Abstract
The hippocampus is essential for representing spatiotemporal context and establishing its association with the sensory details of daily life to form episodic memories. The olfactory cortex in particular shares exclusive anatomical connections with the hippocampus as a result of their common evolutionary history. Here we selectively inhibit hippocampal projections to the anterior olfactory nucleus (AON) during behavioural tests of contextually cued odour recall. We find that spatial odour memory and temporal odour memory are independently impaired following inhibition of distinct, topographically organized hippocampal-AON pathways. Our results not only reveal a longstanding unknown function for the AON but offer new mechanistic insights regarding the representation of odours in episodic memory.
Hippocampus is necessary for integrating the context with sensory cues to retrieve memory for unique episodes. Here, the authors show that inhibiting topographically organized projections from hippocampus to the anterior olfactory nucleus independently impairs spatial and temporal odour memory recall.
Introduction
What happened, when, and where? The ability to readily integrate elements of a unique event into a single representation is a fundamental property of episodic memory1. Encoding, storing, and retrieving contextually unique episodes is crucial for making sense of the present, forming predictions about the immediate future, and selecting behavioural responses relevant to survival2–4.
Lesion and recording studies in humans and non-human animals have highlighted a central role of the hippocampus (HPC) in mediating episodic memory5,6. The spatial and temporal context of an event are first encoded within the HPC as the collective activity of place and time cells7,8. Contextual information later serves as a potent retrieval cue, bringing about the rich multisensory details of the original experience9,10. An emerging theory holds that the HPC conducts this retrieval process by reinstating patterns of cortical activity observed during learning11,12. Yet, it is not known how hippocampal transmission of contextual information can reproduce the sensory aspects of episodic memory.
Olfaction is considered the most evolutionarily ancient sense as evidenced by the direct anatomical connections between the olfactory cortex and the limbic system13,14. In particular, hippocampal projections to the olfactory cortex offer a unique experimental model for understanding the context-driven recollection of previously encountered sensory stimuli. Recently, we revealed a dense and topographically organized projection from the dorsoventral extent of the HPC to the anterior olfactory nucleus (AON)15 (Fig. 1a). The AON receives unidirectional, monosynaptic inputs from the CA1, in contrast to other primary sensory areas that receive hippocampal inputs indirectly via adjacent medial temporal lobe structures16.
The AON is an ideal site of convergence for olfactory and contextual information given its anatomical position as the initial recipient of input from the olfactory bulb and the largest source of feedback projections within the olfactory cortex17,18. Consistently, it has been shown that hippocampal inputs to the AON can alter olfactory perception and odour-guided behaviours19. However, the functional role of the HPC-AON pathway remains unexplored. Here we combine chemogenetic and optogenetic approaches to demonstrate that information regarding the spatial and temporal context of odour memory is delivered by topographically organized hippocampal inputs to the AON.
Results
AON-projecting HPC cells support context-dependent odour memory recall
Using c-Fos as a proxy for neural activity we measured the AON’s response to a novel odour and context, separately or in combination. We found that the AON exhibits a widespread increase in c-Fos-positive neurons following exposure to a novel context-odour pairing, yet neither stimulus alone was sufficient to evoke an increase in the number of labelled cells compared with homecage controls (Supplementary Fig. 1). These results are in line with previous work on the response properties of AON principal cells and suggest that the AON has a role in context-odour coincidence detection20,21.
To manipulate activity in the HPC-AON pathway, we infused the retrogradely propagating canine adenovirus encoding Cre recombinase (CAV2-Cre) into the AON, followed by HPC infusions of an Adeno-associated virus (AAV) vector carrying a Cre-dependent inhibitory hM4D-mCherry (AAV8-hSyn-FLEX-hM4D-mCherry) or mCherry alone (AAV8-hSyn-FLEX-mCherry) (Fig. 1b). This allowed us to selectively inhibit AON-projecting HPC cells upon the administration of clozapine-N-oxide (CNO), thereby limiting the AON’s response to a context-odour pairing (Supplementary Fig. 2). Cre-mediated viral mCherry expression was observed throughout the ventral two thirds of the hippocampal CA1 (Fig. 1c). Three weeks after viral infusion, mice underwent behavioural tests to evaluate memory for the associations between odours and the spatiotemporal context in which they occurred. Assessing the retrieval of episodic odour information was made possible by capitalizing on the innate tendency of mice to preferentially investigate novel stimuli22. Thus, mice can behaviourally express correct memory by investigating odours paired with a novel position in space, or temporal sequence, more so than familiar configurations.
We first tested the ability to remember where specific odours occurred in context. In two encoding phases of a spatial odour memory test, mice were presented with two different odours (odour 1 and 2) placed on opposite ends of a distinct context (A) for 5 min. Mice were then placed in a separate context (B) where the same odours were positioned in reversed locations. Each exposure was followed by a 15 min retention delay, the second of which was preceded by a CNO injection. In the subsequent retrieval phase, animals were reintroduced to context A wherein two copies of either odour 1 or 2 were presented on both sides. In this paradigm, correct memory expression would drive mice to investigate the odour found at the novel location (NL) within the context, as seen in control mice expressing mCherry alone (Fig. 1d). In contrast, CNO-treated hM4D mice investigated both, otherwise identical odours for a similar proportion of time. These results cannot be explained by differences in total investigation time or distance travelled as both groups displayed similar measures in each (Supplementary Fig. 3).
