Skip to main content
NeuroImage : Clinical logoLink to NeuroImage : Clinical
. 2018 Jun 19;20:35–41. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2018.06.024

Resting-state fMRI reveals network disintegration during delirium

Simone JT van Montfort a,, Edwin van Dellen b,c, Aletta MR van den Bosch a,d, Willem M Otte e,f, Maya JL Schutte b, Soo-Hee Choi g, Tae-Sub Chung h, Sunghyon Kyeong i, Arjen JC Slooter a, Jae-Jin Kim i,⁎⁎
PMCID: PMC6037687  PMID: 29998059

Abstract

Delirium is characterized by inattention and other cognitive deficits, symptoms that have been associated with disturbed interactions between remote brain regions. Recent EEG studies confirm that disturbed global network topology may underlie the syndrome, but lack an anatomical basis. The aim of this study was to increase our understanding of the global organization of functional connectivity during delirium and to localize possible alterations. Resting-state fMRI data from 44 subjects were analyzed, and motion-free data were available in nine delirious patients, seven post delirium patients and thirteen non-delirious clinical controls. We focused on the functional network backbones using the minimum spanning tree, which allows unbiased network comparisons. During delirium a longer diameter (mean (M) = 0.30, standard deviation (SD) = 0.05, P = .024) and a lower leaf fraction (M = 0.32, SD = 0.03, P = .027) was found compared to the control group (M = 0.28, SD = 0.04 respectively M = 0.35, SD = 0.03), suggesting reduced functional network integration and efficiency. Delirium duration was strongly related to loss of network hierarchy (rho = −0.92, P = .001). Connectivity strength was decreased in the post delirium group (M = 0.16, SD = 0.01) compared to the delirium group (M = 0.17, SD = 0.03, P = .024) and the control group (M = 0.19, SD = 0.02, P = .001). Permutation tests revealed a decreased degree of the right posterior cingulate cortex during delirium and complex regional alterations after delirium. These findings indicate that delirium reflects disintegration of functional interactions between remote brain areas and suggest long-term impact after the syndrome resolves.

Keywords: Delirium, fMRI, Resting-state, Brain networks, Functional connectivity, Minimum spanning tree

Graphical abstract

Unlabelled Image

Highlights

  • Resting state fMRI network organization was evaluated during and after delirium

  • Integration and efficiency of the network were reduced during delirium

  • Network alterations were strongly correlated with delirium duration

  • Hub function of the right PCC was impaired in delirious patients

  • After delirium connectivity strength decreased and regional differences occurred

1. Introduction

Delirium is a common and serious problem affecting >15% of the hospitalized elderly patients (Marcantonio, 2017). This acute neuropsychiatric syndrome is a direct consequence of another medical condition, and is predominantly characterized by decreased attention and altered awareness with other cognitive disturbances (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). Delirium is related to poor outcomes such as prolonged length of hospital stay (Brooks, 2012; Pompei et al., 1994) and long-term cognitive impairment (Pandharipande et al., 2013). Previous studies suggested neurotransmitter imbalances, an aberrant stress response, and persistent neuroinflammation as potential underlying mechanisms (Zaal et al., 2015), but the pathogenesis remains poorly understood.

As adequate cognitive functioning requires interaction or functional connectivity between remote brain regions (Bullmore & Sporns, 2009; Stam & van Straaten, 2012), a recent hypothesis stated that delirium is a disconnection syndrome (van Dellen et al., 2014; Numan et al., 2017) caused by breakdown of functional brain networks (Sanders, 2011). In previous electroencephalography (EEG) studies, we found that delirious patients have a decrease of alpha band (8–13 Hz) global functional connectivity strength, and a more random, less integrated network organization compared to control patients without delirium (van Dellen et al., 2014; Numan et al., 2017). This was based however on scalp EEG-registrations only, which has low spatial resolution.

Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) – which maps the entire brain rather than only the surface – could potentially provide important additional information on the anatomical basis of these altered network organization. However, due to the symptomatology of delirium it is very challenging to acquire fMRI scans from delirious patients. Nonetheless, one fMRI study comparing delirious, post delirious and comparison subjects has been performed, showing abnormal interactions between brain areas involved in attention and awareness in delirious patients.(Choi et al., 2012). Activity of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and the posterior cingulate cortex was positively correlated in delirium patients, whereas a negative correlation was found in comparison participants. Yet this study was focusing on a limited set of a priori defined brain regions.

