Abstract
Mental schemas provide a framework into which new information can easily be integrated. In a series of experiments, we examined how incongruence that stems from a prediction error modulates memory for multicomponent events that instantiated preexisting schemas as noted in a previous study. Each event consisted of four stimulus pairs with overlapping components, presented in four blocks (A–B, B–C, C–D, D–A). A–B pairs elicited contextual expectations (A: Farm, B: Tractor) that were either met by a congruent C component (C: Farmer) or violated by an incongruent one (C: Lawyer). The baseline condition included unrelated pairs, where the C component was neither congruent nor incongruent. In experiment 2, events were presented in successive trials instead of blocks, and eye movements were recorded to analyze allocation of attention. Memory was tested through old–new item recognition followed by cued recall. Across experiments, recognition and recall performance for incongruent components was reduced compared to congruent components. Incongruent items were in some cases more accurately retrieved compared to unrelated ones, depending on task demands. Additionally, better recall was observed in the incongruent D–A pairs, compared to congruent and unrelated ones, because of reduced interference from C components. Eye-tracking revealed an increased number of fixations on C components in the incongruent and unrelated conditions. These results suggest that the integration of incongruent items into an episode is impaired, compared to congruent items, despite the contextual surprise and increased attention they elicited at encoding. However, there was a beneficial effect of prediction error on memory performance, compared to a baseline, depending on the task used.
Schemas and their role in supporting memory have been a topic of intensive research over several decades (Bartlett 1932). Schema effects can benefit memory in two ways; first, the mere existence of a schema could enhance learning of new information. Previous research suggests memory performance is improved when the items can be assimilated into an existing schema (Tse et al. 2007, 2011; McClelland 2013). Second, performance can be assessed across congruent and incongruent information. A common observation, referred to as the congruency effect, is that schema-congruent information is better recognized and recalled than incongruent information (Craik and Tulving 1975; Staresina et al. 2009; Atienza et al. 2010; van Kesteren et al. 2010). However, van Kesteren et al. (2012) proposed a model (SLIMM) showing schemas can enhance memory for both congruent and incongruent information, via different mechanisms. The key modulator in this model is the prediction error elicited by the incongruent item (Greve et al. 2017). A prominent aspect that remains elusive is to what extent memory is enhanced by the prediction error (e.g., whether memory performance is equivalently good for congruent and incongruent items). Here, we test behavioral predictions of this model by combining contextual surprise with schema-incongruent items, embedded in multicomponent events (Horner and Burgess 2013; Horner et al. 2015).
SLIMM posits that incongruence could lead to superior memory when the contextual schema provides a strong constraint (van Kesteren et al. 2012). In such cases, the incongruent item elicits a prediction error, leading to better memory through the creation of new representations. Importantly, the degree to which incongruence benefits memory, compared to a schema-less, baseline level, remains unclear. The model also accounts for congruency effects, as encountering congruent information results in medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) activation of the schema, which in turn facilitates encoding in the medial temporal lobe (MTL; for similar ideas see Brod et al. 2013; Preston and Eichenbaum 2013). A key aspect of the SLIMM model is that the fate of incongruent information in memory is determined by the context it is embedded in. Most of the studies examining schema effects build upon preexperimental knowledge (van Kesteren et al. 2010; Bayen and Kuhlmann 2011; Bein et al. 2014) and the relationship between a pair of items, or their level of semantic relatedness (Staresina et al. 2009; van Kesteren et al. 2013; Bein et al. 2014). For example, purple-banana would be an incongruent pair, whereas yellow-banana constitutes a congruent one. However, this design does not necessarily allow for predictions to develop at encoding. Subsequently, during retrieval, there is no episodic contextual setting that would reinstate the schema (van Kesteren et al. 2012).
Incongruence that stems from a prediction error should be accompanied by better retention, supported by MTL engagement (van Kesteren et al. 2012; Greve et al. 2017). Importantly, for a prediction to be wrong, it must first be elicited (Kumaran and Maguire 2007). We therefore utilized interleaved learning of events comprising pairs of components, to allow for predictions to be developed and violated. Previous research suggests such multicomponent events are well suited for this purpose, as they promote binding of components into contextual events (Horner and Burgess 2013; Horner et al. 2015). Therefore, the first pair of components presented can be used to implicitly set the schema-related predictions (Schlichting and Preston 2015). Additionally, this paradigm makes it possible to test how incongruent components affect the rest of the contextual event. Specifically, whether incongruent components can be integrated into an otherwise congruent event (Bein et al. 2015), relying on interactions between mPFC and MTL (Schlichting and Preston 2015, 2017). Thus, we can measure not only the independent recollection of incongruent components, but also their indirect effect on adjacent congruent components. According to SLIMM (van Kesteren et al. 2012), if incongruent representations are reactivated at retrieval (similarly to congruent ones), we would expect equivalent levels of interference from incongruent and congruent C components in adjacent A–B and D–A pairs.
