Abstract
The similarities between the structures built by social insects and by humans have led to a convergence of interests between biologists and architects. This new, de facto interdisciplinary community of scholars needs a common terminology and theoretical framework in which to ground its work. In this conceptually oriented review paper, we review the terms ‘information’, ‘space’ and ‘architecture’ to provide definitions that span biology and architecture. A framework is proposed on which interdisciplinary exchange may be better served, with the view that this will aid better cross-fertilization between disciplines, working in the areas of collective behaviour and analysis of the structures and edifices constructed by non-humans; and to facilitate how this area of study may better contribute to the field of architecture. We then use these definitions to discuss the informational content of constructions built by organisms and the influence these have on behaviour, and vice versa. We review how spatial constraints inform and influence interaction between an organism and its environment, and examine the reciprocity of space and information on construction and the behaviour of humans and social insects.
This article is part of the theme issue ‘Interdisciplinary approaches for uncovering the impacts of architecture on collective behaviour’.
Keywords: space, information, architecture, perception, social systems
1. Introduction
Living systems are both constructions and constructors [1,2]. At the fundamental level, organic molecules self-assemble into organic compounds (e.g. proteins and DNA) that build organelles and cells [1]. Cells, in turn, can assemble themselves into tissues, organs and ultimately fully functional organisms [3–8]. Organisms modify their environment to build functional structures that will protect them (e.g. bird nests) and help them acquire the resources that they need for their development, survival and reproduction (e.g. spider web) [9–11]. Finally, organisms in societies can combine their building efforts to achieve constructions that no single individual could produce on its own, as is exemplified by termite mounds and human skyscrapers, which can be several hundreds—or even thousands—times larger than the individuals that build them [11,12].
Social insects, in particular, have long fascinated biologists by their ability to mould their environment to their needs [13–16]. Some species of ants are known to clear debris and vegetation to form large trail networks the size of a football field, connecting their multiple nests to various resources [17–19]. Others have mastered the art of tunnelling to build underground networks of galleries connecting chambers housing their workforce, brood, food stockpiles and even subterranean fungus garden [20–25]. Many species of ants, termites, bees and wasps build structures by accumulating material (e.g. wax, saliva-imbibed soil or vegetable fibres) that will form walls, pillars, floors and ceilings [14,26–35]. Finally, some ants and bees use their own bodies as construction material, attaching to each other and creating dynamical structures such as bridges, ladders, holds and temporary nests [12,36–49].
The complexity and diversity of structures built by social insects is reminiscent of that of human beings [50]. Their construction rules are however radically different. Unlike human-made constructions that are most often composed of inert and standardized units assembled in a precise order, social insect constructions are built from more plastic and irregular components, and their assemblage results from distributed processes of self-organization with little to no supervision [13,51,52]. As a result, their structures are less standardized, but more capable of adjusting their conformation in response to changes in the conditions in which they are placed [12,37,38,53,54].
The parallels and divergences between the structures built by social insects and by humans have sparked a lot of interest in the architectural community [55–60]. The natural world has been an inspiration for architects since antiquity, with biology becoming a key influence on design thinking at the turn of the nineteenth century, when the analogical influence turned to interest in how biological systems develop and evolve [61,62]. Coupled with the computational capacity to simulate natural systems, architects are today exploring the self-organizing and emergent morphologies of biological phenomena to rethink how buildings and cities are designed [63–71]. The emergent, adaptable and situated structures built by social insects offer intriguing insights in particular for architects to re-evaluate not only the sustainable aspects of the human-built environment but also to question the distinction between cognitive phases of human architecture (i.e. between design, construction and occupancy stages) and to think about these as continuous. [72].
Recently, biologists and architects have started coming together to form a new community, interested in understanding the construction mechanisms used by social insects and their potential applications in human-made structures [55,73]. As is to be expected between two disciplines that have existed in parallel with little interaction, terminology has quickly become the first obstacle to creating a theoretical framework in which to ground the emerging field. During discussions preceding the writing of this manuscript, the authors have identified three concepts, in particular, that rendered their mutual understanding difficult: architecture, space and information. In what follows, we will first try to reconcile the somewhat liberal use by biologists of the concept of architecture with the more institutional definition that architects have of it. We will then discuss the concept of space in architecture and biology, and how social systems use space both as a source of information and a means to encode social information. Finally, we will discuss the idea of information itself and the effects of architecture on information flow and processing in social systems.
2. The scope of the review
One of the problems with interdisciplinary work is language, and is what may be termed the baggage individual disciplines bring to the table. Essentially, terminology can be a barrier for interdisciplinary exchange. Key terms, such as architecture, space and information have long conceptual histories, such that even their everyday use is awkward. Closer inspection only muddies the water further because of the way different disciplines claim the high ground with regards their specific outlook. ‘Space’, for example, is from one side an enclosure (i.e. it has boundaries) and from the other the void (i.e. the volume contained within these boundaries). Our capacity to mathematically articulate spatial scenarios gives the impression that ‘space’ is something we have generally mastered conceptually, but the fact that a concise definition evades us implies otherwise.
Another case in point is the title of this paper, which is loaded with conceptual connotations. ‘Architecture’, for example, is principally concerned with the human-built environment. It is the practice of designing buildings and articulating how to build the design; not forgetting how to explain the rationale behind the design to demonstrate why that design should be built. Professional architectural societies, such as the Royal Institute of British Architects (founded to facilitate and promote the advancement of architecture) guard the term specifically as referring to buildings designed by architects, and the Architects Registration Board, the statutory body for the registration of architects in the UK, protect the term in law. Yet, these terms (architecture and architect) are often borrowed to refer to complicated structures and artefacts, such as software applications and circuit boards, recognized as products of intentional design. This trend is particularly apparent within the frame of this special issue, which is concerned with constructions built, particularly, by social insects and comparisons that may be drawn between such structures and the human-built environment.
The authors, a biologist and an architect, brought together through their interest in the natural world and specifically the structures that creatures (other than humans) construct, have sought to establish a ground on which interdisciplinary exchange may be better served by discussing definitions of fundamental terms that span biology and architecture. Our primary goal is to aid better cross-fertilization between disciplines, working in the areas of collective behaviour and analysis of the structures and edifices constructed by non-humans; and to facilitate how this area of study may better contribute to the field of architecture.
3. Towards an interdisciplinary framework
(a). Are social insects architects?
Architecture has many meanings. For instance, Steven Holl said, during his acceptance speech for the 2012 American Institute of Architects Gold Medal, that ‘architecture is an art bridging the humanities and sciences' [74]. Thomas Mayne, at his Pritzker Prize acceptance speech, said that ‘architecture is a way of seeing, thinking and questioning our world and our place in it’ [75]. Claiming social responsibility as its most definitive attribute, Samuel Mockbee asserts ‘architecture is a social art. And as a social art, it is our social responsibility to make sure we are delivering architecture that meets not only functional and creature comforts, but also spiritual comfort’ [76]. Diebedo Francis Kere echoes Mockbee: ‘architecture is not just about building. It's a means of improving people's quality of life’ (see Hales 2005 [77]).
One thing that is, however, common to all these quotes is that architecture is something other than just a building. Jay A. Pritzker claimed that architecture ‘is intended to transcend the simple need for shelter and security by becoming an expression of artistry’ [78]. In this context, a building is considered as no more than the sum of its parts. Architecture, however, is other than that. The whole is perceived to have an independence distinct from the objects it is composed of. If this is what the architects claim then how do the term, and mindset, transfer to edifices formed by non-humans? If architects and biologists are indeed concerned with developing interdisciplinary collaborations (to study, for example, ant nests), we need to dispel the notion of architecture being exclusive to humans and consider it from a non-anthropocentric perspective.
Vitruvius (ca 80–70 BC to ca 15 BC) wrote De Architectura libri decem (commonly referred to as ‘The Ten Books on Architecture’ [79]), which is regarded as the first book on architectural theory. Often referred to as the first architect, he asserted architecture to have three qualities: Firmitas, Utilitas and Venustas. Henry Wotton, a seventeenth century translator, interpreted these terms as ‘firmness’ (well-constructed) and ‘commodity’ (functional) for the first two, with Venustas being less well defined and often interpreted as ‘beauty’ or ‘delight’. We take on the latter version on the premise that it implies something ephemeral and other than the sum of the parts, while beauty has connotations of the beholder's eye and is tied to subjective concerns of taste and style. The first two concepts are unlikely to cause controversy between architects and biologists; both disciplines actually express them in similar terms, as we will discuss below. Delight, however, will require more consideration on our part. Indeed aesthetics—which makes the whole ‘other’ than the sum of its parts—is a concept difficult to operationalize in the scientific study of animal behaviour, and we will attempt to find a middle-ground that biologists and architects can build upon.
(i). Firmness
Vitrivius’ ‘firmness’ is understood as the physical properties of a construction that guarantee its structural soundness, at the very least for the time the building is needed. These properties depend on trade-offs between many factors including construction material and methods, technological advances, substrate composition, environmental conditions and costs. Architects use tools from physics, engineering and economics to balance these different factors and plan accordingly the construction process. Biologists use a similar set of tools to measure biological structures, characterize their construction process and ultimately determine the balance of constraints made by the animals.
Architects and biologists are, for instance, equally interested in measuring the physical properties of construction material. Weight, density, strength and deformability are all determining factors in choosing construction material for buildings. Animals themselves are sensitive to the physical properties of the construction material. Termites, for example, preferentially dig through non-load-bearing over load-bearing wood, and build thicker load-bearing clay walls when attacking loaded wood [32]. Architects rely on tools from materials science and engineering to select materials with desirable physical properties, and from applied physics for combining these materials in a structurally sound manner. Software tools like Oasys' GSA Building (see https://www.oasyssoftware.com/products/structural/gsa-building/) enables detailed analysis of structural solutions providing an accurate prediction of material performance, how a structure interacts with the ground and the impact of footfall on irregular structures. Autodesk's Insight 360 platform (see https://insight360.autodesk.com/oneenergy) permits architects to simulate and analyse building energy and environmental performance so they can approach the design process with the understanding of factors leading to better building performance outcomes throughout the building life cycle [80]. Biologists rely on similar tools to quantify the physical properties of animal constructions. For instance, Cole et al. [26] conducted a comparative study of the physical properties of nest paper in three species of wasps, showing that the fibre composition of the paper might explain differences in thickness and tensile strength between nests. In termite mounds, King et al. [81,82] used structural (e.g. mound geometry) and dynamic (e.g. air flow) measurements to demonstrate that a ‘simple combination of geometry, heterogeneous thermal mass, and porosity allows the mounds to use diurnal ambient temperature oscillations for ventilation’ ([82], p.11589). Finally, and somewhat bridging architecture and biology, the physical qualities of termite mound soil have inspired researchers to evaluate their use in human-made constructions, such as in compressed earth bricks [83] and pavement material [84].
This commonality of tools and approaches provides opportunities for direct interactions between biology and architecture. Indeed, the standardized language of physics and engineering is particularly useful to transfer ‘technology’ between the two disciplines. Case in point, the passive ventilation system of termite mounds has inspired the design of several buildings [85], such as the Eastgate Centre in Harara, Zimbabwe, for instance [86]. The study of the physical and mechanical properties of social insect constructions may, therefore, be the most obvious starting point for collaborations between architects and biologists, and the one that is most likely to generate direct applications of the building principles of natural systems.
(ii). Commodity
Vitrivius’ ‘commodity’ refers to the efficient organization of spaces and systems that support the functions of the construction. It determines how the different parts of the building are used by its occupants and the benefits that they receive from it, relative to other possible organizations of the building. This concept is critical to both human constructions and biological structures, as it links form and function with each other. Unlike ‘firmness’, which is mainly studied with tools from physics and engineering, ‘commodity’ in architecture and biology is more often characterized with methods from behaviour and psychology, with a particular interest in the interaction between the organization of the structure and the distribution of behaviours within.
A first concern of both architects and biologists is the spatial separation of functions that might have an adverse effect on each other. An obvious example is the spatial segregation of feeding locations from excretory areas in order to reduce the spread of infections. In human-made buildings, this segregation is achieved by the physical separation of food storage, cooking and consumption areas from the lavatories. Segregation of function can also be enforced by social conventions and regulations that make certain behaviours acceptable in some locations only (e.g. smoking bans inside publicly accessible buildings). Similarly, the spatial separation of functions is also present in structures built by social insects (see §3b(ii)).
Another common interest of architects and biologists is in determining how efficiently a structure is used, and how its organization balances different, often contradictory uses. In architecture, this can have important implications in terms of, for instance, building safety (e.g. during an evacuation) [87], economic consequences (e.g. time spent by customers in store aisles) [88] and access (e.g. to favour space use by certain categories of users). In social insect constructions, researchers more often look at issues of resource accessibility [17], information flow [89] and nest defensibility [90]. In any case, biologists and architects again use similar tools here to measure and predict the efficiency of a structure relative to one or more of these objectives. For instance, researchers and practitioners in both disciplines regularly employ agent-based models to determine how the spatial organization of a structure affects the distribution of individuals, be they ants in a network of galleries [91] or humans in an art gallery [63]. Fitting such models to data from human and non-human systems allows for direct comparison between them, as has been done multiple times in studies of building evacuation, for instance [92–96].
Finally, tools from graph theory can be used to measure the efficiency of a structure in terms of connectivity between its different parts. The tools have been used to characterize structures built by social insects such as ant and termite nests [23,90], and ant foraging trails [17,18], but also human-made constructions such as urban settlements [71,97], communication networks [98], water distribution systems [99,100] and transportation networks [101]. More specifically, graph theory has been applied in architectural design as a method of describing building form and a way of automatically generating plan arrangements [62,102]. For instance, Space Syntax theory describes how connectivity and integration of areas within buildings and cities epitomize human social relations, and through mapping, the heterogeneity within architectural forms correlates topological relationships between building and settlement configurations and people [103,104]. Such approaches also allow for direct comparisons between human-made and insect-made networks that can be indicative of common building principles. For instance, Buhl et al. [97] showed that street networks in non-planned settlements have similar cost–efficiency trade-offs to the emergent structure of ant tunnelling networks. As in the previous section on ‘firmness’, this commonality of tools and analysis language should allow for more frequent collaborations between architects and biologists.
