Abstract
Although anger and aggression can have wide-ranging consequences for social interactions, there is sparse knowledge as to which brain activations underlie the feelings of anger and the regulation of related punishment behaviors. To address these issues, we studied brain activity while participants played an economic interaction paradigm called Inequality Game (IG). The current study confirms that the IG elicits anger through the competitive behavior of an unfair (versus fair) other and promotes punishment behavior. Critically, when participants see the face of the unfair other, self-reported anger is parametrically related to activations in temporal areas and amygdala – regions typically associated with mentalizing and emotion processing, respectively. During anger provocation, activations in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, an area important for regulating emotions, predicted the inhibition of later punishment behavior. When participants subsequently engaged in behavioral decisions for the unfair versus fair other, increased activations were observed in regions involved in behavioral adjustment and social cognition, comprising posterior cingulate cortex, temporal cortex, and precuneus. These data point to a distinction of brain activations related to angry feelings and the control of subsequent behavioral choices. Furthermore, they show a contribution of prefrontal control mechanisms during anger provocation to the inhibition of later punishment.
Introduction
Although anger and aggression have been researched since decades, there are still few studies on the neural functions that dissociate feelings of anger from the regulation of aggressive responses or reactive punishment behaviors. Anger and aggression are conceptually related, but anger does not always result in aggression1,2. In fact, the emotion of anger, which is defined as a negative emotional response to goal-blockage and unfair behavior by others3,4, is conceptually distinct from aggression, which is defined as an action intended to cause harm to another individual5. Although aggression is often perceived as maladaptive, certain forms of aggression may actually serve evolutionary adaptive purposes, such as securing resources or defending against attacks6.
The neural correlates of both anger and aggression have previously been investigated with computer-based paradigms such as the Taylor Aggression Paradigm7,8. In this competitive reaction time task, there is an alternation between provocation periods in which participants receive painful stimuli by another player and periods in which participants can retaliate by administering electric shocks to the other player. In this task, activations in amygdala and superior temporal sulcus (STS) when watching the opponent suffer were found to correlate with the intensity of the stimulus administered by the participant to the other player7. In a different study using this paradigm, activations in the anterior insula and the anterior cingulate cortex were related to reactive aggression8. However, in spite of the valuable insights obtained in these studies, there is so far no consensus about the brain activations associated with anger itself and the subsequent behavior toward anger-eliciting others. This may partly be due to the alternation of provocation and aggression inherent in the Taylor Aggression Paradigm, which makes it difficult to disentangle angry feelings from social behavior like punishment. Furthermore and as detailed below, most extant studies have focused on the neural correlates of either anger or aggression.
Pertaining to the brain activation patterns underlying anger, the majority of neuroimaging studies have investigated this emotion indirectly by showing angry faces9,10, or by using recall11, imagery12,13 or rumination14 of anger-eliciting situations. The results of these studies are very divergent in their findings. Whereas some findings point to an involvement of the orbitofrontal cortex10, other results suggest reduced activations in the orbitofrontal cortex and somatosensory cortex11 as well as increased activations in the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) and insula11. Still other studies found an association of the temporal poles12,13 or the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex14 with anger.
The divergent nature of these results may have arisen from the indirect way of evoking anger, which may not be representative of actual feelings of anger. Paradigms in which participants are looking at angry faces, for instance, may simply activate the concept of anger15 or rather induce other emotions, such as fear. Moreover, studies employing paradigms relying on recall, imagery, or rumination rely heavily on the capacity of participants to internally relive a situation and this capacity may vary depending on the participants’ personality and the situation. In order to circumvent these shortcomings, it is important to measure neural activations while participants actually feel anger (for instance when seeing the face of a person who actually induced anger as opposed to seeing a face that merely expresses anger without necessarily inducing it in the viewer).
With regard to aggression, animal experiments and lesion studies in humans suggest that aggressive behavior is governed by subcortical brain circuits in the hypothalamus, amygdala, and brainstem, and that the frontal cortex, which integrates social information and can modulate activations in the hypothalamus and amygdala16. Whereas aggressive responses to a frustrating or threatening event are mediated by limbic systems, more controlled and goal-oriented aggression instead is regulated by higher order cortical systems16. In accordance with this assumption, evidence from lesion studies points to a causal role for the orbitofrontal and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) in modulating the expression of aggressive behaviors17. Meta-analytic evidence also suggests that the DLPFC is an important brain region for the regulation of emotional responses18. Accordingly, it has been suggested that the DLPFC might be crucial for inhibiting aggression, as aggression may result as a consequence of deficits in emotion regulation19. Moreover, structural or functional impairments in the DLPFC, temporal lobe, and ACC have been observed in antisocial populations20. Although the above-mentioned lesion and neuroimaging studies on emotion regulation suggest that the DLPFC should play a role in aggression inhibition, functional neuroimaging data supporting this claim is so far missing.
In order to investigate brain activations related to angry feelings and the inhibition of related punishment behavior, we conducted an fMRI experiment with a final sample of 25 male participants who played the Inequality Game (IG), a novel paradigm recently introduced and validated in a previous study2. This interactive economic game (Fig. 1) has been shown to induce anger by confronting the participant with an unfair other player and to elicit different patterns of subsequent behavioral responses, including punishment of the unfair other, aggressive behavior towards the fair other as well as cooperative behavior towards both the fair and the unfair other2.