Next, we examined memory for when specific odours were encountered. Mice were presented with a sequence of odours in three successive encoding phases followed by a retrieval phase where two of the previously encountered odours were reintroduced. Control mice preferentially investigated the odour encountered earlier in the sequence, yet CNO-treated hM4D mice investigated both odours to a largely equal extent (Fig. 1e). Importantly, the spatial context was consistent throughout the test where novelty was only conferred by temporal distance (TD) between the two odours. Lastly, to examine whether hippocampal function can be extended to memory for odours regardless of context, we conducted a novel odour recognition test where animals were presented with a familiar and previously unexplored odour. Both groups preferentially investigated the novel odour despite CNO administration (Fig. 1f). Discrimination ratios obtained for both groups under all conditions were compared to zero (chance performance) and the results detailed in Supplementary Table 1. Together, our findings indicate that AON-projecting HPC cells mediate the retrieval of odour memory only when it is tied to spatiotemporal context.
Spatiotemporal information is transmitted along distinct HPC-AON pathways
The ring of cells which constitute the AON is typically partitioned into four subregions based on their relative cardinal positions: pars medialis (mAON), pars dorsalis (dAON), pars lateralis (lAON), and pars ventralis (vAON)17,23. Previous studies have found that each subregion displays distinct cytoarchitectural, morphological, neurochemical, and connectivity patterns, thereby providing critical foundations for defining AON subregional boundaries23 (for a review, see ref. 17). Our recent neuroanatomical study revealed a previously unknown topographic gradient in HPC-AON projections such that the ventral-most part of the HPC innervates most heavily the medial aspect of the AON and, progressively, more dorsal parts of the HPC innervate increasingly more lateral positions at the AON15. As a result, our definition of the mAON encompasses the pars medialis, yet includes a portion of what is traditionally accepted to be pars dorsalis.
Such topographically organized HPC terminals at the AON may transmit distinct contextual cues depending on where they arise within the HPC. To explore this idea, we employed archaerhodopsin (ArchT) to optogenetically inhibit intermediate HPC (iHPC) or ventral HPC (vHPC) terminals that innervate separate AON subregions. One group of mice received bilateral infusions of AAV-CaMKIIa-ArchT-eYFP into the iHPC with optic fibre implantations in the lateral AON (lAON) for inhibiting the iHPC-lAON pathway and another group underwent viral infusions into the vHPC with optic fibre implantations in the medial AON (mAON) for inhibiting the vHPC-mAON pathway (Fig. 2a; Supplementary Fig. 4). A control group given counterbalanced infusions and implantations were prepared using AAV expressing green fluorescent protein (GFP) only. The efficacy of optogenetic inhibition was confirmed in a separate group of mice unilaterally infused with ArchT, where hippocampal terminal inhibition during a novel context-odour encounter selectively reduced the density of c-Fos positive cells within the AON ipsilateral to the manipulation, leaving the contralateral side unaffected (Supplementary Fig. 5).
Animals were tested in the same behavioural paradigms used for hM4D experiments, except inhibition was mediated by light (532 nm, 12 mW) limited to the retrieval phase. Illumination of the iHPC-lAON and vHPC-mAON pathways disrupted memory for where odours were encountered, as both groups were unable to discriminate odours tied to a spatial location within a specific context (Fig. 2b). In contrast, only vHPC-mAON, but not iHPC-lAON pathway inhibition impaired memory for when odours occurred in a temporal sequence (Fig. 2c). Importantly, all groups showed similar levels of novelty preference in a context-independent novel odour recognition test (Fig. 2d). A comparison of the discrimination ratios with chance performance can be found as Supplementary Table 2. The identified impairments in episodic odour memory retrieval suggest that representations of space and time are differentially distributed across the AON whereby both iHPC and vHPC inputs deliver spatial information, but information regarding the temporal context is supported only by vHPC inputs.
The behavioural tests employed thus far were designed to separately examine the spatial and temporal elements of episodic odour memory. However, given the unified nature of recollected episodes, it is necessary to probe memory-based behavioural judgements while the animal concurrently weighs both parameters. To investigate further the spatiotemporal contributions of hippocampal inputs to the AON, we adopted an episodic memory test where recollection of an odour, its spatial location, and temporal occurrence (what–when–where) were tested simultaneously24,25. The test involved two encoding phases and one retrieval phase, each separated by a 1 h delay (Fig. 3a). Light-mediated inhibition was limited to the retrieval phase. During the encoding phases, mice sampled two distinct odours placed at adjacent corners of an open field, and then sampled a new set of odours positioned on the opposite side of the arena. In the retrieval phase, all four odours were presented, each possessing a unique spatiotemporal configuration. Encountered odours were either in a novel location and appeared earlier in temporal distance (NL/TD), familiar in location/temporally distant (FL/TD), novel in location/temporally recent (NL/TR), or familiar in location/temporally recent (FL/TR).
The control group displayed a pattern of investigation indicative of their novelty preference such that the greatest proportion of time was spent investigating the odour encountered in the most novel spatiotemporal combination (NL/TD), while investigation time for the remaining odours decreased in a familiarity-dependent manner: (NL/TD) > (FL/TD) ≃ (NL/TR) > (FL/TR) (Fig. 3a, b). The iHPC-lAON group spent the greatest proportion (~50%) of time investigating the odour with the FL/TD configuration, while the NL/TR odour was investigated least. Strikingly, inhibition of the vHPC-mAON pathway produced an inverse pattern of investigation to the control group. The odour presented in the FL/TR configuration was investigated most, while the NL/TD odour was investigated least. A separate analysis delineating the spatial and temporal contributions to memory revealed that iHPC-lAON pathway inhibition impaired spatial but not temporal odour associations, while the vHPC-mAON pathway inhibition impaired both components to a similar extent (Fig. 3c). Collectively, these findings confirm the differential roles of the iHPC-lAON and vHPC-mAON pathways in conveying spatiotemporal information during olfactory episodic memory retrieval.
