Abstract
Background and Objectives
To examine the involvement of persons with dementia (PWDs) in everyday decision making from the perspectives of hospitalized PWDs and their family caregivers, and to identify determinants thereof.
Research Design and Methods
Using multilevel modeling, we examined cross-sectional data collected prospectively from 42 family care dyads regarding the care values of the PWD.
Results
Both members of the dyad rated the PWD, on average, as being “somewhat involved”. There was a significant amount of variability around the average perceptions of PWD involvement in decision making for both PWDs (χ2 = 351.02, p < .001) and family caregivers (χ2 = 327.01, p < .001). Both PWDs and family caregivers were significantly more likely to perceive greater PWD involvement in decision making when the family caregiver reported the PWD as valuing autonomy. Additionally, PWDs were significantly more likely to report greater involvement when they had greater cognitive function. Finally, family caregivers perceived significantly greater involvement of the patient in decision making when they reported less strain in the relationship. Together, autonomy, relationship strain, cognitive function, and care-related strain accounted for 38% and 46% of the variability in PWDs’ and family caregivers’ perceptions, respectively, of the PWD’s decision-making involvement.
Discussion and Implications
Although research indicates that decision-making abilities decline with advancing dementia, these results imply that working with families to support PWDs in their value of autonomy and mitigate strain in the dyad’s relationship may help prolong PWDs’ decision-making involvement.
Keywords: Interpersonal context, Informal caregiving, Multilevel modeling, Patient autonomy
Involving older adult patients in decisions about their health and future care is becoming standard practice, particularly through the promotion of models of shared decision making (Elwyn et al., 2012) and a sustained focus on improving advance care planning and end-of-life decision making (Bischoff, Sudore, Miao, Boscardin, & Smith, 2013; Drought & Koenig, 2002; Schmid, Allen, Haley, & Decoster, 2010). The continued involvement of a person with dementia (PWD) in decision making and care planning, though not standard practice, is an important goal for both the PWD and the family caregiver (Fetherstonhaugh, Tarzia, & Nay, 2013; Samsi & Manthorpe, 2013). Although researchers have often examined the involvement of PWDs in medical and treatment decisions (Hirschman, Xie, Feudtner, & Karlawish, 2004; Horton-Deutsch, Twigg, & Evans, 2007; Karel, Gurrera, Hicken, & Moye, 2010; Karlawish, Casarett, Propert, James, & Clark, 2002), it is the involvement in “everyday” decisions about daily care, activities, and functioning that is often most important to PWDs (Feinberg & Whitlatch, 2002; Menne & Whitlatch, 2007; Murphy & Oliver, 2013; Samsi & Manthorpe, 2013). Involvement in these everyday decisions give PWDs a sense of purpose and help them to avoid feeling marginalized (Fetherstonhaugh et al., 2013). There is also evidence that there are fewer depressive symptoms in the family caregiver and better quality of life in both PWD and family caregiver when the PWD is more involved in everyday decision making (Menne, Judge, & Whitlatch, 2009; Menne, Tucke, Whitlatch, & Feinberg, 2008; Samsi & Manthorpe, 2013).
The substantial challenges of involving PWDs in decision making within the acute care setting have recently been highlighted (Greener et al., 2012; Nilsson, Rasmussen, & Edvardsson, 2013). Compared to other older adults, PWDs experience 3 times as many hospitalizations (Thies & Bleiler, 2013). Readmission and mortality rates in hospitals are also higher among PWDs than other older adults (Callahan et al., 2012). Some PWDs have described the experience of a hospitalization as stressful and threatening (Edvardsson & Nordvall, 2008), and the loss of independence in completing activities of daily living is common for PWDs during a hospitalization. For family caregivers, the hospitalization of a relative with dementia has been associated with high levels of burden and depressive symptoms (Epstein-Lubow et al., 2012; Shankar, Hirschman, Hanlon, & Naylor, 2014). The adverse effects of hospitalizations on PWDs and their family caregivers likely create substantial challenges to decision making among PWD-family caregiver dyads. It is critical to understand decision making in this context due to the unique time pressures of hospital discharge planning, and the opportunity to include both members of the dyad. Discharge planning begins at admission for hospitalized patients, and decisions about everyday aspects of the PWD’s life (e.g., choosing who to help with care or where to live) are an inevitable part of the discharge plan. Yet, there are no known studies that have examined the involvement of hospitalized PWDs in everyday decisions from either the PWD’s or the family caregiver’s point of view.
Dementia is a shared context for the PWD-family caregiver dyad. A dyadic perspective (PWD and family caregiver as a unit) is important to the understanding of decision making about everyday aspects of life with dementia, particularly since the lives of this type of care dyad are so intertwined (Samsi & Manthorpe, 2013). Whereas the family caregiver’s perspective has historically been used in research to represent the PWD (Cotrell & Schulz, 1993), the PWD’s perspective is important to include because involvement of PWDs in decision making is important to the family caregiver and it is also associated with better quality of life for the dyad (Menne et al., 2008, 2009; Samsi & Manthorpe, 2013). Furthermore, including the PWD’s perspective is important to maintaining personhood (Bartlett & O’Connor, 2007; Woods, 2001), and it is warranted given the evidence supporting the reliability of PWDs’ self-report. For example, PWDs have consistently and reliably reported on their own quality of life (Logsdon, Gibbons, McCurry, & Teri, 2002), well-being (Mak, 2011), pain (Fisher et al., 2002), depressive symptoms (Parmelee, Lawton, & Katz, 1989), and care values and preferences (Whitlatch et al., 2005), even with a moderate amount of cognitive impairment. At the same time, impairments to executive function and insight, which are common in dementia, affect PWDs’ abilities to make decisions independently (Kensinger, 2009; Orfei et al., 2010; Sorensen, Mak, & Pinquart, 2011), and as a result, family caregivers’ are often required to make the crucial link between what a PWD values and the particular decisions at hand (Gillick, 2013). Thus, when considered together as a dyad, the two perspectives allow for an examination of decision making that reflects the joint involvement of PWD and family caregiver, which is often necessary and optimal in the context of dementia.