(iii). Delight
Finally, Vitrivius’ ‘Delight’ is generally understood as an aesthetic quality, defined in terms of style, proportion or visual beauty, and is symptomatic of how architecture is a visually dominant discipline. That architecture is dominated by a concern for the visual is long held [105], and the visual sense has played a significant role in our evolution as a species. This emphasis has driven cultural and technological development, which has in turn reinforced the prominence of our visual sense [106]. But ‘delight’ is not specifically attuned to the visual and there is a growing sense that architects should account for a wider sensorial domain in the artefacts they create [107,108]. Indeed ‘delight’ infers something of pleasure or joy, which is open to all sensation and sources of stimulation, and thus encompasses all senses.
If we follow the definition professed by Frederick Kiesler, that architecture is emotional, what distinguishes architecture from the building is that the former evokes emotion [109]. Such a definition sidesteps the moral high ground of architectural practice and schools, because it states simply that architecture affects and causes emotion. Understanding architecture as such allows one (i) to transcend boundaries, because it relates to the sensing emotive capacity of the observer and (ii) to consider architecture a product of perceptual systems that perceive stimuli [110].
So, whether a construction, built by social insects or humans, can be considered architecture or not is open to interpretation. As such we are faced instead with philosophical traditions and how one sees the world, and thus one's place among those things we share it with. We must ask, then, if we are to accept the term ‘social insect architecture’ whether ants, for example, have aesthetically triggered emotions? We cannot sidestep this question.
While it is obvious that the nests of social insects have specialized functional dimensions [111–113], the question of whether they are also built aesthetically is difficult to address scientifically. There is no doubt that in the eyes of a human observer, social insect nests are beautiful objects [16]. However, whether they are in the eyes of an ant or a honeybee is more complicated to answer. Social insects can react and associate meaning to a wide variety of stimuli [114–118], but whether they derive emotions from these stimuli is unknown—or at least undiscussed in the literature. Some species of social insects seem to be decorating their nests with artefacts whose function is not immediately evident (e.g. the pebbles and twigs on meat ant nests [119]). But are these true aesthetic artefacts built with the intention of triggering emotions, or more simply construction patterns resulting from the evolutionary history of the organism, for instance, as a mechanism for nest recognition? [119]. If the latter, does this not apply to human artefacts as well? After all, our senses and cognitive processes are also the products of our evolutionary history, therefore our aesthetic experiences should be as well [120].
Taking a non-anthropocentric view, we need to relinquish the idea that aesthetics is an intellectual pursuit, and that it may be a judgement (or act) based on the assignment of value to something. The concept of aesthetics was originally coined by the philosopher Alexander von Baumgarten (1741–1762), who argued aesthetics is the study of the plenitude and complexity of sensations [121] (also, cf. [110]). When Kant took up the concept he drained it of its sensory plenitude, revising its significance to contemplation and judgement of beauty (see Howes & Classen 2013 [122]). If we take a step back (to Baumgarten), we may consider the edifices built by social insects, as having some aesthetic quality from the organism’s perspective—whatever that might be. We may conclude then that architecture (in its widest sense) is a product of behaviours that support and enhance physiological and social needs. On the one side, to provide protection and shelter and on the other, to shape and manage activity. The former applies to all constructions by humans and animals, the latter to social organisms in particular (humans and most typically social insects) that use their constructions as a form of enabling the device to organize actions and define social conditions [112,123].
Therefore, we propose that what truly separates construction from architecture is that the reaction of an organism to the former cannot be distinguished from its reaction to a similar artefact resulting from extraneous processes (that is, processes foreign to that organism). Architecture, on the contrary, carries social information that has the potential of affecting the behaviour of organisms beyond the simple physical constraints imposed by the organization of the structure on them. A builder assembles a construction, but makes it architecture by embedding messages in it—be they intentional to prompt or provoke behaviour or unintentional, in which case they may be a by-product of the builders’ behaviour or happenstance.
(b). Construction as a way to shape space
One of the main outcomes of construction is, arguably, the organization of spatial relationships between individuals, their activities and their environment. Through construction, organisms—be they humans or social insects—partition their environment into distinct zones that can support different functions (e.g. feeding versus excreting) and separate different habitats (e.g. outdoors versus indoors) or different populations (e.g. employees versus customers). This partitioning necessarily creates spatial relationships between the separated elements. This may seem obvious to the reader, yet the idea of space only appeared in architectural discourse in the late nineteenth century, when it became important in two ways: first as the embodiment of human activity inside the architectural form [124], and second when it became aligned to aesthetic ideas in an attempt to define beauty [125]. The issue of space thereafter became a central topic in architecture, initially in terms of sensorial engagement with the environment [126]. (See van de Ven [127] for a concise history of how the idea of space has developed in architectural theory.)
The issue of space is also central to biology at all levels of biological organization. From the partitioning of biochemical reactions within cells [128] to the influence of large-scale environmental patterns on species distribution [129], measuring spatial relationships is critical to understanding life in general. In the context of this review, we are more specifically interested in how organisms reshape their environment through their building behaviour, and how in return the resulting constructions impose spatial constraints that direct further behaviours. These two questions apply similarly to humans and social insects, and the main goal of this section is, therefore, to identify research themes common to biologists and architects and to draw comparisons between their respective approaches.
For this purpose, we propose here that the spatial character of built constructions can be approached from three complementary and non-mutually exclusive angles. By no means do we claim that these angles are the only possible, but we think that they should encompass most of the research issues related to space and construction:
1. First, we will consider that constructions almost always separate an outside from an inside world, most often for reasons linked to protecting the organisms from some aspects of their environment.
2. We will also discuss the role of the spatial organization of the construction and its interaction with behaviour in segregating functions within a population and in channelling the individuals' activities.
3. Finally, we will examine how the spatial configuration of the construction can itself generate functions that benefit the organisms without necessarily requiring their active participation.
(i). Constructions provide protection
The primary function of construction is arguably to provide shelter to organisms from adverse conditions in their environment. An enclosed, insulated space will, for instance, be less subject to climatic variations such as changes in temperature and humidity levels, thereby facilitating an organism's homeostatic regulation. Walls and ceilings also offer barriers that can shield—for a time at least—an organism from any physical threat, such as falling objects or predators. Therefore, one of construction's most important purposes is to create a separation between an outside, often unsafe and unpredictable world, and an inside, more stable and less dangerous one.
Social insects are masters at building fortresses to protect their colonies from intruders. Their nests range from simple holes in the ground or in vegetation [130,131], to vast underground complexes of chambers interconnected by tunnels and housing sometimes several millions of individuals [132,133]. Like human strongholds, the nests of social insects are organized to limit outside access, with only a small number of entrances (often a single one). In many species, specialized workers—often called soldiers and morphologically distincts from the other workers—are found guarding these entrances against intruders [134,135]. In some species of ants and termites, these ‘guards’ have even evolved morphological and/or behavioural adaptations allowing them to plug the entrances with their own bodies, quickly preventing access to the inside of the nest when under attack [12,130,131,136,137]. Outside the fortress, several species of social insects also build protected passages that connect the nest to resources sometimes hundreds of metres away. These passages can be underground tunnels as in leaf-cutting ants and some termite species [132,138–140], mud tunnels (shelter tubes) built by termites along tree trunks [141,142], or even ‘living’ walls that Dorylus ants form along with their trails out of their own bodies [143].
The nests of social insects are not built to resist physical threats only. Indeed many social insect species regulate the micro-climate within their nests in order to maintain stable living conditions, independent from variations of the outside environment [144]. Termite mounds are arguably the most striking examples of constructions by social insects capable of shielding the colony from changes in the external weather conditions [81,82,144–146]. The structure itself of the mound creates temperature gradients that, in turn, generate air currents, balancing the temperature within the nest and ensuring stable gas exchanges [81,82]. A similar phenomenon can be found in some leaf-cutting ant nests, which regulate the oxygen/carbon dioxide balance through passive air movements [35,147–150]. Social insects also regulate the internal conditions of the nest in a more active fashion. Bees, for instance, aggregate at the entrance of their hive on hot days and use their wings to move hot air outside the hive and cooler air inside [151–153]. Army ants, which form temporary nests called bivouacs out of their own bodies, increase or decrease the spacing between each other to regulate the internal temperature of the colony [154]. Finally, in many ant species digging nests into the ground, the workers regularly relocate their brood away from or towards the surface as it heats up or cools down, in order to maintain the brood near their optimal development temperature [155,156].
Protection from the outside world comes at a cost for the colony. Evidently, the constant upkeep and remodelling of the nest structure takes away workers from other essential tasks such as foraging or taking care of the brood. A balance must, therefore, be found between maintaining the nest's integrity and carrying on the other activities of the colony. It is evident that some species invest a lot of time and energy in building and maintaining their nests (e.g. African and Australian termite mounds; the vast underground nests of Atta ants), while others barely improve the pre-existing cavities in which they nest (e.g. rock ants and turtle ants). Do complex—and therefore costly to build and maintain—nests evolve only in species with a strong need for protection—against predators or the environment—or is nest complexity secondary to evolving efficient behaviours to accomplish the other tasks necessary for the survival of the colony? To the best of our knowledge, there has been no systematic study of this trade-off.
Like the ants, humans have long built structures for defence and protection from the climate. Both functions are fundamental form-generating forces in human architecture, but as architects have embraced advancements in technology the influence climate has on human construction has lessened. Similar to the strategies of ants described above, humans have occupied hollows in the ground, carved out underground buildings and networks, and capitalized on features of the landscape to regulate the micro-climate within dwellings and maintain stable living conditions. Dwellings built in the ground, such as the Matmata houses in the Sahara and the Opal miners' houses in Australia use a layer of the Earth as coolant, and Réso, a network of underground tunnels in Montreal provide protection during the long winter. In Naours, France, an underground settlement includes a bakery and chapel. In southern China, the circular Tulou buildings are designed to offer protection from the monsoon rain, and in Normandy aerodynamic roofs provide protection from harsh Atlantic winds (see Piesik [157] for a review).
While societies have long constructed buildings using local materials and inherited construction techniques (vernacular architecture) to provide protection, innovation in the use of materials means the result is not simply a consequence of assembling gathered materials in a rudimentary way, but creatively transforming them. Ashanti huts, for example, have a wooden frame with a roof of branches on top, on which a layer of beaten mud is supported. Contrary to what you might expect, the thick heavy walls do not support the roof, so structurally they act as curtain walls. This may be due to cultural influence, but it is also likely a result of climatic reasoning. An advantage of this construction is the phasing, providing shelter quickly while the walls are erected [158].
Glass is perhaps one the most important innovations in modern building, and has changed the way we perceive the difference between inside and outside space. It blurs the lines between the two by providing physical protection but visual connection. In turn this changes the way we behave and how we think about space. It is interesting to look back at how the issue of space arose in architectural discourse and came to inform the modernist ideal of how space is deemed to flow from one area to another. The conflation of inside and outside was central to the architectural ideology of Leberecht Migge (1881–1935), who promoted the interpenetration of architecture and landscape through rational geometric lines with extensive use of glass to connect the two. Glazed doors and windows formed the Zwischenglieder (interstices) between inside and outside to provide connection with nature, and greenhouses encircling houses providing thermal protection in winter [159]). Migge's interstitial notion of space does not compartmentalize and it does not follow the general tendency to categorize the world into discrete units: between internal and external, and, for example, rooms by function. This controlled and ordered categorization transfers to how we perceive and consequently organize space. We will come back to this in the next section.
(ii). Organization
Division of labour is a landmark of social life. Most social insect species are characterized by a strong behavioural, and also often physical differentiation between groups of individuals specialized in performing different tasks (e.g. foraging, brood tending, etc.) inside the colony [160–163]. In many species, this division of labour is also characterized by the spatial segregation of tasks within the nest, with specialized areas dedicated to specific activities [160,164,165]. A typical example of this spatial organization of activities is the nest of leaf cutter Atta ants [20,132,133,166]. They are composed of a network of tunnels connecting chambers that are all dedicated to a specific task. Some chambers house fungus gardens that serve as primary food source for the colony. Others contain the brood at different stages of development. Finally, rubbish dumps are created inside and outside the nest, isolating the colony from the waste material it produces [167,168].
The spatial segregation of tasks has important consequences for the organization of the colony. Indeed, it has been shown that interactions are much more frequent between ants performing similar tasks [165], and that interaction rates are important regulatory signals for activating and inhibiting workers to perform particular tasks [169–172]. Because activities are segregated within the nest, workers specializing on a particular set of tasks are therefore more likely to interact with other workers with a similar behavioural profile, increasing their ability to share relevant information about their preferred tasks. Moreover, as workers transition towards other behavioural profiles as they age, they might relocate progressively within the nest towards areas better suited to their new preferences, possibly helped by the rate of interactions with workers of the same or of different behavioural profiles.
It is interesting to note here that the spatial segregation of tasks is not necessarily accompanied by the building of barriers to physically separate them. In ants and honeybees, for instance, the brood is often grouped by type (e.g. workers versus drones) or developmental stage within a single space, without walls separating them [30,173,174]. Similarly, the content of honeybee comb cells is often organized spatially, with brood-containing cells grouped together in the centre of the comb, surrounded by a band of pollen-containing cells, and then a larger peripheral region of honey-containing cells, but again with no physical barrier between these different areas [30,163].
The existence of a spatial segregation of tasks without physical barriers is understood to be the result of simple self-organizing processes of differential aggregation [174–177]. This suggests that different areas within a nest—with or without physical separation—might specialize in a particular type of task, not because of their intrinsic characteristics, but because of social feedback loops between the workers: the more a task is performed at a location, the more likely it will be performed again at that location. For instance, in a recent study, Czaczkes et al. showed that Lasius ants will preferentially drop their faeces at specific locations within their nest (usually a specific corner of a specific chamber) [111], separate from other waste materials that are gathered in piles outside the nest (the ‘trash’) [111]. This behaviour is most likely driven by social signals contained in the faeces (e.g. pheromones) that stimulate ants to leave their faeces where other ants have done it, leading to the creation of, effectively, toilets. This self-organized spatial segregation of tasks [178–182] is at odds with the way it is achieved in human constructions. Indeed, buildings built by humans are planned ahead and each room is pre-assigned a type of task, and then fitted with all the required features for users to accomplish these tasks.