More specifically, in the IG, participants are iteratively paired with one of two other players (a fair and an unfair other) to engage in economic choices or feedback choices. A photograph of the other player is displayed each time the pairing changes. As illustrated in Fig. 1, the IG consists of three phases in which power is manipulated (high or low). The IG begins with a high power phase at baseline in which the participant is in control of choosing cooperative (i.e., high gain for both players) or competitive (i.e., high gain for participant and low gain for the other) economic outcomes for himself and one of the other players. In addition, the participant can decide whether to send nice (e.g., “You are very nice”) or derogatory (e.g., “You are annoying”) feedback messages to each of the two other players. This high power phase serves to assess participants’ behavioral preferences for engaging in cooperative or competitive behavior at baseline. It is followed by a low power phase with provocation in which the participant (low power) is confronted with the unfair other’s competitive economic choices (i.e., high gain for himself and low gain for the participant) and derogatory feedback messages (e.g., “You are annoying”). Conversely, in this low power phase, the fair other chooses cooperative economic choices (i.e., high gain for himself and the participant) and nice feedback messages (e.g., “You are very nice”). In line with previous literature3,4,14,21, this confrontation with the unfair as compared to the fair other during the low power phase serves to provoke anger in participants. Finally, participants engage in a high power phase after provocation in which their behavioral choices after anger provocation are measured. To isolate the impact of the players’ intentional choices from high or low monetary outcomes per se, control conditions are included in which there are only high or low gains for both players to choose from.
In addition to measuring participants’ brain activation by means of fMRI, we assessed the effects of the unfair other’s behavior on participants’ feelings through questionnaire-based measures of participants’ emotions (see Methods). The extent to which participants engaged in cooperative as opposed to competitive economic choices towards the unfair other in the high power phase following anger provocation (compared to baseline) served to assess the degree of participants’ punishment inhibition.
Overall, our study had three main objectives. First, to test whether the behavioral patterns and feelings evoked by the IG2 can be replicated in a neuroimaging study. Second, to validate the IG with neuroimaging data by investigating the brain activations related to the different events and phases of the IG. Thirdly, we aimed at delineating brain activations related to anger, aggression and punishment inhibition.
Results
Replication of behavioral patterns in the Inequality Game
In the current study, the behavioral characteristics of the IG2 could be replicated in a sample of participants with low scores of trait aggression as assessed by questionnaires (for details, see Supplementary Table 1) who on average did not engage in aggressive behavior towards the other players (i.e. competitive decisions) during the high power baseline phase of the IG (Fig. 2 and Supplementary Tables 1–5 and Supplementary Figs S1–4 in Part A–Behavioral Data).
To investigate participants’ feelings in response to the confrontation with a fair versus an unfair other in the IG, questionnaire-based data were submitted to a repeated-measure MANOVA with the within-subject factor other (2 levels: fair and unfair) and the self-reported feelings of anger, joy, disappointment, and sadness in response to the other’s economic choices as dependent variables. Results revealed a main effect of other across all emotions (F(4,21) = 13.75, P < 0.001, ƞ² = 0.72) As depicted in Fig. 2a, follow-up tests confirmed previous behavioral results2 by showing that exposure to competitive choices of the unfair other player during the low power phase reliably evoked feelings of anger, disappointment, and sadness, whereas exposure to cooperative choices by the fair other reliably induced feelings of joy. The same pattern of feelings (more anger, disappointment and sadness and less joy) was observed in response to derogatory as opposed to nice feedback as well as towards the unfair as opposed to the fair other player as a person (Supplementary Figs S1 and S2, respectively).
Behavioral decisions were analyzed with a 2 × 2 repeated-measure ANOVA with the within-subject factors other (fair and unfair) and phase (high power at baseline and high power after provocation), using the dependent variable economic choice (amount of cooperative minus competitive choices for the other player). This revealed a significant main effect of other (F(1,24) = 16.46, P < 0.001; ƞ² = 0.41) and phase (F(1,24) = 12.62, P < 0.01; ƞ² = 0.35), as well as an interaction between other and phase (F(1,24) = 7.44, P < 0.05; ƞ² = 0.24). As depicted in Fig. 2b follow-up comparisons showed that while economic choices towards the fair and unfair other were mainly cooperative at baseline and did not differ, participants displayed more punishment towards the unfair other after provocation (P < 0.01), while there were no changes in participants’ behavior towards the fair other player. As depicted in Supplementary Fig. S3, the same pattern of behavior was observed for feedback messages (nice, derogatory) sent to the other. These data indicate that exposure to unfair behavior did not only evoke feelings of anger (and reduced joy), but also promoted more punishment behavior toward the unfair other.
In spite of this effect, binomial tests revealed that participants’ cooperative economic choices towards both other players in the high power phase at baseline were significantly more frequent than expected by chance distribution (P < 0.001) and that in spite the low power phase with provocation there was an overall tendency for prosocial behavior to be maintained during the high power phase after provocation (P = 0.05, Supplementary Table 5).
Interestingly, self-reported feelings of anger towards the unfair other predicted the change in punishment behavior towards the unfair other from the first high-power phase at baseline to the subsequent high-power phase after provocation (r = 0.54, P = 0.005). In line with previous findings2, we also found that empathic concern was positively correlated with the extent to which participants inhibited their aggressive behavior towards the unfair other in the high power phase after provocation (rs = 0.41, P < 0.05), i.e., the frequency of cooperative rather than competitive economic decisions in this phase. A similar trend was observed for perspective taking (rs = 0.37, P = 0.07).
Validation of the Inequality Game with neuroimaging data
As a first manipulation check of our neuroimaging data, we examined brain activation patterns evoked when, across all phases, participants saw the face of another player at the start of a given trial (Supplementary Table 6 in Part B–Neuroimaging Data). This revealed significant increases in occipital cortex and fusiform gyrus, two brain regions typically engaged during visual processing and face perception22,23, respectively.
As a second manipulation check, we tested which brain activity was elicited during economic choices or the selection of feedback messages across all phases. As detailed in Supplementary Table 7 in Part B–Neuroimaging Data, activations were increased in the precuneus, DLPFC, ACC and mid cingulate cortex (MCC; P < 0.05 FWE corrected). The DLPFC is a key area for attention and action selection24, while ACC and MCC play an important role in response conflict monitoring25 and social decision making26,27. Together with the precuneus, which has rich connections to other brain areas involved in sensorimotor, memory, and cognitive processing28, this distributed activation pattern suggests an integration of decision-making processes with motor planning and self-monitoring processes.