In the studies reported here, we used events consisting of four pairs. Each pair shared a common component (A–B, B–C, C–D, D–A), to promote retrieval of the previous pair during study (Caplan et al. 2014), as well as to allow for components to be integrated into an event (Burton et al. 2017; Schlichting and Preston 2017). We extend previous literature on schema effects by actively eliciting contextual predictions that stem from existing schemas, as opposed to relying solely on the level of relatedness of two items. On this basis, we hypothezised that incongruence will modulate memory both of the incongruent components and the event they are incorporated in. While SLIMM predicts memory enhancement of incongruent components (van Kesteren et al. 2012), it remains unclear to what extent. By including an unrelated “baseline” condition, where there is no schema, we will test not only whether the presence of a prediction error supports schema-related memory, but also to what extent. For example, congruent and incongruent items could be equally better than unrelated ones, show a graded response (congruent > incongruent > unrelated), or be equivalent to unrelated events.
To assess differences in memory performance between congruent and incongruent items, compared to an unrelated baseline, we used contextual events (see Experiment 1a in the Supplemental Materials for comparison between congruent-incongruent alone). The first pair in the event is location-object, as it easily instantiates the schema (or lack thereof) for the following items (farm-tractor immediately brings to mind other farm-related items, whereas golf course-torch does not intrinsically belong to a specific context or schema, see Bar and Aminoff 2003). Both congruent and incongruent events had three components that were schema-congruent (A,B,D; see Fig. 1 for examples). In the schema-based conditions, A-B pairs elicited contextual expectations that were either met by a congruent C component or violated by an incongruent one. In unrelated events, components did not share any common contextual information. Memory was tested in two steps, first a yes/no item recognition task for each component, followed by a cued recall for the adjacent component (e.g., B–? or ?–D, see Fig. 1). Cued recall was tested only for initially recognized components. Multiple retrieval trials were used to test effects of task demand (recognition versus recall, forward versus backward cued recall).
Results and discussion
Experiment 1
Item recognition
A three (congruence: congruent, incongruent, unrelated) by four (components: A, B, C, D) repeated measures ANOVA (Fig. 2C) was conducted for corrected recognition responses (hits—false alarms). Greenhouse–Geisser correction for the sphericity assumption of ANOVAs are reported where appropriate, and all post-hoc analyses reported are Bonferroni corrected. When multiple t-tests were computed, a threshold P value is reported for all of them (e.g., all P’s < 0.05). Despite near-ceiling recognition performance (overall average of 93% accuracy), a main effect of congruence was found F(2,60) = 3.87 P = 0.026, , with follow-up paired t-tests indicating congruent components were more easily recognized than incongruent ones t(30) = 2.95, P = 0.006, Cohen's d = 0.53, and unrelated cues t(30) = 0.258, P = 0.015, Cohen's d = 0.464. No differences were observed between incongruent and unrelated components t(30) = 0.851, P = 0.4.
Cued recall
We first tested whether there were any effects of order on trials (AB–forward, BA–backward) from the same pair (A–B). The three (congruence) by four (pair) by two (order) repeated measures ANOVA yielded both a main effect of order F(1,30) = 11.2, P = 0.002, and a significant three-way interaction F(6,180) = 4.2, P = 0.008, . This suggests there were different patterns of performance in each condition between the forward and backward trials (see Fig. 2A,B). Therefore, we carried out two separate three (congruence) by four (pair) ANOVAs, one for each order (forward X–?, and backward ?–X). The forward ANOVA (Fig. 2A) revealed a significant interaction between congruence and pairs F(6,180) = 5.37, P < 0.001, . Similarly, the ANOVA for backward pairs (Fig. 2B) revealed an interaction effect F(6,180) = 4.01, P < 0.001, .