Since recent findings suggest that delirium may be an expression of a disrupted global network organization, it would be relevant to extend these finding into a more globally oriented study on network organization. Recent findings strongly suggest that delirium and related cognitive alterations are associated with global network modifications rather than specific regional changes (van Dellen et al., 2014; Numan et al., 2017; Stam, 2014; van Dellen et al., 2015). To understand the nature of delirium it is therefore crucial to also map the global network changes.

Traditional graph analysis has however methodological challenges, including a significant bias in most group comparisons (Fornito et al., 2013; Stam et al., 2014; van Wijk et al., 2010). A robust network backbone is captured by the minimum spanning tree (MST). MST analysis has recently been shown to capture clinically relevant network changes in challenging acquisition conditions (Stam et al., 2014; Tewarie et al., 2015). MSTs connect all nodes with the highest possible weights without forming loops. Therefore, the MST always consists of a fixed number of connections, which avoids the methodological bias of spurious connections or arbitrary thresholding in group comparisons of network topology.

The aim of this study was to increase our understanding of the global organization of functional connectivity after transition to and recovery from delirium, and relate possible alterations to anatomical regions. Secondly, we aimed to explore how network alterations relate to delirium severity measures. We hypothesized that the functional brain network during delirium is less efficient and disintegrated network that partly restores after recovery from delirium.

2. Methods

2.1. Patient population and clinical assessment

The study protocol was approved by the Institutional Review Board of Yonsei University and informed consent was signed, either given by the participant or a caretaker (Choi et al., 2012).

Twenty two delirious patients were recruited from the Gangnam Severance Hospital at Yonsei University in Seoul, Korea. Delirium was screened daily and diagnosed according to DSM-IV criteria for delirium by trained psychiatrists using the Memorial Delirium Assessment Scale and the Delirium Rating Scale-Revised-98 (DRS-R-98) (Breitbart et al., 1997; Trzepacz et al., 2001). Scans obtained from delirium patients included an anatomical T1-weighted scan and a resting-state BOLD fMRI scan. The day after recovery from delirium (i.e. a score of <10 on the Memorial Delirium Assessment Scale or a score of <15 on the DRS-R-98), a follow-up scan was made of thirteen of these patients using the same scan protocol. The total number of days a patient was diagnosed as delirious was used as a score for delirium duration. The DRS-R-98 score on the day of MRI scanning was used as a measure of severity of delirium. Only low-dose antipsychotics were administered in the delirium and post delirium groups, the dose of antipsychotics was similar in both conditions. The causes of delirium in our sample were multifactorial and mostly included systemic or metabolic disease.

A control group of 22 non-delirious clinical controls, matched on age, sex and extent of leukoaraiosis, was recruited from the Databank for Brain Imaging at Gangnam Severance Hospital. The data bank included functional and structural brain MR images of inpatients or outpatients who had various medical or neurological conditions, who were scanned with the same MR sequences as the patients with delirium and who consented to provide the data.

Exclusion criteria for all groups were a history of cognitive decline such as dementia, a history of seizure or traumatic brain injury, or previously identified focal lesions larger than 3  cm. Since motion artifacts can influence the fMRI data and cause spurious results (Power et al., 2015; Pruim et al., 2015a; Satterthwaite et al., 2013), a strict motion correction was performed (see additional motion correction paragraph below). After motion correction, our sample consisted of nine delirium patients, of whom seven patients were scanned post delirium and thirteen controls.

2.2. Image acquisition

High-resolution anatomical images were obtained using a spoiled gradient-echo sequence (matrix = 512 × 512, echo time (TE) = 1.7 msec, repetition time (TR) = 7.0 msec, field of view = 210 mm, slice thickness = 1.2 mm, flip angle = 20°, number of slices = 240) to serve as an anatomical reference. Functional images were obtained over 400 s using gradient-echo echo-planar imaging sequences in a Signa EXCITE 3-T MR system (GE, Milwaukee; matrix = 64 × 64, TE = 17.6 msec, TR = 2500 msec, field of view = 240 mm, slice thickness = 3 mm, flip angle = 90°, number of slices = 50). All participants were instructed to rest with their eyes closed during each scan.