Conceptual Framework
In order to represent both dyad members’ perspectives conceptually, the framework for this study was derived from the Stress Process Model (SPM) for family caregivers of PWDs (Pearlin, Mullan, Semple, & Skaff, 1990), as well as subsequent versions that have been operationalized for individuals with chronic illness (Menne & Whitlatch, 2007), and more recently, for persons with dementia (Judge, Menne, & Whitlatch, 2010). The three iterations share in common a model encompassing primary stressors and secondary strains that are influential upon one another and together on an outcome of wellbeing such as quality of life or depression (Pearlin et al., 1990), or decision-making involvement of PWDs (Menne & Whitlatch, 2007). The three main components that influence such outcomes are: (a) primary stressors related to dementia/dementia caregiving, both objective (e.g., pathologies of the disease) and subjective (e.g., overload of care tasks or psychological distress of living with dementia), (b) secondary role strains (i.e., strains that occur as a result of adapting to life with dementia), and (c) secondary intrapsychic strains, described by Pearlin and colleagues (1990) as strains on “dimensions of self-concept” (e.g., self-esteem).
In the conceptualization of stressors and strains for this study we drew upon the three versions of the SPM (Judge et al., 2010; Menne & Whitlatch, 2007; Pearlin et al., 1990) and integrated them with the resulting literature to maintain a dyadic perspective of the illness experience. Thus, we operationalized the primary objective stressor related to the dementia illness experience as cognitive function (Judge et al., 2010; Pearlin et al., 1990), the primary subjective stressor as care-related strain (Menne et al., 2009; Pearlin et al., 1990), the secondary role strain as dyadic relationship strain (Judge et al., 2010; Menne & Whitlatch, 2007; Sebern & Whitlatch, 2007), and the secondary intrapsychic strain as the importance of autonomy to the PWD (Menne et al., 2009; Menne & Whitlatch, 2007). Other studies outside the SPM literature have also shown that cognitive impairment and care-related strain are significantly associated with the family caregiver’s perception of the PWD’s decision-making involvement (Hirschman et al., 2004; Karlawish et al., 2002). Few studies, however, have examined the PWD’s involvement in everyday decision-making from both perspectives (Adler, 2010; Boyle, 2013; Menne & Whitlatch, 2007; Samsi & Manthorpe, 2013) and no prior studies have focused on decision-making of the dyad during hospitalization—an event that often calls into question aspects of the PWD’s independence (e.g., activities of daily living) and the dyad’s life at home (e.g., the care arrangement).
The purpose of this study was to examine the involvement of PWDs in everyday types of decisions according to the perspectives of hospitalized patients with dementia and their family caregivers, and to identify factors associated with the dyad’s perception of greater involvement of PWDs in decision making.
Design and Methods
Participants for this study were recruited from three adult inpatient acute care units in a university hospital in the Pacific Northwest. Approval was obtained from the Institutional Review Board for this study. Patients were admitted to hospital units where recruitment took place for a wide range of medical and surgical diagnoses (e.g., pneumonia, cardiac dysrhythmias, and hip fracture). A convenience sample of 42 dyads was enrolled that met the following eligibility criteria.
Inclusion Criteria
PWDs were eligible if they were aged 65 or older, admitted to an acute care unit (with any diagnosis), had symptoms consistent with mild to moderate dementia, and self-reported a probable or current diagnosis of an irreversible progressive dementia: Alzheimer’s disease, vascular dementia, Lewy body dementia, or frontotemporal dementia. Family caregivers were eligible if they were aged 21 or older, nominated by the PWD as the primary family caregiver (primary family caregiver was defined as the family member who is most involved in care at home). To be eligible, PWDs had to score at least 13 on the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE), which corresponds to reliable, consistent reporting in previous studies using this criterion (Feinberg & Whitlatch, 2001; Logsdon, Gibbons, McCurry, & Teri, 2002; Parmelee, Lawton, & Katz, 1989; Whitlatch, Feinberg, & Tucke, 2005).
Exclusion Criteria
Dyads were ineligible if either the PWD or family caregiver was unable to speak English, or if the PWD had unresolved delirium or altered level of consciousness, which was assessed by the direct care registered nurse (RN) prior to screening for interest.
After the investigator screened patient records for potentially eligible participants (confirming dementia diagnosis through chart review), the PWD’s direct care RN screened patients and family caregivers for interest in the study. The researcher met with each member of the dyad to confirm interest, assess eligibility, and obtain informed consent. Individual members of each dyad completed one private interview in-person within the acute care unit, typically within 2–3 days following admission. Participants were provided with written cards to aid memory about response scales and answered verbal questions from the researcher. Responses were recorded by the researcher on a laptop using RedCap, an electronic data capture and storage system.
Measures
Outcomes
Decision-making involvement of the PWD was measured in PWDs and family caregivers using equivalent versions of the Decision-Making Involvement Scale developed specifically for the dementia care dyad (Menne et al., 2008). The measure consists of 15 items, scored on a 4-point scale from 0 (not involved at all), to 3 (very involved). Each member of the dyad responded with their perception of how involved the PWD is in everyday types of decisions (e.g., where to live, when to get medical care, what to eat at meals). The reliability in this study was excellent (PWD α = .89; family caregiver α = .87). Further details of the process for scoring of the measure is described under “parallel scales” in the Analytic Approach section.