HPC-AON input is necessary for context-driven odour recall
The ability to explicitly recollect sensory information is a hallmark of episodic memory, particularly when lacking a physical sensory cue26. Context alone can drive the activity of primary olfactory regions to form internal representations of odours although the underlying neural circuit is unknown9,10. To this end, we examined whether the HPC-AON pathway could support this function. Mice were allowed to explore a rich spatial context in the presence of a pure odour emitted from a cotton swab tip for 30 min per day over nine consecutive days. On day 10, mice were reintroduced to the context in the absence of the applied odour. The mismatch between the odour-paired context and the lack of an emitted scent drives an increase in the investigation time for the cotton swab (Supplementary Movie 1). Indeed, control mice exhibited a marked increase in investigation of the cotton swab upon failure to find the expected odour (Fig. 4a; Supplementary Fig. 6). In contrast, hM4D- and ArchT-mediated inhibition of the HPC-AON pathway abolished this behaviour. All groups showed no increase in investigation time when similarly trained but tested in a novel context (Fig. 4b). In a separate group of mice trained on an odour-context association and later exposed to the context alone, immunostaining for c-Fos revealed a context-driven activation of the AON, complimentary to similar activity patterns observed in the olfactory bulb and piriform cortex (Supplementary Fig. 7)9,10. Together, these results indicate that HPC-AON communication is necessary for mediating the context-driven recollection of odours.
Discussion
The HPC has an essential role in organizing memory for sensory events within the framework of spatiotemporal context, a function integral to episodic memory27. Memory for sensory experiences are theorized to be distributed in modality-specific neocortical networks12,26,28–31. The scattered components of the engram are bound by the HPC which acts as an integrative hub, providing storage for context-based associative indices32. Later during retrieval, the HPC uses the associative index to reinstate patterns of cortical activity that were observed during encoding11. One functional magnetic resonance imaging study in particular provided initial evidence for the reactivation of the olfactory cortex during odour memory recollection9. In their study, human subjects were instructed to learn associations between an odour and a context after which they were examined for the effect of the context alone on neural activity during memory retrieval. Both the primary olfactory cortex and the anterior HPC were activated, adding further support for the role of the HPC in binding sensory memory traces preserved in modality-specific neural regions.
We show that HPC inputs to the AON are necessary for mediating the context-driven recollection of odours (Fig. 4). In our context-driven odour recollection paradigm, the context alone has been shown to produce an internal representation of the associated odour and drive behavioural investigation of the empty odour source10. Whether the activity of place and time cells within the HPC are themselves sufficient to trigger retrieval of the odour memory remains unknown. It is possible that the increased firing rate of ‘misplace’ or mismatch cells within the HPC facilitates the reinstatement of odour representations within the AON, particularly when mice failed to find the odour paired with the context. These hippocampal principal neurons were first characterized in O’Keefe’s33 landmark paper where place units were also described. Misplace cells fire maximally after encountering novel stimuli or at the mismatch between experience and expectation (i.e., when rats sniffed in a particular location where a novel stimulus was found or when rats failed to find the expected stimulus which had been there)33. Thus, the firing of misplace cells may serve as a signal to instigate odour recollection when the odour is no longer present. Inhibition of the HPC-AON pathway may have interrupted the downstream propagation of this signal thereby preventing the exploratory behaviour usually displayed.
To our knowledge, this work provides the first functional demonstration that hippocampal inputs to the olfactory cortex are necessary for episodic odour memory. Specifically, we show that inhibition of hippocampal inputs to the AON results in a loss of odour memory when it is tied to spatiotemporal context (Fig. 1). Next, we demonstrated that hippocampal representations of space and time are differentially distributed across the AON such that both iHPC and vHPC inputs transmit spatial information, whereas information regarding the temporal context is supported by vHPC inputs (Figs. 2, 3). The segregation of hippocampal inputs by their spatiotemporal contribution is consistent with recording studies which suggest that each dimension of an event is coded independently within overlapping hippocampal neural populations34. Although the dorsal HPC is commonly thought to dominate spatial processing, particularly when contrasted to the ventral aspect, this dichotomic perspective is being revised in light of growing evidence for a hippocampal model characterized by multiple levels of functional long-axis gradients and discrete domains35. This alternative perspective better accommodates recent findings demonstrating iHPC and vHPC involvement in transmitting spatial information and is in accordance with our results24,36. Lastly, in addition to the AON, HPC axon fibres were found, albeit to a much lesser extent, within the tenia tecta, an extension of the HPC into the olfactory peduncle. Little is known about the function of this structure, thus the contribution of HPC inputs to the tenia tecta remains undetermined.
Ultimately, our findings support a model of episodic odour memory whereby information regarding odour quality and spatiotemporal context merge at the level of the AON and, as a natural consequence of Hebbian synaptic plasticity, produce cellular populations that represent previously encountered odours within the context in which they occurred (Fig. 5). Such a system maintains the fidelity of the original memory and allows access for retrieval of the full trace via partial cues from either olfactory or contextual inputs.