The basic purpose of any building is to satisfy the physiological and social needs of the organism: on the one side, to provide protection and shelter, as discussed above; on the other, to shape and manage activity. The former transmits to all constructions: human and animal; the latter to social organisms (humans and most typically social insects), which build structures that act as a form of enabling device to organize activity and define social conditions. Scrutinizing built structures enables us to consider space retrospectively as a system of social relations from which rules, or patterns, of inhabitation may be extrapolated. For instance, Bill Hillier and Julienne Hanson analysed the organization of built forms and illustrated how the configuration of space changes when specified from the perspective of each distinct area constituting planned arrangements [103]. Identifying the heterogeneity of built forms, they revealed buildings to be systems of activity defined by the dynamics of social and cultural goings-on. Similarly, analysis of social insect nest structures illustrates intricate spatial arrangements and the social structure of the colony [90,183].
Working out the organization of a building is one of the most important and taxing aspects of architectural design. The task of organizing the numerous criteria of a building programme was identified by Rittel and Weber [184] as ‘wicked’, because planning problems tend to be combinatorially hard. The typical approach to organizing a building is to flatten the problem, so that the activities to be housed can be planned. This has led some, like Paul Coates, to claim the way architects traditionally organize a building is most unnatural [66]. Inspired by the way natural systems are understood as pattern making and problem-solving, architects are today looking to the replication of phenomena in biology and computer science (such as flocking [185], stigmergy [185–188], branching systems [188], food foraging and nest construction [189], replication [190] and so forth) as an alternative approach to modelling form and structure that evades the traditional top–down centralized decision-making process of configuration. This has opened up a whole new way of thinking about configuration in architecture, which is bottom–up and generative, and reminds us of Migge's interstitial notion of space whereby internal and external domains are conflated and flow into one another (see the previous section).
The architect Frederick Kiesler (1890–1965), who was strongly influenced by biology [61,191], promoted a notion of space extending Migge. He considered space to be continuous, or endless—not in sense of the void but in terms of a line for which both ends meet. This notion of space, which is evident in both the organization and materiality of his work [192], was informed by what he saw as a fundamental distinction between how humans construct and what he observed in nature. ‘Nature [he says] builds by cell division towards continuity while man can only build by joining together into a unique structure without continuity’ [193, p. 67]. His point is that humans construct through brute force (connecting parts together to form a whole: we bolt, glue and force elements together). In non-human constructions parts merge, overlap and conjoin one another as a consequence of self-organizing and emergent processes. The concept of stigmergy describing social insect nest construction is a case in point, which we will come back to in §3c(ii). Kiesler sought to emphasize that how we organize space and devise the arrangement of matter is tied to how we comprehend space and distinguish spatial relations.
(iii). Function building
An organism's fitness is not determined by its personal morphological, physiological and behavioural phenotypes only. It is also influenced by phenomena that result from its activity, but are not a physical part of its being [194]. This ‘extended phenotype’ includes structures built by the organism and that provide it with services increasing its survival and reproductive success. The nests of social insects' colonies are exemplars of extended phenotypes that have played a critical role in their evolutionary history [195,196]. Besides providing protection (as discussed in §3b(i)) and a means to organize the colony's activity (as discussed in §3b(ii)), the architecture of the nest itself can generate other complex emergent functions for the benefit of the colony.
Perhaps the most well-known example of a function that it ‘outsourced’ to the nest architecture by social insects is that of ventilation, permitting the regulation of temperature, humidity and respiratory gas composition within the nest [35,81,82,145,149,197–200]. This is a common occurrence in large ant and termite nests, in which depth—and therefore insulation—could render air exchanges with the surface difficult in the absence of dedicated ventilation mechanisms. While ventilation can be actively performed by some social insects (e.g. in bees [144,151,153]), it is often achieved passively by nest structures that can harvest naturally occurring physical phenomena. For instance, it was shown that the interaction between wind and nest structure—and in particular the orientation of nest openings relative to wind direction—was responsible for ventilation in the large nests of the leaf-cutting ant Atta vollenweideri [35,149,200]. A similar mechanism was found to be responsible for nest ventilation in the termite Macrotermes michaelseni [198]. In termites, the mound that covers the nest can also be built so that daily temperature fluctuations caused by the sun heating part of the mound generate convective flow driving the ventilation of the nest [81,82].
In all the examples above, the structure of the nest itself performs the function, independently from the behaviour of the organisms that built it. In many cases, however, the function of the structure only becomes apparent when in interaction with the behaviour of the organism. For instance, topological and geometrical features of ant and termite networks of foraging trails and nest tunnels have been shown to guide the movement behaviour of the workers [19,23,90,91,201–206], for instance, facilitating the collective selection of the most efficient route within the network. In this case, the structure does not have a function by itself, but one is created when interacting with the behaviour of the organisms.
Similarly, the structure of human constructions performs functions independently to provide and maintain suitable living conditions and support physiological and social needs. A classic example of the former is passive ventilation, termed ‘natural ventilation’ to emphasize the lack of mechanical equipment to provide air exchange. The Eastgate Centre, mentioned earlier, is one example. Another is the Palace of Westminster's historic ventilation system designed in the 1840s by physician David Boswell Reid to serve the House of Commons and the House of Lords. These two debating chambers are internal spaces that have no external walls of their own. Reid's elaborate scheme includes more than 2000 vertical shafts, smoke flues and ventilation channels, some up to 200 m long, providing fresh air collected from towers and led through an intricate network to the basement of the building, where it was heated during winter, and released through outlets in the chambers. This included outlets placed in the seating, so fresh air was delivered directly to occupants [207].
More recently, Mesiniagra tower, designed by Ken Yeang, is a bio-climatic skyscraper in Malaysia, where the sun is a prime factor in design. Louvres provide protection from the sun, but Yeang's design was informed by the path of the sun, so the building's form also acts as a shading device reducing solar gain [208]. The form and shape of buildings can also act as a device to distribute people and control the flow of movement. Crowd disasters are a prevailing issue [65,209,210] that has led to extensive data collection to investigate the dynamics of crowd behaviour [211,212]. Serial incidents at the Hajj, Mecca, have resulted in the reorganization of the Hajj, and specifically a new design for the Jamarat bridge. Different levels serve pilgrims coming from different areas and directions to reduce crowding on the Jamarat plaza.
Control is a fundamental factor of institutional buildings, which is clearly evident in Jeremy Bentham's Panopticon. His design is a system of control allowing observation of prison inmates by a single watchman, without the inmates being able to tell whether or not they are being watched. The building acts as a device to prevent, or reduce, the likelihood of undesirable behaviour [208,213]. On a grander scale, Haussmann's plan for Paris remodelled the city to modernize it and also provide physical control of the population. He replaced many narrow streets, which allowed the revolutionaries to establish barricades, with broad boulevards and avenues. Less obviously, the wider streets function as a form of psychological crowd control—a mob may be less likely to revolt due to the expanse making them feel less powerful [214].
(c). Constructions as a way to shape information
All living systems communicate in some shape or form, be it through chemical emission (e.g. scent and pheromone), visual display (e.g. form, colour and movement), sound production (e.g. vocalization and vibration) or electric currents, to inform others of their own state (e.g. mating status) or of the state of their environment (e.g. incoming danger) [215,216]. As hinted at in the previous section, communication can also be achieved through the building. Indeed, each construction act, by modifying the content or configuration of the environment, has the potential of constraining or guiding future behaviours. In Batesonian epistemology, it is ‘a difference which makes a difference’, that is an ‘elementary unit of information’ [217]. If we accept that each feature of a construction potentially holds information—or even is information—then we need to discuss the meaning of this concept in biology and architecture. In particular, in this section, we will attempt to identify possible points of agreement and disagreement between the two fields in order to facilitate communication—no pun intended—and collaboration between researchers across the aisle.
The concept of information is rather proteiform in both the scientific and philosophical literature [218]. Scholars in all disciplines have already proposed an uncountable number of definitions of information. With this manuscript, it is neither our intent to introduce a new one, nor to discuss the relative merits of each existing definition. However, in the following sections, we will often refer explicitly and implicitly to two of the most prominent definitions of information—that of Claude Shannon and that of Gregory Bateson—and we think it necessary to briefly describe and contrast them here.
Claude Shannon's idea of information [219] is motivated by the need to measure and mathematically describe information in order to quantify differences between messages (e.g. to detect transmission errors) and degrees of dependence between different signals (e.g. to detect phase synchronization between separate sources of information). Rooted in statistics and probability theory, Shannon's information has been hugely influential in many disciplines in science and engineering, because of the analytical tools it provides for measuring and comparing the information content of random variables independently of their meaning. As Gibson points out, Shannon's information excludes the meaning of a stimulus to focus on the quality of message transmission from source to the receiver [110].
Gregory Bateson's ecological view of information is rooted in the cybernetic idea of communication and organization. The elementary unit of information, he claims, is a difference that makes a difference. He states, a difference that makes a difference is an idea. It is a ‘bit’, a ‘unit’ of information [217]. This somewhat paradoxical statement deserves unpacking. While Shannon's concept of information is about the reduction of uncertainty, Bateson implies a process of distinction. Both imply an observer, making choices, but Bateson infers a system classifying inputs or sensations subsequent to the ability to discriminate, initially between self and other, between things [220]. He describes a referencing system that perceives and thereby distinguishes [221,222], and accounts for how entities, be they cells, organisms or agents in a computer model, engage with their world. Bateson's unit of information is thereby also a unit of survival, whereby a difference is a matter of trial and error through which habits emerge. His concept of information is the basis for a theory of learning.
With these two approaches of information in mind, we will examine three general areas concerned with construction and information:
1. First, we will examine biological communication and information, and in particular the concepts of cues and signals and how they provide some evolutionary context to the present discussion.
2. We will then consider the concept of stigmergy and how construction can shape social systems by embedding information in the environment.
3. Finally, we will discuss the importance of explicitness in the perception of information and how this might help explain fundamental differences between constructions in humans and social insects.
(i). Cues, signals and biological information
In the behavioural sciences, information generated by an organism is traditionally separated into two categories: cues and signals [215,216,223]. Signals are any information transferring features that have evolved specifically to convey information about the signaller or its environment to receivers. It is generally understood as resulting from the coevolution of emitting and receiving apparatuses, as well as associated behavioural responses. Signals are also often—though not always—associated with the notion of intentionality, that is the organism controls when and where to broadcast the signal.
On the other hand, cues are features that can be used by an organism to guide its behaviour, but that were not evolved specifically to convey information between a signaller and receivers. Think, for instance, of a predator following the scent of a prey animal. The prey animal has not evolved its scent nor does it intentionally release it to inform the predator, yet the predator can evolve an apparatus to perceive the scent, as well as associated behavioural responses. If a cue provides an evolutionary advantage to the emitting organism (e.g. if it attracts potential mates), it can then be selected for and become a signal. However, while signals are intrinsically biological in nature (i.e. a product of evolution), cues can also be obtained from nonliving entities, like the position of the stars in the sky or the direction of the wind.
Cues and signals play an integral role in the construction behaviour of social insects. For instance, the construction behaviours of some ant and termite species have been shown to depend on environmental cues such as the strength and direction of air currents or the presence of physical heterogeneities in the landscape (see, for instance, Jost et al. [224]). These cues can influence both the initiation of the construction process (e.g. environmental heterogeneities serving as anchor points of constructions in ants, termites and wasps) [14,29,225] and the final result of the building activity (e.g. walls aligned along the direction of air currents in ants and termites) [224]. Signals, on the other hand, are more often associated with coordinating the actions of the individuals in the colony. For instance, the addition of pheromones to the construction material in ants and termites has arguably evolved to encourage individuals to add to structures built by nest-mates rather than to random environmental heterogeneities [14]. It could also represent the freshness of the material, therefore indicating structures under construction requiring additional actions by workers.
Similarly, environmental and contextual cues are fundamental factors influencing the building and formation of human constructions. Vernacular architecture perhaps best illustrates how determinants such as climate, availability of local construction materials and the influence of local traditions have informed the design of human constructions. One of the most significant determinants is the climate (see §3b(i)). Buildings in cold climates typically have few openings, windows are small or non-existent to prevent heat loss, and have high thermal mass or significant amounts of insulation. Conversely, buildings in warm climates tend to be constructed of light materials to allow cross-ventilation through openings in the fabric of the building. The different aspects of human behaviour and the environment have led to different building forms, evident in the variable contexts and cultures around the world [157,158,226]. Despite these variations, all buildings are subject to the same laws of physics and hence demonstrate significant similarities, which are evident also in social insect constructions: see §3a(i).
However, human constructions differ from that of insects in that they are also the product of socio-cultural factors that escape largely natural selection. As technology has advanced and human socio-culture has progressed with it, methods of construction have become more sophisticated and the form of buildings has evolved. Innovation and technological advancement allow architects to overcome constraints, such as those determining vernacular architecture. For example, the Gothic flying buttress was an innovation transferring gravitational forces to ground in a way that allowed walls to become lighter, which permitted greater expanses of glass and thereby daylight to flood an interior of buildings. Applied to churches and cathedrals this technique of building provided a means to denote divinity and promote the authority of the church. So, human construction is not only informed by environmental/contextual information—like in social insects—but also enables cultural signs to be embedded in the construction itself. These signs develop through a process typically referred to as ‘cultural evolution’ [227–231], whereby knowledge, beliefs, languages, etc., are passed on from generation to generation (inheritance), modified over time, and may enter in competition with each other, leading to selection pressures not unlike that underlying natural selection.