2.3. Preprocessing

Image preprocessing was performed using the FMRIB's Software Library (FSL) (Jenkinson et al., 2012; Smith et al., 2004; Woolrich et al., 2009). The brain was automatically extracted from the T1-weighted scan (Smith, 2002). Time series were motion corrected with MCFLIRT.(Jenkinson et al., 2002; Jenkinson & Smith, 2001). This removes subject's head movement and allows calculation of the mean relative displacement. Participants with a mean relative displacement larger than 0.2 mm were excluded (Power et al., 2012). Six motion components were extracted, i.e. three voxel-wise displacement parameters and the temporal signal intensities obtained from white matter, cerebrospinal fluid, and global time courses (Satterthwaite et al., 2013). The functional scan was registered to the high-resolution anatomical image by using rigid registration. The anatomical scan was subsequently matched with the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) 152 T1-weighted 2 mm image in standard space with affine registration. Functional scans were slice-time corrected, spatial smoothed to reduce noise (5 mm full-width-half-maximum). The first 15 volumes were deleted to ensure stabilized magnetization. Average time series from the cerebral spinal fluid, the white matter and grey matter intensities were determined after tissue segmentation Tool (FAST) (Zhang et al., 2001).

2.4. Additional motion correction

It has been recognized that motion can have large impact on the resting-state fMRI signal and can induce systematically bias inference (Power et al., 2015; Satterthwaite et al., 2013; Power et al., 2012; Pruim et al., 2015b; Ciric et al., 2017). The standard FSL pipeline is not sufficient to remove motion artifacts from the data, therefore additional motion correction is necessary (Power et al., 2015; Satterthwaite et al., 2013; Power et al., 2012; Pruim et al., 2015b; Ciric et al., 2017). Out of two additional motion correction methods, i.e. independent component analysis-based strategy for Automatic Removal of Motion Artifacts (ICA-AROMA) (Pruim et al., 2015a; Pruim et al., 2015b) and spike regression (Satterthwaite et al., 2013; Power et al., 2012), we selected the most effective one in controlling motion effects in our dataset for final correction, i.e. spike regression (see Supplementary Fig. 1). Volumes that exceeded the threshold of 0.2 mm framewise displacement (Power et al., 2014) were removed and a regression analysis with 24 motion components was done. Motion components were: three voxel-wise displacement parameters and their white matter, cerebrospinal fluid, global time courses, the quadrates, temporal derivatives and quadrates of the derivatives of these six parameters (Yan et al., 2013). A band-pass filter (0.01–0.08 Hz) was applied (Satterthwaite et al., 2013). If the remaining data was <300 s, the subject was excluded from further analysis (Birn et al., 2013).

2.5. Global network analysis

We selected 90 regions from the AAL brain atlas (V4) that cover the cortical and subcortical regions (Tzourio-Mazoyer et al., 2002). To estimate 90 regional mean time series, voxel time series within each AAL region were averaged. A correlation network was constructed for every participant, based on those time series using Pearson's correlation. Only positive correlations were taken into account as a result of the MST analysis, thus avoiding the problematic interpretation of negative BOLD correlations (Stam et al., 2014; Tewarie et al., 2015). Mean connectivity strength of the network was calculated per subject. MST network backbones were extracted with Kruskal's algorithm (Kruskal Jr., 1956) (MATLAB, version R2016b). For each MST the diameter, kappa, tree hierarchy and leaf fraction were calculated (see Table 1 for the definitions and explanation (Tewarie et al., 2015)). See Fig. 1 for an explanation on MST structure.

Table 1.

Definition and explanation of the MST outcomes.

Outcome Definition Explanation
Global measures
Diameter Number of edges connecting the most remote nodes in the MST Gives an indication of the efficiency of global network organization. In a network with a low diameter, information is efficiently processed between remote brain regions.
Kappa Measure of the broadness of the degree distribution Related to resilience against attacks, epidemic spreading and the synchronizability of complex networks.
Tree hierarchy Quantifies hierarchy as the trade-off between large scale integration in the MST and the overload of central nodes This measure was defined to the hypothesized optimal topology of the brain, where information is transferred between nodes in the fewest possible steps, while preventing information overload of central nodes.
Leaf fraction Fraction of leaf nodes in the MST: a leaf node is defined as a node with only one connection Describes to what extend the network has a central, integrated organization. A high leaf fraction indicates that the network is largely dependent on central nodes.



Regional measures
Degree Number of edges for a given node Reflects the importance of a node in the network. A node with a high degree is a more important node in the network.
Betweenness centrality Fraction of all shortest paths that pass through a particular node Betweenness centrality ranges between 0 (leaf node) and 1 (central node in a star-like network).

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

Schematic representation of a line-like, hierarchically tree structure and star-like network. Each network type has 7 nodes and 6 edges. Leaf nodes, the blue nodes in the figure, are nodes that only have one edge and the diameter is the length of the path between the two nodes that are furthest apart. The orange nodes in the figure have a high betweenness centrality. (A) Line-like network: few leaf nodes + long diameter, (B) hierarchical tree structure, (C) star-like network: many leaf nodes + short diameter + a central node with a high betweenness centrality.