Our findings reveal a previously unreported function for the AON, a structure that has long remained elusive in its role in olfactory information processing. The AON, in its position immediately posterior to the olfactory bulb and anterior to the piriform cortex, maintains connections at nearly every synaptic step in the olfactory pathway17. Its central arrangement and extensive command over activity within the olfactory cortex makes the structure a conceivable repository of episodic odour engrams. Adding further weight to this possibility is the AON’s well-documented involvement in the early pathophysiology of Alzheimer’s disease, a debilitating disorder of episodic memory37–41. In fact, the AON has repeatedly been identified as the earliest site of neurodegeneration, including cell loss and the formation of neurofibrillary tangles, senile plaques, and other associated histological markers42–45. Complementing these observations is a vast body of work which reports olfactory dysfunction, specifically olfactory memory loss and hyposmia, as symptoms of the onset of Alzheimer’s disease46–49. Consistently, our previous findings have also implicated the AON in the top-down modulation of olfactory sensitivity19. Studies of olfactory deficits associated with Alzheimer’s disease have now prompted measurements of olfactory ability to be used as clinical biomarkers in the early diagnosis of the disease50–52.
Our findings implicate the AON as the storehouse of episodic odour engrams and suggest that its accelerated degeneration may underlie the olfactory dysfunction observed in Alzheimer’s disease. Thus, the HPC-AON pathway provides a novel circuit model for studying fundamental aspects of human episodic memory and the odour memory deficits commonly found in neurodegenerative conditions.
Methods
Animals
Male C57BL/6 mice (Charles River Laboratories) were used in all behavioural tests. All mice were 8–10 weeks old at the time of surgery and 12–14 weeks old at the time behavioural testing began. Before surgery, mice were group-housed in a temperature-controlled room on a 12 h light/dark cycle with ad libitum access to food and water. Following surgery, mice were individually housed. A total of 60 mice were distributed into two groups for hM4D experiments (hM4D-mCherry: n = 10, mCherry control: n = 8) and three groups for ArchT experiments (GFP-control: n = 14, iHPC-lAON: n = 12, vHPC-mAON: n = 16). An additional 29 animals were used for c-Fos immunoreactivity experiments. All procedures were performed in accordance with the guidelines of the Canadian Council on Animal Care and the University of Toronto Animal Care Committee.
Surgical procedures
CAV2-Cre viral vector was purchased from the Plateforme de Vectorologie de Montpellier, AAV2/8-hSyn-FLEX-hM4D-mCherry (hM4D) and AAV2/8-hSyn-DIO-mCherry (mCherry control) from the vector core at University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, and AAV2/5-hSyn-hChR2-mCherry (ChR2-mCherry), AAV2/5-hSyn-hChR2-eYFP (ChR2-YFP), AAV2/5-CaMKIIa-ArchT-eYFP (ArchT), and AAV2/8-CB7-CI-EGFP-RBG (GFP-control) from the University of Pennsylvania Vector Core. Stereotaxic surgery was conducted on mice anaesthetized with isoflurane and administered ketoprofen (5 mg/kg) for pain management. For chemogenetic experiments, CAV2-Cre viral vector was bilaterally infused into the mAON (10° angle toward midline; anterior/posterior (AP): + 2.90, medial/lateral (ML): ± 1.10, dorsal/ventral (DV): − 3.42) and lAON (no angle; AP: + 3.20, ML: ± 1.10, DV: − 3.90) at a volume of 0.1–0.2 μL and hM4D into the iHPC (no angle; AP: − 2.70, ML: ± 2.20, DV: − 2.00) and vHPC (10° angle away from midline; AP: − 2.92, ML: ± 2.15, DV: − 4.90) at a volume of 0.3–0.4 μL. The position immediate to the rhinal fissure was used as a landmark to delineate intermediate and ventral parts. For optogenetic experiments, ArchT or GFP-control was bilaterally infused into the iHPC or vHPC in a volume of 0.3–0.4 μL, and optical fibres (200 μm core diameter, 0.39 NA; Thorlabs, Newton, NJ, USA) threaded through 1.25 mm-wide zirconia ferrules (Thorlabs) were bilaterally implanted into the lAON or mAON, respectively. For anterograde tracing experiments, ChR2-mCherry was infused into the vHPC and ChR2-YFP was infused into the contralateral iHPC. All infusions were made by means of pressure ejection at a rate of 0.1 μL/min through a cannula connected by Tygon tubing to a 10 μL Hamilton syringe (Hamilton, Reno, NV). A 15 min interval was allotted after each infusion to limit the viral spread.
Drugs
CNO obtained from the NIH was dissolved in a solution of 10% dimethyl sulphoxide and 0.9% saline. A dose of 5 mg/kg of CNO was used in all behavioural experiments using hM4D-mCherry- and mCherry-only-expressing animals. All CNO treatments were separated by a minimum of 72 h.
Apparatus for optogenetic experiments
Inhibition of hippocampal terminals at the AON was conducted by illumination with green light (532 nm, 12 mW) generated by a diode-pumped solid state laser (Laserglow, Toronto, ON, Canada). The laser was connected to a 1 × 2 optical commutator (Doric Lenses, Quebec, QC, Canada), which divided the light path into two arena patch cables attached to the implanted optical fibres.
