Abstract
It is critical that the regulatory system functions well in space’s microgravity. However, the “intrinsic” cardiovascular regulatory system (β), estimated by the fractal scaling of heart rate variability (HRV) (0.0001–0.01 Hz), does not adapt to the space environment during long-duration (6-month) space flights. Neuroimaging studies suggest that the default mode network (DMN) serves a broad adaptive purpose, its topology changing over time in association with different brain states of adaptive behavior. Hypothesizing that HRV varies in concert with changes in brain’s functional connectivity, we analyzed 24-hour HRV records from 8 healthy astronauts (51.8 ± 3.7 years; 6 men) on long (174.5 ± 13.8 days) space missions, obtained before launch, after about 21 (ISS01), 73 (ISS02), and 156 (ISS03) days in space, and after return to Earth. Spectral power in 8 frequency regions reflecting activity in different brain regions was computed by maximal entropy. Improved β (p < 0.05) found in 4 astronauts with a positive activation in the “HRV slow-frequency oscillation” (0.10–0.20 Hz) occurred even in the absence of consciousness. The adaptive response was stronger in the evening and early sleep compared to morning (p = 0.039). Brain functional networks, the DMN in particular, can help adapt to microgravity in space with help from the circadian clock.
Introduction
As humans venture into space, it is critical that the regulatory system should remain fully functional. Humans, including their cardiovascular system must first acclimate to a new environment in order to survive. Spaceflight dramatically alters cardiovascular dynamics1–5. Baroreflex sensitivity fluctuates and blood volume distribution is altered, affecting neural mechanisms involved in dynamic cardiovascular coordination. Cardiac output and stroke volume reportedly increase by 35–41% after 3–6 months in space4. Parasympathetic activity is reduced in space3, with no major difference in sympathetic nerve activity. Blood pressure is reduced by 8–10 mmHg related to a 39% decrease in systemic vascular resistance, not resulting from sympathetic nervous activity suppression, while the nocturnal dip is maintained. Understanding how organisms – and their neural cardiovascular coordination – adjust in space remains a challenge, as mechanisms are diverse and complex5–9.
The “intrinsic” cardiovascular regulatory system, estimated by the fractal scaling of heart rate variability (HRV) (slope “β”), did not adapt to microgravity after 6 months in space10–12, whereas the circadian rhythm of heart rate (HR) did improve10, an important feature since circadian disruption adversely affects health13–17. Beyond circadian rhythms, multi-frequency oscillations12,18–20 include the ~90-minute basic rest-activity cycle (BRAC)12. Most HRV changes in space apparently relate to the BRAC, which was amplified ~3-fold for specific HRV endpoints12, perhaps as a way to adapt to microgravity during long-duration spaceflight.
Being extremely complex, adaptation to a constantly changing environment occurs flexibly through several mechanisms, including physiological, behavioral, cognitive, and environmental demands21. The default mode network (DMN)22–25 and gut feeling26 may serve a broad adaptive process. The DMN consists of large-scale brain dynamics27–29, comprising interacting subsystems linked by hubs (see Fig. 1), such as the anterior medial prefrontal cortex (anterior mPFC) and posterior cingulate cortex (PCC)30. The dorsal medial subsystem includes the temporoparietal junction (TPJ), important for adaptation in nature31–34.
DMN activity is also driven by highly-coupled areas of the parietal cortex (see Fig. 1), linked to the medial orbitofrontal cortex (mOFC)35–38, which shares reciprocal connections with the basolateral amygdala, the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), the hippocampus, and the posterior para hippocampal cortex. The lateral orbitofrontal cortex (lOFC) coactivates with cognitive control regions of the salience network (SN), including the bilateral anterior insula cortex (AIC) and rostral ACC, which project to the dorsomedial and ventroanterior nuclei of the thalamus. The mOFC may be necessary for encoding subjective stimuli and for reward-guided learning36, while the lOFC may be crucial for reversal learning28,29.
Of the major brain networks, the DMN, the executive control network (ECN), and the SN may be involved in the adaptation to a novel environment39–42 (see Fig. 1). While the DMN’s activity is increased in response to internally focused cognitive processes, the ECN (distributed in the frontoparietal areas) engaged in externally directed tasks, is implicated in the management of exogenous cognitive functions. The SN, anchored primarily in the bilateral AIC and dorsal ACC, allocates resources to the most salient events among internal (DMN) and external (ECN) activities43.
The DMN’s organization may vary with the time of day between the usual default mode and an alerted default mode across different brain states during resting and task states (see Fig. 1)30,44,45. Lin et al.30 showed that static and dynamic DMN nodal topology is associated with upcoming cognitive task performance, and suggested that the core node PCC within the DMN plays a key role in supporting cognitive function. In response to stress, the DMN exhibits increased resting-state functional connectivity to key nodes of the SN (AIC, dorsal ACC [dACC] and amygdala), sensorimotor regions and higher-order visual areas, indicative of a shift to an alerted default mode44. Exposure to prolonged stress leads to functional changes in several large-scale brain networks30,46,47.
Multimodal integration of internal and external sensory signals occurs in the AIC through salience processing and interceptive predictions48. The AIC is responsible for body awareness and gut feeling26,49. In particular, the rostral-frontal insular cortex50 plays a major role in the integration of visceral sensory information arising from baroreceptors and chemoreceptors within the cardiovascular system, and it is involved in the efferent control of cardiovascular functions, behaviors usually processed by the unconscious mind.
The DMN plays an adaptive role in internal mentation24. Subliminal stimuli can alter behavior via non-conscious processes51–59. Behavior adjustment is flexible and occurs on a trial-to-trial bias. Neural models of behavior elicited by non-conscious stimuli implicate the prefrontal and cingulate cortices in regulation of subcortical brain regions linked to impulsive and largely non-conscious stimulus perception over time.
Non-consciously processed stimuli activate the visual cortex, insula (a brain area involved in eventual interceptive awareness) and ACC51, forming a basis for conscious perception. The insular cortex, receiving sensory inputs from both interoceptive and exteroceptive sources, is thought to integrate these multimodal signals. fMRI studies showed that internal bodily signals processing in the insular cortex modulates exteroceptive awareness. The AIC has been implicated in temporal processing, which may relate to its role in integrating internal interoceptive temporal cues with external signals. Consciously and unconsciously acquired relational memories may be harbored within a single, cohesive hippocampal-neocortical memory space, where they interact with each other52,53.
The human brain oscillates in harmony with frequency-specific subcomponents of several brain regions60–67. Brain oscillatory activity is usually measured by blood oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) signal, in terms of power within various frequency bands. BOLD oscillatory activity has been examined in the resting-state and during stimulus-evoked activity. The frequency-dependent power differs in different brain regions. The DMN is also composed of distinct frequency-dependent regions (see Fig. 2). According to resting-state fMRI studies, functional integration among brain regions occurs over multiple frequency bands ranging from slow-5 (0.01–0.027 Hz) to slow-1 (0.50–0.75 Hz). Dynamic functional connectivity patterns may be tightly linked to adaptive behavior25,45.
Resting-state networks may consist of frequency-specific subcomponents. Shifts in brain oscillations’ power during a task occur in specific frequency bands and in a spatially-specific manner. Frequencies in the range of 0.10–0.20 Hz may be important for functional connectivity61,63,65,66, mediating interactions within and among the DMN, SN and ECN nodes. The 0.10–0.20 Hz frequency band may reflect intrinsic oscillations in the insula.
The intimate brain-heart connection enunciated by Claude Bernard can be studied by analyzing HRV21, which may reflect the activity of the coordinating system (see Fig. 2), notably brain functional connectivity, including the DMN68–74, which integrates the brainstem nuclei that directly regulate the heart. HRV may provide information on how the brain coordinates with the periphery, and thus may serve to obtain information about the extent of adaptive adjustment.
The circadian system, allowing the measurement of time and the anticipation of environmental changes between light and dark, offered a survival advantage throughout evolution12,17,18. The circadian system drives numerous physiological and behavioral processes17–19; similar observations have been made in space10–12,16. The suprachiasmatic nuclei (SCN) coordinate circadian physiology and behavior by using neuronal and humoral signals that synchronize local clocks within the cells of most organs and tissues. Some of the SCN output pathways serve as input pathways for peripheral tissues15. The circadian gene network is present in most living organisms, from eubacteria to humans, and most cells and tissues express autonomous clocks. Disruption of clock genes results in physiological and metabolic dysregulation at neural, molecular, and cellular levels14,15,18–20; dysregulation has been verified in astronauts in space4,10–12,16.
Briefly, neuroimaging22–25,30–32 showed that dynamic functional connectivity patterns are tightly linked to adaptive behavior during rest/sleep22,25,30,35,75, and while awake. Adaptation processes are carried out mainly by the DMN23,24,29–32,44–47 and operate regardless of consciousness51–59. Adaptive responses of the DMN represent a change towards a more salient brain function (see Fig. 1). The DMN selectively shows an elevated connectivity with hubs of the SN, higher-order visual areas that co-vary with new environments24,44–50, reflecting an alert default state of the brain44, while SN and ECN connectivity remain stable. Adaptation-related changes are associated with shifts in the frequency of brain region-specific BOLD signals (see Fig. 2)60–67, which may be reflected in HRV changes mediated by cortico-subcortical pathways regulating the parasympathetic and sympathetic branches of the autonomic nervous system68–74.
Understanding the interplay between circadian changes in dynamic fluctuations of brain activity remains a major challenge7,8, notably in relation to the resting-state functional connectivity, including the DMN. This question is investigated herein, using HRV endpoints derived from repeated 24-hour ECG records provided by 8 astronauts. We explore (1) ways by which the brain adapts to microgravity; (2) whether DMN space adaptation occurs preferentially at certain circadian stages; and (3) whether there is a common or diverse modes of adaption. We posit that: HRV covaries with diverse fluctuations of brain regions and/or with temporal changes in whole-brain functional connectivity, reflecting adaptation behaviors to microgravity in space.