Independent Variables
Cognitive status was screened and measured in PWDs with the MMSE (Folstein, Folstein, & McHugh, 1975). The MMSE is designed for clinician assessment of 11 cognitive domains spanning aspects of orientation, working memory, language, delayed recall, attention, and comprehension. The scale range is 0–30, with higher scores indicating higher cognitive function. It is used widely in research and has good reliability (test–retest r = .89) and validity (predictive and concurrent validity) among PWDs (Fillenbaum, Heyman, Wilkinson, & Haynes, 1987; Mitchell, 2009; Tombaugh & McIntyre, 1992).
Care-related strain was measured in family caregivers using the Role Overload scale (Pearlin et al., 1990), which assesses the extent to which caregiver’s time and energy are exhausted by the demands of caring for the person with dementia. Caregivers responded to 3 items regarding how worn-out and overloaded their care role makes them feel using a Likert-type scale from 1 (not at all) to 4 (very much). The items were summed for a scale range of 3–12. Higher scores indicate high levels of care-related strain. The reliability in this sample was adequate (α = .75)
Relationship strain was measured in family caregivers using the 5-item Dyadic Strain subscale of the Dyadic Relationship Scale (Sebern & Whitlatch, 2007). Each item is a statement of a potential source of strain in the relationship, for which family caregivers rated their level of agreement from 1 (strongly disagree) to 4 (strongly agree). An example item is: “Because of helping my family member, I feel angry toward her/him.” Items were averaged for a scale range of 1–4, with higher scores indicating more perceived relationship strain. The reliability in this sample was good (α = .85).
The PWD’s value of autonomy was measured in family caregivers using the autonomy subscale of the Care Values scale, which was developed specifically for caregiving dyads in which the care recipient is a person with cognitive impairment (Whitlatch, Piiparinen, & Feinberg, 2009). The autonomy subscale has 7 items that describe care values around autonomy (e.g., do things for him/herself). The items were rated according to the importance of each value to the PWD on a 3-point scale. The total score was averaged for a scale range of 1–3, with higher scores indicating that the family caregiver perceived the PWD to place more importance on their autonomy. The reliability in this sample was adequate (α = .79).
Analytic Approach
Descriptive statistics and Pearson’s correlations between study variables were conducted using the software program Stata, version 14 (Statacorp, 2015). Analysis of the dyadic data was conducted using multilevel modeling and the software program HLM, version 7 (Raudenbush, Bryk, & Congdon, 2011). The multivariate outcomes model (separate PWD/family caregiver outcomes) can be achieved using multilevel modeling while still estimating and controlling for the degree of shared variance in the dyad. In this study, level 1 data included PWDs and family caregivers, which were nested within the level 2 PWD-family caregiver dyad (the unit of analysis). HLM uses full information maximum likelihood (FIML) to estimate parameter values, given all existing data. In this study there were less than 1% missing data on the outcome variable for PWDs and less than 6% missing data on the outcome variable for family caregivers. The level 1 (unconditional) model estimated the average values and the variability around the averages for both the PWD’s and family caregiver’s perceptions of the PWD’s decision-making involvement. Predictors were introduced in level 2 to explain the variability around the average.
Level 1 Model
Within-dyad variation was modeled at level 1, where the outcome is the sum of the true score and measurement error using the following formula, originally described by Barnett, Marshall, Raudenbush & Brennan (1993), and more recently adapted by other dyadic researchers (Lyons & Sayer, 2005; Pruchno, Wilson-Genderson, & Cartwright, 2009). In the equation,
Decisionij represents the outcome parallel score i in dyad j. PWD is an indicator variable taking on a value of 1 if the response was obtained from the PWD, or taking on a value of 0 if the response was obtained from the family caregiver. Similarly, CG is an indicator variable taking on a value of 1 if the response was obtained from the family caregiver, or taking on a value of 0 if the response was obtained from the PWD. The latent true scores of perceptions of the PWD’s decision-making involvement for PWDs and family caregivers are represented by β1j and β2j, respectively. The within-dyad residuals, rij, are estimated separately for PWD and family caregiver. Thus, PWDs’ average perceptions of their own decision-making involvement (Decision) is the sum of their latent true score (β1j) plus measurement error (rij); or, family caregivers’ average perceptions of PWDs’ decision making involvement (Decision) is the sum of their latent true score (β2j) plus measurement error (rij).
Parallel Scales
In order to provide adequate information to estimate measurement error variances for both PWD and the family caregiver, parallel scales were created for both members of the care dyad. These procedures (Barnett, Marshall, Raudenbush, & Brennan, 1993; Sayer & Klute, 2005) entailed matching items from the decision-making involvement measure into pairs based upon the closeness of their standard deviations to create 7 pairs with one item from each pair randomly assigned to one of two scales. This process resulted in two parallel scales with equal variance and reliability for each member of the dyad (a total of four scores for each dyad). Although the parallel scales method is not always possible when there are too few items on the measure, or when variances of parallel scales are too dissimilar, we were confident that our use of parallel scales was appropriate given that these conditions were satisfied in our study. Finally, the level 1 model also produces a tau correlation, capturing the correlation between PWDs’ and family caregivers’ scores of the PWD’s decision-making involvement.
Level 2 Model
Between-dyad variation was modeled at level 2. In the equations,
the parameters for latent true scores of PWDs (β1j) and family caregivers (β2j) became the outcome variables. Based upon the SPM and supporting literature from studies previously conducted in the community setting, independent variables (MMSE = the PWD’s cognitive function; STRAIN = care-related strain, RELAT = relationship strain; and AUTON = the PWD’s value of autonomy) were included in level 2 conditional models. The proportion of variability explained by these independent variables was calculated for each of the outcome variables (the PWD’s and family caregiver’s perceptions of the PWD’s decision-making involvement) as follows: (τ00 [Level 1 unconditional model]−τ00 [Level 2 conditional model])/τ00 (Level 1 unconditional model), where τ00 represents the variance component (either PWD or caregiver).
