Abstract
Objective: Purposes of the present study were to assess the social cognitive impairment in schizophrenia and to detect if some clinical variables (particularly age at onset) are predictive of general/social cognitive deficit in schizophrenia patients.
Method: Thirty-five clinically stabilized schizophrenia outpatients were assessed by the Brief Assessment of Cognition in Schizophrenia (BACS) and by Torralva’s social cognition battery. Binary logistic models were performed to find an eventual association between continuous clinical variables and cognitive test failures. The total sample was divided in groups according to dichotomous variables (gender, diagnostic subtypes and type of abuse) and the presence of cognitive deficits was compared between groups by χ2 tests.
Results: An earlier age at onset was found to be predictive of frontal cognitive impairment (Tower of London p=0.038, OR=0.702). Female gender was more probably associated with mistakes at MET-HV (χ2= 4.80, p=0.05, phi=0.40) and HOTEL tests (χ2= 5.25, p=0.04, phi=0.4) than male one. Cannabis abusers showed more frequently deficits on verbal fluency (χ2= 9.35, p=0.04, phi=0.52) and executive functioning (Tower of London) (χ2= 11.67, p=0.02, phi=0.58) than alcohol/cocaine ones.
Conclusion: Female patients with an early age at onset and cannabis abuse seem to have the worst general and social cognitive profile among patients suffering from schizophrenia.
Key Words: Age at Onset, Cannabis Abuse, Gender, Schizophrenia, Social Cognition
Several studies have shown that global cognitive impairment and deficits in the processing of emotions are typical features of schizophrenia (1-6). Schizophrenia patients appear to have deficits in different neurocognitive domains and, among them, memory, attention, and executive functions would be the most compromised (7, 8). In addition, schizophrenia patients often experience low level of performances and a reduced ability to live independently despite the remission of the acute symptomatology (9-11), with a negative impact on the social and occupational functioning and poor quality of life (10, 12).
Of note, social cognition, defined as the mental operations underlying social interactions (10), has been recently interpreted as a mediator between neurocognition and functional outcome in schizophrenia (13). Furthermore, in light of the importance of cognitive impairment in affecting schizophrenia outcome, the interest in cognitive functioning has raised in the last twenty years (14,15) and cognitive impairment has been considered as an important pharmacological target for the development of new drugs in schizophrenia, having the available molecules no or small effect on cognition (16).
With regard to clinical symptoms, the available data show that cognitive impairment seems to be more strongly associated with the severity of clinical negative symptoms respect to positive ones (17-19). In addition, higher estimated premorbid IQ, years of education and predominant disorganized symptoms seem to be important predictors of poor neurocognitive performances (20-22).
In schizophrenia patients, gender differences have been found in verbal learning and recall: a poorer neurocognitive performance, particularly in verbal memory, has been associated with male gender (20). Furthermore, altered neurocognitive functioning in several domains has been reported in schizophrenia patients with lifetime cannabis abuse (3, 23 and 24). In contrast, a meta-analysis found superior neurocognitive performances in cannabis-using patients compared to non-using ones (25). Moreover, a further study did not find significant differences on neurocognitive functioning or number of lost days of work between moderate/severe drug users and mild users or abstainers (26). Finally, alcohol abuse in schizophrenia is associated with more impaired functioning across many domains, including memory (27). It is currently debated if diagnostic subtypes are associated with severity of cognitive impairment. In two studies paranoid patients resulted to have better Verbal IQ, executive functioning and memory compared to undifferentiated ones (28-30), while another study showed that paranoid patients did not have higher intellectual functioning than those with a non-paranoid sub-type (31).
Duration of illness probably influences the severity and type of cognitive deficits: a study showed that chronic patients presented more severe cognitive impairment especially in verbal memory, visual memory and attention respect to recent-onset schizophrenia patients (22). Furthermore, a longer duration of illness was found to correlate with impairment on psychomotor processing speed, verbal fluency and verbal learning (32(.
Duration of Untreated Psychosis (DUP)/ Untreated Illness (DUI) has been largely studied that an important factor in influencing cognitive abilities of schizophrenia patients. Longer DUP has been associated with deficits in Digit Symbol and Comprehension subtests (20), and, in first-episode schizophrenia, with deficits in verbal IQ, verbal learning, and verbal working memory (33).