(ii). Stigmergy and spatial embedding of information
The notion discussed above that construction—whether by humans or insects—embeds information (or in other words, that it can influence future actions of the builders or the users) is reminiscent of the concept of stigmergy in biology. This idea was first introduced by Pierre-Paul Grassé in 1959 to describe the construction behaviour of termites [186,232]. Grassé explains that the organization of the building activity does not depend on direct coordination between the workers, but rather on indirect coordination achieved through the modification of the structure under construction. Each time a termite worker adds or removes material from the structure, it changes the configuration of the local environment around it. This change will influence subsequent building activities at or around its location, either by the same worker or other workers in the colony. Coordination at the colony level emerges from the repetition of such stigmergic processes, giving the impression that the colony is following some sort of well-defined plan.
As Grassé's original insight, stigmergic coordination has been found to play a role in most constructions built by social insects. For instance, the primitively eusocial wasp Polistes builds its nest out of paper it produces by mixing its saliva with plant fibres [26]. This paper is then turned into walls that will ultimately form a comb of hexagonal cells. During the building of the comb, cells are not added randomly to the structure under construction: wasps are more likely to add new cells where existing cells already form three or more adjacent walls [13,233]. As a consequence of this preference, multiple wasps can coordinate their building activity and will first complete existing rows of cells in the comb before starting a new one. The result of this indirect coordination is a round-shaped comb with approximately 150 cells and, more importantly, without holes. Other examples of social insect construction relying on stigmergic coordination include internal and external structures of nests in ants and honeybees [14,163], trail networks in ants and termites [234–236] and cemeteries and refuse piles in ants [113,224].
While it can be argued that stigmergy is a dominant organizational force in social insects' construction, they also rely on other modes of coordination during building. In particular, environmental and social templates play an important role—often in combination with stigmergy—in determining the final shape of the construction [13,51]. For instance, Macrotermes termites adjust the size of their queen's chamber to match her size as she grows [237,238]. Similarly, rock ants (Temnothorax albipennis) adjust the size of their nest to the quantity of their brood [239–241]. In both cases, it is believed that volatile pheromones produced by the queen and the brood establish a chemical gradient around them that can be used as a template by the workers to determine the size of the construction. Environmental heterogeneities and gradients can also be used as templates by social insects, determining for instance the location at which a construction is initiated or its final orientation. Finally, social insects can use direct coordination to organize their building activity. This is the case, for instance, for the self-assemblages built by some species of ants (e.g. temporary nests, bridges and ladders) and bees (e.g. swarms and festoons) by attaching to each other [12,37,38,47,49]. While limited to a few species, these—quite literally—living architectures built through direct coordination have the advantage over stigmergic structures of being extremely plastic and reactive, sometimes assembling and disassembling in a matter of minutes or even seconds.
As a concept to describe the coordinated building activity of social insects, the concept of stigmergy does not, on the first inspection, easily transfer to human society and its architecture. However, Grassé's idea of stigmergy can be extended to encompass all forms of cues and signals that organisms—including humans—leave in their environment that have the potential of mediating indirect interactions between individuals [51,186,187]. Stigmergic traces represent the information that organisms embed in the spatial context and, together with environmental influences, they define a large part of the information landscape accessible to each organism.
In the social sciences, Grasse's original insight has been studied in the context of numerous forms of human activity, including the stock market, economics, traffic patterns, urban development and more besides [242–245]. One may claim even that the way architects design traditionally, through drawing sketches, is stigmergic, whereby a line drawn on the page breaks the homogeneity of the blank surface, and influences scribing the next line. Successive lines are added influenced by and influencing the developing pattern to mediate the development of an idea. Working in a team, the same sketch is referred to and developed by others who are influenced by what they see and add to, adapt or emphasize aspects of the sketch. Building Information Modelling uses a stored digital model, which is accessible to all members of a design team, who work on and develop the model in parallel, detecting clashes and developing the model collectively. For an explanation see the National Building Specification (NBS) at https://www.thenbs.com/knowledge/what-is-building-information-modelling-bim. Recently architects have begun investigating stigmergy as a mechanism of coordinating design and construction [245] and experimenting with stigmergy as a method of generating form [246–248,249] and organising activities [250, 251].
As mentioned earlier (see §3b(ii)), the capacity to use the computer to simulate the autonomy, emergence and distributed functioning of natural systems provides architects with a new way of producing form and structure, and to think about the organization of areas constituting a building or city. Adjacency and circulation are fundamental concerns in organizing architectural layouts, because of factors like the movement of people, material and information between areas, and/or the need to control or supervise one area from another. The nature of such problems has been characterized as ‘wicked’ [184] because of the interrelatedness of the factors involved. The food foraging behaviour of ants, for example, has been explored as an alternative method of organizing distribution networks in buildings and cities. Instead of placing activity areas in relation to one another based on convention, the stigmergic behaviour of assorted artificial ant colonies has been used as a method of self-aggregation, and applied to generating the desired arrangements between activities in a building [252], and to generate primitive room arrangements [250]. Puusepp proposed a model whereby circulation is developed as an emergent by-product of global morphogenesis of the built form [253], and proposed a tool for generating outline urban arrangements often associated with unplanned settlements [254]. The stigmergic behaviour evident in insect societies and animals has also been adopted as a method of form finding [247,249,255]. Carranza and Coates, for example, used the trails left behind by a population of swarming agents as a scaffold to wrap a continuous surface around [247].
While stigmergy has been applied as an alternative approach to organizing buildings and form finding, the casual form of urban aggregation evident in medieval villages, Brazillian favelas and Chinese Hutongs exemplifies stigmergic configuration driven by environmental constraints, as with vernacular architecture, but urban aggregation of this type is also driven by associations with one's neighbour. While cities are prone to top–down planning by the authorities, they have been shown to operate as a dynamic, adaptive system based on interactions with neighbours, feedback and decentralized distribution of people, goods, information and energy [70,256,257]. Consequently, urban growth has been evaluated computationally and illustrated to replicate natural systems [66,258]. Coates demonstrated how the formation of early human settlements is underpinned by geometrical constraints that inform the arrangement of unplanned as well as planned urban arrangements through a combination of environmental feedback and simple local rules [259]. The algorithmic approach driving contemporary architectural design today is motivated by this comprehension of geometrical rules and stigmergic behaviour of agent-systems evident in shaping urban settlements and the configuration of buildings. Coupled with the capacity of social insect societies to unscramble the wickedness of certain problems (like searching for food), architects are today looking to the decentralized and distributed control evident in the behaviour of social insects and how they form the structures they build [13,51,189].
(iii). Explicit and implicit information
In the previous two sections, we discussed information from the point of view of the signaller: signals and cues are categorized based on whether the signaller has evolved them specifically to convey information about itself or its environment—or not (§3c(i)); and stigmergic traces are characterized by whether they persist in the environment even in the absence of the signaller (§3c(ii)). In this section, we would like to shift the focus toward the receiver of the information. In particular, we would like to argue that information can influence the behaviour of the receiver in either an explicit manner, or in an implicit one. We consider information as being explicit if the receiver has evolved—through natural or cultural evolution—perceptual and/or cognitive abilities to specifically give a meaning to this information. In other words, the organism has acquired dedicated processes to operate on the content of a piece of information (e.g. neural pathways) and react to it accordingly. This corresponds to all forms of information for which the organisms possess a receptor and mechanisms to interpret the output of the receptor.
Implicit information, on the other hand, corresponds to features that can modify the behaviour of an organism without requiring this organism to process or even perceive the associated stimuli. In other words, they are features of the physical and social environment that do not have a meaning for the organism—the organism might not even be able to perceive them—yet they may influence its actions in a manner that the organism cannot control. These are often external physical forces applied on the organism without its knowledge (e.g. the tide pushing planktonic organisms toward the shore) [260] or barriers that constrain the movement of the organism. In some species of ants, for instance, it was found that the geometry of their networks of foraging trails is asymmetrical: when a forager comes back towards its nest and reaches a branching point, the trail heading towards the nest after the branching point deviates less (approx. 30°) from the ant's original direction than the other trail (approx. 120°) that leads away from the nest [17,203,204,206,261]. While one species of ant may be able to use this information explicitly to navigate its trail network [204], others do not seem to perceive the difference and simply follow the path of ‘least resistance’ [91,203]. As a result, they are more likely to find their way back to the nest and their foraging output will be increased up to three times, all of this without requiring any navigational capabilities, spatial awareness or even the ability to detect the configuration of the branching point (as demonstrated using robots) [205].
Most studies on the building behaviour and construction use of social insects involve characterizing explicit forms of information: pheromone deposits, tactile contacts, air movements, etc. [14,224,262]. Few, however, have considered the importance of implicit information in shaping the collective behaviour of the colony. Indeed, one difficulty with studying implicit information is that it is not always obvious to an external observer given the disconnection between this form of information and the sensory and cognitive apparatus of the organism. Yet, as in the example mentioned above, there is strong evidence that the topology and geometrical organization of the environment have an influence on the spatial distribution of organisms, even when they are imperceptible to said organisms. Therefore, it should be explored more systematically in the context of social insect constructions.
Similarly, we can see examples of information that is embedded within the human-built environment, and in architectural form, and how it too can have an influence on the behaviour of the perceiver. Again, this impact may be described as implicit or explicit. Winston Churchill's adage ‘we shape our buildings; thereafter they shape us' exemplifies the built environment as a chief factor in determining behaviour. The correlation between perception of the environment and its implicit effects on well-being and behaviour has long interested psychologists [263]. The complexity of the built environment is a crucial factor contributing to human behaviour. Experiments measuring how the brain and body respond to different kinds of settings show people are bored and unhappy when faced with extensive bland facades, and by contrast, happy and stimulated by varied and permeable building frontages, which will in turn have an influence on where a person will choose to spend their time [264,265].
Quantitative theories and methods of analysing urban configurations, such as Space Syntax [266], illustrate the correlation between the geometrical composition of the built environment and social behaviour [103,104]. Graph-based representations and statistical analysis of the structural properties of built form illustrate that there is a direct correlation between the topology and geometrical organization of the environment and the spatial distribution of people and movement [267–269]. For example, the least angular deviation along a route suggests the structure of the street network is itself the key determinant of pedestrian flow. A pedestrian will tend to choose routes that require the least amount of turns, and this will correlate to their perception of how well integrated the street is within a network, and consequently to pedestrian density. The implication is that configuration can have effects on movement that are independent of attractors [270,271].
The role of explicit information in the built environment is both more literal and more formalized. Road signs and the demarcation of pathways are obvious examples. In extreme cases, the function of the building is literally interpreted by the observer, such as ‘Big Duck’: a shop selling ducks and duck eggs that is built in the shape of a duck. However, a particular aspect that distinguishes the human use of information is our capacity to build arbitrary associations between things and to think metaphorically. Symbolism enables humans to communicate with other humans they do not meet: i.e. symbols are an indirect form of communication, which are embedded and perceived throughout the built environment and have developed their associations (or meanings) through cultural evolution. A structure is symbolic when it acts as a vehicle of arbitrary content and the observer reads the embedded meaning, making architecture ‘other than’ just a building, as discussed in §3a(iii).
4. Conclusion
Humans have long since looked on the natural world as a source of inspiration, and observation of what other animals can do has driven us to achieve feats beyond our natural capabilities; such as being able to fly. The idea of late that simple creatures build complex and dynamic constructions has spurred researchers to investigate the mechanisms behind such phenomena, from the building of social insects' nests to the formation of cells, tissues, organs and ultimately organisms. The complex and coordinated behaviours resulting from interactions between individuals in a collective has led scientists and engineers to question how this understanding may be applied to human-related problems. Architects, on the other hand, who are becoming more aware of the parallels between biological processes and design, as well as the artefact-making capacities of animals, are turning more to biology to explore innovative methods of problem-solving and designing.
While there is a long history of biology influencing architectural endeavour, only recently have biologists and architects begun to meet and collaborate. As indicated at the start of this paper, this union brings inherent difficulties as each discipline claims its own high ground and concepts fundamental to both are viewed distinctly from either side—perhaps none more so than the concepts of ‘architecture’, ‘space’ and ‘information’, which are not only fundamental to the sciences and humanities but to everyday understanding. Consequently, we set out in this review to cross-examine these concepts in biology and architecture and to establish a framework within which fundamentals that span both disciplines are apparent and beneficial to both, with the view to better enabling cooperation in the study of constructions built by social organisms and how these structures influence, direct and manage behaviour of social systems.
The primitive framework established here provides a basis on which to build. Having examined the notion of architecture, we have proposed an open definition spanning human and non-human constructs and reviewed the concepts of ‘space’ and ‘information’ in relation to human and social insect constructions. Additional concepts, such as ‘emotion’, may be scrutinized and included to facilitate and bolster interdisciplinary discourse. The notion of delight is perhaps beyond scientific reason, but aesthetics (if we refer to Baumgarten [121,122]) may be considered a fundamental aspect of all living systems. The key, we suggest, is to analyse the occurrence of internal–external relations established by perceptual systems in the process of distinguishing information about their world. The real issue is to avoid anthropomorphizing the social insect and consider how the insect's perceptual system conveys information about its world. In so doing, we should avoid seeking the meaning and establish the internal–external relations that inform, direct and lead to, for example, the termites' pillar building activity. Living systems are embedded in their environment, which, we have proposed, from the organism's perspective, is a matter of relations and forms that influence behaviour. These features, which may be evolved (signals) or not (cues), perceptible (explicit) or otherwise (implicit), constitute environmental pressures that constrain and coerce the activity of organisms. Spatial constraints are a fundamental feature of living systems, both in their development and in their unfolding engagement with the world [272,273]. Evident, for example, in the building of self-ventilating mounds in termites, the rules that govern construction can be seen as productive constraints because they are sensed by the organism that responds to it, giving it a meaning, and ultimately creating a functional pattern (the mound and its passive ventilation) that improves the colony's fitness. It is a fundamental character of natural systems that spans scales from abiotic to social systems. This semiotic perspective unifies architecture and biology and, we hope, could be the basis for a more formal collaborative language between the two disciplines.