Experimental design
Unless otherwise noted, all tests took place in a 50 cm × 25 cm × 20 cm plexiglass open-topped cage. Odours were presented mixed with woodchip bedding in 3 cm wide, 1 cm high aluminium cups. Multiple identical odour cups were used such that an animal never investigated the same cup twice. The odours used included nutmeg, vanillin, coriander, banana, garlic, cinnamon, thyme, almond, onion, curry, ginger, savoury, cumin, dill, jasmine, coffee, oregano, sage, and rosemary. The odours presented and the order of their presentation between animals was pseudorandomized. For habituation, mice were given 15 min of exploration time for each unique context prior to initial exposure. Each exposure was 5 min in length and inter-trial intervals were 15 min. All tests were video-recorded at 60 fps using a NIKON D5200 equipped with a 30 mm lens. An additional overhead video was recorded using a Logitech webcam. All videos were subsequently scored blind to the treatment groups. Exploration was strictly defined as head up sniffing, directed towards and within 1 cm of the odour source. This definition excludes the use of the odour cup for sitting or as support during rearing.
Olfactory spatial memory test: In this paradigm adapted from Eacott and Norman53, mice were tested for memory of odour location in context. The test chamber was altered to produce two distinct contexts. Zebra-patterned paper was used to line the walls of context A, whereas context B had transparent walls surrounded by red plastic cups and bedding on the floor. Each animal underwent two encoding phases and one retrieval phase. During the first encoding phase, mice explored context A where two highly distinct odours were placed at opposite ends of the chamber (odour 1 on the left and odour 2 on the right). Next, mice were removed from the chamber and placed in a holding cage. The mice were then returned to the chamber, except it was now configured as context B and contained both odours in opposite positions (odour 2 on the left and odour 1 on the right). The animals were allowed to explore both odours in their new positions before being placed back into the holding cage. For the retrieval phase, the chamber was reconfigured as context A but now two copies of one odour were presented on both sides of the chamber. Time spent investigating the odour cups was measured. The novel configuration consists of the familiar odour in a novel position within the original context. The initial context and left/right position of the odour cups were pseudorandomized.
Olfactory temporal order memory test: This paradigm is based on similar tests used previously to measure memory for the temporal order of objects54,55. Mice were tested in a transparent chamber with spatial cues kept constant throughout the session. Each animal underwent three encoding phases and one retrieval phase. In the first exposure, mice were placed in the chamber with two copies of one odour presented on opposite sides of the arena. After exploring both copies, the animal was removed from the chamber and placed in a holding cage. This process was repeated two more times using different odours each time. During the retrieval phase the animal was returned to the chamber, but this time earlier and recently explored odours were presented on opposite sides. In this case, the odour explored earlier is more novel given its TD compared with the recently explored odour. The left–right positions of the first and last odours during the test were pseudorandomized. Time spent investigating both odours was measured.
Novel odour recognition test: This test was given 72 h after examining performance on temporal order memory and followed a similar paradigm. The animals underwent three encoding phases where two copies of a unique odour were presented in each. On the retrieval phase, mice were presented with the initially encountered odour and a previously unexplored odour on opposite sides of the chamber. Time spent investigating each odour was measured.
Olfactory episodic memory test: Animals were first habituated to the apparatus, which consisted of a 50 cm × 50 cm × 20 cm transparent plexiglass open field for a 30 min period. The animals were then exposed to two encoding phases and one retrieval phase each separated by a 1 h delay. In the first encoding phase, animals were given 10 min to explore two different odours located at two adjacent corners of the arena. In the second encoding phase, the animals were given an additional 10 min to explore another set of unique odours presented on the opposite adjacent corners. During the retrieval phase, all four odours were presented with the spatial position of one odour from each set exchanged. This presentation results in each odour possessing a unique spatiotemporal configuration—NL/TD, FL/TD, NL/TR, and FL/TR. Successful memory for an integrated (what, when, and where) memory results in a pattern of exploration such that the odour with the NL/TD configuration is preferentially investigated the most while the FL/TR configuration is investigated the least. Time spent investigating all four odours was measured for 5 min in the retrieval phase. Overhead videos were analysed using the ANY-maze software to produce average heat maps of each treatment group’s position within the arena.
Context-driven odour recall test: In this paradigm adapted from Mandairon et al.10, mice were trained to associate a visually distinct context with an odour and subsequently tested for recollection of the odour when exposed to the context alone. The testing apparatus consists of a 50 cm × 30 cm × 20 cm plexiglas cage with colourful visual patterns pasted on the outside of the walls. A wooden applicator with a cotton swab tip was positioned 3 cm from the floor and 5 cm from one end of the chamber. Before introducing mice into the chamber, 100 μL of a pure odourant was applied to the cotton tip. Each mouse was randomly assigned a monomolecular odourant to be trained with among limonene, isoamyl acetate, nonane, and 1-pentanol. Mice were allowed to explore the context and the odorized cotton swab for 30 min per day for 9 consecutive days. On Day 10, mice were once again placed into the cage, however no odour was added to the cotton swab. Investigation time of the cotton swab was measured on day 9 and 10 for the first 5 min of their exposure to the context. Upon failure to detect the expected odour, mice behaviourally expressed memory by spending a greater amount of time investigating the cotton swab compared to their investigation time when the odour was present on Day 9.
General histology
After behavioural testing, mice were transcardially perfused with phosphate-buffered saline (PBS, pH 7.4), followed by 4% paraformaldehyde in phosphate buffer. Brain tissue was extracted and postfixed overnight at 4 °C. The brains were then cryoprotected using a 30% sucrose in PBS solution. Coronal 40 µm-thick sections were collected using a cryostat (Leica, Germany). The sections were slide-mounted, counterstained with 4′,6-diamidino-2-phenylindole 135 for 5 min and subsequently coverslipped with Aquamount (Polysciences, Inc., Warrington, PA). Wide-field fluorescent images were captured using a 4 × objective lens on a fluorescent microscope (Olympus, Japan). Confocal images were captured using a × 20 and × 60 objective through a Quorum spinning disk confocal microscope (Zeiss, Germany). Adobe Photoshop CS6 (Adobe Systems, Incorporated, San Jose, CA) was used to adjust the brightness and contrast of representative sections.