Methods
Subjects
Eight healthy astronauts (6 men, 2 women) participated in this study. Their mean (±SD) age was 51.8 ± 3.7 years. Their mean stay in space was 174.5 ± 13.8 days. Astronauts had passed class III physical examinations from the National Aeronautics and Space Administration (NASA). The study was approved by the NASA and the Japan Aerospace Exploration Agency (JAXA) Institutional Review Boards. Informed consent was obtained from all subjects. A detailed explanation of the study protocol was given to the subjects before they gave written, informed consent, according to the Declaration of Helsinki Principles. All methods were performed in accordance with the JAXA/NASA guidelines and regulations.
Experimental protocol
Ambulatory around-the-clock 24-hour electrocardiographic (ECG) records were obtained by using a two-channel Holter recorder (FM-180; Fukuda Denshi). Measurements were made five times: once before flight (Control); three times during flight on the International Space Station (ISS): ISS01, ISS02, and ISS03; and once after return to Earth (After flight). The control session was conducted on days 226.7 ± 158.7 (63 to 469) before launch in all but one astronaut who had technical problems with his before-flight record. In his case, a replacement control record was obtained 3.5 years after return to Earth. The three sessions in space were performed on days 21.0 ± 3.2 (18 to 28, ISS01), 73.4 ± 3.8 (68 to 78, ISS02) and 156.08 ± 16.6 (139 to 188, ISS03) after launch, the latter corresponding to 18.1 ± 3.4 days (11 to 21) before return to Earth. The last measurement session was performed on days 70.3 ± 28.6 (37 to 106 days) after return to Earth (After flight).
Analysis of HRV and measurement of 1/f fluctuations in HR dynamics
Data collection and measurement procedures were conducted as previously reported10–12. Briefly, for HRV measurements, QRS waveforms were read from continuous ECG records. The RR intervals between normal QRS waveforms were extracted as normal-to-normal (NN) intervals. The measured NN intervals were A/D converted (125-Hz) with 8-ms time resolution. After the authors confirmed that all artifacts were actually removed and that the data excluded supraventricular or ventricular arrhythmia, time-domain HRV indices, including SDNN, SDANN and Triangular Index (TI), and the conventional frequency-domain measures76, including the high frequency (HF) (0.15–0.40 Hz), low frequency (LF) (0.04–0.15 Hz), and very low frequency (VLF) (0.003–0.04 Hz) components, were obtained with the MemCalc/CHIRAM (Suwa Trust GMS, Tokyo, Japan) software77. Time series of NN intervals were also processed consecutively in 180-min intervals, progressively displaced by 5 min, to estimate the ultra-low frequency (ULF) component (0.0001–0.003 Hz), and to evaluate the 1/f β-type scaling in HRV, by plotting the log10[power] (ordinate) against log10[frequency] (abscissa) and fitting a regression line to estimate the slope β. Focus was placed on the frequency range of 0.0001–0.01 Hz (periods of 2.8 hours to 1.6 minutes).
In addition, time series of NN intervals covering 5-min intervals were processed consecutively, and three types of spectral power in eight frequency regions were computed using the Maximum Entropy Method (MEM): 1. LF-band (0.01–0.05 Hz) according to Baria et al.62; 2. LF-component (0.05–0.15 Hz) and HF-component (0.15–0.50 Hz) according to Chang et al.34; and 3. B1- (0.01–0.10 Hz; 100 sec to 10 sec), B2- (0.10–0.20 Hz; 10 sec to 5 sec), B3- (0.20–0.30 Hz; 5 sec to 3.3 sec), B4- (0.30–0.40 Hz; 3.3 sec to 2.5 sec) and B5- (0.40–0.50 Hz; 2.5 sec to 2.0 sec) bands according to Chen and Glover64. Herein, we call Baria et al.’ LF-band “HRV very slow-frequency oscillation (HRV-VSFO)”, Chang et al.’s LF- and HF-components “HRV LF- and HF-components”, and Chen and Clover’s B2-band “HRV slow-frequency oscillation (HRV-SFO)”.
Circadian stage-dependent HRV response to microgravity
Circadian stage-dependent HRV responses to the adaptive processes were assessed by subdividing the 24-hour day into 5 time spans of 3 hours each: late sleep (around 04:30); morning (around 07:30); afternoon (around 14:30); evening (around 21:30); and early sleep (around 00:30).
Fit of single 24-hour cosine model
Single 24-hour cosine curves were fitted to various HRV endpoints by cosinor11,12,18,19 to assess their time structure, especially focusing on the circadian amplitude. The 24-hour cosine model was fitted to 24-hour records of NN intervals, total power (TF), and power in the conventional ULF, VLF, LF, and HF regions of the MEM spectrum.
Statistical analyses
Estimates of HRV endpoints averaged over 24 hours, or over 3-hour spans to assess circadian stage-dependent effects, were expressed as mean ± SE (standard error). Changes in each HRV index during ISS01, ISS02 and ISS03 were compared to pre-flight (control) by the two-tailed Student t-test.
Estimates of HRV-SFO, HRV LF- and HF-components, and HRV-VSFO during ISS01, ISS02 and ISS03 were compared with those before flight. When they were increased in space, we defined them as a positive response. Any improvement in β was assessed by comparing slopes among the 3 sessions in space (ISS01, ISS02 and ISS03).
We hypothesize that a positive HRV response in two specific frequency regions is indicative of how different astronauts adapt to the space environment. One frequency region is the B2-band (0.10–0.20 Hz; “HRV-SFO”), reflecting the intrinsic oscillations in the insula, involving interactions within and among the DMN, SN and ECN nodes61,63,65,66. The other is the LF-band (0.01–0.05 Hz; “HRV-VSFO”), reflecting an activation of the DMN, including the mPFC, posterior parietal cortex, posterior portion of precuneus and PCC.
We propose that a positive HRV-SFO response defines astronauts in Group A (n = 4). In the absence of such a positive response, only some astronauts in Group B may show a positive response in HRV-VSFO (B1, n = 2), while others do not (B2, n = 2). The latter may differ from all other astronauts in their 24-hour HRV endpoints (β, TF, SDNN, SDANN and TI, and circadian amplitude of NN-interval, TF-, ULF-, VLF-, LF-, and HF- components), as tested by the two-tailed Student t-test. As data will accumulate in the future, it will be possible to test the validity of our hypothesis based on the relatively small sample of 8 astronauts thus far.
P-values less than 0.05, adjusted for multiple testing according to Sidak’s correction, were considered to indicate statistical significance. The Stat Flex (Ver. 6) software (Artec Co., Ltd., Osaka, Japan) was used.
Results
Characteristics of Group A (increased HRV activity in 0.10–0.20 Hz B2-band)
On the average, astronauts in Group A increased their B2 power by 36.9 ± 8.4% during ISS03 as compared to pre-flight (paired t = 4.374, p = 0.022), whereas astronauts in Group B decreased their B2 power by 22.5 ± 7.5% (paired t = 2.993, p = 0.058). An increase in the HRV-VSFO power was also observed in all 4 astronauts of Group A (see Table 1), suggesting an activation of the DMN. In 2 of them, HRV-VSFO activation occurred during ISS03, in another it occurred during both ISS02 and ISS03, and in another one it occurred only during ISS01. Increase in HRV-VSFO power may be associated with activation in the mPFC, the PCC, the precuneus, and the lateral and medial parietal cortex, approximating brain regions comprising the DMN62. In these 4 astronauts of Group A, the intrinsic cardiovascular autonomic regulatory system β improved (see Table 2), confirmed by the fit of a quadratic regression of β as a function of time (x) (R2 = 0.568, p = 0.004; t[x] = −3.213, p = 0.007 t[x2] = 2.337, p = 0.036). The 3 astronauts showing a positive HRV-VSFO response during ISS03 also improved β during ISS03, and the astronaut with a positive HRV-VSFO response during ISS01 improved β during ISS01 (see Table 2). Adaptation to space in these 4 astronauts may have been mediated via the DMN, shifting to the alerted default mode.
Table 1.