Acknowledgements
The authors thank Guy Theraulaz and Noa Pinter-Wollman for their input and feedback on various versions of this paper. We also acknowledge the anonymous reviewers for their comments and suggestions, which have helped to significantly improve the content of this paper. The workshop ‘The effects of architecture on collective behaviour’ organized by Steve Fiore, Guy Theraulaz and Noa Pinter-Wollman that inspired this paper, was fundamental to the development of ideas presented here and to the later collaboration between the authors.
Data accessibility
This article has no additional data.
Competing interests
We declare we have no competing interests.
Funding
We thank the National Academies Keck Futures Initiative for funding the workshop.
References
- 1.Kushner DJ. 1969. Self-assembly of biological structures. Bacteriol. Rev. 33, 302–345. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Whitesides GM, Grzybowski B. 2002. Self-assembly at all scales. Science 295, 2418–2421. ( 10.1126/science.1070821) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Smeets B, Alert R, Pešek J, Pagonabarraga I, Ramon H, Vincent R. 2016. Emergent structures and dynamics of cell colonies by contact inhibition of locomotion. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 113, 14 621–14 626. ( 10.1073/pnas.1521151113) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Friedl P, Gilmour D. 2009. Collective cell migration in morphogenesis, regeneration and cancer. Nat. Rev. Mol. Cell Biol. 10, 445–457. ( 10.1038/nrm2720) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Méhes E, Vicsek T. 2014. Collective motion of cells: from experiments to models. Integr. Biol. 6, 831–854. ( 10.1039/C4IB00115J) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Abercrombie M, Heaysman JEM. 1954. Observations on the social behaviour of cells in tissue culture. Exp. Cell Res. 6, 293–306. ( 10.1016/0014-4827(54)90176-7) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Abercrombie M, Heaysman JE. 1953. Observations on the social behaviour of cells in tissue culture. I. Speed of movement of chick heart fibroblasts in relation to their mutual contacts. Exp. Cell Res. 5, 111–131. ( 10.1016/0014-4827(53)90098-6) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Gonzalez-Rodriguez D, Guevorkian K, Douezan S, Brochard-Wyart F. 2012. Soft matter models of developing tissues and tumors. Science 338, 910–917. ( 10.1126/science.1226418) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Hansell MH. 1984. Animal architecture and building behaviour. London, UK; New York, NY: Longman. [Google Scholar]
- 10.Hansell MH. 2005. Animal architecture. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- 11.Hansell M. 2007. Built by animals: the natural history of animal architecture. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- 12.Anderson C, Theraulaz G, Deneubourg J-L. 2002. Self-assemblages in insect societies. Insectes Soc. 49, 99–110. ( 10.1007/s00040-002-8286-y) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Camazine S, Deneubourg J-L, Franks NR, Sneyd J, Theraulaz G, Bonabeau E. 2001. Self-Organization in biological systems. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. [Google Scholar]
- 14.Khuong A, Gautrais J, Perna A, Sbaï C, Combe M, Kuntz P, Jost C, Theraulaz G. 2016. Stigmergic construction and topochemical information shape ant nest architecture. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 113, 1303–1308. ( 10.1073/pnas.1509829113) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Tschinkel WR. 2004. The nest architecture of the Florida harvester ant, Pogonomyrmex badius. J. Insect Sci. 4, 21 ( 10.1093/jis/4.1.21) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Tschinkel WR. 2015. The architecture of subterranean ant nests: beauty and mystery underfoot. J. Bioecon. 17, 271–291. ( 10.1007/s10818-015-9203-6) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Buhl J, Hicks K, Miller ERER, Persey S, Alinvi O, Sumpter DJT. 2008. Shape and efficiency of wood ant foraging networks. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 63, 451–460. ( 10.1007/s00265-008-0680-7) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Ellis S, Franks DW, Robinson EJH. 2014. Resource redistribution in polydomous ant nest networks: local or global? Behav. Ecol. 25, 1–9. ( 10.1093/beheco/aru108) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Farji-Brener AG, Chinchilla F, Umaña MN, Ocasio-Torres ME, Chauta-Mellizo A, Acosta-Rojas D, Marinaro S, Curth MdeT, Amador-Vargas S. 2015. Branching angles reflect a trade-off between reducing trail maintenance costs or travel distances in leaf-cutting ants. Ecology 96, 510–517. ( 10.1890/14-0220.1) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Forti LC, Moreira AA, Camargo RdaS, Caldato N, Castellani MA. 2017. Nest architecture development of grass-cutting ants. Rev. Bras. Entomol. 62, 46–50. ( 10.1016/j.rbe.2017.10.002) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Gravish N, Garcia M, Mazouchova N, Levy L, Umbanhowar PB, Goodisman MAD, Goldman DI. 2012. Effects of worker size on the dynamics of fire ant tunnel construction. J. R. Soc. Interface 9, 3312–3322. ( 10.1098/rsif.2012.0423) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Buhl J, Gautrais J, Deneubourg J-L, Theraulaz G. 2004. Nest excavation in ants: group size effects on the size and structure of tunneling networks. Naturwissenschaften 91, 602–606. ( 10.1007/s00114-004-0577-x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Buhl J, Gautrais J, Louis Deneubourg J, Kuntz P, Theraulaz G. 2006. The growth and form of tunnelling networks in ants. J. Theor. Biol. 243, 287–298. ( 10.1016/j.jtbi.2006.06.018) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Minter NJ, Franks NR, Brown KAR. 2012. Morphogenesis of an extended phenotype: four-dimensional ant nest architecture. J. R. Soc. Interface 9, 586–595. ( 10.1098/rsif.2011.0377) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Toffin E, Kindekens J, Deneubourg J-L. 2010. Excavated substrate modulates growth instability during nest building in ants. Proc. R. Soc. B 277, 2617–2625. ( 10.1098/rspb.2010.0176) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Cole MR, Hansell MH, Seath CJ. 2001. A quantitative study of the physical properties of nest paper in three species of Vespine wasps (Hymenoptera, Vespidae). Insectes Soc. 48, 33–39. ( 10.1007/PL00001742) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Jeanne RL. 1975. The adaptiveness of social wasp nest architecture. Q. Rev. Biol. 50, 267–287. ( 10.1086/408564) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Smith AP. 1978. An investigation of the mechanisms underlying nest construction in the mud wasp Paralastor sp. (Hymenoptera: Eumenidae). Anim. Behav. 26(Part 1), 232–240. ( 10.1016/0003-3472(78)90023-4) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Karsai I, Theraulaz G. 1995. Nest building in a social wasp: postures and constraints. Sociobiology 26, 83–114. [Google Scholar]
- 30.Seeley TD, Morse RA. 1976. The nest of the honey bee (Apis mellifera L.). Insectes Soc. 23, 495–512. ( 10.1007/BF02223477) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Pratt SC. 1998. Condition-dependent timing of comb construction by honeybee colonies: how do workers know when to start building? Anim. Behav. 56, 603–610. ( 10.1006/anbe.1998.0811) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Oberst S, Lai JCS, Evans TA. 2016. Termites utilise clay to build structural supports and so increase foraging resources. Sci. Rep. 6, 20990 ( 10.1038/srep20990) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Green B, Bardunias P, Turner JS, Nagpal R, Werfel J. 2017. Excavation and aggregation as organizing factors in de novo construction by mound-building termites. Proc. R. Soc. B 284, 20162730 ( 10.1098/rspb.2016.2730) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Zachariah N, Das A, Murthy TG, Borges RM. 2017. Building mud castles: a perspective from brick-laying termites. Sci. Rep. 7, 4692 ( 10.1038/s41598-017-04295-3) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Cosarinsky MI, Roces F. 2011. The construction of turrets for nest ventilation in the grass-cutting ant Atta vollenweideri: import and assembly of building materials. J. Insect Behav. 25, 222–241. ( 10.1007/s10905-011-9290-8) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Graham JM, Kao AB, Wilhelm DA, Garnier S. 2017. Optimal construction of army ant living bridges. J. Theor. Biol. 435, 184–198. ( 10.1016/j.jtbi.2017.09.017) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Reid CR, Lutz MJ, Powell S, Kao AB, Couzin ID, Garnier S. 2015. Army ants dynamically adjust living bridges in response to a cost–benefit trade-off. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 112, 15 113–15 118. ( 10.1073/pnas.1512241112) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Garnier S, Murphy T, Lutz M, Hurme E, Leblanc S, Couzin ID. 2013. Stability and responsiveness in a self-organized living architecture. PLoS Comput. Biol. 9, e1002984 ( 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1002984) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Baudier KM, O'Donnell S. 2016. Structure and thermal biology of subterranean army ant bivouacs in tropical montane forests. Insectes Soc. 63, 467–476. ( 10.1007/s00040-016-0490-2) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Schneirla TC, Brown RZ, Brown FC. 1954. The bivouac or temporary nest as an adaptive factor in certain terrestrial species of army ants. Ecol. Monogr. 24, 269–296. ( 10.2307/1948466) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Schneirla TC. 1972. Army ants: A study in social organization. San Francisco, CA: W. H. Freeman & Co Ltd. [Google Scholar]
- 42.Rettenmeyer CW. 1963. Behavioral studies of army ants. Univer. Kansas Sci. Bull. 44, 281–465. [Google Scholar]
- 43.Lioni A, Deneubourg JL. 2004. Collective decision through self-assembling. Naturwissenschaften 91, 237–241. ( 10.1007/s00114-004-0519-7) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Peeters C, De Greef S. 2015. Predation on large millipedes and self-assembling chains in Leptogenys ants from Cambodia. Insectes Soc. 62, 471–477. ( 10.1007/s00040-015-0426-2) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Lioni A, Sauwens C, Theraulaz G, Deneubourg J-L. 2001. Chain formation in Oecophylla longinoda. J. Insect Behav. 14, 679–696. ( 10.1023/A:1012283403138) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Theraulaz G, Bonabeau E, Sauwens C, Deneubourg J-L, Lioni A, Libert F, Passera L, Solé R. 2001. Model of droplet dynamics in the Argentine ant Linepithema Humile (Mayr). Bull. Math. Biol. 63, 1079–1093. ( 10.1006/bulm.2001.0260) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Phonekeo S, Mlot N, Monaenkova D, Hu DL, Tovey C. 2017. Fire ants perpetually rebuild sinking towers. R. Soc. open sci. 4, 170475 ( 10.1098/rsos.170475) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Mlot NJ, Tovey C, Hu DL. 2012. Dynamics and shape of large fire ant rafts. Commun. Integr. Biol. 5, 590–597. ( 10.4161/cib.21421) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Mlot NJ, Tovey CA, Hu DL. 2011. Fire ants self-assemble into waterproof rafts to survive floods. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 108, 7669–7673. ( 10.1073/pnas.1016658108) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Ben-Alon L, Sacks R, Grobman YJ. 2014. Similarities and differences between humans' and social insects' building processes and building behaviours. In Proceedings of the 2014 Construction Research Congress, Atlanta, GA, USA, 19–21 May 2014, pp. 51–60. Reston, VA: American Society of Civil Engineers; ( 10.1061/9780784413517.006) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Garnier S, Gautrais J, Theraulaz G. 2007. The biological principles of swarm intelligence. Swarm Intell. 1, 3–31. ( 10.1007/s11721-007-0004-y) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Detrain C, Deneubourg J-L. 2006. Self-organized structures in a superorganism: do ants ‘behave’ like molecules? Phys. Life Rev. 3, 162–187. ( 10.1016/j.plrev.2006.07.001) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Perna A, Theraulaz G. 2017. When social behaviour is moulded in clay: on growth and form of social insect nests. J. Exp. Biol. 220, 83–91. ( 10.1242/jeb.143347) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Korb J, Linsenmair KE. 1998. The effects of temperature on the architecture and distribution of Macrotermes bellicosus (Isoptera, Macrotermitinae) mounds in different habitats of a West African Guinea savanna. Insectes Soc. 45, 51–65. ( 10.1007/s000400050068) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Holbrook CT, Clark RM, Moore D, Overson RP, Penick CA, Smith AA. 2010. Social insects inspire human design. Biol. Lett. 6, 431–433. ( 10.1098/rsbl.2010.0270) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Ellingsen E.2017. Designing buildings, using biology. The Scientist. See https://www.the-scientist.com/?articles.view/articleNo/25290/title/Designing-buildings-using-biology/ (accessed on 23 December 2017).
- 57.Sabin JE, Jones PL. 2017. Labstudio: design research between architecture and biology. Abingdon, UK: Routledge. [Google Scholar]
- 58.Agkathidis A. 2017. Biomorphic structures: architecture inspired by nature (form+technique). 2nd Revised edn London, UK: Laurence King Publishing. [Google Scholar]
- 59.Imhof B, Gruber P (eds) 2015. Built to grow—blending architecture and biology (edition angewandte). Basel, Switzerland: Birkhauser. [Google Scholar]
- 60.Ball P. 2010. For sustainable architecture, think bug. New Sci. 205, 35–37. ( 10.1016/S0262-4079(10)60423-X) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Mertins D. 2007. Where architecture meets biology: an interview with Detlef Mertins. In Interact or Die! (eds Brouwer J, Mulder A), pp. 110–131. Rotterdam, Holland: V2 Publishing. [Google Scholar]
- 62.Steadman P. 1983. Architectural morphology: an introduction to the geometry of building plans. London, UK: Pion Ltd. [Google Scholar]
- 63.Batty M. 1997. Predicting where we walk. Nature 388, 19–20. ( 10.1038/40266) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Decraene J, Monterola C, Lee GKK, Hung TGG, Batty M. 2013. The emergence of urban land use patterns driven by dispersion and aggregation mechanisms. PLoS ONE 8, e80309 ( 10.1371/journal.pone.0080309) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Haase K, Al Abideen HZ, Al-Bosta S, Kasper M, Koch M, Müller S, Helbing D. 2016. Improving pilgrim safety during the hajj: an analytical and operational research approach. Interfaces 46, 74–90. ( 10.1287/inte.2015.0833) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Coates P. 2010. Programming.architecture. Abingdon, UK: Routledge. [Google Scholar]
- 67.Frazer J. 1995. An evolutionary architecture. London, UK: Architectural Association. [Google Scholar]
- 68.Spuybroek L. 2009. Research and design: the architecture of variation. London, UK: Thames & Hudson. [Google Scholar]
- 69.Derix C, Izaki A. 2014. Empathic space: the computation of human-centric architecture. New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons. [Google Scholar]
- 70.Batty M. 2013. The new science of cities. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
- 71.Barthelemy M. 2018. Morphogenesis of spatial networks. Berlin, Germany: Springer International Publishing. [Google Scholar]
- 72.Soar R. 2016. Part 2: Pushing the envelope. A process perspective for architecture, engineering and construction. Intell. Build. Int. 8, 90–105. ( 10.1080/17508975.2015.1014461) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Theraulaz G, Solé RV, Kuntz P (eds) 2009. From insect nest to human architecture: Proc. of the int. workshop engineering principles of innovation in swarm-made architectures. See https://market.android.com/details?id=book-7j2VoAEACAAJ.