Results
Patients with dementia were mean age 80 ± 8 years, predominantly non-Hispanic white ethnicity/race (95%), had an average MMSE score of 21 ± 4, and a slight majority (55%) were male. The most common dementia diagnosis among patients was Alzheimer’s disease (40%), followed by vascular dementia (29%), mixed or unknown dementia type (24%), fronto-temporal dementia (5%), and Lewy body dementia (2%). Family caregivers were age 61 ± 13 years, predominantly non-Hispanic white ethnicity/race (93%), mostly female (75%), and were either adult children (70%) or spouses (30%) of patients. See Table 1 for additional demographic and descriptive data. Bivariate correlations (Table 2) between study variables and other potentially influential variables were also examined in order to better understand the sample characteristics and directions for future research. Although there were no statistically significant correlations in this small sample, the PWD’s perception of greater involvement in decision making was negatively correlated with greater years spent caregiving (r = −.19, p = .234), and positively correlated with the PWD’s years of education (r = .217, p = .185) and female gender (r = .252, p = .107). The family caregiver’s perception of greater involvement of the PWD in decision making was negatively correlated with being in a non-spousal relationship (r = −.181, p = .263).
Table 1.
PWD | CG | |
---|---|---|
Mean (SD) or % | Mean (SD) or % | |
Age in years | 79.81 (7.76) | 61 (12.95) |
Female | 45.24% | 75% |
Education (> high school diploma) | 69% | 84.60% |
Race/Ethnicity | ||
White (Non-Hispanic) | 95.24% | 92.50% |
Black/African American | 2.38% | 2.50% |
Native American/Pacific Islander | 0% | 0% |
Asian | 2.38% | 5% |
Hispanic/Latino | 2.38% | 2.50% |
Marital status (married/partnered) | 40.00% | 62.50% |
Relationship to patient | ||
Wife | — | 22.50% |
Husband | — | 7.50% |
Adult daughter | — | 50.00% |
Adult son | — | 17.50% |
Daughter-in-law | — | 2.50% |
Dementia type | ||
Alzheimer’s disease | 40.48% | — |
Vascular | 28.57% | — |
Fronto-temporal | 4.76% | — |
Lewy bodies | 2.38% | — |
Other (Mixed or Unknown) | 23.81% | — |
Cognitive function (MMSE, range 12–27) | 20.55 (3.86) | — |
Care-related strain (Role overload, scale 3–12) | — | 7.9 (2.62) |
Relationship strain (scale 1–4) | — | 2.02 (.72) |
PWD’s value of autonomy (scale 1–3) | — | 2.19 (.50) |
Note: CG = family caregiver; PWD = person with dementia; MMSE = Mini-mental state examination; SD = standard deviation. Ages 90 years or older were all recorded as 90+ to protect identity.
Table 2.
1. PWD DMI scale | 1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | 7 | 8 | 9 | 10 |
2. Spouse/Non-spouse | 0.001 | — | ||||||||
3. FM years caregiving | −0.193 | 0.125 | — | |||||||
4. PWD age in years | −0.103 | −0.326* | −0.14 | — | ||||||
5. PWD female gender | 0.252 | −0.298 | −0.159 | 0.329* | — | |||||
6. PWD years education | 0.217 | 0.108 | 0.216 | −0.212 | −0.530*** | — | ||||
7. Cognitive impairment | 0.411** | 0.025 | −0.108 | −0.223 | 0.383* | −0.183 | — | |||
8. FM role overload | −0.093 | 0.046 | 0.252 | −0.092 | −0.101 | −0.012 | 0.108 | — | ||
9. FM relationship strain | −0.242 | 0.146 | −0.056 | −0.148 | −0.138 | −0.146 | −0.084 | 0.199 | — | |
10.PWD’s autonomy | 0.323* | −0.173 | −0.312* | −0.057 | 0.017 | 0.098 | 0.095 | −0.036 | 0.039 | — |
11. Family DMI scale | 0.674*** | −0.181 | −0.005 | −0.065 | 0.145 | 0.124 | 0.236 | 0.122 | −0.17 | 0.558*** |
Note: DMI Scale = decision-making involvement scale Menne et al., 2008; FM = family member; PWD = person with dementia.
*p < .05; **p < .01; ***p < .001.
Multilevel Modeling Results
At level 1 (see Model 1, Table 3), average ratings of the PWD’s decision-making involvement were β1j= 2.11 ± 0.10, p < .001 and β2j = 2.09 ± 0.10, p < .001 for PWDs and family caregivers, respectively, indicating that, on average, both members of the dyad perceived the PWD as being “somewhat” involved in everyday decisions. The tau correlation between PWD and family caregiver ratings of the PWD’s decision-making involvement was high at .76. There was a significant amount of variability around the average perceptions of PWD involvement in decision-making from the perspective of both PWDs (χ2 = 351.02, p < .001) and family caregivers (χ2 = 327.01, p < .001).
Table 3.
