovius M, Patterson K. A PET study of visual and semantic knowledge about objects. Cortex. 2005;41:121–132. doi: 10.1016/s0010-9452(08)70887-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Roy JE, Buschman TJ, Miller EK. Prefrontal cortex neurons reflect categorical decisions about ambiguous stimuli. J Cogn Neurosci. 2014;26:1283–1291. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00568. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Aggleton JP, Albasser MM, Aggleton DJ, Poirier GL, Pearce JM. Lesions of the rat perirhinal cortex spare the acquisition of a complex configural visual discrimination yet impair object recognition. Behav Neurosci. 2010;124:55–68. doi: 10.1037/a0018320. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Antunes M, Biala G. The novel object recognition memory: neurobiology, test procedure, and its modifications. Cogn Process. 2012;12:93–110. doi: 10.1007/s10339-011-0430-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Watson HC, Lee ACH. The perihinal cortex and recognition memory interference. J Neurosci. 2013;33:4192–4200. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2075-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Klenberg L, Korkman M, Lahti-Nuuttila P. Differential development of attention and executive functions in 3- to 12-year-old Finnish children. Dev Neuropsychol. 2001;20:407–428. doi: 10.1207/S15326942DN2001_6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Brocki KC, Bohlin G. Executive functions in children aged 6 to 13: a dimensional and developmental study. Dev Neuropsychol. 2004;26:571–593. doi: 10.1207/s15326942dn2602_3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Luna B, Garver KE, Urban TA, Lazar NA, Sweeney JA. Maturation of cognitive processes from late childhood to adulthood. Child Dev. 2004;75:1357–1372. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00745.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Hooper CJ, Luciana M, Conklin HM, Yarger RS. Adolescents’ performance on the development of decision making and ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Dev Psychol. 2004;40:1148–1158. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.40.6.1148. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Fuster JM. Frontal lobe and cognitive development. J Neurocytol. 2002;31:373–385. doi: 10.1023/a:1024190429920. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Sowell ER, Thompson PM, Holmes CJ, Jernigan TL, Toga AW. In vivo evidence for post-adolescent brain maturation in frontal and striatal regions. Nat Neurosci. 1999;2:859–861. doi: 10.1038/13154. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Sowell ER, Thompson PM, Tessner KD, Toga AW. Mapping continued brain growth and gray matter density reduction in dorsal frontal cortex: inverse relationships during post adolescent brain maturation. J Neurosci. 2001;21:8819–8829. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-22-08819.2001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Blakemore SJ, Choudhury S. Development of the adolescent brain: implications for executive function and social cognition. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2006;47:296–312. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01611.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]