- 74.Holl S.2012. Steven Holl receives AIA gold medal with ceremony in Washington DC. STEVEN HOLL ARCHITECTS. See http://www.stevenholl.com/press/99. (accessed on 24 December 2017)
- 75.Mayne T.2005. Acceptance speech: Thom Mayne. The Pritzker Architecture Prize. See http://www.pritzkerprize.com/2005/ceremony_speech1. (accessed on 24 December 2017).
- 76.Mockbee S.2007. His impact on architecture. The official website of Samuel Mockbee. See http://samuelmockbee.net/impact/architecture/ (accessed on 24 December 2017).
- 77.Hales L. 2005. A lesson in simple but edifying architecture. The Washington Post, 29 January. See http://www.washingtonpost.com/wp-dyn/articles/A46072-2005Jan28.html.
- 78.Pritzker JA.1985. Ceremony speech: Hans Hollein. The Pritzker Architecture Prize. See http://www.pritzkerprize.com/1985/ceremony_speech2. (accessed on 24 December 2017).
- 79.Vitruvius. 2001. Vitruvius: ‘Ten books on architecture’. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- 80.Chappell E. 2015. Autodesk InfraWorks 360 and autodesk InfraWorks 360 LT essentials. New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons. [Google Scholar]
- 81.Ocko SA, King H, Andreen D, Bardunias P, Turner JS, Soar R, Mahadevan L. 2017. Solar-powered ventilation of African termite mounds. J. Exp. Biol. 220, 3260–3269. ( 10.1242/jeb.160895) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.King H, Ocko S, Mahadevan L. 2015. Termite mounds harness diurnal temperature oscillations for ventilation. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 112, 11 589–11 593. ( 10.1073/pnas.1423242112) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Olaoye GS, Anigbogu NA. 2000. Properties of compressed earth bricks stabilized with termite mound materials. Niger. J. Constr. Technol. Manage. 3, 150–156. [Google Scholar]
- 84.Udoeyo FF, Turman MY. 2002. Mound soil as a pavement material. Glob. J. Eng. Res. 1, 137–144. [Google Scholar]
- 85.Webb R. 1994. Offices that breathe naturally. New Sci. 142, 38–41. [Google Scholar]
- 86.Atkinson J. 1995. Emulating the termite. Zimbabwean Rev. 1, 16–19. [Google Scholar]
- 87.Bode NWF, Kemloh Wagoum AU, Codling EA. 2014. Human responses to multiple sources of directional information in virtual crowd evacuations. J. R. Soc. Interface 11, 20130904 ( 10.1098/rsif.2013.0904) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Park CW, Iyer ES, Smith DC. 1989. The effects of situational factors on in-store grocery shopping behavior: the role of store environment and time available for shopping. J. Consum. Res. 15, 422–433. ( 10.1086/209182) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Cao TT. 2013. High social density increases foraging and scouting rates and induces polydomy in Temnothorax ants. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 67, 1799–1807. ( 10.1007/s00265-013-1587-5) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Perna A, Jost C, Couturier E, Valverde S, Douady S, Theraulaz G. 2008. The structure of gallery networks in the nests of termite Cubitermes spp. revealed by X-ray tomography. Naturwissenschaften 95, 877–884. ( 10.1007/s00114-008-0388-6) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91.Garnier S, Guérécheau A, Combe M, Fourcassié V, Theraulaz G. 2009. Path selection and foraging efficiency in Argentine ant transport networks. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 63, 1167–1179. ( 10.1007/s00265-009-0741-6) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 92.Wang S, Cao S, Wang Q, Lian L, Song W. 2016. Effect of exit locations on ants escaping a two-exit room stressed with repellent. Physica A 457, 239–254. ( 10.1016/j.physa.2016.03.083) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 93.Soria SA, Josens R, Parisi DR. 2012. Experimental evidence of the ‘Faster is Slower’ effect in the evacuation of ants. Saf. Sci. 50, 1584–1588. ( 10.1016/j.ssci.2012.03.010) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 94.Shahhoseini Z, Sarvi M, Saberi M. 2018. Pedestrian crowd dynamics in merging sections: revisiting the ‘faster-is-slower’ phenomenon. Physica A 491, 101–111. ( 10.1016/j.physa.2017.09.003) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 95.von Krüchten C, Schadschneider A. 2017. Empirical study on social groups in pedestrian evacuation dynamics. Physica A 475, 129–141. ( 10.1016/j.physa.2017.02.004) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 96.Heliövaara S, Kuusinen J-M, Rinne T, Korhonen T, Ehtamo H. 2012. Pedestrian behavior and exit selection in evacuation of a corridor – an experimental study. Saf. Sci. 50, 221–227. ( 10.1016/j.ssci.2011.08.020) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 97.Buhl J, Gautrais J, Reeves N, Solé RV, Valverde S, Kuntz P, Theraulaz G. 2006. Topological patterns in street networks of self-organized urban settlements. Eur. Phys. J. B 49, 513–522. [Google Scholar]
- 98.Frank H, Kahn RE, Kleinrock L. 1972. Computer communication network design: experience with theory and practice. In Proc. of the May 16–18, 1972, Spring Joint Computer Conf., Atlantic City, New Jersey, May 16–18, 1972, pp. 255–270. New York, NY: ACM. [Google Scholar]
- 99.Yazdani A, Otoo RA, Jeffrey P. 2011. Resilience enhancing expansion strategies for water distribution systems: a network theory approach. Environ. Model. Softw. 26, 1574–1582. ( 10.1016/j.envsoft.2011.07.016) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 100.Tzatchkov VG, Alcocer-Yamanaka VH, Bourguett Ortíz V. 2008. Graph theory based algorithms for water distribution network sectorization projects. In Proc. of the eighth annual water distribution systems analysis symposium (WDSA), 27–30 August 2006, Cincinnati, OH, USA (eds Buchberger SG, Clark RM, Grayman WM, Uber JG). ( 10.1061/40941(247)172) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 101.Ran B, Boyce D. 2012. Dynamic urban transportation network models: theory and implications for intelligent vehicle-highway systems. Berlin, Germany: Springer Science & Business Media. [Google Scholar]
- 102.March L, Steadman P. 1974. The geometry of environment: an introduction to spatial organization in design. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
- 103.Hillier B, Hanson J. 1989. The social logic of space. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- 104.Hillier B. 1996. Space is the machine: a configurational theory of architecture. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- 105.Pallasmaa J. 2012. The eyes of the skin: architecture and the senses. New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons. [Google Scholar]
- 106.Cairns G. 2017. Visioning technologies: the architectures of sight. London, UK: Taylor & Francis. [Google Scholar]
- 107.Zumthor P, Oberli-Turner M, Schelbert C, Binet H. 2006. Thinking architecture. Boston, MA: Birkhäuser. [Google Scholar]
- 108.Rasmussen SE. 1964. Experiencing architecture. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
- 109.Kiesler FJ. 1996. The electric switch or the switch to process architecture. In Frederick J. Kiesler. Selected writings (eds Gohr S, Luyken G), pp. 110–118. Ostifildern bei Stuttgart: Verlag Gerd Hatje. [Google Scholar]
- 110.Gibson JJ. 1966. The senses considered as perceptual systems. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin. [Google Scholar]
- 111.Czaczkes TJ, Heinze J, Ruther J. 2015. Nest etiquette—where ants go when nature calls. PLoS ONE 10, e0118376 ( 10.1371/journal.pone.0118376) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 112.Heyman Y, Shental N, Brandis A, Hefetz A, Feinerman O. 2017. Ants regulate colony spatial organization using multiple chemical road-signs. Nat. Commun. 8, 15414 ( 10.1038/ncomms15414) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 113.Farji-Brener AG, Elizalde L, Fernández-Marín H, Amador-Vargas S. 2016. Social life and sanitary risks: evolutionary and current ecological conditions determine waste management in leaf-cutting ants. Proc. R. Soc. B 283, 20160625 ( 10.1098/rspb.2016.0625) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 114.Avarguès-Weber A, Dyer AG, Combe M, Giurfa M. 2012. Simultaneous mastering of two abstract concepts by the miniature brain of bees. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 109, 7481–7486. ( 10.1073/pnas.1202576109) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 115.Menzel R, Giurfa M. 2001. Cognitive architecture of a mini-brain: the honeybee. Trends Cogn. Sci. 5, 62–71. ( 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01601-6) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 116.Josens R, Eschbach C, Giurfa M. 2009. Differential conditioning and long-term olfactory memory in individual Camponotus fellah ants. J. Exp. Biol. 212, 1904–1911. ( 10.1242/jeb.030080) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 117.Giurfa M. 2013. Cognition with few neurons: higher-order learning in insects. Trends Neurosci. 36, 285–294. ( 10.1016/j.tins.2012.12.011) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 118.Avargues-Weber A, Giurfa M. 2013. Conceptual learning by miniature brains. Proc. R. Soc. B 280, 20131907 ( 10.1098/rspb.2013.1907) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 119.van Wilgenburg E. 2007. The influence of relatedness, neighbourhood and overall distance on colony mate recognition in a polydomous ant. Ethology 113, 1185–1191. ( 10.1111/j.1439-0310.2007.01431.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 120.Miller GF. 1998. How mate choice shaped human nature: a review of sexual selection and human evolution. In Handbook of evolutionary psychology: ideas, issues and application (eds Crawford CB, Krebs DL), pp. 87–129. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. [Google Scholar]
- 121.Gregor MJ. 1983. Baumgarten's ‘Aesthetica’. Rev. Metaphys. 104, 357–385. [Google Scholar]
- 122.Howes D, Classen C. 2013. Ways of sensing: understanding the senses in society. Abingdon, UK: Routledge. [Google Scholar]
- 123.Sendova-Franks AB, Franks NR. 1995. Spatial relationships within nests of the ant Leptothorax unifasciatus (Latr.) and their implications for the division of labour. Anim. Behav. 50, 121–136. ( 10.1006/anbe.1995.0226) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 124.Semper G, Herrmann W, Mallgrave HF, Rykwert J. 1989. The four elements of architecture and other writings. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- 125.Schwarzer MW, Schmarsow A. 1991. The emergence of architectural space: August Schmarsow's theory of ‘Raumgestaltung’. Assemblage 15, 49–61. [Google Scholar]
- 126.Vischer R. 1873. On the optical sense of form: a contribution to aesthetics. Empathy, form, and space: problems in German aesthetics 1893, 89–124. [Google Scholar]
- 127.van de Ven C. 1987. Space in architecture: the evolution of a new idea in the theory and history of the modern movements. Assen, The Netherlands: Van Gorcum. [Google Scholar]
- 128.Alberts B., et al. 2014. Molecular biology of the cell. New York, NY: Garland Science. [Google Scholar]
- 129.Guisan A, Thuiller W. 2005. Predicting species distribution: offering more than simple habitat models. Ecol. Lett. 8, 993–1009. ( 10.1111/j.1461-0248.2005.00792.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 130.Powell S. 2016. A comparative perspective on the ecology of morphological diversification in complex societies: nesting ecology and soldier evolution in the turtle ants. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 70, 1075–1085. ( 10.1007/s00265-016-2080-8) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 131.Matsuura K. 2002. Colony-level stabilization of soldier head width for head-plug defense in the termite Reticulitermes speratus (Isoptera: Rhinotermitidae). Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 51, 172–179. ( 10.1007/s00265-001-0426-2) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 132.Moreira AA, Forti LC, Andrade AP, Boaretto MAC, Lopes JFS. 2004. Nest architecture of Atta laevigata (F. Smith, 1858) (Hymenoptera: Formicidae). Stud. Neotrop. Fauna Environ. 39, 109–116. ( 10.1080/01650520412331333756) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 133.Jonkman J. 1980. The external and internal structure and growth of nests of the leaf-cutting ant Atta vollenweideri Forel, 1893 (Hym.: Formicidae). J. Appl. Entomol. 89, 217–246. [Google Scholar]
- 134.Whitehouse MEAA, Jaffe K. 1996. Ant wars: combat strategies, territory and nest defence in the leaf-cutting ant Atta laevigata. Anim. Behav. 51, 1207–1217. ( 10.1006/anbe.1996.0126) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 135.Noirot C, Darlington JPEC. 2000. Termite nests: architecture, regulation and defence. In Termites: evolution, sociality, symbioses, ecology (eds Abe T, Bignell DE, Higashi M), pp. 121–139. Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Springer. [Google Scholar]
- 136.Huang MH. 2010. Multi-phase defense by the big-headed ant, Pheidole obtusospinosa, against raiding army ants. J. Insect Sci. 10, 1 ( 10.1673/031.010.0101) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 137.Szabó-Patay J. 1928. A kapus-mangay. Termeszettudományi Közlöny 60, 215–219. [Google Scholar]
- 138.Darlington JPEC. 2009. The underground passages and storage pits used in foraging by a nest of the termite Macrotermes michaelseni in Kajiado, Kenya. J. Zool. 198, 237–247. ( 10.1111/j.1469-7998.1982.tb02073.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 139.Moreira AA, Forti LC, Boaretto MAC, Andrade APP, Lopes JFS, Ramos VM. 2004. External and internal structure of Atta bisphaerica Forel (Hymenoptera: Formicidae) nests. J. Appl. Entomol. 128, 204–211. ( 10.1111/j.1439-0418.2004.00839.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 140.Mintzer A. 1979. Foraging activity of the Mexican leafcutting ant Atta mexicana (F. Smith), in a sonoran desert habitat (Hymenoptera, Formicidae). Insectes Soc. 26, 364–372. ( 10.1007/BF02223555) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 141.Mizumoto N, Matsuura K. 2013. Colony-specific architecture of shelter tubes by termites. Insectes Soc. 60, 525–530. ( 10.1007/s00040-013-0319-1) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 142.Anderson C, McShea DW. 2001. Intermediate-level parts in insect societies: adaptive structures that ants build away from the nest. Insectes Soc. 48, 291–301. ( 10.1007/PL00001781) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 143.Weissflog A, Sternheim E, Dorow W, Berghoff S, Maschwitz U. 2000. How to study subterranean army ants: a novel method for locating and monitoring field populations of the South East Asian army ant Dorylus (Dichthadia) laevigatus Smith, 1857 (Formicidae, Dorylinae) with observations on their ecology. Insectes Soc. 47, 317–324. ( 10.1007/PL00001723) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 144.Jones JC, Oldroyd BP. 2006. Nest thermoregulation in social insects. In Advances in insect physiology (ed. SJ Simpson.), pp. 153–191. New York, NY: Academic Press. [Google Scholar]