As a third step of validating the IG on a neuroimaging level, we tested whether, during the low power phase with provocation, cooperative choices of the fair other would elicit different brain activations than competitive choices of the unfair other. When participants were confronted with competitive choices of the unfair other (as opposed to the control condition with the same monetary outcome), increased activations in the inferior occipital cortex were measured (Supplementary Table 8, P < 0.05 FWE corrected), presumably due to enhanced visual attention to the displayed outcome. Conversely, cooperative choices of the fair other (as opposed to the control condition with the same monetary outcome) were associated with increased activations in distributed areas spanning the temporal cortex, frontopolar cortex, posterior cingulate cortex (PCC), and the ventral striatum (Supplementary Table 8, P < 0.05 FWE corrected). This suggests an interaction between brain networks implicated in social cognition29 and the ventral striatum that is crucial for reward processing in general30 and social reward processes in particular31,32.
Brain activity related to anger, aggression and punishment inhibition
To test the brain activations elicited by being confronted with an unfair other, we contrasted BOLD activations when participants saw the face of the unfair versus the fair other player in the low power phase with provocation. This revealed that seeing the unfair as opposed to the fair other’s face produced significant activations in somatosensory cortices (Supplementary Table 9, P < 0.05 FWE corrected). No other significant activations were observed when contrasting fair and unfair faces in each of the phases of the IG (Supplementary Table 9).
In a next step, we investigated in how far neural responses to an unfair other in the low power phase with provocation were related to feelings of anger and to subsequent aggression and punishment inhibition.
First, we tested how brain activations when seeing the unfair versus fair other were correlated to self-reported feelings of anger towards the unfair other. This revealed that self-reported anger was associated with higher activation in this contrast in the amygdala, superior temporal sulcus (STS), and fusiform gyrus (Fig. 3a, Supplementary Table 9, P < 0.05 FWE corrected). These brain areas are involved in processing social information29, faces22,23, and affectively relevant cues33,34. Interestingly, higher activity in bilateral anterior DLPFC and ACC – two interconnected regions24 that are important for emotion regulation35 – in the same contrast predicted more frequent inhibition of subsequent punishment behavior by the participant (Fig. 3b, Supplementary Table 9, P < 0.05 FWE corrected). This effect was specific for the low power phase. In fact, there was no correlation between brain activations when seeing the unfair versus fair other’s face and the inhibition of aggressive behavior after provocation during the high power phase at baseline or the high power phase after provocation (for details, see Supplementary Table 9).
In order to test which brain activations are involved in the actual behavioral choices for the unfair as opposed to the fair other, we compared conditions when participants made active economic choices (as opposed to control choices) for the unfair as opposed to the fair other in the high power phase after provocation. This comparison revealed higher activations in several areas involved in social cognition29 and behavioral adjustment36, comprising PCC, precuneus and middle temporal gyrus (Supplementary Table 10, P < 0.05 FWE corrected). As depicted in Fig. 4, higher involvement of PCC, precuneus and superior temporal gyrus were also present when testing the interaction of the factors phase and other by comparing choices made for the unfair versus the fair other in the high power phase after provocation, relative to the high power phase at baseline (Supplementary Table 10, P < 0.05 FWE corrected). In addition, this contrast also revealed activations in the DLPFC (Supplementary Table 10, P < 0.05 FWE corrected). This finding suggests that when participants make choices for the unfair as opposed to the fair other player in the high power phase after provocation, DLPFC activations important for emotion regulation35 still play a role together with activations related to social cognition29 and behavioral adjustment36 encompassing the PCC, precuneus and temporal gyrus. The specificity of these results was confirmed by showing that no other contrast exhibited these activation patterns (for details, see Supplementary Table 10).
In an exploratory analysis, we wanted to test to what degree the brain activations in bilateral DLPFC in response to the face of the unfair other during the low power phase with provocation that were predicitive of later punishment inhibition were linked with brain activations during the actual economic choices for the unfair other in the high power phase after provocation. To this end, we carried out the following analysis: Using marsbar, we extracted activations from a 9 mm sphere around the peaks of the R and L DLPFC from the regression of the contrast Unfair – Fair Other’s Face in low power phase with provocation with the inhibition of subsequent punishment behavior (from Supplementary Table 9). In a next step, these values were entered in a regression analysis with the contrast of participants’ economic choices for the unfair versus fair other in the high power phase after provocation (from Supplementary Table 10). As detailed in Supplementary Table 10, this regression revealed that the R DLPFC activations when seeing the unfair other during the low power phase with provocation that were related to the later inhibition of punishment correlated with activations in the left primary and supplementary motor cortex when participants actually made their choices for the unfair other (P < 0.05, FWE corrected, cluster level threshold). In other words, brain activations related to emotion regulation35 during the provocation phase are positively correlated with activations in motor cortices of the contralateral side to the hand with which participants indicated their decisions for the unfair other in the high power phase after provocation, suggesting a long-term link between processes related to emotion regulation during provocation and later action selection. Supporting the consistency of this finding, an equivalent relation was found for L DLPFC activations, although these activations were only marginally significant with P = 0.06 (FWE corrected, cluster level threshold).
Discussion
In the present study we do not only replicate the validity of the IG to elicit anger and punishment behavior in controlled settings, but also extend it to the neuroimaging level by highlighting specific neural substrates for emotional and behavioral processes recruited in these conditions. Specifically, we unveil dissociable networks related to feelings of anger and the control of subsequent punishment, implicating brain activations involved in social cognition and inhibitory control, respectively. We also report that social decision making differentially involves a distributed network including the PCC, precuneus, and superior temporal gyrus when directed to an unfair (versus fair) other.