Frequency range (Hz) | Pre | ISS01 | ISS02 | ISS03 | ISS01 vs. Pre | ISS02 vs. Pre | ISS03 vs. Pre | ||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mean | SE | Mean | SE | Mean | SE | Mean | SE | t | p | t | p | t | p | ||
Group A: B2-band response (+) | |||||||||||||||
LF-band response (+) | |||||||||||||||
Case 1 | |||||||||||||||
B2 | 0.10–0.20 | 149.8 | 7.07 | 138.7 | 4.70 | 160.8 | 4.86 | 191.6 | 7.33 | 4.10 | 0.0005 | ||||
LF-b | 0.01–0.05 | 1007.5 | 54.7 | 749.0 | 38.2 | 952.9 | 47.9 | 1235.2 | 57.1 | 2.88 | 0.0408 | ||||
LF-c | 0.05–0.15 | 459.4 | 18.8 | 449.4 | 14.6 | 543.9 | 16.8 | 664.5 | 20.5 | 3.35 | 0.0085 | 7.37 | <0.0001 | ||
HF-c | 0.15–0.50 | 135.9 | 6.9 | 102.1 | 3.4 | 109.1 | 3.0 | 138.3 | 3.8 | ||||||
Case 2 | |||||||||||||||
B2 | 0.10–0.20 | 216.7 | 8.75 | 346.3 | 9.00 | 273.2 | 8.41 | 341.9 | 10.33 | 10.32 | <0.0001 | 4.65 | <0.0001 | 9.25 | <0.0001 |
LF-b | 0.01–0.05 | 1119.3 | 52.1 | 1370.5 | 53.6 | 797.7 | 33.8 | 1035.5 | 41.1 | 3.36 | 0.0084 | ||||
LF-c | 0.05–0.15 | 641.9 | 22.2 | 1035.1 | 26.9 | 761.0 | 21.8 | 837.0 | 20.7 | 11.27 | <0.0001 | 3.83 | 0.0014 | 6.43 | <0.0001 |
HF-c | 0.15–0.50 | 177.1 | 6.9 | 242.3 | 6.3 | 170.9 | 6.2 | 242.8 | 10.0 | 6.95 | <0.0001 | 5.41 | <0.0001 | ||
Case 3 | |||||||||||||||
B2 | 0.10–0.20 | 25.5 | 0.90 | 22.1 | 0.80 | 33.6 | 1.28 | 30.4 | 1.04 | 5.20 | <0.0001 | 3.60 | 0.0036 | ||
LF-b | 0.01–0.05 | 392.9 | 20.0 | 443.4 | 22.5 | 488.8 | 24.9 | 526.6 | 34.8 | 3.00 | 0.0277 | 3.34 | 0.0092 | ||
LF-c | 0.05–0.15 | 120.0 | 3.8 | 106.9 | 3.3 | 161.4 | 5.0 | 146.4 | 6.3 | 6.59 | <0.0001 | 3.60 | 0.0037 | ||
HF-c | 0.15–0.50 | 42.8 | 1.0 | 24.0 | 0.6 | 34.1 | 1.0 | 35.2 | 1.1 | ||||||
Case 4 | |||||||||||||||
B2 | 0.10–0.20 | 121.0 | 5.12 | 147.2 | 5.77 | 150.0 | 5.43 | 172.7 | 6.28 | 3.39 | 0.0076 | 3.88 | 0.0011 | 6.37 | <0.0001 |
LF-b | 0.01–0.05 | 880.7 | 30.8 | 812.7 | 36.2 | 915.3 | 43.4 | 1143.9 | 66.9 | 3.57 | 0.0040 | ||||
LF-c | 0.05–0.15 | 571.0 | 23.7 | 538.6 | 21.0 | 543.6 | 19.1 | 595.8 | 21.2 | ||||||
HF-c | 0.15–0.50 | 73.6 | 2.3 | 97.9 | 3.5 | 89.3 | 3.8 | 161.3 | 5.4 | 5.79 | <0.0001 | 3.56 | 0.0041 | 15.03 | <0.0001 |
Group B: B2-band response (−) | |||||||||||||||
LF-band response (+) | |||||||||||||||
Case 5 | |||||||||||||||
B2 | 0.10–0.20 | 169.6 | 5.98 | 93.9 | 3.03 | 84.5 | 2.50 | 96.8 | 3.59 | ||||||
LF-b | 0.01–0.05 | 640.8 | 26.2 | 1077.1 | 69.9 | 1121.6 | 72.0 | 937.8 | 64.6 | 5.85 | <0.0001 | 6.23 | <0.0001 | 4.26 | 0.0003 |
LF-c | 0.05–0.15 | 488.2 | 15.1 | 295.4 | 10.0 | 280.1 | 8.6 | 266.3 | 9.4 | ||||||
HF-c | 0.15–0.50 | 120.5 | 3.1 | 94.3 | 3.2 | 77.5 | 2.1 | 214.4 | 10.4 | 8.64 | <0.0001 | ||||
Case 6 | |||||||||||||||
B2 | 0.10–0.20 | 132.1 | 4.74 | 103.3 | 3.61 | 107.6 | 2.88 | 113.0 | 3.29 | ||||||
LF-b | 0.01–0.05 | 715.3 | 40.5 | 715.4 | 32.9 | 787.1 | 38.0 | 881.3 | 39.3 | 2.94 | 0.0335 | ||||
LF-c | 0.05–0.15 | 583.0 | 20.7 | 448.5 | 13.9 | 407.2 | 10.6 | 482.7 | 16.7 | ||||||
HF-c | 0.15–0.50 | 59.9 | 2.0 | 60.7 | 1.7 | 79.6 | 1.9 | 75.4 | 1.8 | 7.11 | <0.0001 | 5.64 | <0.0001 | ||
LF-band response (−) | |||||||||||||||
Case 7 | |||||||||||||||
B2 | 0.10–0.20 | 172.9 | 7.36 | 152.9 | 4.81 | 107.2 | 3.90 | 130.9 | 4.02 | ||||||
LF-b | 0.01–0.05 | 2661.7 | 159.3 | 2251.2 | 143.9 | 1420.1 | 84.6 | 2305.5 | 133.4 | ||||||
LF-c | 0.05–0.15 | 683.7 | 22.9 | 617.2 | 19.6 | 467.7 | 16.6 | 557.8 | 19.6 | ||||||
HF-c | 0.15–0.50 | 174.2 | 5.0 | 165.0 | 3.4 | 107.9 | 2.6 | 128.3 | 2.8 | ||||||
Case 8 | |||||||||||||||
B2 | 0.10–0.20 | 355.5 | 12.29 | 254.9 | 8.61 | 327.6 | 10.06 | 325.2 | 11.96 | ||||||
LF-b | 0.01–0.05 | 2907.3 | 188.9 | 2137.7 | 129.9 | 2165.7 | 131.8 | 2719.0 | 194.6 | ||||||
LF-c | 0.05–0.15 | 1110.4 | 38.1 | 878.4 | 34.2 | 1025.4 | 34.7 | 997.1 | 38.2 | ||||||
HF-c | 0.15–0.50 | 213.8 | 8.2 | 156.9 | 4.8 | 194.1 | 5.4 | 213.3 | 8.0 |
B2-band (B2-b), HRV-SFO; LF-band: HRV-VSFO; LF-c: HRV LF-component; HF-c: HRV HF-component.
N between 260 and 287; t: Student t; p: P-value (Sidak correction).
Table 2.
Case | Pre | ISS01 | ISS02 | ISS03 | ISS02 vs. ISS01 | ISS03 vs. ISS01 | ISS03 vs. ISS02 | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mean | SE | Mean | SE | Mean | SE | Mean | SE | t | p | t | p | t | p | |
Group A: B2-band response (+) | ||||||||||||||
LF-band response (+) | ||||||||||||||
1 | −1.0584 | 0.0275 | −1.0564 | 0.0213 | −0.8186 | 0.0150 | −0.9728 | 0.0140 | 9.14 | <0.0001 | 3.28 | 0.0111 | −7.53 | <0.0001 |
2 | −1.1645 | 0.0199 | −0.9698 | 0.0123 | −1.1248 | 0.0171 | −0.9050 | 0.0075 | −7.34 | <0.0001 | 4.48 | 0.0001 | 11.73 | <0.0001 |
3 | −1.3017 | 0.0205 | −0.8700 | 0.0144 | −0.7900 | 0.0267 | −0.8832 | 0.0173 | 2.64 | 0.0843 | −0.59 | 0.9997 | −2.93 | 0.0356 |
4 | −1.1761 | 0.0157 | −1.0009 | 0.0121 | −0.9159 | 0.0139 | −1.0134 | 0.0191 | 4.61 | 0.0001 | −0.55 | 0.9998 | −4.13 | 0.0004 |
Group B: B2-band response (−) | ||||||||||||||
Group B1: LF-band response (+) | ||||||||||||||
5 | −1.0258 | 0.0140 | −0.8483 | 0.0232 | −0.6810 | 0.0192 | −0.7753 | 0.0292 | 5.55 | <0.0001 | 1.96 | 0.4082 | −2.70 | 0.0699 |
6 | −1.2171 | 0.0140 | −1.1413 | 0.0209 | −1.0638 | 0.0116 | −1.0722 | 0.0182 | 3.25 | 0.0126 | 2.50 | 0.1212 | −0.39 | 1.0000 |
Group B2: LF-band response (−) | ||||||||||||||
7 | −1.0698 | 0.0267 | −0.8535 | 0.0242 | −0.9988 | 0.0260 | −0.9741 | 0.0258 | −4.09 | 0.0005 | −3.41 | 0.0071 | 0.68 | 0.9990 |
8 | −0.9864 | 0.0270 | −1.0370 | 0.0228 | −0.8830 | 0.0147 | −0.8484 | 0.0249 | 5.67 | <0.0001 | 5.58 | <0.0001 | 1.19 | 0.9303 |
B2-band: HRV-SFO; LF-band: HRV-VSFO.
N between 230 and 255; t: Student t; p: P-value (Sidak correction).
In 3 of the 4 astronauts of Group A the power of the HRV LF-component increased (see Table 1), suggesting an activation of the TPJ, which serves a broader adaptive purpose. The TPJ provides widespread connectivity to the dACC, insula, amygdala, thalamus and brainstem, likely playing a role on salience processing and autonomic control during adaptation to space.
The HRV HF-component power also increased in 2 of the 4 astronauts of Group A (see Table 1). Such an increase is accompanied by increases in functional connectivity between the dACC and cingulate cortex, basal ganglia, thalamus, amygdala and the brainstem, independent of connectivity between dACC and TPJ34.
These results illustrate the complexity of adaptive responses, even though the DMN played a key role in astronauts of Group A with increased activity of the “HRV-SFO”. The response to long-term exposure to microgravity, achieved diversely in different astronauts, elicits profound changes needed towards a more salient mode of brain function requiring a higher interconnectivity among the DMN, SN, and/or ECN, switching to the alerted default mode (see Fig. 1).
Circadian challenges of Group A astronauts in adaptation to microgravity in space
When subdividing the 24-hour day into 5 time spans of 3 hours each, centered around 04:30 (late sleep), 07:30 (morning), 14:30 (afternoon), 21:30 (evening) and 00:30 (early sleep), the adaptive behavior involving the DMN (switch to the alerted default mode) was found to be circadian stage-dependent in the Group A but not in the Group B astronauts (see Table 3). Overall, LF power in Group A was increased during ISS3 compared to pre-flight by 159 ms2 (41%) (paired t = 4.399, p = 0.022), more so during early sleep (335 ms2, paired t = 3.493, p = 0.040) than at other circadian stages. In Group B, however, LF power was decreased overall by 128 ms2 (38%) (paired t = 4.133, p = 0.026), particularly in the morning (236 ms2, p = 0.054) and afternoon (309 ms2, p = 0.004). Adaptive behavior was thus activated preferentially in the evening as well as during early sleep in the 4 astronauts of Group A.
Table 3.