Some studies showed that early age at onset is associated with severe cognitive impairment in schizophrenia patients (34-36). A study found a relationship between early age at onset and poor performances on IQ, Digit Symbol Coding and Tower of London tests (37). These findings are in agreement with a study (38), which demonstrated that worse Tower of London Task scores may primarily characterize early-onset schizophrenia. In line with these results, late-onset schizophrenia could be associated with better socio-functional outcome and higher possibility to get married (39).
On the basis of the mentioned data, the objectives of the present study are: 1) to assess the social cognition in a sample of stabilized schizophrenic outpatients and 2) to find an eventual relation between clinical variables and general/social cognitive deficits in schizophrenic patients.
Materials and Methods
Thirty-five outpatients (twenty-nine males and six females) recruited from community services afferent to the Department of Psychiatry (University of Milan), with a diagnosis of schizophrenia according to DSM-5 (40) and treated with an antipsychotic mono-therapy, were included into the study.
Structured Clinical Interview for DSM –SCID-5-CV– (40), Global Assessment of Functioning – GAF (41), The Calgary Depression Scale for Schizophrenia - CDSS (42) and Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale - PANSS (43) were administered to the patients: those who showed a re-exacerbation of the disorder, as defined by a PANSS score > 60 (44), were excluded. Other exclusion criteria were comorbidity with mental retardation or other neurological conditions involving Central Nervous System (e.g. cerebral tumors), presence of a comorbid Axis I disorder (except substance or alcohol misuse), comorbidity with medical diseases (e.g. hypothyroidism) or metabolic disorders causing psychiatric symptoms, pregnancy and breastfeeding.
The design of the study was naturalistic and cross-sectional. General cognitive functioning and social cognitive functioning was assessed by this way:
General Cognitive assessment
General cognitive assessment was obtained using the Brief Assessment of Cognition in Schizophrenia - BACS (45, 46) that is a neuropsychological battery which includes Verbal Fluency, Token Motor Task, Symbol Coding, Tower of London Test, Verbal Memory (list learning) and Working memory (digit sequencing).
Verbal Fluency: patients have to say that many words as possible from a category in a given time (in our case 60 seconds). This category can be semantic such as furniture or fruits, or phonemic, such as words that begin with letter b.
Token Motor Task: patients are given 100 plastic tokens and asked to place them into a container as quickly as possible for 60 seconds.
Symbol Coding: it consists of nine digit-symbol pairs followed by a list of digits. Under each digit the patient should write down the corresponding symbol as fast as possible.
Tower of London: it consists of two boards with pegs and several beads with different colors. The rater uses the board in a variety of ways to test problem solving skills.
Verbal Memory (list learning): patients are presented with 15 words and then asked to recall as many as possible.
Working Memory (digit sequencing): patients are presented with clusters of numbers of increasing length.
Social cognition battery
In light of an overlapped cognitive impairment between schizophrenia and fronto-temporal dementia (47), social cognition was assessed using a 5-test battery sensitive in detecting executive and social cognitive impairment in early stages of the behavioral variant of Frontotemporal dementia (48).
1. Multiple Errands Test for Use in Hospital Settings (MET-HV): the test requires subjects to carry out a number of tasks simulating “real life” situations where minor inconveniences can take place (49).
2. The Hotel Task: the task comprises six activities that would plausibly need to be completed in the course of running a hotel (50).
3. Iowa gambling task: this test mimics real-life personal decision-making activities that include reward and punishment (51).
4. Reading the Mind in the Eyes: participants are required to choose between four options (adjectives) that best describes what the individual in the presented photo are thinking or feeling (52, 53).
5. Faux Pas Test: participants have to find something inappropriate in tales that they have to read and that may contain a social “faux pas” (a violation of accepted social norms) (54).
Statistical analysis
Descriptive statistics were performed in order to evaluate general and social cognition in the total sample. Binary logistic models were then performed. In these analyses, the failure in a single test was considered that the dependent variable, while age, age at onset, duration of illness, DUI, Calgary scores, GAF scores and PANSS scores were the covariates. Χ2 tests were used to compare dichotomous variables (gender, diagnostic subtypes, type of abuse) and cognitive test failures. SPSS for Windows (version 22.0) was used as statistical program.
Results
Descriptive statistics of the total sample are reported in table 1. In our sample patients showed impairment in different several domains. The worst performances resulted in Faux Pas Test (failure in 90.3% of patients), Token Motor Task (failure in 82.9% of patients) and Symbol Coding (failure in 80.0% of patients).
Table1.