2.6. Regional network analysis

To localize possible alterations in network organization to anatomical regions, regional network characteristics were analyzed. Since global network characteristics were analyzed based on the MST, we also focused on MST characteristics for regional analysis, i.e. MST degree and MST betweenness centrality were used (Tewarie et al., 2015) (Table 1).

2.7. Statistical analyses

The mean connectivity strength and the MST outcomes leaf fraction, diameter, kappa and tree hierarchy were compared between i) delirium and controls, ii) post delirium and controls, and iii) delirium and post delirium. For these analyses a bootstrapped linear mixed models with 'group' (i.e. delirium, post delirium, control) as independent variable and ‘subject’ as random variable were used. This approach was corrected for within-subject correlation allowing calculation of robust 95% confidence intervals of group differences. Modeling was done within the R statistical software (version 3) with the lmeresampler package. A False Discovery Rate correction was used to control for multiple comparisons. As exploratory analysis, spearman's correlation analyses were conducted to evaluate the association between the five global MST network outcomes (of the delirium group) and the duration or severity of delirium. These exploratory analyses were not corrected for multiple comparisons.

Regional MST characteristics were tested between groups (P < .05), by using permutation tests in Matlab (Monte Carlo 2-sided test, 1000 permutations), corrected for multiple comparisons (Groppe et al., 2011). Nodal degree and nodal betweenness centrality of all 90 regions were tested for group differences between i) delirium and controls, ii) post delirium and controls, and iii) delirium and post delirium.

3. Results

3.1. Demographics

Table 2 shows the demographic and clinical data of the delirium, post delirium and control group. The delirium and control group were comparable on age, sex and extent of leukoaraiosis. The average duration of an episode of delirium was 9.00 days (standard deviation (SD) 2.73). After spike regression, no differences were found in motion, i.e. mean frame-wise displacement, between the three groups (F = 1.35, P = .60). No correlation was found between our network outcome measures and motion, i.e. mean relative displacement (see Supplementary Fig. 2).

Table 2.

Demographic and clinical characteristics of delirium patients, the post delirium patients and comparison subjects.

Delirium (N = 9) Post delirium (N = 7) Control (N = 13) P-valuea
Age (years) 75.56 (6.88) 75.43 (8.00) 72.69 (6.65) 0.34
Gender (male) 55.6% (N = 4) 85.7% (N = 6) 46.2% (N = 6) 0.68
PVH (range 0–6) 2.44 (1.33) 2.14 (1.22) 1.46 (1.39) 0.11
DWMH (range 0–24) 7.44 (6.32) 4.71 (5.90) 7.85 (5.47) 0.88
Focal lesions
(part of group)
55.6% (N = 4) 28.6% (N = 2) 30.8% (N = 4) 0.54
MDAS score 15.44 (3.61) 5.00 (2.83)

Mean (SD) is shown.

a

Statistical difference between delirium and control group. Abbreviations: PVH = periventricular hyperintensities, DWMH = deep white matter hyperintensities, MDAS = Memorial Delirium Assessment Scale.

3.2. Global network organization

Fig. 2 shows boxplots of the overall connectivity strength and MST outcomes diameter, kappa, leaf and tree hierarchy for the delirium, post delirium and control group. Mean network outcomes are displayed in Supplementary Table 1. Connectivity strength was significantly decreased in the post delirium group (M: 0.16, SD: 0.01) compared to the control group (M: 0.19, SD: 0.02) with a difference of −0.04 (95% CI -0.05 – -0.02, corrected P = .001) and compared to the delirium group (M: 0.17, SD: 0.03) with a difference of −0.02 (95% CI -0.02–0.00, corrected P = .027). Diameter was significantly increased during delirium (M: 0.30, SD: 0.05) compared to the control group (M: 0.28, SD: 0.04) with a difference of 0.04 (95% CI -0.01–0.08, corrected P = .024). Leaf fraction was significantly decreased during delirium (M: 0.32, SD: 0.03) compared to the control group (M: 0.35, SD: 0.03), with a difference of −0.02 (95% CI -0.04–0.02, corrected P = .027).