Case | Pre | ISS03 | t | p | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mean | SE | Mean | SE | ||||
Group A | |||||||
Late Sleep (04:30) | |||||||
1 | LF | 914.7 | 84.3 | 682.1 | 39.6 | ||
HF | 363.1 | 22.1 | 103.7 | 6.0 | |||
2 | LF | 749.6 | 57.4 | 732.3 | 61.1 | ||
HF | 283.3 | 24.4 | 276.8 | 23.5 | |||
3 | LF | 84.0 | 7.5 | 199.1 | 14.9 | 6.89 | <0.0001 |
HF | 49.6 | 2.3 | 40.7 | 2.5 | |||
4 | LF | 318.5 | 38.7 | 825.6 | 83.5 | 5.51 | <0.0001 |
HF | 63.7 | 4.3 | 225.7 | 14.7 | 10.60 | <0.0001 | |
Morning (07:30) | |||||||
1 | LF | 404.1 | 35.6 | 552.4 | 53.4 | ||
HF | 103.1 | 10.8 | 103.3 | 8.3 | |||
2 | LF | 704.8 | 46.7 | 873.3 | 59.5 | ||
HF | 216.1 | 24.1 | 156.6 | 7.0 | |||
3 | LF | 130.0 | 9.8 | 138.6 | 12.8 | ||
HF | 48.9 | 2.9 | 46.9 | 3.8 | |||
4 | LF | 596.4 | 60.1 | 764.7 | 46.9 | ||
HF | 74.4 | 6.3 | 136.2 | 9.1 | 5.6 | <0.0001 | |
Afternoon (14:30) | |||||||
1 | LF | 332.0 | 33.5 | 412.2 | 43.5 | ||
HF | 73.2 | 6.2 | 107.6 | 10.8 | |||
2 | LF | 710.8 | 66.5 | 771.4 | 51.1 | ||
HF | 151.6 | 12.0 | 173.0 | 7.3 | |||
3 | LF | 115.3 | 6.9 | 124.5 | 8.2 | ||
HF | 41.7 | 1.9 | 31.7 | 1.8 | |||
4 | LF | 480.5 | 47.5 | 481.4 | 40.7 | ||
HF | 60.4 | 4.1 | 137.3 | 6.1 | 10.49 | <0.0001 | |
Evening (21:30) | |||||||
1 | LF | 206.0 | 21.0 | 560.6 | 33.0 | 9.06 | <0.0001 |
HF | 74.6 | 4.5 | 126.8 | 5.9 | 7.02 | <0.0001 | |
2 | LF | 481.2 | 46.2 | 847.4 | 63.3 | 4.67 | 0.0002 |
HF | 146.5 | 8.0 | 201.9 | 16.1 | 3.08 | 0.0335 | |
3 | LF | 136.4 | 11.2 | 102.1 | 10.7 | ||
HF | 46.6 | 3.1 | 27.6 | 1.5 | |||
4 | LF | 345.5 | 71.9 | 488.2 | 60.8 | ||
HF | 61.3 | 6.5 | 165.1 | 11.9 | 7.64 | <0.0001 | |
Early Sleep (00:30) | |||||||
1 | LF | 471.3 | 43.0 | 938.6 | 65.9 | 5.94 | <0.0001 |
HF | 190.5 | 16.8 | 180.6 | 8.7 | |||
2 | LF | 456.4 | 47.1 | 987.9 | 70.0 | 6.30 | <0.0001 |
HF | 190.8 | 17.6 | 558.5 | 35.4 | 9.31 | <0.0001 | |
3 | LF | 90.7 | 9.2 | 275.7 | 30.0 | 5.90 | <0.0001 |
HF | 39.6 | 3.2 | 29.8 | 2.1 | |||
4 | LF | 642.0 | 74.5 | 798.6 | 66.8 | ||
HF | 74.7 | 9.8 | 287.3 | 18.3 | 10.25 | <0.0001 | |
Group B1 | |||||||
Late Sleep (04:30) | |||||||
5 | LF | 494.0 | 44.2 | 426.5 | 34.7 | ||
HF | 142.8 | 10.7 | 469.7 | 13.4 | 19.04 | <0.0001 | |
6 | LF | 645.6 | 63.4 | 505.2 | 63.6 | ||
HF | 90.6 | 8.1 | 81.9 | 2.6 | |||
Morning (07:30) | |||||||
5 | LF | 362.5 | 25.3 | 199.3 | 17.2 | ||
HF | 109.9 | 5.5 | 87.3 | 6.7 | |||
6 | LF | 574.0 | 74.6 | 506.4 | 33.2 | ||
HF | 59.6 | 7.0 | 80.0 | 4.6 | |||
Afternoon (14:30) | |||||||
5 | LF | 645.7 | 35.3 | 232.5 | 21.0 | ||
HF | 171.9 | 9.3 | 89.4 | 7.5 | |||
6 | LF | 489.4 | 31.8 | 267.5 | 36.3 | ||
HF | 45.4 | 2.8 | 38.6 | 4.7 | |||
Evening (21:30) | |||||||
5 | LF | 558.1 | 41.4 | 269.2 | 21.1 | ||
HF | 98.5 | 5.0 | 261.9 | 17.5 | 8.96 | <0.0001 | |
6 | LF | 391.8 | 43.0 | 459.6 | 23.1 | ||
HF | 58.5 | 6.2 | 67.0 | 2.5 | |||
Early Sleep (00:30) | |||||||
5 | LF | 378.0 | 35.1 | 258.5 | 23.3 | ||
HF | 125.2 | 8.3 | 426.6 | 18.5 | 14.83 | <0.0001 | |
6 | LF | 630.9 | 61.6 | 506.5 | 70.1 | ||
HF | 87.8 | 7.2 | 87.2 | 3.7 | |||
Group B2 | |||||||
Late Sleep (04:30) | |||||||
7 | LF | 666.0 | 62.6 | 757.2 | 68.4 | ||
HF | 244.7 | 14.3 | 169.8 | 8.5 | |||
8 | LF | 1411.9 | 72.8 | 1894.1 | 91.0 | 4.14 | <0.0001 |
HF | 428.6 | 22.3 | 435.1 | 20.2 | |||
Morning (07:30) | |||||||
7 | LF | 865.5 | 95.1 | 569.9 | 41.3 | ||
HF | 184.4 | 14.6 | 141.8 | 6.7 | |||
8 | LF | 1301.9 | 80.4 | 885.0 | 69.1 | ||
HF | 256.0 | 16.3 | 180.2 | 11.4 | |||
Afternoon (14:30) | |||||||
7 | LF | 778.8 | 58.1 | 494.5 | 37.9 | ||
HF | 135.7 | 8.8 | 109.8 | 4.6 | |||
8 | LF | 1090.8 | 106.1 | 772.7 | 61.2 | ||
HF | 150.5 | 8.6 | 164.8 | 13.7 | |||
Evening (21:30) | |||||||
7 | LF | 410.8 | 35.2 | 529.8 | 70.0 | ||
HF | 118.0 | 4.6 | 114.6 | 5.7 | |||
8 | LF | 444.5 | 40.3 | 518.4 | 48.8 | ||
HF | 135.5 | 13.3 | 100.6 | 9.0 | |||
Early Sleep (00:30) | |||||||
7 | LF | 612.1 | 55.0 | 646.4 | 60.7 | ||
HF | 183.9 | 13.5 | 159.8 | 8.1 | |||
8 | LF | 1696.6 | 96.2 | 1184.5 | 76.1 | ||
HF | 329.7 | 19.7 | 264.3 | 10.2 |
LF: HRV LF-component (0.05–0.15 Hz).
HF: HRV HF-component (0.15–0.50 Hz).
N between 34 and 37; t: Student t; p: P-value (Sidak correction).
Characteristics of Group B astronauts (without increased HRV activity in 0.10–0.20 Hz B2-band)
In astronauts of Group B, β also improved, but sporadically (see Table 2). They tried to adapt to microgravity differently. There was no positive response of the HRV LF-component, suggesting that the TPJ did not participate in their adaptive process (see Table 1).
Two astronauts with a positive HRV-VSFO response (Group B1) responded positively in their HRV HF-component (0.15–0.50 Hz) during ISS03 (see Table 1). They showed statistically significant positive responses in B3 (0.20–0.30 Hz), B4 (0.30–0.40 Hz), and B5 (0.40–0.50 Hz) (see Table 4) either during the daytime (see Table 4, top) or during the night (see Table 4, bottom). Since the HRV HF-component reportedly relates to connectivity between dACC, AIC and amygdala to thalamus and the brainstem, brain oscillatory activity may shift from lower-frequency to higher-frequency bands during adaptation processes. Adaptation to microgravity in Group B1 astronauts may thus be initiated by the SN.
Table 4.