Immunohistochemistry
For c-Fos immunostaining, free-floating tissue sections were obtained and washed with PBS with Triton X-100 (PBS-T) and then blocked with normal donkey serum (5 in 0.1% PBS-T) for 1 h. The sections were subsequently incubated with rabbit polyclonal anti-c-Fos antibody (1:1000 in PBS-T; Santa Cruz Biotechnology, California) for 72 h on a nutating mixer at 4 °C. After incubation with the primary antibody the sections were submerged in Alexa Flour 594-conjugated donkey anti-rabbit secondary antibody (1:500 in PBS-T; Invitrogen) for 90 min at room temperature.
Validation of chemo- and optogenetic inhibition
To characterize the AON’s response patterns we exposed groups of three animals to one of four conditions: homecage only, homecage with an applied odour (100 µL of limonene on a cotton ball), novel context only, or novel context with an applied odour. With the exception of the homecage control group, all animals were put into each condition for a total duration of 30 min.
To validate our chemogenetic and optogenetic manipulations, we prepared two groups of four animals—one group received unilateral CAV2-Cre infusions into the mAON and Cre-responsive hM4D in the vHPC; another received unilateral ArchT infusions into the vHPC and optic fibre implantations in the mAON. Each group was exposed to a novel odour-context pairing for 10 min (reduced from 30 min to prevent prolonged illumination in the optogenetic experiment). hM4D-expressing mice were injected with CNO 15 min prior to exposure whereas ArchT-expressing mice were illuminated with green light throughout the exposure. In the subsequent tissue analysis, the mean density of c-Fos-labelled neurons were compared between each hemisphere.
To examine context-driven AON activity, two groups of three mice were trained on a context-odour association by placing the animals in a context (50 × 30 × 20 cm plexiglas closed-lid cage with colourful visual patterns pasted on the outer walls) with an applied odour for 1 h over three consecutive days. The mice were killed the following day after exposure to the context-odour pair or the context alone for 30 min. c-Fos density was measured and compared to a third group trained and exposed to a context in the absence of an applied odour.
All animals were transcardially perfused 75 min after initial exposure to their testing condition.
Cell counting
Every other section from bregma + 3.20 to + 2.34 was collected and immunostained for c-Fos. Sections were mounted and scanned using a 4 × objective lens on a fluorescent microscope. c-Fos-positive cell counting was performed using the cellSens software (Olympus). The area of each AON section was outlined to form a region of interest (ROI) and the number of mCherry-expressing cells were counted. Calibration parameters were established using randomly chosen prominent neurons and adjusted by raising the threshold of detection to a conservative level, excluding the majority of false positives. Once set, the calibration was kept constant. The density of neurons was calculated by dividing the number of labelled cells in the ROI by the area for each AON section. The mean density was determined for each animal and for all animals in each experimental group.
Calculations and statistical analysis
The discrimination ratios were derived from the exploration time of odour-context pairings during the retrieval phase of each test. For all tests based on the spontaneous novelty preference paradigm, the discrimination ratio was calculated as the difference between the times spent exploring the novel and familiar odour-context configurations divided by the total amount of time investigating both odours. Here, a value of zero indicates that the animal investigated both odours to an equal extent. Positive values up to one indicate preference for the novel combination, whereas a negative value indicates greater investigation of the familiar odour-context pair. The discrimination ratios of each group obtained using the behavioural paradigms detailed in Fig. 1 and Fig. 2 were compared to zero (chance performance) using a (two-tailed) one-sample t-test.
For the episodic memory test, percent investigation time was calculated by dividing the amount of time spent investigating an individual odour-spatiotemporal configuration by the time investigating all odours and multiplied by 100%:
For analysing spatial memory, the difference in the amount of time spent investigating odours with novel and familiar positions was divided by the total investigation time:
For temporal memory, the difference in the time spent investigating odours experienced earlier and later was divided by the total investigation time:
Performance in novelty preference-based paradigms was compared using t-test in hM4D experiments and a one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) with testing group as the factor in optogenetic experiments. Percent investigation time data collected in the episodic memory test was analysed using a two-way ANOVA with testing group and odour-spatiotemporal configuration as factors. A two-way ANOVA was used to analyse data in the contextually cued odour recall test for hM4D and optogenetic experiments, respectively, using experimental day and treatment group as factors. Where appropriate, Tukey’s multiple comparisons test was used for post hoc comparisons. Significance was defined as *P < 0.05, **P < 0.005, ***P < 0.0005, ****P < 0.0001.
Data availability
All relevant data are available from the corresponding authors upon reasonable request.
Electronic supplementary material
Acknowledgements
We thank Drs. Rutsuko Ito, Boyer Winters, Kaori Takehara, Paul Whissell, and John Yeomans for their valuable input regarding the collection and interpretation of data. This research was funded by operating grants to J.C.K. from the Canadian Institutes for Health Research (CIHR) (MOP 496401) and the Natural Sciences and Engineering Council of Canada (NSERC) (MOP 491009).
Author contributions
A.J.A. and J.C.K. carried out the study conceptualization and experimental design. A.J.A. performed and analysed behavioural experiments. A.J.A. and J.C.K. wrote the manuscript.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Electronic supplementary material
Supplementary Information accompanies this paper at 10.1038/s41467-018-05131-6.