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2

Overview of global network results comparing the delirium group, post delirium group and control group. (A) Boxplots of overall connectivity strength and minimum spanning tree outcomes diameter, kappa, leaf and tree hierarchy for the control, delirium and post delirium group. A significant difference was found in connectivity strength between the control and the post delirium group and between the delirium and the post delirium group. A significant difference was found in diameter and leaf fraction between the delirium and the control group. (B)* A significant negative correlation was found between delirium duration and tree hierarchy. (C)* A significant negative correlation was found between delirium duration and leaf fraction. Abbreviations: M = mean, SD = standard deviation, leaf = leaf fraction, th = tree hierarchy, C = control group, D = delirium group, PD = post delirium group. *Note that data on these variables is only shown for subjects included in the delirium group.

In order to increase the comparability with previous studies, we additionally calculated classical graph measures, i.e. clustering coefficient, density and path length from the weighted connectivity matrix (for definitions see Supplementary Table 2). This resulted in similar results as our MST findings, however these analyses additionally showed decreased clustering, decreased density and increased path length in the post delirium group (Supplementary Fig. 3).

We found significant negative correlations between delirium duration and leaf fraction (rho = −0.73, P = .039), and between delirium duration and tree hierarchy (rho = −0.92, P = .001) (Fig. 2). No significant correlations were found between delirium severity and the global MST network measures.

3.3. Regional network organization

We performed permutation analyses to test for possible alterations of the nodal characteristics degree and betweenness centrality. We found that the degree of the right posterior cingulate cortex was lower in the delirium group compared to the control group (corrected P = .039) (Fig. 3).

Fig. 3.

Fig. 3

Visualization of the mean degree in the minimum spanning tree network of the delirium, post delirium and the control group. The anatomical labeling atlas with 90 regions was used. The size of the nodes corresponds to the degree. Red nodes mark regions with a significant group difference in degree. The degree of the right posterior cingulate was significantly lower in the delirium group compared to the control group.

Several differences in regional betweenness centrality were found between groups (Supplementary Fig. 5). Betweenness centrality of the right inferior temporal gyrus was lower in the delirium group compared to the control group (corrected P = .004). Betweenness centrality of the left anterior cingulum and the right pallidum were lower in the post delirium group compared to the control group (both corrected P = .016). Betweenness centrality of the orbital part of the right middle frontal gyrus, the right medial orbitofrontal cortex and the left anterior cingulate were lower in the delirium group compared to the post delirium group (corrected P = .030, corrected P = .016, corrected P = .031, resp.).

4. Discussion

This study shows a less efficient, disintegrated network during delirium, which was in accordance with our hypothesis. More profound network disintegration was strongly associated with longer duration of delirium, suggesting that this mechanism is pivotal in the pathophysiology of the syndrome. Connectivity strength was declined after recovery of delirium. In addition, several differences between regional network characteristics were found after delirium was resolved, including a lower degree of the right posterior cingulate cortex and a lower centrality of the right inferior temporal gyrus during delirium and complex regional alterations after delirium. Taken together, these findings suggest long-term impact of delirium on functional connectivity and brain network organization after remission of the syndrome.

Our findings of decreased network integration during delirium are in line with previous EEG graph theoretical studies (van Dellen et al., 2014; Numan et al., 2017). The loss of network integration and efficiency may reflect the underlying neural mechanism for the cognitive dysfunction during the syndrome. Interestingly, the transition to a more path-like network during delirium is similar to findings in MST studies on dementia (van Dellen et al., 2015; Yu et al., 2016). Previous EEG studies additionally suggested a change in connectivity strength during delirium (van Dellen et al., 2014; Numan et al., 2017). In the present study we found a similar trend, but this did not reach statistical significance. This may be explained by our small sample size. Another possibility may be that fMRI is less sensitive than EEG in picking up functional connectivity strength alterations during delirium. Nevertheless, we did find a difference in connectivity strength between the post delirium group and the control group, and between the post delirium and the delirium group. Delirium is associated with long-term cognitive decline (Pandharipande et al., 2013; Inouye et al., 2016; Wolters et al., 2017), while decreased functional connectivity was related to cognitive impairment in previous work (van Dellen et al., 2015; Yu et al., 2016). We speculate that an irreversible decrease of connectivity strength due to delirium may be an underlying pathophysiological mechanism of this association between delirium and cognitive decline.