Band | Pre | ISS01 | ISS02 | ISS03 | Post | ISS01 vs. Pre | ISS02 vs. Pre | ISS03 vs. Pre | ||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mean | SE | Mean | SE | Mean | SE | Mean | SE | Mean | SE | t | p | t | p | t | p | |
Daytime | ||||||||||||||||
Case 5 | ||||||||||||||||
B1 | 952.9 | 32.7 | 892.4 | 32.9 | 844.3 | 26.7 | 781.9 | 33.4 | 1093.9 | 38.9 | ||||||
B2 | 180.1 | 6.88 | 93.3 | 3.91 | 79.4 | 2.89 | 79.4 | 3.60 | 141.1 | 5.44 | ||||||
B3 | 39.7 | 1.73 | 29.5 | 1.46 | 29.2 | 1.24 | 47.4 | 2.94 | 32.6 | 1.73 | ||||||
B4 | 21.7 | 0.90 | 13.0 | 0.77 | 10.3 | 0.54 | 25.4 | 1.55 | 19.0 | 0.82 | ||||||
B5 | 13.1 | 0.34 | 9.1 | 0.78 | 7.4 | 0.51 | 21.2 | 1.29 | 6.1 | 0.18 | 6.11 | <0.001 | ||||
Case 6 | ||||||||||||||||
B1 | 1113.8 | 47.6 | 1079.6 | 39.1 | 1114.2 | 45.8 | 1271.7 | 48.6 | 1300.4 | 50.3 | ||||||
B2 | 127.7 | 4.85 | 108.7 | 4.05 | 106.7 | 3.25 | 112.7 | 3.62 | 134.2 | 5.67 | ||||||
B3 | 18.0 | 0.74 | 21.4 | 0.86 | 28.8 | 0.85 | 26.2 | 0.86 | 36.3 | 1.63 | 3.01 | 0.027 | 9.62 | <0.001 | 7.28 | <0.001 |
B4 | 6.9 | 0.27 | 7.6 | 0.25 | 9.8 | 0.34 | 10.1 | 0.35 | 12.6 | 0.48 | 6.75 | <0.001 | 7.19 | <0.001 | ||
B5 | 4.3 | 0.21 | 4.4 | 0.13 | 5.8 | 0.19 | 5.9 | 0.19 | 7.3 | 0.31 | 5.50 | <0.001 | 5.63 | <0.001 | ||
Nighttime | ||||||||||||||||
Case 5 | ||||||||||||||||
B1 | 1174.0 | 92.7 | 2203.4 | 188.8 | 2232.1 | 174.5 | 2018.7 | 194.2 | 1533.9 | 134.6 | 4.89 | <0.001 | 5.35 | <0.001 | 3.93 | 0.002 |
B2 | 126.8 | 9.85 | 95.2 | 4.59 | 93.6 | 4.55 | 138.6 | 6.70 | 119.7 | 10.70 | ||||||
B3 | 87.9 | 6.41 | 50.4 | 2.32 | 26.1 | 1.26 | 215.2 | 8.90 | 100.8 | 7.52 | 11.61 | <0.001 | ||||
B4 | 15.0 | 0.76 | 12.1 | 0.32 | 9.0 | 0.22 | 89.6 | 2.13 | 11.1 | 0.75 | 32.97 | <0.001 | ||||
B5 | 11.7 | 0.57 | 6.8 | 0.25 | 4.2 | 0.11 | 65.7 | 1.31 | 8.5 | 0.70 | 37.85 | <0.001 | ||||
Case 6 | ||||||||||||||||
B1 | 1551.4 | 176.6 | 1092.1 | 97.5 | 1117.0 | 93.0 | 1324.0 | 125.3 | 1665.1 | 167.1 | ||||||
B2 | 151.7 | 14.11 | 88.9 | 7.47 | 110.6 | 6.27 | 114.4 | 7.78 | 175.1 | 12.64 | ||||||
B3 | 30.1 | 3.07 | 31.3 | 2.07 | 45.5 | 3.41 | 32.2 | 1.77 | 57.1 | 4.37 | 3.36 | 0.011 | ||||
B4 | 9.5 | 0.65 | 6.7 | 0.24 | 9.4 | 0.31 | 10.3 | 0.43 | 24.4 | 0.81 | ||||||
B5 | 6.7 | 0.68 | 4.6 | 0.18 | 5.2 | 0.24 | 6.6 | 0.36 | 14.3 | 0.46 |
N between 181 and 233 (daytime), between 52 and 104 (nighttime); t: Student t; p: P-value (Sidak correction).
The other two astronauts without a positive HRV-VSFO response (Group B2) did not respond in terms of their HRV LF-component or HRV HF-component, except during late sleep (case 8) (see Tables 1 and 3). As compared to the other 6 astronauts, they had statistically significantly larger spectral power of the TF-component measured before flight (p = 0.0007), during ISS01 (p = 0.0102), ISS02 (p = 0.0010), and ISS03 (p = 0.0023) (see Table 5, top). Before flight, they had a higher SDNN (p = 0.0001), SDANN (p = 0.0001) and Triangular Index (p = 0.0002), suggesting a better prognosis in terms of cardiovascular outcome. These HRV indices were significantly larger also during ISS01, ISS02 and ISS03, except for the Triangular Index during ISS03 (see Table 5, top), suggesting that adaptation processes in Group B2 astronauts were independent and different from the brain’s functional connectivity and/or regional cerebral blood flow.
Table 5.
24-hour HRV | Group A and B1 (n = 6) | Group B2 (n = 2) | Student t-test | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mean | SD | Mean | SD | t | p | ||
Pre | HR | 75.2 | 9.4 | 59.5 | 11.5 | −1.962 | 0.0974 |
β | −1.157 | 0.102 | −1.028 | 0.059 | 1.647 | 0.1507 | |
TF | 4725.0 | 1282.6 | 15810.2 | 4353.5 | 6.379 | 0.0007 | |
SDNN | 108.50 | 15.99 | 211.85 | 1.06 | 8.668 | 0.0001 | |
SDANN | 92.78 | 12.53 | 190.90 | 7.50 | 10.152 | 0.0001 | |
TI | 28.22 | 2.41 | 52.84 | 7.19 | 8.219 | 0.0002 | |
ISS01 | HR | 68.9 | 7.7 | 60.8 | 15.6 | −1.040 | 0.3386 |
β | −0.981 | 0.111 | −0.945 | 0.130 | 0.384 | 0.7145 | |
TF | 4744.7 | 1677.6 | 9369.6 | 276.9 | 3.689 | 0.0102 | |
SDNN | 133.65 | 18.07 | 195.00 | 19.66 | 4.096 | 0.0064 | |
SDANN | 114.58 | 14.10 | 148.20 | 14.99 | 2.888 | 0.0278 | |
TI | 32.32 | 5.07 | 51.60 | 4.13 | 4.791 | 0.0030 | |
ISS02 | HR | 68.9 | 7.7 | 61.2 | 5.7 | −1.276 | 0.2490 |
β | −0.899 | 0.170 | −0.941 | 0.082 | −0.323 | 0.7574 | |
TF | 4020.1 | 1040.7 | 9736.0 | 1650.0 | 6.011 | 0.0010 | |
SDNN | 118.07 | 26.42 | 211.90 | 60.95 | 3.316 | 0.0161 | |
SDANN | 100.87 | 24.08 | 186.85 | 35.85 | 3.988 | 0.0072 | |
TI | 25.64 | 3.88 | 41.60 | 9.81 | 3.656 | 0.0106 | |
ISS03 | HR | 68.7 | 7.9 | 60.4 | 8.5 | −1.267 | 0.2519 |
β | −0.937 | 0.105 | −0.911 | 0.089 | 0.307 | 0.7694 | |
TF | 5122.1 | 1364.1 | 12612.3 | 3217.9 | 5.068 | 0.0023 | |
SDNN | 134.10 | 26.25 | 210.90 | 49.78 | 2.994 | 0.0242 | |
SDANN | 113.47 | 22.09 | 186.95 | 44.05 | 3.331 | 0.0158 | |
TI | 27.92 | 5.01 | 38.24 | 8.18 | 2.231 | 0.0672 | |
Post | HR | 72.7 | 7.0 | 65.0 | 16.5 | −1.008 | 0.3524 |
β | −1.164 | 0.170 | −1.066 | 0.100 | 0.749 | 0.4819 | |
TF | 5606.2 | 1412.6 | 11090.0 | 1920.2 | 4.451 | 0.0043 | |
SDNN | 129.07 | 34.90 | 198.15 | 31.32 | 2.464 | 0.0488 | |
SDANN | 107.22 | 32.46 | 178.50 | 37.34 | 2.620 | 0.0396 | |
TI | 34.03 | 7.64 | 43.54 | 10.90 | 1.408 | 0.2088 | |
Circadian amplitude | Group A and B1 (n = 6) | Group B2 (n = 2) | Student t-test | ||||
Mean | SD | Mean | SD | t | p | ||
Pre | NN | 69.1 | 25.9 | 200.5 | 40.8 | 5.560 | 0.0014 |
TF | 1240.8 | 513.7 | 7781.2 | 461.3 | 15.851 | <0.0001 | |
ULF | 1140.8 | 586.3 | 5957.1 | 763.8 | 9.523 | 0.0001 | |
VLF | 417.7 | 239.2 | 3304.5 | 1640.5 | 5.019 | 0.0024 | |
LF | 119.6 | 92.6 | 350.7 | 265.9 | 2.057 | 0.0854 | |
HF | 33.9 | 36.5 | 85.9 | 58.1 | 1.559 | 0.1701 | |
ISS01 | NN | 103.1 | 37.2 | 182.7 | 28.4 | 2.720 | 0.0347 |
TF | 2571.6 | 990.0 | 3279.8 | 1427.6 | 0.807 | 0.4507 | |
ULF | 2567.7 | 1064.1 | 3030.3 | 2730.7 | 0.383 | 0.7148 | |
VLF | 611.3 | 374.4 | 1561.2 | 127.7 | 3.365 | 0.0151 | |
LF | 91.4 | 77.5 | 339.4 | 425.5 | 1.620 | 0.1564 | |
HF | 33.2 | 31.4 | 47.5 | 26.9 | 0.572 | 0.5881 | |
ISS02 | NN | 88.7 | 39.5 | 179.0 | 9.4 | 3.040 | 0.0227 |
TF | 2329.4 | 1892.0 | 7434.7 | 3286.6 | 2.859 | 0.0288 | |
ULF | 2026.4 | 2056.6 | 5318.0 | 4264.9 | 1.574 | 0.1664 | |
VLF | 587.6 | 262.7 | 1658.6 | 844.6 | 3.123 | 0.0205 | |
LF | 95.1 | 53.2 | 364.7 | 310.5 | 2.432 | 0.0510 | |
HF | 27.6 | 13.8 | 53.4 | 47.8 | 1.363 | 0.2219 | |
ISS03 | NN | 107.8 | 28.0 | 190.6 | 32.9 | 3.513 | 0.0126 |
TF | 3405.3 | 2320.0 | 6082.1 | 3484.3 | 1.285 | 0.2462 | |
ULF | 3153.1 | 2045.1 | 4921.5 | 4064.2 | 0.867 | 0.4192 | |
VLF | 621.8 | 405.3 | 2695.9 | 2236.2 | 2.579 | 0.0418 | |
LF | 134.9 | 100.1 | 461.5 | 418.3 | 2.065 | 0.0844 | |
HF | 69.0 | 66.2 | 78.9 | 74.6 | 0.180 | 0.8630 | |
Post | NN | 88.7 | 50.5 | 175.8 | 91.4 | 1.798 | 0.1223 |
TF | 1752.8 | 613.5 | 4717.8 | 1049.4 | 5.150 | 0.0021 | |
ULF | 1525.0 | 626.5 | 3277.0 | 518.5 | 3.519 | 0.0125 | |
VLF | 485.8 | 228.3 | 2394.0 | 1567.7 | 3.472 | 0.0133 | |
LF | 155.0 | 75.1 | 425.7 | 494.5 | 1.555 | 0.1709 | |
HF | 39.4 | 19.7 | 41.6 | 45.8 | 0.103 | 0.9215 |
HR: Heart Rate; NN: NN-interval.