Demographic and Clinical Variables of the Clinically Stabilized Patients with Schizophrenia
Variables | N=35 | |
---|---|---|
Gender | Male | 29 (82.9) |
Female | 6 (17.1) | |
Age | 41.40 + 10.22 | |
Age at onset | 20.94 + 3.70 | |
Prevalent Symptoms | Psychotic | 11 (31.4) |
Negative | 15 (42.9) | |
Disorganization | 9 (25.7) | |
Abuse before the onset | No | 13 (37.1) |
Yes | 22 (62.9) | |
Duration of illness | 20.46 + 11.29 | |
Type of abuse | None | 13 (37.1) |
Alcohol | 5 (14.3) | |
Cannabis | 11 (31.4) | |
Cocaine | 4 (11.4) | |
Heroin | 2 (5.7) | |
Duration of untreated illness | 2.80 + 4.78 | |
Treatment | Quetiapine | 3 (8.6) |
Olanzapine | 5 (14.3) | |
Risperidone | 8 (22.9) | |
Aripiprazole | 5 (14.3) | |
Zuclopenthixol | 7 (20.0) | |
Haloperidol | 5 (14.3) | |
Paliperidone | 2 (5.7) | |
PANSS total score | 48.06 + 3.92 | |
CDSS | 4.74 + 3.88 | |
GAF | 43.34 + 11.79 |
Note: Standard deviations for continuous variables and percentages for dichotomous ones are reported into brackets
PANSS: Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale
CDSS: The Calgary Depression Scale for Schizophrenia
GAF: Global Assessment of Functioning
In table 2 the mean test scores are reported together with the percentage of failing patients.
Table2.
Summary of the Results in Cognitive Domains in Clinically Stabilized Patients with Schizophrenia
COGNITIVE TESTS Sample N=35 |
Minimum Score |
Maximum Score |
Mean Score |
Standard Deviation |
% of Failure |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
BACS | |||||
Verbal Memory vn > 33.01 |
21 | 57 | 35.69 | ± 8.477 | 60.0 |
Working Memory vn > 14.93 |
3.25 | 26.25 | 16.87 | ± 5.339 | 51.4 |
Token Task vn > 68.77 |
25 | 90 | 61.15 | ± 16.126 | 82.9 |
Symbol Coding Task vn > 40.49 |
15 | 63 | 38.56 | ± 10.858 | 80.0 |
Verbal Fluency vn > 31.68 |
16.3 | 58.5 | 34.04 | ± 9.587 | 68.6 |
Tower of London vn > 12.37 |
0 | 20 | 12.79 | ± 4.010 | 68.6 |
SOCIAL COGNITION | |||||
MET | |||||
Tasks attempted | 4 | 12 | 10.07 | ± 2.288 | 56.7 |
Task failures | 0 | 8 | 1.93 | ± 2.288 | 56.7 |
Inefficiencies | 0 | 3 | 1.47 | ± 0.776 | 56.7 |
Rule breaks | 0 | 2 | 0.63 | ± 0.615 | 0.0 |
Interpretation failure | 0 | 4 | 0.63 | ± 0.999 | 40.0 |
Total failures | 0 | 11 | 4.67 | ± 3.294 | 30.0 |
HOTEL | |||||
Tasks attempted | 0 | 5 | 3.17 | ± 1.315 | 53.3 |
Tasks correct | 0 | 5 | 3.17 | ± 1.315 | 53.3 |
Time deviation (sec) | 0 | 720 | 277.93 | ± 209.415 | 24.1 |
Button pressing | 0 | 2 | 1.3 | ± 0.915 | 40.0 |
Opening deviation | 0 | 4 | 2.67 | ± 1,826 | |
Closing deviation | 0 | 4 | 2.17 | ± 1,913 | |
Garage time deviation | 0 | 8 | 4.83 | ± 3.534 | 50.0 |
IOWA GAMBLING TASK | -42 | 52 | -1.1 | ± 21.806 | 58.1 |
THEORY OF MIND TESTS | |||||
THE EYES TEST | 10 | 25 | 19.68 | ± 4.482 | 35.5 |
FAUX PAS TEST | 0 | 20 | 12.52 | ± 3.846 | 90.3 |
BACS: Brief Assessment of Cognition in Schizophrenia
HOTEL: The Hotel Task
MET: Multiple Errands Test
The goodness-of-fit test results (Hosmer and Lemeshow Test: χ2=8.83, df =7, p=0.265) showed that the model including continuous variables/scale scores as possible predictors of Tower of London failure was adequate, allowing for a correct classification of 77.1% of the cases.