Although classical graph analyses showed persisting organizational changes after delirium was resolved, MST network analyses did not. The difference might be due to the strong dependency of classical graph measures on connectivity strength, which makes the interpretation of the classical graph measures difficult (van Wijk et al., 2010). As simulation studies suggest that the MST overcomes this bias (Tewarie et al., 2015), we will focus on the MST findings in the evaluation of network alteration after delirium. Interestingly, we found a large variation in post delirium patients in MST outcomes that were affected during delirium. This may be due to the smaller size of the group, but could also indicate heterogeneity. Considering that delirium is associated with long-term cognitive impairment (Pandharipande et al., 2013; Inouye et al., 2016; Wolters et al., 2017), it could be that the sub-group of delirious patients who will develop this long-term problems shows remaining alterations in global brain organization, while the patients without lasting problems will recover from global brain organization disturbances. In line with this hypothesis, we observed that network integration was negatively associated with delirium duration. The post delirium patients also showed regional network alterations compared to the control group, which may reflect (more localized) long-term network impairments.

This fMRI study showed a lower nodal MST degree, i.e. number of connections, in the right posterior cingulate cortex during delirium. The posterior cingulate cortex is an important structure in the default mode network and previous studies have shown impaired activity in this region in patient groups with disorders of cognition, attention or consciousness (Leech & Sharp, 2014). A previous fMRI study on network alteration during delirium in a priori chosen brain regions, showed an abnormal interaction between the posterior cingulate cortex and the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, i.e. regions involved in attention and cognition, which resolved after delirium. The lower degree in the right posterior cingulate cortex might reflect loss of hub function of this region, related to a development of long-term cognitive impairment.

We observed that betweenness centrality of post delirium patients was lower in a part of the default mode network, i.e. the middle prefrontal cortex, and in regions involved in emotional and cognitive processes, i.e. the orbitofrontal cortex, anterior cingulate and globus pallidus. These regions are not known in the literature as hub regions (van den Heuvel & Sporns, 2013). We would therefore not expect a high betweenness centrality, which makes it difficult to interpret what the importance of a lower betweenness centrality in the post delirium group is. The complex regional alterations might reflect a process of neural plasticity in order to recover from delirium, or might be an indication of lasting impairment related to the vulnerability for negative outcomes after delirium.

Strengths of our study were the strict methodology, using groups matched on age, sex and the extent of leukoaraiosis, strict motion correction and bias limiting MST network comparisons. Furthermore, we had access to a unique dataset of fMRI scans during delirium, which is very challenging to perform.

This study also has several limitations. It is difficult to instruct delirious, sometimes restless, patients, to undergo resting-state fMRI measurements. Therefore, the reliability of the ‘resting-state’ may not be assured in this study. Due to the limited scanning time and the need for strict motion correction for global brain network analysis, a considerable part of our study population had to be excluded. It can be hypothesized that the most restless patients that needed to be excluded were the patients with most severe delirium. This would have resulted in an underestimation of the true effect. Samples and preprocessing steps may differ from the previous study on this dataset (Choi et al., 2012), due to recent improvements in processing of resting-state fMRI data for connectivity analysis. Another limitation might be that antipsychotic medication could have influenced our results (Roder et al., 2010). It should however be noted that the use of antipsychotics was equally distributed between the delirium and post delirium scans and that only low-dose antipsychotics were administered in both scan conditions. Future research should focus on global and regional network studies during delirium, with larger sample sizes and follow-up measurements to evaluate the long-term cognitive effects of network changes due to delirium.

5. Conclusion

This fMRI study revealed a disintegrated, less efficient resting-state network during delirium, which correlates with the duration of the disorder, and loss of hub function of the right posterior cingulate cortex. Connectivity strength was declined after delirium was resolved. Additional complex regional alterations were shown after delirium, indicating a process of persistent vulnerability or ongoing recovery. These findings provide further evidence that delirium reflects a disintegration of functional interactions between remote brain areas and indicate long-term impact after remission of the syndrome.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by a grant from the Korea Health Technology R&D Project through the Korea Health Industry Development Institute (KHIDI), funded by the Ministry of Health & Welfare, Republic of Korea (grant number: HI16C0132).

Footnotes

Appendix A

Supplementary data to this article can be found online at https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nicl.2018.06.024.

Contributor Information

Simone J.T. van Montfort, Email: s.j.t.vanmontfort-2@umcutrecht.nl.

Jae-Jin Kim, Email: jaejkim@yonsei.ac.kr.