Model 1 | Model 2 | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
β | SE | t | β | SE | t | |
Fixed effects (robust SE) | ||||||
PWD intercept | 2.11 | .10 | 21.10*** | 2.09 | .08 | 24.83*** |
Cognitive impairment | 0.07 | .03 | 2.87** | |||
Care-related strain | −0.02 | .03 | −0.69 | |||
Perception of PWD’s autonomy | 0.43 | .15 | 2.85** | |||
Perception of relationship strain | −0.20 | .11 | −1.84 | |||
Family member intercept | 2.09 | .10 | 21.20*** | 2.09 | .08 | 26.47*** |
Cognitive impairment | 0.02 | .02 | 1.14 | |||
Care-related strain | 0.05 | .03 | 1.58 | |||
Perception of PWD’s autonomy | 0.74 | .16 | 4.75*** | |||
Perception of relationship strain | −0.21 | .09 | −2.17* | |||
Random effects | Variance component | χ 2 | Variance component | χ 2 | ||
Patient | 0.37 | 351.02*** | 0.23 | 217.38*** | ||
Family member | 0.35 | 327.01*** | 0.19 | 191.26*** |
Note: PWD = person with dementia; SE = standard error. Note that cognitive impairment was measured from the PWD’s self-report, whereas care-related strain, perceptions of the PWD’s autonomy, and perceptions of relationship strain were measured from family caregiver’s reports only.
* p < .05; **p < .01; ***p < .001.
Based on level 2 results (Model 2, Table 3), both PWDs and family caregivers were significantly more likely to perceive greater PWD involvement in decision making when the family caregiver reported the PWD as valuing autonomy (see Table 3). PWDs were significantly more likely to report greater involvement when they had better cognitive function. Family caregivers were significantly more likely to report greater PWD involvement when they perceived less strain in their relationship with the PWD. Together, autonomy, relationship strain, cognitive function, and care-related strain accounted for 38% and 46% of the variability in PWDs’ and family caregivers’ perceptions, respectively, of the PWD’s decision-making involvement. According to Cohen’s F2, the effect sizes corresponding to the proportion of variance explained by Level 2 results were f2 = .61 for the PWDs’ model and f2 = .85 for the family caregivers’ model.
Discussion
The current study was a dyadic examination of the decision-making involvement of PWDs in everyday types of decisions, and it is one of the first such studies known to include hospitalized PWDs and their family caregivers. There are several important findings. First, PWDs and family caregivers shared similar average ratings of the PWD as being “somewhat” involved in decision making, which is comparable to studies of dyads in the community setting (Menne & Whitlatch, 2007). Second, there was significant variability around the average perceptions for both PWDs and family caregivers, confirming that the use of a method such as MLM is necessary in order to further examine this variability and providing evidence that there is heterogeneity across dyads. Third, several determinants chosen according to the SPM were identified as being significantly associated with the variability in perceptions of the hospitalized PWD’s decision-making involvement. This finding indicates that dyads’ appraisals of the PWD’s involvement in decision-making may be affected by the stress process. Finally, determinants differed across PWD/family caregiver models, reinforcing the need for a dyadic examination of decision making within the care dyad with dementia.
The family caregiver’s perception of the PWD’s value of autonomy was a significant determinant of both PWD and family caregiver ratings of the PWD’s decision-making involvement. According to the SPM, the diminishing value of autonomy in the PWD can place an intrapsychic strain on perceptions of the PWD’s involvement in making decisions, potentially lowering both PWD and family caregiver ratings (Menne & Whitlatch, 2007). As other researchers have pointed out, preserving autonomy in PWDs and supporting their involvement in decision-making are related goals (Fetherstonhaugh et al., 2013; Menne et al., 2008; Samsi & Manthorpe, 2013). However, in this study we included the family caregiver’s perception of how important autonomy is to the PWD, the difference being that we measured the value of autonomy rather than a perception of how much independence remains in the PWD. This difference is critical since dementia continually threatens autonomy, but may not diminish how important autonomy is to a PWD. Recent perspectives on achieving person-centered care underscore the importance of moving away from how to compensate for what PWDs cannot do, and instead focus on how to promote the PWD’s contributions and identity (Vernooij-Dassen & Moniz-Cook, 2016). Working with family caregivers to show that they understand and support PWDs in their value of autonomy may be an important protective factor helping to prolong PWDs’ decision-making involvement.
Family caregivers in this study perceived greater involvement of the PWD in decision making when they rated strain in their relationship with the PWD as being lower. This finding is similar to another study using the SPM as a framework, where relationship strain is considered a secondary role strain that is influential on the perceptions of the PWD’s decision-making involvement (Menne & Whitlatch, 2007). It is likely that family caregivers are in a better position to support PWDs’ decision-making involvement when they enjoy less strain in the relationship. Since this is cross-sectional data, it is also possible that when PWDs are more involved in decision making, the family caregiver perceives the dyad’s relationship as less strained. Future longitudinal work is needed to untangle these associations.
We found support in this study for the conceptualization of cognitive impairment as a primary stressor on PWDs’ perceptions of their decision-making involvement, as described previously in the SPM literature (Judge et al., 2010; Pearlin et al., 1990). Previous studies conducted in the community setting have also demonstrated that PWDs’ perceptions of their decision-making involvement were higher when the PWD had greater cognitive function (Karlawish et al., 2002; Menne et al., 2008). A recent literature review of dementia decision making found that across six studies, the severity of cognitive impairment was strongly correlated, and even predictive of, lower levels of the PWD’s decision-making involvement (Miller, Whitlatch, & Lyons, 2016). However, in this study family caregivers did not associate the PWD’s decision-making involvement with cognitive ability, indicating that cognitive impairment may not be as influential on family caregivers’ perceptions as it is on PWDs’ perceptions when other influential stressors and strains (i.e., relationship strain) are included in the analyses. The PWDs included in this study had mild to moderate dementia, with an average MMSE score of 21 ± 4. Whereas PWDs’ cognitive function may weigh on their own perceptions of their decision-making involvement from early in the disease process, family caregivers’ perceptions may not be significantly affected by cognitive function until the extent of impairment is greater and more observable (e.g., moderate to severe dementia).