In addition, the model was overall significant (Omnibus test: χ2=13.22, df=6, p=0.04). An early age at onset was found to be associated with BACS Tower of London failure (OR=0.702, p=0.038). No further statistically significant associations were found (table 3). With regard to dichotomous variables, female schizophrenia patients showed more frequently failures in MET (total errors: χ2=4.80, p=0.05, Phi=0.40) (Figure 1) and in the HOTEL task (number of performed activities: χ2=5.25, p=0.04, Phi=0.42; number of correctly performed activities: χ2=5.25, p=0.04, Phi=0.42). In addition, cannabis abusers showed more frequently deficits on verbal fluency (χ2= 9.35, p=0.04, phi=0.52) (Figure 2) and executive functioning (Tower of London) (χ2= 11.67, p=0.02, phi=0.58) than alcohol/cocaine abusers. No further statistically significant associations were found.
Table3.
Summary of the Statistics for the Best-Fit Logistic Regression Model Applied
Clinical Variables/ Scale Scores | B | S.E. | Wald | df | p | Exp (B) |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Age | 0.013 | 0.055 | 0.058 | 1 | 0.809 | -1.013 |
Age at onset | -3.54 | 0.170 | 4.321 | 1 | 0.038 | 0.702 |
DUI | 0.056 | 0.117 | 0.230 | 1 | 0.632 | 1.058 |
CDSS scores | -0.236 | 0.143 | 2.718 | 1 | 0.099 | 0.790 |
GAF scores | 0.037 | 0.045 | 0.658 | 1 | 0.417 | 1.037 |
PANSS scores | 0.285 | 0.187 | 2.331 | 1 | 0.127 | 1.33 |
In this analysis the dependent variable was the Tower of London success
B=coefficient; S.E.= standard error of B; Wald=Wald statistics; df= degree of freedom; p=significance; Exp (B)=odds ratio.
CDSS= Calgary Depression Scale for Schizophrenia
GAF= Global Assessment of Functioning
PANSS= Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale
Omnibus Test of Model Coefficients: χ2=13.22, df=6, p=0.04
Hosmer and Lemeshow Test: χ2=8.83, df=7, p=0.265
Figure1.
Gender Differences in Multiple Errands Test (MET) Performances
Figure2.
Substance Abuse and Differences in Verbal Fluency Performances
Discussion
The first result of our study is that clinically stabilized schizophrenia patients present impairment in several cognitive domains particularly in language comprehension (Token Test), social sensitivity (Faux Pas Test) and memory (Digit Symbol Coding). This finding supports the statement that schizophrenia cognitive dysfunction is not only associated to acute re-exacerbations, but it remains during the course of illness (55, 56). This is the reason why specific cognitive impairments have been recently proposed that neurocognitive markers of schizophrenic illness by international biological societies (57).
The main result of the present paper is that an early age at onset is associated with severity of frontal cognitive impairment (Tower of London). Frontal cognitive disability limits rehabilitation programs and impairs quality of life so that our findings confirm the view that early-onset schizophrenia patients have very poor prognosis (58). It is actually debated if cognitive dysfunction can be associated with brain changes that have been found in early-onset schizophrenia patients by recent neuroimaging researches (59-63). Interestingly, our data show that frontal dysfunctions do not result to be associated with age so that they probably emerge in first years of illness and keep stable over time. On the basis of this hypothesis, primary (identification of high-risk population) and secondary prevention programmes (early diagnosis) become preeminent to prevent or at least limit cognitive impairment and improve outcome of early-onset patients (64).
The second result is a worse social cognition in female schizophrenia patients respect to male ones. This finding is surprising as female gender is generally considered having a better outcome than schizophrenic males especially in case of a long duration of illness (65, 66). Perhaps these are specific cognitive deficits of female gender as in our knowledge this is the first study assessing social cognition in a sample of schizophrenia patients by Torralva’s neuropsychological battery. The results suggest that endophenotypes in schizophrenia may be sex-specific (67).
Finally, cannabis abuse appears to be associated with a more severe verbal/executive impairment in comparison with alcohol/cocaine abuse. It is traditionally stated that schizophrenic cannabis abusers show better neuropsychological performances than non-abusers in light of a baseline minor cognitive impairment (68). The discordant results of this paper can be explained by the long duration of illness of our sample: it can be hypothesized that cannabis-related cognitive impairment may be progressive during the course of schizophrenia, while cognitive impairment may be static in cocaine abusers (69).