Planned comparisons between the congruence conditions for each trial were carried out to examine differences between conditions. Congruent BC and CD trials (forward order) were better recalled than incongruent and unrelated ones (all P’s ≤0.011). No significant differences were observed between incongruent and unrelated components in these two trials (all P’s ≥ 0.32). In the backward order trials, a benefit of incongruent over unrelated trials was found for DC t(30) = 2.774, P = 0.009, Cohen's d = 0.5, and a trend toward it was observed in CB trials t(30) = 1.75, P = 0.091, Cohen's d = 0.31. These results show the order effect was due to a graded pattern (congruent > incongruent > unrelated) in the backward, but not forward order. Finally, in DA and AD trials, incongruent components were better recalled than unrelated and congruent ones and incongruent AB were more accurately retrieved compared to congruent ones (all P’s ≤ 0.01).
Interference analysis
To further elucidate the benefit of incongruence on D–A and A–B pairs, we examined the erroneous answers for cued recall trials (Fig. 2D). We inspected both trials comprising the D–A pairing (AD and DA) together, to have a sufficient number of trials and participants included (due to order effects in previous analysis we also carried out this analysis separated by order, which showed similar results despite a lower number of trials and participants included, see Supplemental Materials). We examined how many of the errors were due to interference from C items, compared to a baseline (erroneous recall of B/D). One out of the 31 participants had missing values for one pair, therefore data from 30 participants were analyzed. A three (congruence: congruent, incongruent, and unrelated) by two (item: C and D/B) by two (pair: A–B and D–A) repeated measures ANOVA revealed interaction effects of congruence by item F(2,58) = 7.37, P = 0.001, , and item by pair F(1,29) = 10.36, P = 0.003, . The three-way interaction effect was not significant F(2,58) = 1.1, P = 0.34.
Post-hoc tests revealed more C errors in congruent D–A and A–B pairs compared to incongruent and unrelated ones (all P’s < 0.004). There were also more C errors in incongruent D–A and A–B pairs compared to unrelated ones (all P’s < 0.038). When comparing interference between C items and baseline B/D items, in D–A pairs we observed less interference from incongruent C compared to incongruent B components (t(29) = 2.14, P = 0.041, Cohen's d = 0.391). In A–B trials, on the other hand, interference to incongruent pairs is equivalent between C and D items (t(29) = 0.084, P = 0.934), but there is more interference from congruent C items compared to congruent D ones (t(29) = 2.43, P = 0.021, Cohen's d = 0.444).
The results described above show that memory performance for congruent components is superior to incongruent and unrelated ones, in all testing formats. Incongruent items are, in backward cued recall, more accurately retrieved than unrelated ones. The interference analysis showed better recall performance for congruence-matched pairs (A–B, D–A) of incongruent events is due to reduced interference from C components, suggesting they are less integrated into the event. Additionally, incongruent C items caused more interference than unrelated ones, showing a similar pattern of responses as in backward cued recall. Overall, these findings suggest that congruence benefits memory performance across the board, but memory for incongruent components was modulated by task demands. Differences in performance between incongruent and unrelated pairs, stemming from the prediction error associated with incongruence, were observed in backward cued recall and the interference analysis. In both cases, a graded response was observed (congruent > incongruent > unrelated). In the recognition and forward recall, on the other hand, memory for incongruent components was equivalent to unrelated ones.
Experiment 2
To address the integration account of reduced performance for incongruent components, we devised Experiment 2 to allow for easier integration of the pairs into a cohesive event. To do so, events were presented as trial-by-trial pairs, rather than across blocks. Trial-by-trial presentation differs in the contextual setting of learning. In Experiment 1, each pair was compared to other pairs of the same kind (i.e., B–C pairs from different events were always studied together). In Experiment 2, on the other hand, the “reference point” is the previous pair from the same event (B–C will follow A–B from the same event). This change would allow for a quicker buildup of predictions, as the event pairs would now be temporally closer than in the previous experiments. We added an associative inference task between A and C items, which were not shown together, to test differences in integration levels. To examine whether unexpected pairs are processed differently at encoding, we measured eye movements during this stage. We reasoned that changes in fixation patterns observed at encoding would indicate a different allocation of resources to components that are more difficult to encode and integrate into the event.