This study was limited in its generalizability by small sample size and lack of diversity. Sample size dictated that few independent variables be entered into models, which constrained the study in several ways. Firstly, potentially confounding variables such as education level (Hirschman et al., 2005; Menne & Whitlatch, 2007), age (Hirschman et al., 2004; Menne et al., 2008), gender (Menne & Whitlatch, 2007), and kinship type (Hirschman et al., 2005) were not included due to sample size, which was constrained by the number of dyads per parameter estimated (in this case 4 determinants for 42 dyads). Secondly, the small sample in this study limited the selection of independent variables to only one variable representing each type of stressor or strain from the SPM, and to only one dyad member’s perception representing each variable. This latter limitation constrained the ability to simultaneously examine actor and partner effects on the outcome of decision-making involvement of PWDs. In order to fully explore the influence that dyad members’ thoughts and perceptions have on one another in this area, future research is needed with larger samples and reports from both members of the care dyad on potential determinants of decision-making involvement of PWDs.
Yet, there were notable strengths to the study, including the novel inpatient hospital setting, dyadic measures and data collection, and an analytic approach (i.e., MLM) appropriate for dyadic data. By including the perspective of both members of the dyad at the outcome-level, we highlighted the complexities of decision making for dyads with dementia, which clearly extend beyond the question of whether or not a PWD is involved in the process. Other researchers have emphasized the importance of including the PWD and family caregiver together as a dyad in discharge planning during a hospitalization (Bloomer, Digby, Tan, Crawford, & Williams, 2016). This study adds to the literature by providing evidence of extensive variability in PWD involvement in decision making in the acute care setting, and by suggesting that there are differential determinants of perceptions of PWD involvement (e.g., cognitive impairment, relationship strain) depending on whose perspective of decision making is solicited (i.e., PWD’s or family caregiver’s). Future studies should thus continue to include both PWD and family caregiver perspectives while working toward addressing the modifiable aspects of decision-making involvement of PWDs.
The inpatient hospital setting is one place in which many decisions about post-hospital care and everyday living are made, and where most PWDs will find themselves at some point during the dementia trajectory. Although hospital discharge planning in the context of dementia is complex—and our measure of decision making involvement was not specific to all the decisions that go into the discharge plan—this study has implications for using a hospitalization as an opportunity to bring together patients with dementia and their family members to make decisions as a dyad. A hospitalization may also be an important time to focus on assisting dyads in planning together for everyday care at home while there are multiple resources, specialists, and other providers on hand. The results from this study suggest that, in order to appreciate the challenges of including the PWD in decision making, the nurse or clinician may need to first assess the extent of cognitive impairment in PWDs and the amount of strain in the dyad’s relationship, since both of these factors have the potential to diminish the dyad’s perceptions of the PWD’s decision-making involvement. Relationship strain is a modifiable factor, capturing the salient interpersonal context of the dyad. Providing family caregivers with resources to address strain in the dyad’s relationship could lead to improvements in the PWD’s involvement in decision-making. Finally, assessing family caregivers’ perceptions of the importance of autonomy to the PWD, and encouraging their support of it, may help sustain the PWD’s involvement in decision-making despite the challenges of doing so in an acute care environment. Ultimately, this study provides evidence that the family caregiver’s perception of the PWD’s values is a significant aspect of including the PWD in the types of decisions that make up the discharge plan. Thus, the family caregiver could be the PWD’s most crucial advocate for remaining involved in decision-making and optimizing care planning for the dyad with dementia in the acute care setting.
Funding
This work was supported in part by grants from the National Institute of Nursing Research of the National Institutes of Health [F31NR015195; T32NR013456]. Study data were collected and managed using REDCap electronic data capture tools hosted at Oregon Health & Science University, which is supported by a grant from Oregon Clinical and Translational Research Institute [1 UL1 RR024140 01]. The content is solely the responsibilities of the authors and does not necessarily represent the views of the National Institutes of Health.
Acknowledgments
The authors are sincerely grateful to the patients and families who participated, and for the support of nursing staff during the study’s recruitment and enrollment. The authors also acknowledge Jill A. Bennett, PhD, RN, for her contributions to the study design and insightful reviews of the manuscript.