Limitation
Limits of the present study have to be shortly described. First the sample size is small, but this is partially due to the selection of patients in mono-therapies to limit the impact of medications on cognition. Second, possible confounding factors such as different antipsychotic mono-therapies might be biased the results, even though the available antipsychotics do not have a clear effect on cognition. Third gender imbalance in our sample might have influenced the study results. In contrast the naturalistic study design has the advantage to be more adherent with clinical practice. Studies with larger samples and possibly drug-naives could be useful to confirm the
data of this article.
Conclusion
Stabilized schizophrenia patients show marked cognitive impairment especially regarding memory and social sensitivity. Early age at onset is associated with even a more compromised neuropsychological state with worse outcomes on executive functioning. Female gender appears to be associated with a poorer social cognitive functioning and cannabis abuse with impairment in verbal fluency and executive functioning.
Acknowledgment
This research did not receive any funding source.
Conflict of Interest
The authors do not have conflicts of interest to disclose
References
- 1.Bergh S, Hjorthøj C, Sørensen HJ, Fagerlund B, Austin S, Secher RG, Jepsen JR, Nordentoft M. Predictors and longitudinal course of cognitive functioning in schizophrenia spectrum disorders, 10 years after baseline: The OPUS study. Schizophr Res. 2016;175:57–63. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2016.03.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Gur R. Neuropsychiatric aspects of schizophrenia. CNS Neurosci Ther. 2011;17:45–51. doi: 10.1111/j.1755-5949.2010.00220.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Heinrichs RW, Zakzanis KK. Neurocognitive deficit in schizophrenia: a quantitative review of the evidence. Neuropsychology. 1998;12:426–445. doi: 10.1037//0894-4105.12.3.426. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Puig O, Penadés R, Baeza I, Sánchez-Gistau V, De la Serna E, Fonrodona L, Andrés-Perpiñá S, Bernardo M, Castro-Fornieles J. Processing speed and executive functions predict real-world everyday living skills in adolescents with early-onset schizophrenia. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2012;21:315–326. doi: 10.1007/s00787-012-0262-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Schaefer J, Giangrande E, Weinberger DR, Dickinson D. The global cognitive impairment in schizophrenia: consistent over decades and around the world. Schizophr Res. 2013;150:42–50. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2013.07.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Tuulio-Henriksson A, Perälä J, Saarni SI, Isometsä E, Koskinen S, Lönnqvist J, Suvisaari J. Cognitive functioning in severe psychiatric disorders: a general population study. Eur. Arch. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2011;261:447–456. doi: 10.1007/s00406-010-0186-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Cornblatt B, Keilp J. Impaired attention, genetics, and the pathophysiology of schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 1994;20:31–46. doi: 10.1093/schbul/20.1.31. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Kuperberg G, Heckers S. Schizophrenia and cognitive function. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2000;10:205–210. doi: 10.1016/s0959-4388(00)00068-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Green MF, Kern RS, Braff DL, Mintz J. Neurocognitive deficits and functional outcome in schizophrenia: are we measuring the ‘right stuff’? Schizophr Bull. 2000;26:119–136. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a033430. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Green MF, Olivier B, Crawley JN, Penn DL, Silverstein S. Social cognition in schizophrenia: recommendations from the measurement and treatment research to improve cognition in schizophrenia new approaches conference. Schizophr Bull. 2005;31:882–887. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbi049. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.San L, Cuidad A, Álvarez E, Bobes J, Gilaberte I. Symptomatic remission and social/vocational functioning in outpatients with schizophrenia: prevalence and associations in a cross-sectional study. Eur Psychiatry. 2007;22:490–498. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2007.06.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Bellack AS, Sayers M, Mueser KT, Bennet M. Evaluation of social problem solving in schizophrenia. J Abnorm Psychol. 1994;103:371–378. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.103.2.371. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Schmidt SJ, Mueller DR, Roder V. Social cognition as a mediator variable between neurocognition and functional outcome in schizophrenia: empirical review and new results by structural equation modelling. Schizophr Bull. 2011;37:S41–S54. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbr079. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Barch DM, Keefe RS. Anticipating DSM-V: opportunities and challenges for cognition and psychosis. Schizophr Bull. 2010;36:43–47. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbp139. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Heinrichs RW, Goldberg JO, Miles AA, McDermid Vaz S. Predictors of medication competence in schizophrenia patients. Psychiatry Res. 2008;157:47–52. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2007.02.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Bowie CR, Harvey PD. Cognition in schizophrenia: impairments, determinants, and functional importance. Psychiatr Clin North Am. 2005;28:613–633. doi: 10.1016/j.psc.2005.05.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Buoli M, Caldiroli A, Panza G, Altamura AC. Prominent clinical dimension, duration of illness and treatment response in schizophrenia: a naturalistic study. Psychiatry Investig. 2012;9:354–60. doi: 10.4306/pi.2012.9.4.354. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Harvey P, Keefe R. Studies of cognitive change in patients in schizophrenia following novel antipsychotic treatment. Am J Psychiatry. 2001;158:176–184. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.158.2.176. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Palmer B, Dawes S, Heaton R. What do we know about neuropsychological aspects of schizophrenia? Neuropsychol Rev. 2009;19:365–384. doi: 10.1007/s11065-009-9109-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Amminger GP, Edwards J, Brewer WJ, Harrigan S, McGorry PD. Duration of untreated psychosis and cognitive deterioration in first-episode schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2002;54:223–230. doi: 10.1016/s0920-9964(01)00278-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Bruno N, Sachs N, Demily C, Franck N, Pacherie E. Delusions and metacognition in patients with schizophrenia. Cogn Neuropsychiatry. 2012;17:1–18. doi: 10.1080/13546805.2011.562071. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Liu SK, Hsieh MH, Huang TJ, Liu CM, Liu CC, Hua MS, Chen WJ, Hwu HG. Patterns and clinical correlates of neuropsychologic deficits in patients with schizophrenia. J Formos Med Assoc. 2006;105:978–991. doi: 10.1016/S0929-6646(09)60282-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Leeson VC, Harrison I, Ron MA, Barnes TR, Joyce EM. The effect of cannabis use and cognitive reserve on age at onset and psychosis outcomes in first-episode schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 2012;38:873–880. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbq153. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Ringen PA, Vaskinn A, Sundet K, Engh JA, Jónsdóttir H, Simonsen C, Friis S, Opjordsmoen S, Melle I, Andreassen OA. Opposite relationships between cannabis use and neurocognitive functioning in bipolar disorder and schizophrenia. Psychol Med. 2010;40:1337–1347. doi: 10.1017/S0033291709991620. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Rabin RA, Zakzanis KK, George TP. The effect of cannabis use on neurocognition in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. Schizophr Res. 2011;128:111–116. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2011.02.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Kerfoot KE, Rosenheck RA, Petrakis IL, Swartz MS, Keefe RS, McEvoy JP, Stroup TS CATIE Investigators. Substance use and schizophrenia: adverse correlates in the CATIE study sample. Schizophr Res. 2011;132:177–182. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2011.07.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Bowie CR, Serper MR, Riggio S, Harvey PD. Neurocognition, symptomatology, and functional skills in older alcohol-abusing schizophrenia patients. Schizophr Bull. 2005;31:175–182. doi: 10.1093/jschbul/sbi001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Dillon C, Taragano F, Sarasola D, Iturry M, Serrano C, Raczkowski A, Allegri R. Cognitive performance in schizophrenia (paranoid vs residual subtype) Vertex. 2007;18:170–175. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Hill SK, Ragland JD, Gur RC, Gur RE. Neuropsychological differences among empirically derived clinical subtypes of schizophrenia. Neuropsychology. 2001;15:492–501. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Seltzer J, Conrad C, Cassens G. Neuropsychological profiles in schizophrenia: paranoid versus undifferentiated distinctions. Schizophr Res. 1997;23:131–138. doi: 10.1016/S0920-9964(96)00094-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Zalewski C, Johnson-Selfridge MT, Ohriner S, Zarrella K, Seltzer JC. A review of neuropsychological differences between paranoid and nonparanoid schizophrenia patients. Schizophr Bull. 1998;24:127–145. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a033305. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Bajs M, Janović S, Bajs M, Dordević V, Jevtović S, Radonić E, et al. Correlation of cognitive functions with some aspects of illness, treatment and social functioning in recurrently hospitalized schizophrenic patients. Coll Antropol. 2011;35(Suppl 1):39–44. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Lappin JM, Morgan KD, Morgan C, Dazzan P, Reichenberg A, Zanelli , et al. Duration of untreated psychosis and neuropsychological function in first episode psychosis. Schizophr Res. 2007;95:103–110. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2007.05.037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Basso MR, Nasrallah HA, Bornstein RA. Cognitive deficits distinguish patients with adolescent- and adult-onset schizophrenia. Neuropsychiatry Neuropsychol Behav Neurol. 1997;10:107–112. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Ito S, Nemoto T, Tsujino N, Ohmuro N, Matsumoto K, Matsuoka H, Tanaka K, Nishiyama S, Suzuki M, Kinoshita H, Ozawa H, Fujita H, Shimodera S, Kishimoto T, Matsumoto K, Hasegawa T, Mizuno M. Differential impacts of duration of untreated psychosis (DUP) on cognitive function in first-episode schizophrenia according to mode of onset. Eur Psychiatry. 2015;30:995–1001. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2015.08.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Linke M, Jankowski KS, Ciołkiewicz A, Jędrasik-Styła M, Parnowska D, Gruszka A, Denisiuk M, Jarema M, Wichniak A. Age or age at onset? Which of them really matters for neuro and social cognition in schizophrenia? Psychiatry Res. 2015;225:197–201. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2014.11.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Rajji TK, Ismail Z, Mulsant H. Age at onset and cognition in schizophrenia: meta-analysis. Br J Psychiatry. 2009;195:286–293. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.108.060723. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Kravariti E, Morris RG, Rabe-Hesketh S, Murray RM, Frangou S. The Maudsley Early-Onset Schizophrenia Study: cognitive function in adolescent-onset schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2003;65:95–103. doi: 10.1016/s0920-9964(03)00067-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Jeste DV, Harris MJ, Krull A, Kuck J, McAdams LA, Heaton R. Clinical and neuropsychological characteristics of patients with late-onset schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. 1995;152:722–730. doi: 10.1176/ajp.152.5.722. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.First MB, Williams JBW, Karg RS, Spitzer RL. Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-5 Disorders (SCID-5-CV) Washington DC: American Psychiatric Publishing; 2016. [Google Scholar]
- 41.Endicott J, Spitzer RL, Fleiss JL, Cohen J. The Global Assessment scale. A procedure for measuring overall severity of psychiatric disturbance. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1976;33:766–771. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1976.01770060086012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Addington D, Addington J, Schissel B. A depression rating scale for schizophrenics. Schizophr Res. 1990;3:247–251. doi: 10.1016/0920-9964(90)90005-r. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Kay S, Fiszbein A, Opler L. The positive and negative syndrome scale (PANSS) for schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 1987;13:261–276. doi: 10.1093/schbul/13.2.261. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Opler MG, Yang LH, Caleo S, Alberti P. Statistical validation of the criteria for symptom remission in schizophrenia: preliminary findings. BMC Psychiatry. 2007;7:35. doi: 10.1186/1471-244X-7-35. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Anselmetti S, Poletti S, Ermoli E, Bechi M, Cappa S, Venneri A, Smeraldi E, Cavallaro R. The Brief Assessment of Cognition in Schizophrenia. Normative data for the Italian population. Neurol Sci. 2008;29:85–92. doi: 10.1007/s10072-008-0866-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Keefe RS, Goldberg TE, Harvey PD, Gold JM, Poe MP, Coughenour L. The Brief Assessment of Cognition in Schizophrenia: reliability, sensitivity, and comparison with a standard neurocognitive battery. Schizophr Res. 2004;68:283–297. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2003.09.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Sanders F, Smeets-Janssen MM, Meesters PD, van der Vlies AE, Kerssens CJ, Pijnenburg YA. Frontotemporal dementia and schizophrenia in later life: a comparison of executive and general cognitive functioning. Tijdschr Psychiatr. 2012;54:409–417. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Torralva T, Roca M, Gleichgerrcht E, Bekinschte T, Manes F. A neuropsychological battery to detect specific executive and social cognitive impairments in early frontotemporal dementia. Brain. 2009;132:1299–1309. doi: 10.1093/brain/awp041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Burgess P. Development of a simplified version of the multiple errands test for use in hospital settings. Neuropsychol Rehabil. 2002;12:231–255. [Google Scholar]
- 50.Manly T, Hawkins K, Evans J, Woldt K, Robertson IH. Rehabilitation of executive function: facilitation of effective goal management of complex tasks using periodic auditory alerts. Neuropsychologia. 2002;40:271–281. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(01)00094-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Bechara A, Tranel D, Damasio H. Characterization of the decision-making deficit of patients with ventromedial prefrontal cortex lesions. Brain. 2000;123:2189–2202. doi: 10.1093/brain/123.11.2189. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S, Hill J, Raste Y, Plumb I. The "Reading the Mind in the Eyes" Test revised version: a study with normal adults, and adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2001;42:241–251. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Serafin M, Surian L. Il test degli occhi: uno strumento per valutare la “teoria della mente”. Giornale Italiano di Psicopatologia. 2004;31:213–236. [Google Scholar]