Eye-tracking results
We first subjected the number of fixations to a three (congruence) by four (pair) by 2 (area of interest, AOI) ANOVA (Fig. 3A), which showed a significant main effect of pair F(2.1,56.73) = 73.6, P < 0.001, , a congruence by pair interaction F(6,162) = 5.63, P < 0.001, and a significant AOI by pair interaction F(2.23,60.32) = 25.7, P < 0.001, . Post-hoc comparisons showed the least amount of fixations on the B item of unrelated A–B pairs, when compared to the congruent and incongruent conditions (all P’s ≤ 0.008). Additionally, unrelated B–C pairs were associated with fewer fixations on B, when compared to congruent and incongruent pairs (all P’s < 0.001). For the C components in the B–C pairs, congruent components were associated with the lowest number of fixations compared to incongruent and unrelated ones (all P’s ≤ 0.008). A similar analysis for fixation durations (Fig. 3B) revealed significant main effects of pair F(1.53,42.91) = 76.33, P < 0.001, and AOI F(1,28) = 13.21, P = 0.001, , as well as a pair by congruence interaction F(6,168) = 5.9, P < 0.001, , and a pair by AOI interaction F(3.84) = 19.98, P < 0.001, . Planned comparisons indicated that in unrelated B–C pairs, fixations on the B components were shortest and those on C components were longest, compared to congruent and incongruent pairs (all P’s ≤ 0.003).
Behavioral results
Item recognition
A three (congruence: congruent, incongruent, unrelated) by four (component: A, B, C, D) repeated measures ANOVA was conducted for corrected recognition responses. Again, a main effect of congruence was found F(2,54) = 14.83 P < 0.001, , with follow-up paired t-tests indicating congruent components were more easily recognized than incongruent and unrelated ones (all P’s ≤ 0.002). No differences were observed between incongruent and unrelated components (P > 0.2).
Cued recall
We first tested whether there were any effects of order. Similar to Experiment 1, the three (congruence) by four (pair) by two (order) repeated measures ANOVA yielded a significant three-way interaction F(6,162) = 4.39, P < 0.001, . This suggests there were different patterns of performance in each condition between the forward and backward orders. Therefore, we performed two separate three (congruence) by four (pair) ANOVAs, one for each presentation order (forward X–?, and backward ?–X). The forward ANOVA (Fig. 4A) revealed a significant congruence by pair interaction F(4.14,112) = 5.67, P < 0.001, . The ANOVA for backward trials (Fig. 4B) was akin to that for the forward trials, with an interaction between congruence and pair F(6,162) = 9.37, P < 0.001, .
Planned comparisons between the congruence conditions for each trial were then carried out. For AB and DA trials a similar pattern emerged, whereby there were no significant differences between congruent trials and any of the other conditions (all P’s ≥ 0.02), but a significant benefit of incongruent trials over unrelated ones was observed (all P’s ≤ 0.002). Unrelated BA trials were associated with reduced recall performance compared to congruent and incongruent ones (all P’s ≤ 0.006). For BC, CB, CD, and DC trials there were again similar findings, with congruent trials associated with better recall compared to incongruent and unrelated trials (all P’s ≤ 0.013), but no difference was observed between incongruent and unrelated trials (all P’s ≥ 0.668). These results suggest that performance for trials that are part of incongruent events tracks their level of relatedness.
Associative inference retrieval task (A-C)
A three (congruence) by two (component: A versus C) ANOVA yielded a significant main effect of congruence F(1.57,40.97) = 105.46, P < 0.001, (Fig. 4C). The main effect of component was not significant F(1,26) = 0.016, P = 0.901, as was the interaction effect F(1.43,37.23) = 0.58, P = 0.508. Post-hoc tests revealed that congruent components were associated with better performance compared to incongruent and unrelated ones, and a benefit of incongruent components over unrelated ones (all P’s < 0.001).
Interference analysis
A three (congruence: congruent, incongruent and unrelated) by two (component: C and D/B) by two (pair: A–B and D–A) repeated measures ANOVA (Fig. 4D) revealed a significant interaction between congruence and item F(2,54) = 34.8, P < 0.001, ). The other two-way interactions, as well the three-way interaction were not significant (all P’s > 0.248). Planned comparisons showed increased interference from congruent C components in A–B and D–A pairs, compared to incongruent and unrelated components (all P’s < 0.001). There were no significant differences between incongruent and unrelated C components (all P’s > 0.528). Additionally, there was more interference from incongruent B/D (baseline) components, compared to incongruent C components, in D–A pairs (t(27) = 4.45, P < 0.001, Cohen's d = 0.841) and A–B pairs, respectively (t(27) = 3.44, P = 0.002, Cohen's d = 0.778).