On the behavioral level, this study shows for the first time that participants engage in cooperative economic choices for both other players in the high power phase at baseline. At the same time, the present data confirm previous behavioral findings2 by showing an increase in punishment behavior for the unfair other player in the high power phase after provocation. This indicates that punishment behavior in the IG is a result of the unfair other player’s behavior during the low power phase with provocation. The current study also confirms previous findings on the level of elicited emotions2 by showing that the confrontation with the unfair other’s competitive economic behavior and derogatory feedback also induces anger, disappointment and sadness, while reducing feelings of joy. In addition, we replicate that the higher participants score on empathic concern, the more they inhibit punishment behavior after provocation2.
On the level of brain imaging, previous studies on anger have often employed paradigms that did not necessarily induce true anger in the participant, such as the presentation of angry faces9,10, recall of anger-eliciting events11, imagery12,13, or rumination14. Moreover, these paradigms yielded very divergent findings, which might be due to the indirect and less reliable method of inducing anger.
In the present study, we found that the intensity of experienced anger when seeing the face of the unfair other was parametrically related to activations in amygdala, STS, and fusiform gyrus. While these findings differ from previous results obtained with paradigms indirectly studying anger10,11, they are in line with script-based studies12,13 on the implication of the STS in anger experience. Activations in the amygdala and STS have also been observed to be parametrically modulated by the intensity of a painful stimulus that was administered to an unfair other in a study in which anger and aggression were alternating7. The present study goes beyond the past studies in showing anger-related activations in the amygdala – a crucial component of emotion networks which is linked to affective appraisals and elicitation of defensive behaviors in animals16. In humans, amygdala activity signals motivational relevance33,34, whether for positive or negative events35,37, and promotes learning of associations with rewards or punishments38. Together with temporal areas involved in social cognition and mentalizing29, and activations in the fusiform gyrus, which is important for face processing22,23, these activation patterns may reflect appraisal and learning mechanisms related to provocation, yielding subjective feelings of anger.
Interestingly, unlike activity in the latter affective and social network, stronger activations in the DLPFC and ACC when seeing the unfair other during the provocation phase predicted the inhibition of aggressive behavior in the subsequent economic interaction phase. These two functionally interconnected regions24 are important for conflict resolution24, emotion regulation18,35, and the integration of motivational information for guiding goal-relevant decisions24,39. DLPFC lesions in animals and humans have been linked to increased aggression17 and transient DLPFC inhibition by transcranial magnetic stimulation was shown to reduce fairness behavior40. Previous studies also pointed to DLPFC involvement in third-party punishment41 or norm enforcement in bilateral economic interactions with responsible agents40. Moreover, impairments in the DLPFC and ACC have been reported for antisocial populations20. Their engagement in our paradigm might reflect how aversive experiences with an unfair other are integrated with contextual factors to determine future behavioral intentions, including punishment and the inhibition thereof.
In the economic interaction phase, greater activation was observed in PCC, precuneus and middle temporal gyrus when participants made behavioral decisions directed to the unfair other (as compared to the fair other). This pattern converges with previous observations during social decision making with provocative others8. The precuneus has rich connections to other brain areas involved in processing sensori-motor, visual, and cognitive information28. Together with activations in the PCC, which has been proposed to play a key role in contextual memory, change detection and subsequent behavioral adjustments36, and temporal lobe areas implicated in social cognition29, this activation pattern may be indicative of deeper integration of social and contextual information in decision making for unfair others.
In line with this observation, participants’ economic choices for the unfair as opposed to the fair other in the high power phase after provocation compared to the high power phase at baseline also showed increased activations in PCC, precuneus and temporal gyrus. In addition, this contrast revealed activations in the DLPFC, which is a key region for emotion regulation35. This brain activation pattern may represent the integration of emotion regulation with social decision-making during the inhibition of punishment behavior. In line with our previous study2 the behavioral data of the present study show that although participants on average punish the unfair other more than the fair other (Fig. 2b), the majority of participants can be classified as prosocial (Supplementary Table 5). In other words, even in the high power phase after provocation, patricipants select predominantly cooperative economic outcomes for the fair and the unfair other. In order to implement such an inhibition of punishment, the observed co-activations of regions involved in behavioral decisions, social cognition and emotion regulation may play a crucial role. To test for the robustness of this finding, future research is needed to investigate the role of these brain activations in more detail and with larger sample sizes and a variety of paradigms.
In summary, we extend previous work by dissociating the brain activity related to anger experience during provocation from subsequent regulations of punishment behavior. Anger-related brain activations when seeing an unfair other person were present in amygdala and STS, suggesting an interaction of temporal areas involved in social cognition29 with amygdala activations related to relevance and emotion processing. Stronger activations in DLPFC and ACC when seeing the unfair other predicted less punishment later on, emphasizing the importance of emotion control during the provocation phase in guiding subsequent punishment inhibition. As these networks were previously linked to impaired aggression regulation after brain lesions17 and in antisocial populations20, our findings provide new insights on functional mechanisms of aggression regulation that have important implications for effective clinical interventions in these populations. More generally, our paradigm may provide a novel and ecological tool to explore social and emotional processing in neuropsychiatric conditions associated with altered control of anger and aggressive behaviors.
Methods
Participants
We tested 32 male participants, of whom 7 had to be excluded as they did not believe that they were interacting with other players during the IG. The final sample thus comprised 25 men (mean age = 26.08 years, SD = 4.51). 13 of the participants were students (no students from psychology), 10 employees, and 2 had other jobs. We only tested male volunteers, due to previously reported sex differences in aggression42,43 and in emotional as well as neural responses to unfairness44. In addition to being male, volunteers had to be right-handed, aged 18–40, and without contraindication for magnetic resonance imaging (MRI). Further inclusion criteria comprised an alexithymia score <61 (using the Toronto- Alexithymia Scale, TAS-2045), as well as a depression score <29 (using Beck’s Depression Inventory, BDI-II46). The study protocol was approved by the Research Ethics Committee of the Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences at the University of Geneva. It was carried out in accordance with the approved guidelines and the declaration of Helsinki. Prior to the experiment participants provided informed written consent. They were paid and debriefed after the experiment. Materials and data related to this experiment will be made available upon request.