- 54.Stone VE, Cohen SB, Knight RT. Frontal lobe contribution to theory of mind. J Cogn Neurosci. 1998;10:640–656. doi: 10.1162/089892998562942. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Ekerholm M, Firus Waltersson S, Fagerberg T, Söderman E, Terenius L, Agartz I, Jönsson EG, Nyman H. Neurocognitive function in long-term treated schizophrenia: a five-year follow-up study. Psychiatry Res. 2012;200:144–152. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2012.05.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Green MF, Bearden CE, Cannon TD, Fiske AP, Hellemann GS, Horan WP, Kee K, Kern RS, Lee J, Sergi MJ, Subotnik KL, Sugar CA, Ventura J, Yee CM, Nuechterlein KH. Social cognition in schizophrenia, Part 1: performance across phase of illness. Schizophr Bull. 2012;38:854–864. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbq171. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Stober G, Ben-Shachar D, Cardon M, Falkai P, Fonteh AN, Gawlik M, Glenthoj BY, Grunblatt E, Jablensky A, Kim YK, Kornhuber J, McNeil TF, Muller N, Oranje B, Saito T, Saoud M, Schmitt A, Schwartz M, Thome J, Uzbekov M, Durany N, Riederer P. Schizophrenia: from the brain to peripheral markers. A consensus paper of the WFSBP task force on biological markers. World J Biol Psychiatry. 2009;10:127–155. doi: 10.1080/15622970902898980. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Remschmidt H, Theisen F. Early-onset schizophrenia. Neuropsychobiology. 2012;66:63–69. doi: 10.1159/000338548. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Arango C, Moreno C, Martínez S, Parellada M, Desco M, Moreno D, et al. Longitudinal brain changes in early-onset psychosis. Schizophr Bull. 2008;34:341–353. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbm157. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Arango C, Rapado-Castro M, Reig S, Castro-Fornieles J, González-Pinto A, et al. Progressive brain changes in children and adolescents with first-episode psychosis. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2012;69:16–26. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.150. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Mechelli A, Riecher-Rössler A, Meisenzahl EM, Tognin S, Wood SJ, Borgwardt SJ, et al. Neuroanatomical abnormalities that predate the onset of psychosis: a multicenter study. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2011;68:489–495. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.42. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Reig S, Parellada M, Castro-Fornieles J, Janssen J, Moreno D, Baeza I, et al. Multicenter study of brain volume abnormalities in children and adolescent-onset psychosis. Schizophr Bull. 2011;37:1270–1280. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbq044. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Tang J, Liao Y, Zhou B, Tan C, Liu W, Wang D, et al. Decrease in temporal gyrus gray matter volume in first-episode, early onset schizophrenia: an MRI study. PLoS One. 2012;7:e40247. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0040247. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Altamura AC, Buoli M, Serati M. Duration of illness and duration of untreated illness in relation to drug response in psychiatric disorders. Neuropsychiatry. 2011;1:81–90. [Google Scholar]
- 65.Ochoa S, Usall J, Cobo J, Labad X, Kulkarni J. Gender differences in schizophrenia and first-episode psychosis: a comprehensive literature review. Schizophr Res Treatment. 2012;2012:916198. doi: 10.1155/2012/916198. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Zhang XY, Chen DC, Xiu MH, Yang FD, Haile CN, Kosten TA, et al. Gender differences in never-medicated first-episode schizophrenia and medicated chronic schizophrenia patients. J Clin Psychiatry. 2012;73:1025–1033. doi: 10.4088/JCP.11m07422. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Karilampi U, Helldin L, Archer T. Cognition and global assessment of functioning in male and female outpatients with schizophrenia spectrum disorders. J Nerv Ment Dis. 2011;199:445–448. doi: 10.1097/NMD.0b013e318221413e. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Yücel M, Bora E, Lubman DI, Solowij N, Brewer WJ, Cotton SM, et al. The impact of cannabis use on cognitive functioning in patients with schizophrenia: a meta-analysis of existing findings and new data in a first-episode sample. Schizophr Bull. 2012;38:316–330. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbq079. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Peer J, Bennett ME, Bellack AS. Neurocognitive characteristics of individuals with schizophrenia and cocaine dependence: comparison of currently dependent and remitted groups. J Nerv Ment Dis. 2009;197:631–634. doi: 10.1097/NMD.0b013e3181b08bf4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]