Appendix A. Supplementary data

Supplementary material

mmc1.docx (329.6KB, docx)

References

  1. American Psychiatric Association . American Psychiatric Association; 2013. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. [Google Scholar]
  2. Birn R.M., Molloy E.K., Patriat R., Parker T., Meier T.B., Kirk G.R., Nair V.A., Meyerand M.E., Prabhakaran V. The effect of scan length on the reliability of resting-state fMRI connectivity estimates. NeuroImage. 2013;83:550–558. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.099. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Breitbart W., Rosenfeld B., Roth A., Smith M.J., Cohen K., Passik S. The memorial delirium assessment scale. J. Pain Symptom Manag. 1997;13(3):128–137. doi: 10.1016/s0885-3924(96)00316-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Brooks P.B. Postoperative delirium in elderly patients. AJN, Am J Nurs. 2012;112(9):38–49. doi: 10.1097/01.NAJ.0000418922.53224.36. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bullmore E., Sporns O. Complex brain networks: graph theoretical analysis of structural and functional systems. Nat Rev Neurosci Nat Rev Neurosci. 2009;10(3):186–198. doi: 10.1038/nrn2575. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Choi S.H., Lee H., Chung T.S. Neural network functional connectivity during and after an episode of delirium. Am. J. Psychiatry. 2012;169(5):498–507. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2012.11060976. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Ciric R., Wolf D.H., Power J.D. Benchmarking of participant-level confound regression strategies for the control of motion artifact in studies of functional connectivity. NeuroImage. 2017;154:174–187. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.03.020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Fornito A., Zalesky A., Breakspear M. Graph analysis of the human connectome: promise, progress, and pitfalls. NeuroImage. 2013;80:426–444. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.04.087. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Groppe D.M., Urbach T.P., Kutas M. Mass univariate analysis of event-related brain potentials/fields I: a critical tutorial review. Psychophysiology. 2011;48(12):1711–1725. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2011.01273.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Inouye S.K., Marcantonio E.R., Kosar C.M. The short-term and long-term relationship between delirium and cognitive trajectory in older surgical patients. Alzheimers Dement. 2016;12(7):766–775. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2016.03.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Jenkinson M., Smith S. A global optimisation method for robust affine registration of brain images. Med. Image Anal. 2001;5(2):143–156. doi: 10.1016/s1361-8415(01)00036-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Jenkinson M., Bannister P., Brady M., Smith S. Improved optimization for the robust and accurate linear registration and motion correction of brain images. NeuroImage. 2002;17(2):825–841. doi: 10.1016/s1053-8119(02)91132-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Jenkinson M., Beckmann C.F., Behrens T.E.J., Woolrich M.W., Smith S.M. FSL. Neuroimage. 2012;62(2):782–790. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.09.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Kruskal J.B., Jr. On the shortest spanning subtree of a graph and the traveling salesman problem. Proc. Am. Math. Soc. 1956;7(1):48. [Google Scholar]
  15. Leech R., Sharp D.J. The role of the posterior cingulate cortex in cognition and disease. Brain. 2014;137(1):12–32. doi: 10.1093/brain/awt162. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Marcantonio E.R. Delirium in hospitalized older adults. Solomon CG, ed. N. Engl. J. Med. 2017;377(15):1456–1466. doi: 10.1056/NEJMcp1605501. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Numan T., Slooter A.J.C., van der Kooi A.W. Functional connectivity and network analysis during hypoactive delirium and recovery from anesthesia. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2017;128(6):914–924. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2017.02.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Pandharipande P.P., Girard T.D., Jackson J.C. Long-term cognitive impairment after critical illness. N. Engl. J. Med. 2013;369(14):1306–1316. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa1301372. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Pompei P., Foreman M., Rudberg M.A., Inouye S.K., Braund V., Cassel C.K. Delirium in hospitalized older persons: outcomes and predictors. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 1994;42(8):809–815. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.1994.tb06551.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Power J.D., Barnes K.A., Snyder A.Z., Schlaggar B.L., Petersen S.E. Spurious but systematic correlations in functional connectivity MRI networks arise from subject motion. NeuroImage. 2012;59(3):2142–2154. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.10.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Power J.D., Mitra A., Laumann TO, Snyder A.Z., Schlaggar B.L., Petersen S.E. Methods to detect, characterize, and remove motion artifact in resting state fMRI. NeuroImage. 2014;84:320–341. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.08.048. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Power J.D., Schlaggar B.L., Petersen S.E. Recent progress and outstanding issued in motion correction resting state fmri. NeuroImage. 2015;105:536–551. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.10.044. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Pruim R.H.R., Mennes M., van Rooij D., Llera A., Buitelaar J.K., Beckmann C.F. ICA-AROMA: a robust ICA-based strategy for removing motion artifacts from fMRI data. NeuroImage. 2015;112:267–277. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.02.064. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Pruim R.H.R., Mennes M., Buitelaar J.K., Beckmann C.F. Evaluation of ICA-AROMA and alternative strategies for motion artifact removal in resting state fMRI. NeuroImage. 