Measures
Questionnaires
Prior to the experiment, we collected the following online questionnaires (using Questionnaire Machine, developed by Christoph Hofstetter, University of Geneva): the trait measures of the State-Trait Anger Expression Inventory47,48, the Aggression Questionnaire49,50, the Levenson Self-Report Psychopathy Scale51,52, the Behavioral Inhibition System/Behavioral Activation System Scales53,54, the trait form of the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory55,56, and the Interpersonal Reactivity Index57,58. The psychometric characteristics of the sample are summarized in Supplementary Table 1. In line with our previous behavioral work using the Inequality Game, IG2, we assessed participants’ feelings in relation to the task. In the current study, we used several self-report questionnaires with analogue response scales that ranged between 0 (not at all) to 10 (extremely). After the fMRI measurement, participants filled in these questionnaires to characterize their general feelings towards both other players as well as feelings evoked by the different types of interactions that took place during the three phases of the IG. Participants also evaluated how fair, agreeable, reliable, and good-looking they considered each of the other players, and finally provided information about their subjective involvement in the game.
Inequality Game
We acquired fMRI data while participants completed the IG2. The IG is an interactive game with economic choices and feedback messages in which the participant is sequentially paired with one of two other players (a fair and an unfair other who are counterbalanced across participants). Unbeknownst to the participant, the other players’ behavior is preprogrammed. As depicted in Fig. 1a the employed version of the IG consists of a high power phase at baseline to assess participants’ spontaneous behavior towards the other players prior to provocation, a low power phase with provocation in which anger is elicited and a subsequent high power phase after provocation in which punishment behavior is measured. Inspired by previous work3,4,14,21, the IG induces anger by confronting the participant with an unfair other player who engages in competitive economic choices (e.g., a high gain of 9 or 10 CHF for himself and a low gain of 0 or 1 CHF for the participant) and sends derogatory feedback messages (e.g., “You are annoying”) as opposed to a fair other player who makes cooperative economic choices (e.g., a high gain of 9 or 10 CHF for both, himself and the participant) and sends nice feedback messages (e.g., “You are very nice”). This is done in a low power phase with provocation. In a subsequent high power phase after provocation, participants’ behavioral reactions to the other players are assessed by measuring the degree to which participants choose competitive as opposed to cooperative economic distributions and derogatory as opposed to nice feedback messages.
More specifically (see also Fig. 1), the current paradigm has a 3 × 2 × 2 within-subject design with the factors power (high power phase at baseline, low power phase with provocation, and high power phase after provocation), other (fair and unfair), and event type (economic choices and feedback messages). Power is manipulated in each phase and serves to (i) assess participants spontaneous behavioral choices for the two other players during the high power phase at baseline, (ii) induce anger by confronting the participant with the unfair other players’ behavior in the low power phase with provocation, (iii) assess participants’ behavioral responses to the provocation in the subsequent high power phase. The manipulation of other means that the participant is paired with either the fair or unfair other player during half of the trials. In line with the validated version of the IG2, we implemented two event types: joint economic choices on a 2 by 2 payoff matrix and a choice of one out of four feedback messages. To optimize time for fMRI measurements and maximize repetitions of economic interaction trials, the current fMRI version of the IG focuses on economic interactions (36 economic interactions per phase) and only uses 6 feedback trials per phase. In each phase (Fig. 1b), there are 20 interactions with relevant economic choices. In these relevant economic choices the person in high power can choose between outcomes that are competitive or cooperative. This means that the player with high power is in control of up to 90% of the other’s gain, while the player with low power can only affect up to 10% of the other’s gain. In addition, only the player with high power selects feedback messages. In each of these rounds, the player with high power controls the lines and starts by choosing between a cooperative (e.g., 9 or 10 CHF for both) or competitive outcomes (e.g., 9 or 10 CHF for himself and 0 or 1 CHF for the other). The player with low power chooses next and can only impact up to 1 CHF of the outcome (e.g., by choosing between 10 or 9 CHF for the other and 1 or 0 CHF for himself). The screens that participants see when they are in the high power condition are detailed in Fig. 1b. In order to control for monetary reward per se, 16 economic interactions in each phase have essentially pre-determined monetary outcomes for both players (half of them being “win” trials with high monetary outcomes for both players and half of them being “no win” trials with low or no monetary gains for both players). Participants were told that at the end of the game, the computer will randomly choose two interactions which will be paid out, which implies a gain range between 0 and 20 CHF.
We obtained a measure of punishment inhibition by computing the difference in the number of trials in which participants made cooperative as opposed to competitive decisions towards the unfair other during the high power phase after provocation, relative to the high power phase at baseline: i.e., punishment inhibition = (cooperative-competitive decisions for the unfair other during the high power phase after provocation) - (cooperative-competitive decisions for the unfair other during the high power phase at baseline). Feedback behavior, which was not of primary interest in the current study, was quantified by computing the sum of weighted frequencies with which participants chose the following four feedback sentences for each player: “You are very nice” was assigned a weight of 2, “You are nice” a weight of 1, “You are not nice” a weight of −1, and “You are annoying” a weight of −2. The feedback index thus indicates to what extent participants chose nice (positive values) or derogatory feedback (negative values) for each of the other players.
The Inequality Game can be seen as a variant of mixed-motive games, such as the Prisonner’s Dilemma Game59 to which a power manipulation is added60. However, in contrast to a Prisonner’s Dilemma game or other mixed-motive games, the payoffs of the two players interacting in in the IG are largely independent, thus disentangling self-related choices from other-related choices2. This means that the choice of high or low payoffs for the other player do not affect participants’ own payoffs.