Circadian characteristics of Group B2 astronauts
As compared to the other 6 astronauts, the two astronauts of Group B2 had a statistically significantly larger circadian amplitude of NN-interval (p = 0.0014), TF-component (p < 0.0001), and conventional ULF (p = 0.0001) and VLF (p = 0.0024) components measured before spaceflight. Several of these indices were also significantly larger during ISS01, ISS02, ISS03, and after return to Earth (see Table 5, bottom); circadian amplitudes of the LF and HF components were not significantly different (see Table 5, bottom).
Discussion
Neuroimaging studies suggest that HRV varies in concert with changes in brain functional connectivity34,68–74. If so, HRV could serve as a proxy for ‘vertical integration’ of the brain system in which the DMN widely interacts with other large-scale brain systems and may play a role in the adaptive process to space’s microgravity. Microgravity induces altered blood volume distribution, which disturbs neural mechanisms involved in dynamic cardiovascular coordination1–4,6,10–12. Since urgent action is needed to help the neural cardiovascular coordination adapt to the space environment, adaptive responses were examined, based on 24-hour HRV endpoints.
As expected, coordination of physiological functions was diverse and varied over time, with no common path of accommodation methods for successful adaptation. A thorough and extensive analysis of the data uncovered two HRV frequency bands, the HRV-SFO (0.10–0.20 Hz) and the HRV-VSFO (0.01–0.05 Hz) that we propose may classify astronauts in terms of mechanisms they use to adapt to microgravity in space (see Table 1).
Some limitations need to be noted. By necessity rather than choice, neuroimaging could not be performed in space, preventing the direct investigation of the relationship between the DMN, SN, and the circadian system. Comparisons of HRV endpoints were made on 5-min estimates obtained over 24 hours. As a result, serial correlation may have led to P-values that are too liberal. Comparisons between different sessions provided by each astronaut were made using the Student t test rather than the paired t test because some 5-min estimates are occasionally missing due to artifacts in the data. To identify different kinds of adaptation responses to microgravity, many comparisons were made, between records in space versus pre-flight and among the 3 sessions in space. While P-values were adjusted for multiple testing, results only intend to propose a framework for assessing astronauts’ adaptation process. Data to accumulate in future studies will be critical to determine whether the proposed classification remains valid or whether it needs to be refined.
In astronauts with a positive response in HRV-SFO, the intrinsic cardiovascular autonomic regulatory system, gauged by β, improved (see Table 2). The HRV LF-component power increased by 25 ± 8% (paired t = 3.011, p = 0.057), whereas it decreased by 23 ± 8% in Group B astronauts (paired t = 2.942, p = 0.060). We propose that two types of corticostriatal circuits play key roles in complex adaptive behaviors. One is the DMN circuit, acting via the TPJ networks31–34, and the other is the SN circuit acting via the lOFC loop35–38, including the PFC, anterior insula and ACC. HF-component (0.20–0.50 Hz) activity increased in Group B1, suggesting SN (dACC, anterior insula and amygdala) involvement in the adaptation process39–43,68,71,73. Since changes in brain functional networks were not observed in Group B2 (see Table 1), other adaptation mechanisms may have taken place. Their larger circadian amplitudes of TF, SDNN, SDANN and TI compared to the other 6 astronauts suggest that long-term HRV may be more predictive19,76,77 than short-term HRV indices in their case (see Table 5, top).
As anticipated, changes in HRV activities during adaptation to space were found to be circadian stage-dependent, being strongest during early sleep (see Table 3), implying a role of the circadian system, which was also observed in Group B2 astronauts, who had more prominent circadian rhythms of several HRV indices as compared to the other 6 astronauts (see Table 5, bottom). The circadian organization is thus likely to play a role in the adaptation process of all astronauts. Results illustrate how diverse adaptation processes can be, primarily involving the dynamics of large-scale brain networks, initiated by the DMN (Group A), sometimes supported by the SN (Group B1), but always coordinated by the circadian system and occurring irrespective of consciousness. Several questions need to be addressed.
First, why is the 0.10–0.20 Hz HRV frequency band important in adaptation? Bajaj et al.61 observed that brain activity oscillations dynamically changed along time scales of seconds to minutes. Frequencies in the range of 0.10–0.198 Hz played a significant role for functional connectivity, especially in the DMN. Pfurtscheller et al.66 observed an increase in the frequency range of 0.07–0.20 Hz in association with activation of the insula. The slow oscillation (0.07–0.19 Hz) reportedly mediates the interactions within and among the DMN, SN and ECN nodes for decision-making tasks, the right AIC of the SN causally controlling the DMN and ECN in combination with the dACC63. Social stress responses reportedly upregulate the DMN functional connectivity towards an alerted default mode44, the DMN being intrinsically related to both SN and ECN nodes, mediated by increased activity of the right AIC and dACC. Increase in 0.10–0.20 Hz HRV power, reflecting an activation of the insula, suggests that the DMN shifted towards a state of increased vigilance to prepare astronauts to detect salient stimuli and reorient attention for a successful adaptation to the space environment (see Fig. 1).
Second, what role does the circadian system play in the adaption process? Ubiquitous circadian changes have gained in interest13,15,17–20, notably in relation to neurobehavioral functions. Circadian changes in brain functional connectivity have been observed78, in part using resting-state fMRI79,80. Neural activity of the brain fluctuates over the course of 24 hours and circadian stage is important when interpreting functional connectivity data.
Several factors contribute to inter-individual variability20, including chronotype, age, sleep disturbance and disordered sleep-wake cycle, medication, and circadian disruption caused by lifestyle choices and/or disease conditions. Inter-individual variation was found in the way different astronauts adapt differently to microgravity. DMN activity of Group A astronauts was higher in the evening and early sleep. Increased sleepiness induces neurochemical consequences for the brain at the higher brain performance. Previous studies assumed a priori a link between DMN activity and mind wandering31,37,38, now substantiated by a greater cerebral BOLD response in the evening80, involving the prefrontal cortex (medial and inferior orbital frontal gyrus, and anterior cingulate). In early NREM sleep, the DMN has no measurable change in functional connectivity75, which is important since maintenance of these network connections through ongoing spontaneous activity may be of fundamental importance to the living brain. In Group B1 astronauts, the SN was involved in the adaptation process, as was the circadian system. Differences in the adaptive response between nighttime and daytime may reflect inter-individual differences. The involvement of the circadian system is not surprising since brain’s cortical functions are circadian rhythmic, their phase varying across brain regions81. In Group B2 astronauts, circadian organization was important, independently of the brain’s functional connectivity.
Third, what are implications for an unconscious activation of adaptive processes? Recent empirical findings in psychology suggest that complex behavior, including adaptive processes, could take place without conscious attention54,55. Human senses can handle more than 11 million bits/s: 10 million are processed by the visual system; ~1 million via the ears; 10,000–100,000 via the skin; much fewer by the olfactory system and remaining sensory channels (gustatory and vestibular). Only a minuscule proportion of sensory data (~50 bits/s) is processed by the conscious mind56; remaining data are processed unconsciously; that is ~220,000 (11,000,000/50) times as many as data processed consciously.
Unconscious information processing has been described as flexible, sharing many sophisticated characteristics with its conscious counterpart55,57,58. Several brain regions are involved in unconscious processes, including the PFC, insula, ACC, hippocampus, amygdala, PCC, precuneus, and thalamus, hubs or subsystems of the DMN. The insular cortex particularly receives sensory inputs from both interoceptive and exteroceptive sources and integrates these multimodal signals59.
Fourth, how well does HRV reflect brain oscillatory activity? Several neuroimaging studies provided intimate connections between HRV endpoints and different brain regions, showing bidirectional relations between the heart and brain. Levels of HRV activity are considered to associate with structures and functions of brain’s neural networks, including the DMN, SN and ECN34. Based on this information, we examined how astronauts adapt to microgravity by analyzing HRV changes in specific frequency regions linked to specific brain functional networks in repeated 24-hour ECG records over 6 months in space. These associations are extremely complex and will need to be validated in future studies.
New insights regarding the brain’s functional organization obtained from fMRI signals60 provide information regarding the spontaneous fluctuations of the resting-state brain. The spatial distribution of brain’s oscillatory activity was examined as a function of four frequency bands62: low-frequency (LF: 0.01–0.05 Hz); medium-frequency-1 (MF1: 0.05–0.10 Hz); medium-frequency-2 (MF2: 0.10–0.15 Hz); and high-frequency (HF: 0.15–0.20 Hz). Individual frequency bands had distinct spatial profiles. Frequency-dependent organizing rules for the best characterized resting-state network were observed, LF oscillations having most significant power in the mPFC, PCC, precuneus, lateral and medial parietal cortex, which together approximate brain regions comprising the DMN, the most prominent intrinsically connected network. The MF1 band was localized to the thalamus and basal ganglia, while the MF2 and HF bands associated with the orbitofrontal, insula, temporal cortex and subcortical regions, suggesting that more complex brain areas are dominated by higher-frequency brain oscillations. The DMN, in general resting-state networks, is thus considered to consist of frequency-dependent sub-regions.