2015;112:278–287. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.02.063. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Roder H.C., Marie Hoogendam J.M., van der Veen F. FMRI, antipsychotics and schizophrenia. Influence of different antipsychotics on BOLD-signal. Curr. Pharm. Des. 2010;16(18):2012–2025. doi: 10.2174/138161210791293088. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Sanders R.D. Hypothesis for the pathophysiology of delirium: role of baseline brain network connectivity and changes in inhibitory tone. Med. Hypotheses. 2011;77(1):140–143. doi: 10.1016/j.mehy.2011.03.048. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Satterthwaite T.D., Elliott M.A., Gerraty R.T. An improved framework for confound regression and filtering for control of motion artifact in the preprocessing of resting-state functional connectivity data. NeuroImage. 2013;64:240–256. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.08.052. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Smith S.M. Fast robust automated brain extraction. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2002;17(3):143–155. doi: 10.1002/hbm.10062. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Smith S.M., Jenkinson M., Woolrich M.W. Advances in functional and structural MR image analysis and implementation as FSL. NeuroImage. 2004;23:S208–S219. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.07.051. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Stam C.J. Modern network science of neurological disorders. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2014;15(10):683–695. doi: 10.1038/nrn3801. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Stam C.J., van Straaten E.C.W. The organization of physiological brain networks. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2012;123(6):1067–1087. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2012.01.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Stam C.J., Tewarie P., Van Dellen E., van Straaten E.C.W., Hillebrand A., Van Mieghem P. The trees and the forest: characterization of complex brain networks with minimum spanning trees. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 2014;92(3):129–138. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2014.04.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Tewarie P., van Dellen E., Hillebrand A., Stam C.J. The minimum spanning tree: an unbiased method for brain network analysis. NeuroImage. 2015;104:177–188. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.10.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Trzepacz P.T., Mittal D., Torres R., Kanary K., Norton J., Jimerson N. Validation of the delirium rating scale-Revised-98. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2001;13(2):229–242. doi: 10.1176/jnp.13.2.229. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Tzourio-Mazoyer N., Landeau B., Papathanassiou D. Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical Parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain. NeuroImage. 2002;15(1):273–289. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0978. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. van Dellen E., van der Kooi A.W., Numan T. Decreased functional connectivity and disturbed directionality of information flow in the electroencephalography of intensive care unit patients with delirium after cardiac surgery. Anesthesiology. 2014;121(2):328–335. doi: 10.1097/ALN.0000000000000329. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. van Dellen E., de Waal H., van der Flier W.M. Loss of EEG network efficiency is related to cognitive impairment in dementia with Lewy bodies. Mov. Disord. 2015;30(13):1785–1793. doi: 10.1002/mds.26309. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. van den Heuvel M., Sporns O. Network hubs in the human brain. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2013;17(12):683–696. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.09.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. van Wijk B.C.M., Stam C.J., Daffertshofer A., Stam C., Apkarian A. Comparing brain networks of different size and connectivity density using graph theory. PLoS One. 2010;5(10):e13701. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0013701. Sporns O, ed. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Wolters A.E., Peelen L.M., Veldhuijzen D.S. Long-term self-reported cognitive problems after delirium in the intensive care unit and the effect of systemic inflammation. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 2017;65(4):786–791. doi: 10.1111/jgs.14660. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Woolrich M.W., Jbabdi S., Patenaude B. Bayesian analysis of neuroimaging data in FSL. NeuroImage. 2009;45(1):S173–S186. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.10.055. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Yan C.-G., Cheung B., Kelly C. A comprehensive assessment of regional variation in the impact of head micromovements on functional connectomics. NeuroImage. 2013;76:183–201. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.03.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Yu M., Gouw A.A., Hillebrand A. Different functional connectivity and network topology in behavioral variant of frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer's disease: an EEG study. Neurobiol. Aging. 2016;42:150–162. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2016.03.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Zaal I.J., Devlin J.W., Peelen L.M., Slooter A.J.C. A systematic review of risk factors for delirium in the ICU*. Crit. Care Med. 2015;43(1):40–47. doi: 10.1097/CCM.0000000000000625. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Zhang Y., Brady M., Smith S. Segmentation of brain MR images through a hidden Markov random field model and the expectation-maximization algorithm. IEEE Trans. Med. Imaging. 2001;20(1):45–57. doi: 10.1109/42.906424. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplementary material

mmc1.docx (329.6KB, docx)

Articles from NeuroImage : Clinical are provided here courtesy of Elsevier

RESOURCES