Procedure
The experiment had a total duration of 2 h. Participants were first seated in a multi-computer room and told that during the fMRI scanning they would interact with two other participants who would arrive later. To increase credibility, we ostensibly prepared questionnaire sheets and pens for two other participants in the multi-computer room, to the right and left of the participants’ seat and told participants that the two other players would be seated there while participants were being installed in the MRI. Participants did not see the other players and unknown to the participants, the others´ choices in the IG were preprogrammed.
After explaining the general procedure of the experiment, participants filled in consent forms and questionnaires. The experimenter then took a photograph of the participant in order to allegedly display it to the others during the game. While the experimenter left the room under the pretense to upload the photograph, participants read the instructions of the game. Upon the experimenter’s return, their understanding of the game was tested by means of a questionnaire and oral probing. Participants were reminded that they could win between 0 and 20 CHF during the game in addition to the 35 CHF they obtained for participating in the experiment. Participants were then familiarized with the game through a few test trials allegedly played with the scanner operator who used the computer in the MRI control room. To underline the training purpose of test trials, letters replaced the economic outcomes and feedback sentences were numbered.
Following the preparation phase, participants were guided to the scanner room and brain fMRI data were obtained while they played the IG with the two ostensible other players in the multi-computer room. To increase credibility, the experimenter pretended to double-check whether all players were ready prior to starting the game. During the fMRI measurement, participants played the three phases of the IG (high power at baseline, low power with provocation, and high power after provocation). Each phase served as an fMRI run and lasted about 13 minutes. A structural brain scan (T1) was acquired at the end of the MRI measurement. Following the neuroimaging part, participants were guided to the waiting area of the laboratory where they filled in a few additional questionnaires about their overall feelings towards the other players, the emotions experienced in response to the different events of each phase of the game, and their thoughts on the experiment’s purpose. At the end of the experiment, participants received 45 CHF and a compact disc with their anatomical brain scan. They were then thanked, probed for suspicion, and debriefed.
fMRI
While participants played the IG, blood oxygenation level dependent contrast (BOLD) signals were acquired with a 3 Tesla Siemens Magnetom Trio Tim syngo (MR B17) scanner and a 32 channel head coil. 40 slices were obtained in descending order (TR = 2000 ms, TE = 20 ms; voxel size = 3 × 3 × 2.5 mm³). High resolution anatomical images (1 × 1 × 1 mm³) were recorded using a T1-weighted sagitally oriented GRAPPA sequence with 192 slices. Visual stimuli were presented on a back projection screen inside the scanner bore using an LCD projector (CP-SX1350, Hitachi, Japan).
Analyses
Statistical analyses of behavioral data were carried out using the software SPSS Statistics 21. Neuroimaging data were analyzed using the software SPM 8 (Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging, London) run on Matlab R2012b. Imaging data were preprocessed with the following steps: first, all structural and functional data were manually reoriented, so that the anterior commissure was at the origin of the coordinate system (0, 0, 0). Then, data were realigned and coregistered using standard procedures. Warping parameters extracted from the segmentation of the anatomical images were used to normalize functional and structural images to the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) template brain. Functional images were resampled to a 3 mm³ voxel size and spatially smoothed using a Gaussian kernel of 6 mm³ full-width at half-maximum (FWHM).
First-level general linear models for each participant were computed in a single design matrix using each of the three phases as a run (high power at baseline, low power with provocation, high power after provocation). Regressors were modeled in an event-related design according to the display frames (Fig. 1). For each high power phase, the onsets and durations of the following regressors (events) were separately modeled for interactions with the fair and unfair other (including control conditions with pre-determined “win” or “no win” outcomes): display of the other’s face, payoff display, participant’s decision, display of the participant’s own decision, and display of the other’s decision (which served at the same time as the final result display). In addition, we included participants’ decisions for feedback choices and the related feedback presentation in the model. These conditions were not of interest for the current analysis, because there were too few observations (only 3 feedback interactions with each of the other players per phase, see above). Corresponding regressors were modelled for events in the low power phase. Movement parameters from each run were included as additional regressors.
On the second (group) level, we carried out general linear models on the first level contrast images, after smoothing by a Gaussian kernel of 9 mm³ FWHM. Statistical comparisons between conditions were performed by standard whole-brain analyses with one-sample t-tests, analyses of variance, and linear regressions in SPM. Significance levels were set to P < 0.05, FWE corrected at the voxel or cluster level. In order to obtain robust statistics61,62, while at the same time avoiding the risk of missing true effects (which can be much smaller in affective and social neuroscience than in motor or perceptual research63), we have adopted the following strategy: To test for basic effects related to perceptual or motor phenomena, we use a very conservative threshold of P < 0.05 FWE correction at the voxel level. To study more subtle effects related to affective and social phenomena, we employ a cluster level threshold with a FWE correction of P < 0.05. Note that this thresholding is more conservative than the thresholding of P < 0.005 with a 10 voxel extent that has been suggested for neuroimaging studies of affective and social phenomena63.
Electronic supplementary material
Acknowledgements
We thank Lucien Gyger, Alexandra Anastaze, and Patricia Cernadas Curotto for their help in the study and Sascha Fruehholz for his help in implementing DARTEL. This work was supported by a Marie Curie fellowship to Olga M. Klimecki from the European Union Seventh Framework Programme (FP7/2007–2013) under grant agreement n° 267171. Our research was also supported by the National Center of Competence in Research (NCCR) for the Affective Sciences, financed by a grant from the Swiss National Science Foundation (51NF40-104897), hosted by the University of Geneva.
Author Contributions
O.K., D.S. and P.V. designed the experiment, O.K. carried out the experiment, O.K. analyzed the data, O.K., P.V. and D.S. wrote the paper.