Publisher's note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
References
- 1.Tulving E. Elements of Episodic Memory. Oxford: Oxford Univ. Press; 1985. [Google Scholar]
- 2.Chanes L, Barrett LF. Redefining the role of limbic areas in cortical processing. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2016;20:96–106. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2015.11.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Chu MW, Li WL, Komiyama T. Balancing the robustness and efficiency of odor representations during learning. Neuron. 2016;92:174–186. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.09.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Davachi L, DuBrow S. How the hippocampus preserves order: the role of prediction and context. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2015;19:92–99. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2014.12.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Tulving E, Markowitsch HJ. Episodic and declarative memory: role of the hippocampus. Hippocampus. 1998;8:198–204. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-1063(1998)8:3<198::AID-HIPO2>3.0.CO;2-G. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Eichenbaum H. Memory: organization and control. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2017;68:19–45. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010416-044131. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Moser EI, Kropff E, Moser MB. Place cells, grid cells, and the brain’s spatial representation system. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2008;31:69–89. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.31.061307.090723. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Eichenbaum H. Time cells in the hippocampus: a new dimension for mapping memories. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2014;15:732–744. doi: 10.1038/nrn3827. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Gottfried JA, Smith AP, Rugg MD, Dolan RJ. Remembrance of odors past: human olfactory cortex in cross-modal recognition memory. Neuron. 2004;42:687–695. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(04)00270-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Mandairon, et al. Context-driven activation of odor representations in the absence of olfactory stimuli in the olfactory bulb and piriform cortex. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 2014;8:138. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00138. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Tanaka, et al. Cortical representations are reinstated by the hippocampus during memory retrieval. Neuron. 2014;84:347–354. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.09.037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Nyberg L, Habib R, McIntosh AR, Tulving E. Reactivation of encoding-related brain activity during memory retrieval. PNAS. 2000;97:11120–11124. doi: 10.1073/pnas.97.20.11120. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Isaacson R. The Limbic System. 2nd edn. New York City, NY: Plenum Publishing Corp; 1982. [Google Scholar]
- 14.Rowe TB, Macrini TE, Luo ZX. Fossil evidence on origin of the mammalian brain. Science. 2011;332:955–957. doi: 10.1126/science.1203117. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Aqrabawi AJ, Kim JC. Topographic organization of hippocampal inputs to the anterior olfactory nucleus. Front. Neuroanat. 2018;12:12. doi: 10.3389/fnana.2018.00012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Swanson LW, Köhler C. Anatomical evidence for direct projections from the entorhinal area to the entire cortical mantle in the rat. J. Neurosci. 1986;6:3010–23. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.06-10-03010.1986. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Brunjes PC, Illig KR, Meyer E. A field guide to the anterior olfactory nucleus (cortex) Brain Res. Rev. 2005;50:305–335. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2005.08.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Carson KA. Quantitative localization of neurons projecting to the mouse main olfactory bulb. Brain Res. Bull. 1984;12:629–634. doi: 10.1016/0361-9230(84)90143-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Aqrabawi, et al. Top-down modulation of olfactory-guided behaviours by the anterior olfactory nucleus pars medialis and ventral hippocampus. Nat. Commun. 2016;7:13721. doi: 10.1038/ncomms13721. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Kay RB, Meyer EA, Illig KR, Brunjes PC. Spatial distribution of neural activity in the anterior olfactory nucleus evoked by odor and electrical stimulation. J. Comp. Neurol. 2011;519:277–289. doi: 10.1002/cne.22519. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.McGinley MJ, Westbrook GL. Membrane and synaptic properties of pyramidal neurons in the anterior olfactory nucleus. J. Neurophysiol. 2011;105:1444–1453. doi: 10.1152/jn.00715.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Ennaceur A, Delacour J. A new one-trial test for neurobiological studies of memory in rats. 1: behavioral data. Behav. Brain. Res. 1988;31:47–59. doi: 10.1016/0166-4328(88)90157-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Haberly LB, Price JL. Association and commissural fiber systems of the olfactory cortex of the rat. II. Systems originating in the olfactory peduncle. J. Comp. Neurol. 1978;181:781–808. doi: 10.1002/cne.901810407. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Barker, et al. Separate elements of episodic memory subserved by distinct hippocampal-prefrontal connections. Nat. Neurosci. 2017;20:242–250. doi: 10.1038/nn.4472. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Dere E, Huston JP, De Souza Silva MA. Episodic-like memory in mice: simultaneous assessment of object, place and temporal order memory. Brain Res Protoc. 2005;16:10–19. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresprot.2005.08.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Wheeler ME, Petersen SE, Buckner RL. Reactivation of encoding-related brain activity during memory retrieval. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2000;97:11125–11129. doi: 10.1073/pnas.97.20.11125. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Eichenbaum, H. What versus where: non-spatial aspects of memory representation by the hippocampus. In: Behavioral Neuroscience of Learning and Memory. Current Topics in Behavioral Neurosciences vol. 37 (eds. Clark, R. E & Martin, S.) 101–117 (Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg, 2016). [DOI] [PubMed]