General discussion
In a series of experiments, we found that unexpected incongruent components were less likely to be recognized and recalled, compared to congruent ones. This pattern was observed both when events were presented across blocks, to allow for predictions to develop gradually, and when events were constructed trial-by-trial, to promote easier integration. Examination of fixation patterns during encoding of such events revealed increased fixations on the first unexpected incongruent component, compared to their congruent counterparts, suggesting they were more difficult to encode. Our results suggest the presence of a prediction error in incongruent pairs did not enhance memory to the level observed in congruent events. However, depending on task demands, it did benefit memory compared to unrelated components (showing a graded pattern of responses). Finally, a beneficial effect of incongruence was observed in A–B and D–A pairs, which were only indirectly related to the incongruent C component.
The advantageous role of schema congruence on memory performance, compared to both incongruent and unrelated items, is in line with the congruency effect. Previous findings, and predictions from SLIMM, suggest congruent items benefit from the existing strong representation of the schema (Craik and Tulving 1975; Staresina et al. 2009; van Kesteren et al. 2012). Previous experiences of similar associations (e.g., farm-tractor) are reactivated by the mPFC (Brod et al. 2013; Preston and Eichenbaum 2013; Schlichting and Preston 2015), and proposed to be more readily available during retrieval (Moscovitch and Craik 1976; Hemmer and Steyvers 2009; Steyvers and Hemmer 2012). This notion is in line with our finding that congruent A–B and D–A pairs are more prone to interference from C components, showing reduced recall accuracy. Incongruent components, and unrelated ones even more so, are more difficult to integrate into the event (Craik and Tulving 1975; Bein et al. 2015), resulting in less associative competition (Caplan et al. 2014), or a more constrained search space (Anderson 1981) for retrieving A–B and D–A.
Further evidence for the reduced integration of incongruent C components can be found in forward-order trials in Experiment 1, as well as in all retrieval trials in Experiment 2. Incongruent components show comparable results, in memory performance and eye-fixation patterns, to those of their unrelated counterparts. The eye-tracking results indicate increased effort invested in encoding incongruent items, but this effort does not come into fruition later in retrieval performance. The encoding part of this effect can be attributed to MTL-driven function, showing the prediction error associated with an incongruent component promotes more elaborate encoding. During retrieval, congruent information seems to be dominating, especially in trials where a congruent item (B or D) cues retrieval of an incongruent item. In this case, mPFC involvement would potentially direct retrieval toward congruent representations (van Kesteren et al. 2012; Preston and Eichenbaum 2013). Although previous studies have shown similar encoding and retrieval effects independently (van Kesteren et al. 2010, 2013), future neuroimaging studies could examine the cooccurrence of such effects. Additionally, due to the use of bidirectional cued-recall tests, we could not directly correlate fixations at encoding with later retrieval performance, but this would be an interesting effect to examine.
Incongruent components also served as worse cues for their associates (Schulman 1974), showing impaired recall performance (equivalent to unrelated pairs). This finding is more difficult to interpret in light of SLIMM's predictions (van Kesteren et al. 2012), as successful retrieval in this case requires reactivation of a congruent item. We argue this finding points to difficulty in binding the incongruent component into the event (Craik and Tulving 1975; Bein et al. 2015; Packard et al. 2017), as also indicated by increased fixations. Given that schemas facilitate gist extraction and abstraction of commonalities (Gilboa and Marlatte 2017), the presence of an incongruent component in the event could interrupt this process. This would therefore result in reduced schema instantiation to support binding of the incongruent C component to its congruent pairwise associates, explaining impaired performance on C–B and C–D pairs.
Interestingly, in the backward order retrieval trials and interference analysis (Experiment 1), as well as in the A–C inference task (Experiment 2), a graded pattern of responses was observed (congruent > incongruent > unrelated). To our knowledge, this is the first study to show such pattern. Although van Kesteren et al. (2012) postulated memory for both congruent and incongruent items would be enhanced, the extent to which this effect varies between conditions was unclear. Previous findings pertaining to congruency effects have not used an unrelated baseline condition, making it difficult to account for such differences. Here, we find that while components from congruent events were unequivocally better recalled, incongruent components showed better performance compared to unrelated ones. This pattern of results suggests a prediction error can enhance memory performance (compared to unrelated items), though not to the same extent as congruence does. Interestingly, this result was observed only under specific circumstances, suggesting this effect could be susceptible to task demands (Ghosh and Gilboa 2014).