Competing Interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Electronic supplementary material
Supplementary information accompanies this paper at 10.1038/s41598-018-28863-3.
Publisher's note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
References
- 1.Averill JR. Studies on anger and aggression: Implications for theories of emotion. American Psychologist. 1983;38:1145–1160. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.38.11.1145. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Klimecki OM, Vuilleumier P, Sander D. The Impact of Emotions and Empathy-Related Traits on Punishment Behavior: Introduction and Validation of the Inequality Game. PLoS One. 2016;11:e0151028. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0151028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Dollard, J., Doob, L. W., Miller, N. E., Mowrer, O. H. & Sears, R. R. Frustration and Aggression. Yale University Press, (1939).
- 4.Mikula G, Scherer KR, Athenstaedt U. The role of injustice in the elicitation of differential emotional reactions. Personality and social psychology bulletin. 1998;24:769–783. doi: 10.1177/0146167298247009. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Anderson CA, Bushman BJ. Human aggression. Annual review of psychology. 2002;53:27–51. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135231. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Buss DM, Shackelford TK. Human aggression in evolutionary psychological perspective. Clinical psychology review. 1997;17:605–619. doi: 10.1016/S0272-7358(97)00037-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Lotze M, Veit R, Anders S, Birbaumer N. Evidence for a different role of the ventral and dorsal medial prefrontal cortex for social reactive aggression: An interactive fMRI study. Neuroimage. 2007;34:470–478. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.09.028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Krämer UM, Jansma H, Tempelmann C, Münte TF. Tit-for-tat: the neural basis of reactive aggression. Neuroimage. 2007;38:203–211. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.07.029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Phan KL, Wager T, Taylor SF, Liberzon I. Functional neuroanatomy of emotion: a meta-analysis of emotion activation studies in PET and fMRI. Neuroimage. 2002;16:331–348. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1087. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Murphy FC, Nimmo-Smith I, Lawrence AD. Functional neuroanatomy of emotions: a meta-analysis. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2003;3:207–233. doi: 10.3758/CABN.3.3.207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Damasio AR, et al. Subcortical and cortical brain activity during the feeling of self-generated emotions. Nat Neurosci. 2000;3:1049–1056. doi: 10.1038/79871. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Kimbrell TA, et al. Regional brain activity during transient self-induced anxiety and anger in healthy adults. Biological psychiatry. 1999;46:454–465. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(99)00103-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Dougherty DD, et al. Anger in healthy men: a PET study using script-driven imagery. Biological psychiatry. 1999;46:466–472. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(99)00063-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Denson TF, Pedersen WC, Ronquillo J, Nandy AS. The angry brain: Neural correlates of anger, angry rumination, and aggressive personality. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2009;21:734–744. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21051. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Innes-Ker Å, Niedenthal PM. Emotion concepts and emotional states in social judgment and categorization. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2002;83:804–816. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.83.4.804. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Nelson RJ, Trainor BC. Neural mechanisms of aggression. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2007;8:536–546. doi: 10.1038/nrn2174. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Giancola PR. Evidence for dorsolateral and orbital prefrontal cortical involvement in the expression of aggressive behavior. Aggressive Behavior. 1995;21:431–450. doi: 10.1002/1098-2337(1995)21:6<431::AID-AB2480210604>3.0.CO;2-Q. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Buhle JT, et al. Cognitive Reappraisal of Emotion: A Meta-Analysis of Human Neuroimaging Studies. Cerebral Cortex. 2014;24:2981–2990. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bht154. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Davidson RJ, Putnam KM, Larson CL. Dysfunction in the neural circuitry of emotion regulation–a possible prelude to violence. Science. 2000;289:591–594. doi: 10.1126/science.289.5479.591. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Raine A, Yang Y. Neural foundations to moral reasoning and antisocial behavior. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2006;1:203–213. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsl033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Pillutla MM, Murnighan JK. Unfairness, anger, and spite: Emotional rejections of ultimatum offers. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 1996;68:208–224. doi: 10.1006/obhd.1996.0100. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Kanwisher N, McDermott J, Chun MM. The fusiform face area: a module in human extrastriate cortex specialized for face perception. J Neurosci. 1997;17:4302–4311. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-11-04302.1997. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Kanwisher N, Yovel G. The fusiform face area: a cortical region specialized for the perception of faces. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2006;361:2109–2128. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2006.1934. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Cieslik EC, et al. Is There “One” DLPFC in Cognitive Action Control? Evidence for Heterogeneity From Co-Activation-Based Parcellation. Cerebral Cortex. 2013;23:2677–2689. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs256. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Botvinick MM, Cohen JD, Carter CS. Conflict monitoring and anterior cingulate cortex: an update. Trends Cogn Sci. 2004;8:539–546. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.10.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Rushworth MF, Behrens TE, Rudebeck PH, Walton ME. Contrasting roles for cingulate and orbitofrontal cortex in decisions and social behaviour. Trends Cogn Sci. 2007;11:168–176. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2007.01.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Rushworth MF, Buckley MJ, Behrens TE, Walton ME, Bannerman DM. Functional organization of the medial frontal cortex. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2007;17:220–227. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2007.03.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Margulies DS, et al. Precuneus shares intrinsic functional architecture in humans and monkeys. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2009;106:20069–20074. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0905314106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Bzdok D, et al. Parsing the neural correlates of moral cognition: ALE meta-analysis on morality, theory of mind, and empathy. Brain Structure and Function. 2012;217:783–796. doi: 10.1007/s00429-012-0380-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Haber SN, Knutson B. The reward circuit: linking primate anatomy and human imaging. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2010;35:4–26. doi: 10.1038/npp.2009.129. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Mobbs D, et al. A key role for similarity in vicarious reward. Science. 2009;324:900. doi: 10.1126/science.1170539. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Rilling J, et al. A neural basis for social cooperation. Neuron. 2002;35:395–405. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(02)00755-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Sander D, Grafman J, Zalla T. The human amygdala: an evolved system for relevance detection. Reviews in the Neurosciences. 2003;14:303–316. doi: 10.1515/REVNEURO.2003.14.4.303. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Vuilleumier P. How brains beware: neural mechanisms of emotional attention. Trends Cogn Sci. 2005;9:585–594. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.10.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Phillips ML, Drevets WC, Rauch SL, Lane R. Neurobiology of emotion perception I: the neural basis of normal emotion perception. Biological Psychiatry. 2003;54:504–514. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(03)00168-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Pearson JM, Heilbronner SR, Barack DL, Hayden BY, Platt ML. Posterior cingulate cortex: adapting behavior to a changing world. Trends Cogn Sci. 2011;15:143–151. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.02.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Sergerie K, Chochol C, Armony JL. The role of the amygdala in emotional processing: A quantitative meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2008;32:811–830. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2007.12.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Holland PC, Gallagher M. Amygdala circuitry in attentional and representational processes. Trends Cogn Sci. 1999;3:65–73. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(98)01271-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Shackman AJ, et al. The integration of negative affect, pain and cognitive control in the cingulate cortex. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2011;12:154–167. doi: 10.1038/nrn2994. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Knoch D, Pascual-Leone A, Meyer K, Treyer V, Fehr E. Diminishing reciprocal fairness by disrupting the right prefrontal cortex. Science. 2006;314:829–832. doi: 10.1126/science.1129156. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Buckholtz JW, et al. The neural correlates of third-party punishment. Neuron. 2008;60:930–940. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.10.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Archer J. Sex differences in aggression in real-world settings: A meta-analytic review. Review of general Psychology. 2004;8:291–322. doi: 10.1037/1089-2680.8.4.291. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Björkqvist K. Sex differences in physical, verbal, and indirect aggression: A review of recent research. Sex roles. 1994;30:177–188. doi: 10.1007/BF01420988. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Singer T, et al. Empathic neural responses are modulated by the perceived fairness of others. Nature. 2006;439:466–469. doi: 10.1038/nature04271. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Bagby RM, Parker JD, Taylor GJ. The twenty-item Toronto Alexithymia Scale–I. Item selection and cross-validation of the factor structure. J Psychosom Res. 1994;38:23–32. doi: 10.1016/0022-3999(94)90005-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Beck AT, Steer RA, Ball R, Ranieri W. Comparison of Beck Depression Inventories -IA and -II in psychiatric outpatients. J Pers Assess. 1996;67:588–597. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa6703_13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Spielberger, C. D. Manual for the state-trait anger expression inventory STAXI-2. Psychological Assessment Resources (1999).
- 48.Borteyrou X, Bruchon-Schweitzer M, Spielberger CD. The French adaptation of the STAXI-2, C.D. Spielberger’s State-trait anger expression inventory. Encephale-Revue De Psychiatrie Clinique Biologique Et Therapeutique. 2008;34:249–255. doi: 10.1016/j.encep.2007.06.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Buss AH, Perry M. The Aggression Questionnaire. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1992;63:452–459. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.63.3.452. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Bouchard, J. Validation de la version française du AQ auprès de deux échantillons: étudiants universitaires (étude1) et adultes non-recrutés en milieu universitaire (étude 2) Université du Québec à Chicoutimi (2007).
- 51.Levenson MR, Kiehl KA, Fitzpatrick CM. Assessing Psychopathic Attributes in a Noninstitutionalized Population. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1995;68:151–158. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.68.1.151. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Savard, C., Lussier, Y. & Sabourin, S. French-Canadian validation of the Levenson self-report psychopathy scale. In Annual meeting of the Canadian Psychological Association Montréal, Canada (2005).
- 53.Carver CS, White TL. Behavioral-inhibition, behavioral activation, and affective responses to impending reward and punishment - the BIS BAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1994;67:319–333. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.67.2.319. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Caci H, Deschaux O, Bayle FJ. Psychometric properties of the French versions of the BIS/BAS scales and the SPSRQ. Personality and Individual Differences. 2007;42:987–998. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2006.09.008. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Spielberger, C. D., Gorsuch, R. L., Lushene, R., Vagg, P. R. & Jacobs, G. A. Manual for the State-trait anxiety inventory (form Y) (“self-evaluation questionnaire”). Consulting Psychologists Press (1983).
- 56.Gauthier J, Bouchard S. Adaptation canadienne-française de la forme révisée du State–Trait Anxiety Inventory de Spielberger. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science/Revue canadienne des sciences du comportement. 1993;25:559–578. doi: 10.1037/h0078881. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Davis MH. Measuring Individual-Differences in Empathy - Evidence for a Multidimensional Approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1983;44:113–126. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.44.1.113. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Gilet A-L, Mella N, Studer J, Gruehn D, Labouvie-Vief G. Assessing Dispositional Empathy in Adults: A French Validation of the Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI) Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science-Revue Canadienne Des Sciences Du Comportement. 2013;45:42–48. doi: 10.1037/a0030425. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Rapoport, A. & Chammah, A. M. Prisoner’s dilemma: A study in conflict and cooperation. Vol. 165 University of Michigan press (1965).
- 60.Hokanson JE, Sacco WP, Blumberg SR, Landrum GC. Interpersonal behavior of depressive individuals in a mixed-motive game. Journal of abnormal psychology. 1980;89:320. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.89.3.320. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Eklund, A., Nichols, T. E. & Knutson, H. Cluster failure: why fMRI inferences for spatial extent have inflated false-positive rates. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 201602413 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 62.Flandin G, Friston KJ. Analysis of family-wise error rates in statistical parametric mapping using random field theory. arXiv preprint arXiv. 2016;1606:08199. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23839. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Lieberman MD, Cunningham WA. Type I and Type II error concerns in fMRI research: re-balancing the scale. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2009;4:423–428. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsp052. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.