References
- Adler G. (2010). Driving decision-making in older adults with dementia. Dementia, 9, 45–60. doi:10.1177/1471301209350289 [Google Scholar]
- Barnett R. C. Marshall N. L. Raudenbush S. W., & Brennan R. T (1993). Gender and the relationship between job experiences and psychological distress: A study of dual-earner couples. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 794–806. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.64.5.794 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bartlett R., & O’Connor D (2007). From personhood to citizenship: Broadening the lens for dementia practice and research. Journal of Aging Studies, 21, 107–118. doi:10.1016/j.jaging.2006.09.002 [Google Scholar]
- Bischoff K. E. Sudore R. Miao Y. Boscardin W. J., & Smith A. K (2013). Advance care planning and the quality of end-of-life care in older adults. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 61, 209–214. doi:10.1111/jgs.12105 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bloomer M. Digby R. Tan H. Crawford K., & Williams A (2016). The experience of family carers of people with dementia who are hospitalised. Dementia (London), 15(5), 1234–1245 doi:10.1177/1471301214558308 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boyle G. (2013). She’s usually quicker than the calculator: Financial management and decision-making in couples living with dementia. Health & Social Care in the Community, 21, 554–562. doi:10.1111/hsc.12044 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Callahan C. M. Arling G. Tu W. Rosenman M. B. Counsell S. R. Stump T. E., & Hendrie H. C (2012). Transitions in care for older adults with and without dementia. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 60, 813–820. doi:10.1111/j.1532-5415.2012.03905.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cotrell V., & Schulz R (1993). The perspective of the patient with Alzheimer’s disease: A neglected dimension of dementia research. The Gerontologist, 33, 205–211. doi:10.1093/geront/33.2.205 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Drought T. S., & Koenig B. A (2002). Choice in end-of-life decision making: Researching fact or fiction?The Gerontologist, 42, 114–128. doi:10.1093/geront/42.suppl_3.114 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Edvardsson D., & Nordvall K (2008). Lost in the present but confident of the past: Experiences of being in a psycho-geriatric unit as narrated by persons with dementia. Journal of Clinical Nursing, 17, 491–498. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2702.2006.01826.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Elwyn G. Frosch D. Thomson R. Joseph-Williams N. Lloyd A. Kinnersley P., … Barry M (2012). Shared decision making: A model for clinical practice. Journal of General Internal Medicine, 27, 1361–1367. doi:10.1007/s11606-012-2077-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Epstein-Lubow G. Gaudiano B. Darling E. Hinckley M. Tremont G. Kohn R., … Miller I. W (2012). Differences in depression severity in family caregivers of hospitalized individuals with dementia and family caregivers of outpatients with dementia. American Journal of Geriatrics Psychiatry, 20, 815–819. doi:10.1097/JGP.0b013e318235b62f [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Feinberg L. F., & Whitlatch C. J (2001). Are persons with cognitive impairment able to state consistent choices?The Gerontologist, 41, 374–382. doi:10.1093/geront/41.3.374 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Feinberg L. F., & Whitlatch C. J (2002). Decision-making for persons with cognitive impairment and their family caregivers. American Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease and Other Dementias, 17, 237–244. doi:10.1177/153331750201700406 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fetherstonhaugh D. Tarzia L., & Nay R (2013). Being central to decision making means I am still here!: The essence of decision making for people with dementia. Journal of Aging Study, 27, 143–150. doi:10.1016/j.jaging.2012.12.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fillenbaum G. G. Heyman A. Wilkinson W. E., & Haynes C. S (1987). Comparison of two screening tests in Alzheimer’s disease. The correlation and reliability of the Mini-Mental State Examination and the modified Blessed test. Archives of Neurology, 44, 924–927. doi:10.1001/archneur.1987.00520210026014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fisher S. E. Burgio L. D. Thorn B. E. Allen-Burge R. Gerstle J. Roth D. L., & Allen S. J (2002). Pain assessment and management in cognitively impaired nursing home residents: Association of certified nursing assistant pain report, minimum data set pain report, and analgesic medication use. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 50, 152–156. doi:10.1046/j.1532-5415.2002.50021.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Folstein M. F. Folstein S. E., & McHugh P. R (1975). Mini-mental state. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12, 189–198. doi:10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gillick M. R. (2013). The critical role of caregivers in achieving patient-centered care. JAMA, 310, 575–576. doi:10.1001/jama.2013.7310 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Greener H. Poole M. Emmett C. Bond J. Louw S. J., & Hughes J. C (2012). Value judgements and conceptual tensions: Decision-making in relation to hospital discharge for people with dementia. Clinical Ethics, 7, 166–174. doi:10.1258/ce.2012.012028 [Google Scholar]
- Hirschman K. B. Joyce C. M. James B. D. Xie S. X. Casarett D. J., & Karlawish J (2005). Would caregivers of Alzheimer disease patients involve their relative in a decision to use an AD-slowing medication?American Journal of Geriatrics Psychiatry, 13, 1014–1021. doi:10.1176/appi.ajgp.13.11.1014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hirschman K. B. Xie S. X. Feudtner C., & Karlawish J (2004). How does an Alzheimer’s disease patient’s role in medical decision making change over time?Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry and Neurology, 17, 55–60. doi:10.1177/0891988704264540 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Horton-Deutsch S. Twigg P., & Evans R (2007). Health care decision-making of persons with dementia. Dementia, 6, 105–120. doi:10.1177/1471301207075643 [Google Scholar]
- Judge K. S. Menne H. L., & Whitlatch C. J (2010). Stress process model for individuals with dementia. The Gerontologist, 50, 294–302. doi:10.1093/geront/gnp162 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Karel M. J. Gurrera R. J. Hicken B., & Moye J (2010). Reasoning in the capacity to make medical decisions: The consideration of values. The Journal of Clinical Ethics, 21, 58–71. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Karlawish J. Casarett D. Propert K. J. James B. D., & Clark C. M (2002). Relationship between Alzheimer’s disease severity and patient participation in decisions about their medical care. Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry and Neurology, 15, 68–72. doi:10.1177/089198870201500203 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kensinger E. A. (2009). Cognition in Aging and Age-Related Disease. In Squire L. R. (Ed.), Encyclopedia of Neuroscience (pp. 1055–1061): Elsevier Science. [Google Scholar]