- 145.Korb J. 2003. Thermoregulation and ventilation of termite mounds. Naturwissenschaften 90, 212–219. ( 10.1007/s00114-002-0401-4) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 146.Korb J, Linsenmair KE. 2000. Thermoregulation of termite mounds: what role does ambient temperature and metabolism of the colony play? Insectes Soc. 47, 357–363. ( 10.1007/pl00001731) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 147.Bollazzi M, Roces F. 2010. Control of nest water losses through building behavior in leaf-cutting ants (Acromyrmex heyeri). Insectes Soc. 57, 267–273. ( 10.1007/s00040-010-0081-6) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 148.Bollazzi M, Roces F. 2010. Leaf-cutting ant workers (Acromyrmex heyeri) trade off nest thermoregulation for humidity control. J. Ethol. 28, 399–403. ( 10.1007/s10164-010-0207-3) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 149.Kleineidam C, Ernst R, Roces F. 2001. Wind-induced ventilation of the giant nests of the leaf-cutting ant Atta vollenweideri. Naturwissenschaften 88, 301–305. ( 10.1007/s001140100235) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 150.Bollazzi M, Roces F. 2007. To build or not to build: circulating dry air organizes collective building for climate control in the leaf-cutting ant Acromyrmex ambiguus. Anim. Behav. 74, 1349–1355. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.02.021) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 151.Weidenmüller A. 2004. The control of nest climate in bumblebee (Bombus terrestris) colonies: interindividual variability and self reinforcement in fanning response. Behav. Ecol. 15, 120–128. ( 10.1093/beheco/arg101) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 152.Westhus C, Kleineidam CJ, Roces F, Weidenmüller A. 2012. Behavioural plasticity in the fanning response of bumblebee workers: impact of experience and rate of temperature change. Anim. Behav. 85, 27–34. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.10.003) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 153.Cook CN, Kaspar RE, Flaxman SM, Breed MD. 2016. Rapidly changing environment modulates the thermoregulatory fanning response in honeybee groups. Anim. Behav. 115, 237–243. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2016.03.014) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 154.Franks NR. 1989. Thermoregulation in army ant bivouacs. Physiol. Entomol. 14, 397–404. ( 10.1111/j.1365-3032.1989.tb01109.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 155.Bollazzi M, Roces F. 2002. Thermal preference for fungus culturing and brood location by workers of the thatching grass-cutting ant Acromyrmex heyeri. Insectes Soc. 49, 153–157. ( 10.1007/s00040-002-8295-x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 156.Anderson KE, Munger JC. 2003. Effect of temperature on brood relocation in Pogonomyrmex salinus (Hymenoptera: Formicidae). West. N. Am. Nat. 63, 122–128. [Google Scholar]
- 157.Piesik S. 2017. Habitat: vernacular architecture for a changing planet. London, UK: Thames & Hudson. [Google Scholar]
- 158.Rapoport A. 1969. House form and culture. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. [Google Scholar]
- 159.Haney DH. 2011. Essay: the case of Leberecht Migge. Landscape: The Journal of the Landscape Institute Spring, 36–39. [Google Scholar]
- 160.Beshers SN, Fewell JH. 2001. Models of division of labor in social insects. Annu. Rev. Entomol. 46, 413–440. ( 10.1146/annurev.ento.46.1.413) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 161.Robinson GE. 1992. Regulation of division of labor in insect societies. Annu. Rev. Entomol. 37, 637–665. ( 10.1146/annurev.en.37.010192.003225) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 162.Hölldobler B, Wilson EO. 1990. The ants, 1st edn Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. [Google Scholar]
- 163.Seeley TD. 1995. The wisdom of the hive: the social physiology of honey bee colonies. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. [Google Scholar]
- 164.Jandt JM, Dornhaus A. 2009. Spatial organization and division of labour in the bumblebee Bombus impatiens. Anim. Behav. 77, 641–651. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.11.019) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 165.Mersch DP, Crespi A, Keller L. 2013. Tracking individuals shows spatial fidelity is a key regulator of ant social organization. Science 9, 735–748. ( 10.1126/science.1234316) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 166.Moser JC. 2006. Complete excavation and mapping of a texas leafcutting ant nest. Ann. Entomol. Soc. Am. 99, 891–897. ( 10.1603/0013-8746(2006)99%5B891:CEAMOA%5D2.0.CO;2) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 167.Hart AG, Ratnieks FLW. 2001. Task partitioning, division of labour and nest compartmentalisation collectively isolate hazardous waste in the leafcutting ant Atta cephalotes. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 49, 387–392. ( 10.1007/s002650000312) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 168.Gordon DM, Chu J, Lillie A, Tissot M, Pinter N. 2005. Variation in the transition from inside to outside work in the red harvester ant Pogonomyrmex barbatus. Insectes Soc. 52, 212–217. ( 10.1007/s00040-004-0796-3) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 169.Davidson JD, Gordon DM. 2017. Spatial organization and interactions of harvester ants during foraging activity. J. R. Soc. Interface 14, 20170413 ( 10.1098/rsif.2017.0413) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 170.Prabhakar B, Dektar KN, Gordon DM. 2012. The regulation of ant colony foraging activity without spatial information. PLoS Comput. Biol. 8, e1002670 ( 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1002670) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 171.Huang ZY, Robinson GE. 1992. Honeybee colony integration: worker–worker interactions mediate hormonally regulated plasticity in division of labor. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 89, 11 726–11 729. ( 10.1073/pnas.89.24.11726) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 172.Naug D, Gadagkar R. 1999. Flexible division of labor mediated by social interactions in an insect colony—a simulation model. J. Theor. Biol. 197, 123–133. ( 10.1006/jtbi.1998.0862) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 173.Franks NR, Sendova-Franks AB. 1992. Brood sorting by ants: distributing the workload over the work-surface. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 30, 109–123. ( 10.1007/BF00173947) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 174.Sendova-Franks AB, Scholes SR, Franks NR, Melhuish C. 2004. Brood sorting by ants: two phases and differential diffusion. Anim. Behav. 68, 1095–1106. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2004.02.013) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 175.Gaubert L, Redou P, Harrouet F, Tisseau J. 2007. A first mathematical model of brood sorting by ants: functional self-organization without swarm-intelligence. Ecol. Complex. 4, 234–241. ( 10.1016/j.ecocom.2007.06.004) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 176.Holland O, Melhuish C. 1999. Stigmergy, self-organization, and sorting in collective robotics. Artif. Life 5, 173–202. ( 10.1162/106454699568737) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 177.Deneubourg JL, Goss S, Franks N, Sendova-Franks A, Detrain C, Chrétien L. 1991. The dynamics of collective sorting robot-like ants and ant-like robots In Proc. of the first Int. Conf. on simulation of adaptive behavior on From animals to animats, Paris, France, pp. 356–363. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
- 178.Johnson BR. 2010. Task partitioning in honey bees: the roles of signals and cues in group-level coordination of action. Behav. Ecol. 21, 1373–1379. ( 10.1093/beheco/arq138) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 179.Theraulaz G, Bonabeau E, Sole RV, Schatz B, Deneubourg J-L. 2002. Task partitioning in a ponerine ant. J. Theor. Biol. 215, 481–489. ( 10.1006/jtbi.2001.2518) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 180.Ratnieks FLW, Anderson C. 1999. Task partitioning in insect societies. Insectes Soc. 46, 95–108. ( 10.1007/s000400050119) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 181.Anderson C, Ratnieks FLW. 2000. Task partitioning in insect societies: novel situations. Insectes Soc. 47, 198–199. ( 10.1007/PL00001702) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 182.Bonabeau E, Theraulaz G, Deneubourg J-L. 1998. Fixed response thresholds and the regulation of division of labor in insect societies. Bull. Math. Biol. 60, 753–807. ( 10.1006/bulm.1998.0041) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 183.Quevillon LE, Hanks EM, Bansal S, Hughes DP. 2015. Social, spatial, and temporal organization in a complex insect society. Sci. Rep. 5, 13393 ( 10.1038/srep13393) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 184.Rittel HWJ, Webber MM. 1973. Dilemmas in a general theory of planning. Policy Sci. 4, 155–169. ( 10.1007/BF01405730) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 185.Reynolds CW. 1987. Flocks, herds and schools: a distributed behavioral model. ACM SIGGRAPH Comput. Grap. 21, 25–34. ( 10.1145/37402.37406) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 186.Theraulaz G, Bonabeau E. 1999. A brief history of stigmergy. Artif. Life 5, 97–116. ( 10.1162/106454699568700) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 187.Theraulaz G. 2014. Embracing the creativity of stigmergy in social insects. Archit. Des. 84, 54–59. ( 10.1002/ad.1808) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 188.Prusinkiewicz P, Lindenmayer A. 2012. The algorithmic beauty of plants. Berlin, Germany: Springer Science & Business Media. [Google Scholar]
- 189.Bonabeau E, Dorigo M, Theraulaz G. 1999. Swarm intelligence: from natural to artificial systems. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- 190.Gardner M. 1970. Mathematical games—the fantastic combinations of John Conway's new solitaire game, Life . Sci. Am. 223, 120–123. ( 10.1038/scientificamerican1070-120) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 191.Ireland T. In press Frederick Kiesler: from life to architecture—to life. Signs and Media Journal. [Google Scholar]
- 192.Bogner D. 2003. Friedrich Kiesler: endless house 1947–1961. Berlin, Germany: Hatje Cantz. [Google Scholar]
- 193.Kiesler F. 1939. On correalism and biotechnique: a definition and test of a new approach to building design. Archit. Rec. 86, 60–75. [Google Scholar]
- 194.Dawkins R. 1982. The extended phenotype: the long reach of the gene. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- 195.Hölldobler B, Wilson EO. 2009. The superorganism: the beauty, elegance, and strangeness of insect societies. New York, NY: W.W. Norton & Company. [Google Scholar]
- 196.Wilson EO. 1971. The insect societies. Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press of Harvard University Press. [Google Scholar]
- 197.Turner JS. 1994. Ventilation and thermal constancy of a colony of a southern African termite (Odontotermes transvaalensis: Macrotermintinae). J. Arid Environ. 28, 231–248. ( 10.1016/S0140-1963(05)80060-6) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 198.Turner JS. 2001. On the mound of Macrotermes michaelseni as an organ of respiratory gas exchange. Physiol. Biochem. Zool. 74, 798–822. ( 10.1086/323990) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 199.Bollazzi M, Forti LC, Roces F. 2012. Ventilation of the giant nests of Atta leaf-cutting ants: does underground circulating air enter the fungus chambers? Insectes Soc. 59, 487–498. ( 10.1007/s00040-012-0243-9) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 200.Kleineidam C, Roces F. 2000. Carbon dioxide concentrations and nest ventilation in nests of the leaf-cutting ant Atta vollenweideri. Insectes Soc. 47, 241–248. ( 10.1007/PL00001710) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 201.Haifig I, Jost C, Janei V, Costa-Leonardo AM. 2011. The size of excavators within a polymorphic termite species governs tunnel topology. Anim. Behav. 82, 1409–1414. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.09.025) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 202.Perna A, Valverde S, Gautrais J, Jost C, Solé R, Kuntz P, Theraulaz G. 2008. Topological efficiency in three-dimensional gallery networks of termite nests. Physica A 387, 6235–6244. ( 10.1016/j.physa.2008.07.019) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 203.Gerbier G, Garnier S, Rieu C, Theraulaz G, Fourcassié V. 2008. Are ants sensitive to the geometry of tunnel bifurcation? Anim. Cogn. 11, 637–642. ( 10.1007/s10071-008-0153-4) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 204.Jackson DE, Holcombe M, Ratnieks FLW. 2004. Trail geometry gives polarity to ant foraging networks. Nature 432, 907–909. ( 10.1038/nature03105) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 205.Garnier S, Combe M, Jost C, Theraulaz G. 2013. Do ants need to estimate the geometrical properties of trail bifurcations to find an efficient route? A swarm robotics test bed. PLoS Comput. Biol. 9, e1002903 ( 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1002903) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 206.Acosta FJ, López F, Serrano JM. 1993. Branching angles of ant trunk trails as an optimization cue. J. Theor. Biol. 160, 297–310. ( 10.1006/jtbi.1993.1020) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 207.Schoenefeldt H. 2017. The historic ventilation system of the House of Commons, 1840–52: revisiting David Boswell Reid's environmental legacy. Antiquaries J. 98 (In press.). (See https://kar.kent.ac.uk/65558/) [Google Scholar]
- 208.Safamanesh K. 1995. The Aga Khan Award for Architecture. Technical Review Summary: Menara Mesiniaga.
- 209.Benedictus L. 2015. Hajj crush: how crowd disasters happen, and how they can be avoided. The Guardian, 3 October. See http://www.theguardian.com/world/2015/oct/03/hajj-crush-how-crowd-disasters-happen-and-how-they-can-be-avoided.