- 28.Sakai K, Miyashita Y. Neural organization for the long-term memory of paired associates. Nature. 1991;354:152–155. doi: 10.1038/354152a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Vaidya CJ, Zhao M, Desmond JE, Gabrieli JD. Evidence for cortical encoding specificity in episodic memory: memory-induced re-activation of picture processing areas. Neuropsychologia. 2002;40:2136–2143. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00053-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Kraemer DJM, Macrae CN, Green AE, Kelley WM. Musical imagery: sound of silence activates auditory cortex. Nature. 2005;434:158. doi: 10.1038/434158a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Goldberg RF, Perfetti CA, Schneider W. Perceptual knowledge retrieval activates sensory brain regions. J. Neurosci. 2006;26:4917–4921. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5389-05.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Teyler TJ, Rudy JW. The hippocampal indexing theory and episodic memory: updating the index. Hippocampus. 2007;17:1158–1169. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20350. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.O’Keefe JA. Place units in the hippocampus of the freely moving rat. Exp. Neurol. 1976;51:78–109. doi: 10.1016/0014-4886(76)90055-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Eichenbaum H. On the integration of space, time, and memory. Neuron. 2017;95:1007–1017. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.06.036. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Strange BA, Witter MP, Lein ES, Moser EI. Functional organization of the hippocampal longitudinal axis. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2014;15:655–669. doi: 10.1038/nrn3785. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Spellman T, Rigotti M, Ahmari SE, Fusi S, Gogos JA, Gordon JA. Hippocampal-prefrontal input supports spatial encoding in working memory. Nature. 2015;522:309–314. doi: 10.1038/nature14445. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Averback P. Two new lesions in Alzheimer’s disease. Lancet. 1983;19:1203. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(83)91256-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Esiri MM, Wilcock GK. The olfactory bulbs in Alzheimer’s disease. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry. 1984;47:56–60. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.47.1.56. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.ter Laak HJ, Renkawek K, van Workum FP. The olfactory bulb in Alzheimer disease: a morphologic study of neuron loss, tangles, and senile plaques in relation to olfaction. Alzheimer Dis. Assoc. Disord. 1994;8:38–48. doi: 10.1097/00002093-199408010-00007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Price JL, Morris JC. Tangles and plaques in nondemented aging and “preclinical” Alzheimer’s disease. Ann. Neurol. 1999;45:358–368. doi: 10.1002/1531-8249(199903)45:3<358::AID-ANA12>3.0.CO;2-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Attems J, Jellinger KA. Olfactory tau pathology in Alzheimer disease and mild cognitive impairment. Clin. Neuropathol. 2006;25:265–271. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Price JL, Davis PB, Morris JC, White DL. The distribution of tangles, plaques and related immunohistochemical markers in healthy aging and Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol. Aging. 1991;12:295–312. doi: 10.1016/0197-4580(91)90006-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Kovács T, Cairns NJ, Lantos PL. Olfactory centres in Alzheimer’s disease: olfactory bulb is involved in early Braak’s stages. Clin. Neurosci. Neuropathol. 2001;12:285–288. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200102120-00021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Christen-Zaech S, et al. Early olfactory involvement in Alzheimer’s disease. Can. J. Neurol. 2003;30:20–25. doi: 10.1017/S0317167100002389. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Foveau B, Albrecht S, Bennett DA, Correa JA, LeBlanc AC. Increased Caspase-6 activity in the human anterior olfactory nuclei of the olfactory bulb is associated with cognitive impairment. Acta Neuropathol. Commun. 2016;4:127. doi: 10.1186/s40478-016-0400-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Doty RL, Reyes PF, Gregor T. Presence of both odor identification and detection deficits in Alzheimer’s disease. Brain Res. Bull. 1987;18:597–600. doi: 10.1016/0361-9230(87)90129-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Niccoli-Waller CA, Harvey J, Nordin S, Murphy C. Remote odor memory in Alzheimer’s disease: deficits as measured by familiarity. J. Adult Dev. 1999;6:131–136. doi: 10.1023/A:1021624925786. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Djordjevic J, Jones-Gotman M, De Sousa K, Chertkow H. Olfaction in patients with mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol. Aging. 2008;29:693–706. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2006.11.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Vasavada MM, Martinez B, Wang J, Eslinger PJ, Gill DJ, Sun X, Karunanayaka P, Yang QX. Central olfactory dysfunction in Alzheimer’s disease and mild cognitive impairment: a functional MRI study. J. Alzheimers Dis. 2017;59:359–368. doi: 10.3233/JAD-170310. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Stamps JJ, Bartoshuk LM, Heilman KM. A brief olfactory test for Alzheimer’s disease. J. Neurol. Sci. 2013;333:19–24. doi: 10.1016/j.jns.2013.06.033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Devanand DP, et al. Olfactory deficits predict cognitive decline and Alzheimer dementia in an urban community. Neurology. 2015;84:182–189. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000001132. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Albers DA, et al. Episodic memory of odors stratifies Alzheimer biomarkers in normal elderly. Ann. Neurol. 2016;80:846–857. doi: 10.1002/ana.24792. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Eacott MJ, Norman G. Integrated memory for object, place, and context in rats: a possible model of episodic-like memory? J. Neurosci. 2004;24:1948–1953. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2975-03.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Hunsaker MR, Fieldsted PM, Rosenberg JS, Kesner RP. Dissociating the roles of dorsal and ventral CA1 for the temporal processing of spatial locations, visual objects, and odors. Behav. Neurosci. 2008;122:643–650. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.122.3.643. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Mitchell JB, Laiacona J. The medial frontal cortex and temporal memory: tests using spontaneous exploratory behaviour in the rat. Behav. Brain. Res. 1998;97:107–113. doi: 10.1016/S0166-4328(98)00032-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
All relevant data are available from the corresponding authors upon reasonable request.