- Logsdon R. G. Gibbons L. E. McCurry S. M., & Teri L (2002). Assessing quality of life in older adults with cognitive impairment. Psychosomatic Medicine, 64, 510–519. doi:10.1097/00006842-200205000-00016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lyons K. S., & Sayer A. G (2005). Longitudinal dyad models in family research. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 67, 1048–1060. doi:10.1111/j.1741-3737.2005.00193.x [Google Scholar]
- Mak W. (2011). Self-reported goal pursuit and purpose in life among people with dementia. Journal of Gerontology B Psychology Sciences and Social Sciences, 66(2), 177–184. doi:10.1093/geronb/gbq092 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Menne H. L. Judge K. S., & Whitlatch C. J (2009). Predictors of quality of life for individuals with dementia: Implications for intervention. Dementia, 8, 543–560. doi:10.1177/1471301209350288 [Google Scholar]
- Menne H. L. Tucke S. S. Whitlatch C. J., & Feinberg L. F (2008). Decision-making involvement scale for individuals with dementia and family caregivers. American Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease & Other Dementias, 23, 23–29. doi:10.1177/1533317507308312 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Menne H. L., & Whitlatch C. J (2007). Decision-making involvement of individuals with dementia. The Gerontologist, 47, 810–819. doi:10.1093/geront/47.6.810 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miller L. M. Whitlatch C. J., & Lyons K. S (2016). Shared decision making in dementia: A review of patient and family carer involvement. Dementia, 15, 1141–1157. doi:10.1177/1471301214555542 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mitchell A. J. (2009). A meta-analysis of the accuracy of the mini-mental state examination in the detection of dementia and mild cognitive impairment. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 43, 411–431. doi:10.1016/j.jpsychires.2008.04.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Murphy J., & Oliver T (2013). The use of Talking Mats to support people with dementia and their carers to make decisions together. Health & Social Care in the Community, 21, 171–180. doi:10.1111/hsc.12005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nilsson A. Rasmussen B. H., & Edvardsson D (2013). Falling behind: A substantive theory of care for older people with cognitive impairment in acute settings. Journal of Clinical Nursing, 22, 1682–1691. doi:10.1111/jocn.12177 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Orfei M. D. Varsi A. E. Blundo C. Celia E. Casini A. R. Caltagirone C., & Spalletta G (2010). Anosognosia in mild cognitive impairment and mild Alzheimer’s disease: Frequency and neuropsychological correlates. American Journal of Geriatrics Psychiatry, 18, 1133–1140. doi:10.1097/JGP.0b013e3181dd1c50 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Parmelee P. A. Lawton M. P., & Katz I. R (1989). Psychometric properties of the Geriatric Depression Scale among the institutionalized aged. Psychological Assessment, 1, 331–338. doi:10.1037/1040-3590.1.4.331 [Google Scholar]
- Pearlin L. I. Mullan J. T. Semple S. J., & Skaff M. M (1990). Caregiving and the stress process: An overview of concepts and their measures. The Gerontologist, 30, 583–594. doi:10.1093/geront/30.5.583 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pruchno R. Wilson-Genderson M., & Cartwright F (2009). Self-rated health and depressive symptoms in patients with end-stage renal disease and their spouses: a longitudinal dyadic analysis of late-life marriages. Journal of Gerontology B Psychology Sciences and Social Sciences, 64, 212–221. doi:10.1093/geronb/gbp006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Raudenbush S. W. Bryk A. S., & Congdon R (2011). HLM for Windows (Version 7). Skokie, IL: Scientific Software International, Inc. [Google Scholar]
- Samsi K., & Manthorpe J (2013). Everyday decision-making in dementia: Findings from a longitudinal interview study of people with dementia and family carers. International Psychogeriatrics, 25, 949–961. doi:10.1017/S1041610213000306 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sayer A. G., & Klute M. M (2005). Analyzing couples and families: Multilevel methods. In V. L. Bengtson A. C. Acock K. R. Allen P. Dilworth-Anderson, & D. M. Klein (Eds.), Sourcebook of family theory and research (pp. 289–313). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications. [Google Scholar]
- Schmid B. Allen R. S. Haley P. P., & Decoster J (2010). Family matters: Dyadic agreement in end-of-life medical decision making. The Gerontologist, 50, 226–237. doi:10.1093/geront/gnp166 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sebern M. D., & Whitlatch C. J (2007). Dyadic relationship scale: A measure of the impact of the provision and receipt of family care. The Gerontologist, 47, 741–751. doi:10.1093/geront/47.6.741 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shankar K. N. Hirschman K. B. Hanlon A. L., & Naylor M. D (2014). Burden in caregivers of cognitively impaired elderly adults at time of hospitalization: A cross-sectional analysis. Journal of American Geriatrics Society, 62, 276–284. doi:10.1111/jgs.12657 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sorensen S. Mak W., & Pinquart M (2011). Planning and decision making for care transitions. Annual Review of Gerontology Geriatrics, 31, 111–142. doi:10.1891/0198-8794.31.111 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Statacorp (2015). Stata Statistical Software: Release 14 (Version 14). College Station, TX: StataCorp LP. [Google Scholar]
- Thies W., & Bleiler L (2013). 2013 Alzheimer’s disease facts and figures. Alzheimer’s & Dementia, 9, 208–245. doi:10.1016/j.jalz.2013.02.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tombaugh T. M., & McIntyre N. J (1992). The Mini-Mental State Examination: A comprehensive review. Journal of American Geriatrics Society, 40, 922–935. doi:10.1111/j.1532–5415.1992.tb01992.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vernooij-Dassen M., & Moniz-Cook E (2016). Person-centred dementia care: Moving beyond caregiving. Aging and Mental Health, 20(7),667–668. doi:10.1080/13607863.2016.1154017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Whitlatch C. J. Feinberg L. F., & Tucke S. S (2005). Measuring the values and preferences for everyday care of persons with cognitive impairment and their families. The Gerontologist, 45, 370–380. doi:10.1093/geront/45.3.370 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Whitlatch C. J. Piiparinen R., & Feinberg L. F (2009). How well do family caregivers know their relatives’ care values and preferences?Dementia, 8, 223–243. doi:10.1177/1471301209103259 [Google Scholar]
- Woods R. T. (2001). Discovering the person with Alzheimer’s disease: cognitive, emotional and behavioural aspects. Aging and Mental Health, 5, S7–16. doi:10.1080/713650008 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]