Fluctuations in HRV indices appear in harmony with the behavior of networks in brain regions. The mPFC may be a particularly important part of the neural network21. Following adaptive changes in amygdala functional connectivity of patients with excessive worrying symptoms over 1 year72 showed that HRV represented progressive functional alterations in connectivity between the amygdala and mPFC.
Brain regions were identified where functional connectivity with the dACC and amygdala (key nodes of the salience network) varied in concert with changes in the HRV HF- and LF-components34. Increases in HF-component were accompanied by increases in functional connectivity between the dACC and regions including the basal ganglia, thalamus, midbrain and brainstem, and between the amygdala and regions including the basal ganglia, anterior insula and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Increases in LF-component also related positively to changes in connectivity between the temporoparietal junction (TPJ) and both the dACC and amygdala, which provides connections to the insula, and is associated with changes in vigilance states and attentional shifts34. The TPJ is a key node of the DMN31–34. Increase in LF-component suggests that DMN’s role in adaptation to microgravity and in coordination of coordinating functional connections with other brain networks, including the SN.
The mPFC is another important node in both DMN and SN21,70, providing an anatomical site for shifting from DMN to SN activation. mPFC activation is positively related to HF-HRV. Despite the lack of overall association between HF-HRV and DMN, a positive association was reported between HF-HRV and mPFC71. Merits of long-term HRV assessment over 24 hours in psychophysiological research have been recognized82.
HRV is a biomarker reflecting activities of the brain integration system that regulate adaptive physiological adjustments and autonomic outflow to the periphery. Three brain regions were identified to relate to the HRV HF-component, the ventromedial PFC, ACC and amygdala. Brain regions have also been classified relative to likely indicators of sympathetic and parasympathetic (AIC, hippocampal formation, amygdala and dorsal PCC) control69.
Caution, however, needs to be exercised in interpreting neuroimaging studies investigating dynamic associations between brain connectivity and HRV68. Factors underlying inter-individual differences in HRV have been reported to include brain morphology74. The amount of parasympathetic activity of HRV correlated positively with the cortical thickness of an area within the right anterior midcingulate cortex.
In conclusion, assuming that HRV varies in concert with changes in brain functional connectivity, we analyzed adaptive responses of astronauts to examine how brain functional networks dynamically changed over time in space, using repeated 24-hour HRV. Focusing on 2 HRV frequency bands, the HRV-SFO (0.10–0.20 Hz), reflecting a switch to the alerted default mode, and the HRV-VSFO (0.01–0.05 Hz), showing an activation of the DMN, we found that astronauts adapted to microgravity in diverse ways. We propose that adaptation to microgravity occurs primarily according to two primary processes, one involving the dynamics of large-scale brain networks, initiated by the DMN (Group A) and sometimes supported by the SN (Group B1), and another coordinated by the circadian system (all astronauts, particularly those in Group B2). The adaptation process proceeded even in the absence of consciousness. It may thus not be a dream for humans to live on Mars: together with getting strong cooperation from the circadian system, brain neural networks, the DMN in particular, may facilitate adaptation to the novel space environment.
Acknowledgements
The authors thank Dr. I. Tayama, T. Aiba and S. Ishida from the Space Biomedical Research Group, Japan Aerospace Exploration Agency (JAXA), for cooperation in our study. The authors also acknowledge the cooperation of the astronauts, the engineers, staff and managers of JAXA and NASA. The help of Larry A. Beaty to improve the English language for greater clarity and readability is greatly appreciated. JAXA Chronobiology Project was supported by the Japan Aerospace Exploration Agency (K.M., H.O., S.F., C.M.) and Halberg Chronobiology Fund (G.C.).
Author Contributions
K.O. and C.M. designed the study. K.O. and G.C. wrote the first draft of manuscript and prepared the figures. Y.K., K.S. and M.H. analyzed the data and contributed to the writing and editing of the manuscript. K.M., H.O. and S.F. contributed to the editing of the manuscript. All authors read and contributed to the final version of the manuscripts.
Competing Interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher's note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
References
- 1.Baevsky RM, et al. Autonomic cardiovascular and respiratory control during prolonged spaceflights aboard the International Space Station. J Appl Physiol. 2007;103:156–161. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.00137.2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Verheyden B, Liu J, Beckers F, Aubert AE. Operational point of neural cardiovascular regulation in humans up to 6 months in space. J Appl Physiol. 2010;108:646–654. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.00883.2009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Xu D, et al. Reduced heart rate variability during sleep in long-duration spaceflight. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol. 2013;305:R164–R170. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.00423.2012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Norsk P, Asmar A, Damgaard M, Christensen NJ. Fluid shifts, vasodilatation and ambulatory blood pressure reduction during long duration spaceflight. J Physiol. 2015;593:573–584. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2014.284869. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Crucian B, et al. Alterations in adaptive immunity persist during long-duration spaceflight. npj Microgravity. 2015;1:5013. doi: 10.1038/npjmgrav.2015.13. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Aubert AE, et al. Towards human exploration of space: the THESEUS review series on cardiovascular, respiratory, and renal research priorities. npj Microgravity. 2016;2:16031. doi: 10.1038/npjmgrav.2016.31. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Van Ombergen A, et al. Spaceflight-induced neuroplasticity in humans as measured by MRI: what do we know so far? npj Microgravity. 2017;3:2. doi: 10.1038/s41526-016-0010-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Roberts DR, et al. Effects of spaceflight on astronaut brain structure as indicated on MRI. N Engl J Med. 2017;377:1746–1753. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa1705129. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Fomina EV, Lysova NY, Kukoba TB, Grishin AP, Kornienko MB. One-Year Mission on ISS Is a Step Towards Interplanetary Missions. Aerosp Med Hum Perform. 2017;88:1094–1099. doi: 10.3357/AMHP.4847.2017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Yamamoto N, et al. Effects of long-term microgravity exposure in space on circadian rhythms of heart rate variability. Chronobiology International. 2015;32:327–340. doi: 10.3109/07420528.2014.979940. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11. Otsuka K et al. Intrinsic cardiovascular autonomic regulatory system of astronauts exposed long-term to microgravity in space: observational study. npj Microgravity 1, 15018; 10.1038/npjmgrav.2015.18, Erratum in: npj Microgravity. 8, 12; 10.1038/npjmgrav.2016.31 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 12.Otsuka K, et al. Long-term exposure to space’s microgravity alters the time structure of heart rate variability of astronauts. Heliyon. 2016;2:e00211. doi: 10.1016/j.heliyon.2016.e00211. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Halberg F, Stephens AN. Susceptibility to ouabain and physiologic circadian periodicity. Proc Minn Acad Sci. 1959;27:139–143. [Google Scholar]
- 14.Takahashi JS, Hong HK, Ko CH, McDearmon EL. The genetics of mammalian circadian order and disorder: implications for physiology and disease. Nature Rev. Genet. 2008;9:764–775. doi: 10.1038/nrg2430. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Dibner C, Schibler U, Albrecht U. The mammalian circadian timing system: organization and coordination of central and peripheral clocks. Annu Rev Physiol. 2010;72:517–549. doi: 10.1146/annurev-physiol-021909-135821. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Flynn-Evans EE, Barger LK, Kubey AA, Sullivan JP, Czeisler CA. Circadian misalignment affects sleep and medication use before and during spaceflight. npj Microgravity. 2016;2:15019. doi: 10.1038/npjmgrav.2015.19. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17. van der Veen, D. R. et al. Flexible clock systems: adjusting the temporal programme. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 19, 372 pii: 20160254; 10.1098/rstb.2016.0254 (2017) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 18.Halberg F, et al. Cycles tipping the scale between death and survival (=“life”) Progress of Theoretical Physics Suppl. 2008;173:153–181. doi: 10.1143/PTPS.173.153. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Otsuka, K., Cornélissen, G. & Halberg, F. Chronomics and Continuous Ambulatory Blood Pressure Monitoring – Vascular Chronomics: From 7-Day/24-Hour to Lifelong Monitoring. Tokyo: Springer Japan, 2016, pp. 870+ lxxv; 10.1007/978-4-431-54631-3 (2016)
- 20.Valdez P, Ramírez C, García A. Circadian rhythms in cognitive performance: implications for neuropsychological assessment. ChronoPhysiology Therapy. 2012;2:81–92. doi: 10.2147/CPT.S32586. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Thayer JF, Ahs F, Fredrikson M, Sollers JJ, 3rd, Wager TD. A meta-analysis of heart rate variability and neuroimaging studies: implications for heart rate variability as a marker of stress and health. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2012;36:747–756. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.11.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Buckner RL, Andrews-Hanna JR, Schacter DL. The brain’s default network: anatomy, function, and relevance to disease. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2008;1124:1–38. doi: 10.1196/annals.1440.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Baars B. Spontaneous repetitive thoughts can be adaptive: Postscript on “mind wandering”. Psych Bull. 2010;136:208–210. doi: 10.1037/a0018726. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Andrews-Hanna JR. The brain’s default network and its adaptive role in internal mentation. Neuroscientist. 2012;18:251–270. doi: 10.1177/1073858411403316. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Kucyi A, Davis KD. Dynamic functional connectivity of the default mode network tracks daydreaming. Neuroimage. 2014;100:471–480. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.06.044. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Craig AD. How do you feel–now? The anterior insula and human awareness. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2009;10:59–70. doi: 10.1038/nrn2555. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Raichle ME, et al. A default mode of brain function. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2001;98:676–682. doi: 10.1073/pnas.98.2.676. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Clark L, Cools R, Robbins TW. The neuropsychology of ventral prefrontal cortex: decision-making and reversal learning. Brain Cogn. 2004;55:41–53. doi: 10.1016/S0278-2626(03)00284-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Fellows LK. The role of the orbitofrontal cortex in decision making: a component process account. Ann NY Acad Sci. 2007;1121:421–430. doi: 10.1196/annals.1401.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Lin P, et al. Static and dynamic posterior cingulate cortex nodal topology of default mode network predicts attention task performance. Brain Imaging Behav. 2016;10:212–225. doi: 10.1007/s11682-015-9384-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Andrews-Hanna JR, Reidler JS, Sepulcre J, Poulin R, Buckner RL. Functional-anatomic fractionation of the brain’s default network. Neuron. 2010;65:550–562. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.02.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Kucyi A, Hodaie M, Davis KD. Lateralization in intrinsic functional connectivity of the temporoparietal junction with salience- and attention-related brain networks. J Neurophysiol. 2012;108:3382–3392. doi: 10.1152/jn.00674.2012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Mars RB, et al. Connectivity-based subdivisions of the human right “temporoparietal junction area”: evidence for different areas participating in different cortical networks. Cereb Cortex. 2012;22:1894–1903. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr268. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Chang C, et al. Association between heart rate variability and fluctuations in resting-state functional connectivity. Neuroimage. 2013;68:93–104. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.11.038. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Zald DH, et al. Meta-analytic connectivity modeling reveals differential functional connectivity of the medial and lateral orbitofrontal cortex. Cereb Cortex. 2014;24:232–248. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs308. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Fettes P, Schulze L, Downar J. Cortico-striatal-thalamic loop circuits of the orbitofrontal cortex: promising therapeutic targets in psychiatric illness. Front Syst Neurosci. 2007;27(11):25. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2017.00025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Mason MF, et al. Wandering minds: the default network and stimulus-independent thought. Science. 2007;315:393–395. doi: 10.1126/science.1131295. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Hasenkamp W, Wilson-Mendenhall CD, Duncan E, Barsalou LW. Mind wandering and attention during focused meditation: a fine-grained temporal analysis of fluctuating cognitive states. Neuroimage. 2012;59:750–760. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.07.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Sridharan D, Levitin DJ, Menon V. A critical role for the right fronto-insular cortex in switching between central-executive and default-mode networks. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2008;105:12569–12574. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0800005105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Chen AC, et al. Causal interactions between fronto-parietal central executive and default-mode networks in humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2013;110:19944–19949. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1311772110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Sidlauskaite J, et al. Anticipatory processes in brain state switching - evidence from a novel cued-switching task implicating default mode and salience networks. Neuroimage. 2014;98:359–365. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.05.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Lamichhane B, Dhamala M. The salience network and its functional architecture in a perceptual decision: an effective connectivity study. Brain Connect. 2015;5:362–370. doi: 10.1089/brain.2014.0282. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Liang X, Zou Q, He Y, Yang Y. Topologically reorganized connectivity architecture of default-mode, executive-control, and salience networks across working memory task loads. Cereb Cortex. 2016;26:1501–1511. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhu316. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Clemens B, et al. Alerted default mode: functional connectivity changes in the aftermath of social stress. Sci Rep. 2017;7:40180. doi: 10.1038/srep40180. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Lin P, et al. Dynamic Default Mode Network across Different Brain States. Sci Rep. 2017;7:46088. doi: 10.1038/srep46088. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Kabbara A, El Falou W, Khalil M, Wendling F, Hassan M. The dynamic functional core network of the human brain at rest. Sci Rep. 2017;7:2936. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-03420-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Hermans EJ, Henckens MJ, Joëls M, Fernández G. Dynamic adaptation of large-scale brain networks in response to acute stressors. Trends Neurosci. 2014;37:304–314. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2014.03.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Salomon R, et al. The insula mediates access to awareness of visual stimuli presented synchronously to the heartbeat. J Neurosci. 2016;36:5115–5127. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4262-15.2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Craig AD. Significance of the insula for the evolution of human awareness of feelings from the body. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2011;1225:72–82. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2011.05990.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Cauda F, et al. Functional connectivity of the insula in the resting brain. Neuroimage. 2011;55:8–23. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.11.049. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Meneguzzo P, Tsakiris M, Schloth HB, Stein DJ, Brooks SJ. Subliminal versus supraliminal stimuli activate neural responses in anterior cingulate cortex, fusiform gyrus and insula: a meta-analysis of fMRI studies. BMC Psychol. 2014;2:52. doi: 10.1186/s40359-014-0052-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Henke K, et al. Nonconscious formation and reactivation of semantic associations by way of the medial temporal lobe. Neuropsychologia. 2003;41:863–876. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(03)00035-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Züst MA, et al. Hippocampus is place of interaction between unconscious and conscious memories. PLoS One. 2015;10:e0122459. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0122459. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Bargh JA, Ferguson MJ. Beyond behaviorism: on the automaticity of higher mental processes. Psychol Bull. 2000;126:925–945. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.126.6.925. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Lau HC, Passingham RE. Unconscious activation of the cognitive control system in the human prefrontal cortex. J Neurosci. 2007;27:5805–5811. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4335-06.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Zimmerman, M. The nervous system in the context of information theory, (ed. Schmidt RF, Thews G) Human physiology, 166–173 (Berlin, Springer, 1989)
- 57.van Gaal S, Lamme VA, Ridderinkhof KR. Unconsciously triggered conflict adaptation. PLoS One. 2010;5:e11508. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0011508. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Horga G, Maia TV. Conscious and unconscious processes in cognitive control: a theoretical perspective and a novel empirical approach. Front Hum Neurosci. 2012;6:199. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00199. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Salomon R, et al. The insula mediates access to awareness of visual stimuli presented synchronously to the heartbeat. J Neurosci. 2016;36:5115–5127. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4262-15.2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Biswal B, Yetkin FZ, Haughton VM, Hyde JS. Functional connectivity in the motor cortex of resting human brain using echo-planar MRI. Magn Reson Med. 1995;34:537–541. doi: 10.1002/mrm.1910340409. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Bajaj S, Adhikari BM, Dhamala M. Higher frequency network activity flow predicts lower frequency node activity in intrinsic low-frequency BOLD fluctuations. PLoS One. 2013;8:e64466. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0064466. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Baria AT, Baliki MN, Parrish T, Apkarian AV. Anatomical and functional assemblies of brain BOLD oscillations. J Neurosci. 2011;31:7910–7919. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1296-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Chand GB, Dhamala M. Interactions among the brain default-mode, salience, and central-executive networks during perceptual decision-making of moving dots. Brain Connect. 2016;6:249–254. doi: 10.1089/brain.2015.0379. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Chen JE, Glover GH. BOLD fractional contribution to resting-state functional connectivity above 0.1 Hz. Neuroimage. 2015;107:207–218. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.12.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Gohel SR, Biswal BB. Functional integration between brain regions at rest occurs in multiple-frequency bands. Brain Connect. 2015;5:23–34. doi: 10.1089/brain.2013.0210. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Pfurtscheller G, Andrade A, Koschutnig K, Brunner C, da Silva FL. Initiation of voluntary movements at free will and ongoing 0.1-Hz BOLD oscillations in the insula-a pilot study. Front Integr Neurosci. 2014;8:93. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2014.00093. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Sasai S, et al. Frequency-specific network topologies in the resting human brain. Front Hum Neurosci. 2014;8:1022. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.01022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Allen B, Jennings JR, Gianaros PJ, Thayer JF, Manuck SB. Resting high-frequency heart rate variability is related to resting brain perfusion. Psychophysiology. 2015;52:277–287. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12321. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Beissner F, Meissner K, Bär KJ, Napadow V. The autonomic brain: an activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis for central processing of autonomic function. J Neurosci. 2013;33:10503–10511. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1103-13.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Gianaros PJ, Wager TD. Brain-Body Pathways Linking Psychological Stress and Physical Health. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2015;24:313–321. doi: 10.1177/0963721415581476. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Jennings JR, Sheu LK, Kuan DC, Manuck SB, Gianaros PJ. Resting state connectivity of the medial prefrontal cortex covaries with individual differences in high-frequency heart rate variability. Psychophysiology. 2016;53:444–454. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12586. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Makovac E, et al. Amygdala functional connectivity as a longitudinal biomarker of symptom changes in generalized anxiety. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2016;11:1719–1728. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsw091. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Nikolin S, Boonstra TW, Loo CK, Martin D. Combined effect of prefrontal transcranial direct current stimulation and a working memory task on heart rate variability. PLoS One. 2017;12:e0181833. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0181833. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Winkelmann T, et al. Structural brain correlates of heart rate variability in a healthy young adult population. Brain Struct Funct. 2017;222:1061–1068. doi: 10.1007/s00429-016-1185-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Larson-Prior LJ, et al. Modulation of the brain’s functional network architecture in the transition from wake to sleep. Prog Brain Res. 2011;193:277–294. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-444-53839-0.00018-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Task F. of the European Society of Cardiology and the North American Society of Pacing and Electrophysiology Heart rate variability: standards of measurement, physiological interpretation, and clinical use. Circulation. 1996;93:1043–1065. doi: 10.1161/01.CIR.93.5.1043. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Saito, K., Koyama, A., Yoneyama, K., Sawada, Y. & Ohtomo, N. ed., A recent Advances in Time Series Analysis by Maximum Entropy (Method., Hokkaido University Press, Sapporo, 1994).
- 78.Blautzik J, et al. Classifying fMRI-derived resting-state connectivity patterns according to their daily rhythmicity. Neuroimage. 2013;71:298–306. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.08.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Hodkinson DJ, et al. Circadian and homeostatic modulation of functional connectivity and regional cerebral blood flow in humans under normal entrained conditions. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab. 2014;34:1493–1499. doi: 10.1038/jcbfm.2014.109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Jiang C, et al. Diurnal variations in neural activity of healthy human brain decoded with resting-state blood oxygen level dependent fMRI. Front Hum Neurosci. 2016;10:634. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00634. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Muto V, et al. Local modulation of human brain responses by circadian rhythmicity and sleep debt. Science. 2016;353:687–690. doi: 10.1126/science.aad2993. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Laborde S, Mosley E, Thayer JF. Heart rate variability and cardiac vagal tone in psychophysiological research – Recommendations for experiment planning, data analysis, and data reporting. Front. Psychol. 2017;8:213. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00213. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]