- 210.Moussaid M, Helbing D, Theraulaz G. 2011. How simple rules determine pedestrian behavior and crowd disasters. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 108, 6884–6888. ( 10.1073/pnas.1016507108) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 211.Helbing D, Johansson A, Al-Abideen HZ. 2007. Dynamics of crowd disasters: an empirical study. Phys. Rev. E 75, 046109 ( 10.1103/PhysRevE.75.046109) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 212.Bohannon J. 2005. Directing the herd: crowds and the science of evacuation. Science 310, 219–221. ( 10.1126/science.310.5746.219) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 213.Evans R. 1982. The fabrication of virtue: English prison architecture, 1750–1840. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- 214.Saalman H. 1971. Haussmann: Paris transformed. New York, NY: George Braziller. [Google Scholar]
- 215.Smith JM, Harper D. 2003. Animal signals. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- 216.Bradbury JW, Vehrencamp SL. 1998. Principles of animal communication. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer Associates, Inc. [Google Scholar]
- 217.Bateson G. 1972. Form, substance, and difference. In Steps to an ecology of mind: collected essays in anthropology, psychiatry, evolution, and epistemology, pp. 454–471. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. [Google Scholar]
- 218.Floridi L. 2010. Information: A very short introduction. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press Oxford. [Google Scholar]
- 219.Shannon CE, Weaver W. 1949. The mathematical theory of communication. Champaign, IL: University of Illinois Press. [Google Scholar]
- 220.Salthe SN. 2011. Naturalizing information. Information 2, 417–425. ( 10.3390/info2030417) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 221.Cárdenas-García JF, Romero Castro D, Soria de Mesa B. 2018. Object discernment by ‘a difference which makes a difference’. Biosemiotics ( 10.1007/s12304-018-9315-1) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 222.Schroeder MJ. 2017. The difference that makes a difference for the conceptualization of information In Multidisciplinary Digital Publishing Institute Proceedings 1, 221. [Google Scholar]
- 223.Laidre ME, Johnstone RA. 2013. Animal signals. Curr. Biol. 23, R829–R833. ( 10.1016/j.cub.2013.07.070) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 224.Jost C, Verret J, Casellas E, Gautrais J, Challet M, Lluc J, Blanco S, Clifton MJ, Theraulaz G. 2007. The interplay between a self-organized process and an environmental template: corpse clustering under the influence of air currents in ants. J. R. Soc. Interface 4, 107–116. ( 10.1098/rsif.2006.0156) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 225.Saffre F, Hildmann H, Deneubourg J-L. 2018. Can individual heterogeneity influence self-organised patterns in the termite nest construction model? Swarm Intell. 12, 101–110. ( 10.1007/s11721-017-0143-8) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 226.Rudofsky B. 1964. Architecture without architects: a short introduction to non-pedigreed architecture. New York, NY: UNM Press. [Google Scholar]
- 227.Mesoudi A, Whiten A, Laland KN. 2006. Towards a unified science of cultural evolution. Behav. Brain Sci. 29, 329–347; discussion 347–383 ( 10.1017/S0140525X06009083) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 228.Soltis J, Boyd R, Richerson PJ. 1995. Can group-functional behaviors evolve by cultural group selection?: An empirical test. Curr. Anthropol. 36, 473–494. ( 10.1086/204381) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 229.Danchin E, Giraldeau L-A, Valone TJ, Wagner RH. 2004. Public information: from nosy neighbors to cultural evolution. Science 305, 487–491. ( 10.1126/science.1098254) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 230.Kolodny O, Creanza N, Feldman MW. 2015. Evolution in leaps: the punctuated accumulation and loss of cultural innovations. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 112, E6762–E6769. ( 10.1073/pnas.1520492112) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 231.Whiten A, Hinde RA, Laland KN, Stringer CB. 2011. Culture evolves. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 366, 938–948. ( 10.1098/rstb.2010.0372) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 232.Grassé P-P. 1959. La reconstruction du nid et les coordinations interindividuelles chez Bellicositermes natalensis et Cubitermes sp. La théorie de la stigmergie: Essai d'interprétation du comportement des termites constructeurs. Insectes Soc. 6, 41–80. ( 10.1007/BF02223791) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 233.Theraulaz G, Bonabeau E, Deneubourg J-L. 1999. The mechanisms and rules of coordinated building in social insects. In Information processing in social insects (eds Detrain C, Deneubourg JL, Pasteels JM), pp. 309–330. Basel, Switzerland: Birkhäuser. [Google Scholar]
- 234.Haifig I, Jost C, Fourcassié V, Zana Y, Costa-Leonardo AM. 2015. Dynamics of foraging trails in the neotropical termite Velocitermes heteropterus (Isoptera: Termitidae). Behav. Processes 118, 123–129. ( 10.1016/j.beproc.2015.06.010) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 235.Wilson EO. 1962. Chemical communication among workers of the fire ant Solenopsis saevissima (Fr. Smith) 2. An information analysis of the odour trail. Anim. Behav. 10, 148–158. ( 10.1016/0003-3472(62)90142-2) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 236.Aron S, Deneubourg JL, Goss S, Pasteels JM. 1990. Functional self-organisation illustrated by inter-nest traffic in ants: the case of the Argentine ant. In Biological motion lecture notes in biomathematics, vol. 89, pp. 533–547. Berlin, Heidelberg, Germany: Springer. [Google Scholar]
- 237.Bonabeau E, Theraulaz G, Deneubourg J-L, Franks NR, Rafelsberger O, Joly J, Blanco S. 1998. A model for the emergence of pillars, walls and royal chambers in termite nests. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B 353, 1561–1576. ( 10.1098/rstb.1998.0310) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 238.Ladley D, Bullock S. 2005. The role of logistic constraints in termite construction of chambers and tunnels. J. Theor. Biol. 234, 551–564. ( 10.1016/j.jtbi.2004.12.012) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 239.Aleksiev AS, Sendova-Franks AB, Franks NR. 2007. Nest ‘moulting' in the ant Temnothorax albipennis. Anim. Behav. 74, 567–575. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2006.12.023) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 240.Franks NR, Wilby A, Silverman BW, Tofts C. 1992. Self-organizing nest construction in ants: sophisticated building by blind bulldozing. Anim. Behav. 44, 357–375. ( 10.1016/0003-3472(92)90041-7) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 241.Franks N, Deneubourg J-L. 1997. Self-organizing nest construction in ants: individual worker behaviour and the nest's dynamics. Anim. Behav. 54, 779–796. ( 10.1006/anbe.1996.0496) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 242.Doyle MJ, Marsh L. 2013. Stigmergy 3.0: From ants to economies. Cogn. Syst. Res. 21, 1–6. ( 10.1016/j.cogsys.2012.06.001) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 243.Heylighen F. 2016. Stigmergy as a universal coordination mechanism I: definition and components. Cogn. Syst. Res. 38, 4–13. ( 10.1016/j.cogsys.2015.12.002) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 244.Heylighen F. 2016. Stigmergy as a universal coordination mechanism II: varieties and evolution. Cogn. Syst. Res. 38, 50–59. ( 10.1016/j.cogsys.2015.12.007) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 245.Lewis TG, Marsh L. 2016. Human stigmergy: Theoretical developments and new applications. Cogn. Syst. Res. 38, 1–3. ( 10.1016/j.cogsys.2015.12.001) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 246.Alborghetti P, Erioli A. 2014. The Red Queen hypothesis—chemotaxic stigmergic systems and embodied embedded cognition-based strategies in architectural design. In Fusion—Proceedings of the 32nd eCAADe Conference—Volume 2 (ed Thompson EM.), pp. 97–105. Newcastle upon Tyne, UK: Faculty of Engineering and Environment. [Google Scholar]
- 247.Miranda Carranza P. 2000. Swarm modelling. The use of swarm intelligence to generate architectural form. In Proc. 3rd Generative Art Conference. Generative Art. Milan, Italy: AleaDesign Publisher. [Google Scholar]
- 248.Leach N. 2009. Swarm urbanism. Archit. Des. 79, 56–63. ( 10.1002/ad.918) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 249.Ma Y, Xu W. 2017. PHYSAREALM—a bio-inspired stigmergic algorithm tool for form-finding In Protocols, Flows and Glitches, Proc. 22nd Int. Conf. Association for Computer-Aided Architectural Design Research in Asia (CAADRIA) 2017, Suzhou, China, 5–8 April 2017 (eds Janssen P, Loh P, Raonic A, Schnabel MA), pp. 499–509. Hong Kong, China: The Association for Computer-Aided Architectural Design Research in Asia (CAADRIA). [Google Scholar]
- 250.Ireland T. 2010. Stigmergic planning. In LIFE information: On Responsive Information and Variations in Architecture In Proc. of the 30th Annual Conf. of the Association for Computer Aided Design in Architecture (ACADIA 2010) (eds Sprecher A, Yeshayahu S, Lorenzo-Eiroa P), New York, 21–24 October 2010, pp. 183–189. New York, NY: ACADIA. [Google Scholar]
- 251.Ireland T. 2015. A cell inspired model of configuration. In Computational Ecologies: Design in the Anthropocene Proceedings of the 35th Annual Conference of the Association for Computer Aided Design in Architecture (ACADIA 2015), 19--25 October 2015, Cincinnati, OH, USA (eds Combs L, Perry C), pp. 136–147. New York, NY: ACADIA. [Google Scholar]
- 252.Ireland T. 2008. Sniffing space In Proc. 11th Generative Art Conference, Milan, Italy. [Google Scholar]
- 253.Puusepp R. 2014. Agent-based models for computing circulation In Design Agency: Proceedings of the 34th Annual Conference of the Association for Computer Aided Design in Architecture (ACADIA 2014) October 23–25, 2014, Los Angeles, CA, USA (eds Gerber D, Huang A, Sanchez J), pp. 43–52. Riverside Architectural Press. [Google Scholar]
- 254.Puusepp R. 2014. Spatial agglomerates. Towards synthetic modelling of the ‘unplanned’ In Rethinking Comprehensive Design: Speculative Counterculture, Proc. of the 19th Int. Conf. of the Association of Computer-Aided Architectural Design Research in Asia CAADRIA 2014 (eds Gu N, Watanabe S, Erhan H, Haeusler H, Huang W), pp. 585–594. Kyoto, Japan: Kyoto University Press. [Google Scholar]
- 255.Snooks R. 2018. Behavioural matter. In Computational design (eds Leach N, Yuan PF), pp. 143–155. Tongji, China: Tongji University Press. [Google Scholar]
- 256.Weinstock M. 2013. System city: infrastructure and the space of flows. Archit. Des. 83, 14–23. ( 10.1002/ad.1614) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 257.Weinstock M. 2010. The architecture of emergence: the evolution of form in nature and civilisation. New York, NY: Wiley. [Google Scholar]
- 258.Batty M. 2007. Cities and complexity: understanding cities with cellular automata, agent-based models, and fractals. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
- 259.Coates P, Derix C. 2014. The deep structure of the picturesque. Archit. Des. 84, 32–37. ( 10.1002/ad.1805) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 260.Alldredge AL, Hamner WM. 1980. Recurring aggregation of zooplankton by a tidal current. Estuarine Coastal Mar. Sci. 10, 31–37. ( 10.1016/S0302-3524(80)80047-8) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 261.Ganeshaiahl KN, Veena T. 1991. Topology of the foraging trails of Leptogenys processionalis? Why are they branched? Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 29, 263–270. ( 10.1007/BF00163983) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 262.Smith ML, Koenig PA, Peters JM. 2017. The cues of colony size: how honey bees sense that their colony is large enough to begin to invest in reproduction. J. Exp. Biol. 220, 1597–1605. ( 10.1242/jeb.150342) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 263.Lewin K. 1936. Principles of topological psychology. New York: NY: McGraw-Hill. [Google Scholar]
- 264.Ellard CG, Dzebic V. 2014. The psychology of architectural and urban design: sensor-based field methods based on guided walks In Proc. of the Academy of Neuroscience of Architecture (ANFA) 2014 conference, La Jolla, CA, USA, 18–20 September 2014.
- 265.Montello DR. 2014. Spatial cognition and architectural space: research perspectives. Archit. Des. 84, 74–79. ( 10.1002/ad.1811) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 266.Al-Sayed K, Turner A, Hillier B, Iida S, Penn A. 2014. Space syntax methodology (4th edn). London, UK: Bartlett School of Architecture. [Google Scholar]
- 267.Turner A, Doxa M, O'Sullivan D, Penn A. 2001. From isovists to visibility graphs: a methodology for the analysis of architectural space. Environ. Plann. B Plann. Des. 28, 103–121. ( 10.1068/b2684) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 268.Turner A, Penn A. 2002. Encoding natural movement as an agent-based system: an investigation into human pedestrian behaviour in the built environment. Environ. Plann. B Plann. Des. 29, 473–490. ( 10.1068/b12850) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 269.Hillier B, Major MD, Desyllas J, Karimi K, Campos B, Stonor T. 1996. Tate Gallery, Millbank: a study of the existing layout and new masterplan proposal. London, UK: UCL. [Google Scholar]
- 270.Hillier B, Penn A, Hanson J, Grajewski T, Xu J. 1993. Natural movement: or, configuration and attraction in urban pedestrian movement. Environ. Plann. B Plann. Des. 20, 29–66. ( 10.1068/b200029) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 271.Batty M. 2001. Exploring isovist fields: space and shape in architectural and urban morphology. Environ. Plann. B Plann. Des. 28, 123–150. ( 10.1068/b2725) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 272.Vargas JP, López JC, Salas C, Thinus-Blanc C. 2004. Encoding of geometric and featural spatial information by goldfish (Carassius auratus). J. Comp. Psychol. 118, 206–216. ( 10.1037/0735-7036.118.2.206) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 273.Thinus-Blanc C, Gaunet F. 1999. Spatial processing in animals and humans: the organizing function of representations for information gathering. In Wayfinding behaviour: cognitive mapping and other spatial processes (ed. Golledge RG.), pp. 294–307. Baltimore, MD: The John Hopkins University Press. [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
This article has no additional data.