In Experiment 1, events were created across blocks, thus online comparisons were between B–C pairs from different events. Conversely, in Experiment 2 comparisons were made with A–B pairs from the same event. This difference in temporal context during encoding, could have biased processing of incongruent pairs in Experiment 1 compared to Experiment 2 (Howard and Kahana 2002). Although overall memory performance in Experiment 1 was symmetric between forward and backward trials, graded responses were observed only in backward trials. Asymmetry in memory recall has been suggested to depend on the relationship between paired associates (Li and Lewandowsky 1995; Greene and Tussing 2001; Yang et al. 2013) and to engage the anterior hippocampus (Giovanello et al. 2009). Forward recall is believed to be schema-driven (Geiselman and Callot 1990) and more susceptible to disruptions during formation of associations at encoding (Li and Lewandowsky 1995). In line with this, we find superior memory for congruent components, with no differences between incongruent and unrelated pairs in the forward order. Backward recall, on the other hand, is suggested to be more data-driven (Geiselman and Callot 1990) and thus more susceptible to contextual details at encoding. We therefore suggest backward retrieval in our task promoted the beneficial effect of prediction error, mediated by hippocampal engagement (van Kesteren et al. 2012).
Graded responses were also obtained in the associative inference task in Experiment 2. Successful performance on such tasks is often used as a marker for schematic organization of representations in memory, as it supports novel integration of indirectly related items (Tse et al. 2007; Kumaran et al. 2009; Zeithamova et al. 2012; Preston and Eichenbaum 2013). In this task, incongruent and unrelated pairs were matched on relatedness and differed only on the buildup of expectations from the event's schema. Thus, the presence of a prediction error here could have mediated enhanced inference in this task. Alternatively, the mere existence of a schema in the incongruent condition, as opposed to the unrelated one, could have supported this inference (Tse et al. 2011; Zeithamova et al. 2012; Kumaran 2013; McClelland 2013; Preston and Eichenbaum 2013). Future research on schema effects would benefit from further exploring these effects, specifically in relation to how task demands can modulate memory for incongruent information.
Our main aim was to test behavioral predictions outlined by SLIMM (van Kesteren et al. 2012). We thus utilized a paradigm that allows expectations to gradually develop by using interleaved learning of paired associates. The findings reported above provide some support to the notion prediction errors can enhance memory for incongruent items. An alternative interpretation is that the amount of prediction error associated with incongruent components in our studies was not large enough to result in conclusively improved performance. Critically, our task was designed to implicitly set participants’ predictions. This was done in order to capture the inherent aspect of predictions as they arise in daily life, and to avoid any artificial allocation of attention toward this manipulation. As a result of this manipulation, we could not quantify the amount of prediction error elicited and violated by incongruent components, but only indirectly assert contextual predictions were elicited by the stimuli used (Bar and Aminoff 2003).
In conclusion, our results provide further evidence for the notion that schemas aid memory by providing a structured representation into which congruent information can easily fit. The findings reported here also shed light on the extent to which prediction errors in incongruent items support its presence in memory. Although it requires more effortful encoding, retrieval success of incongruent items is always reduced compared to congruent components. The extent to which incongruent items are better remembered compared to unrelated components, on the other hand, is modulated by task demands. Future research looking into schema-mediated memory may build on the approach and findings highlighted above to better understand factors contributing to these effects.
Materials and Methods
Experiment 1
Participants
Thirty-five participants (12 males) gave informed consent to take part in the experiment. Four participants whose recognition performance was either above or below three times the IQR were excluded from any further analysis. Thus, data from 31 participants between the ages 18–27 (M = 19.8, SD = 2.91) were analyzed.
Materials
The experiment was controlled using E-Prime 1 (Psychology Software Tools). Stimuli were 30 four-components events (10 congruent, 10 incongruent, 10 unrelated events). Each event contained a location (component A, e.g., farm), two objects (components B and D, haystack and a tractor) and a person's profession (component C, farmer). Items were presented as images with labels above them (see Fig. 1 for examples). Congruent and incongruent events were constructed to elicit strong contextual predictions, meaning that their components are most likely to appear in the given context, as established by previous work (Bar and Aminoff 2003). Ten of the events were assigned to be in the incongruent condition, such that the person (C component) was unexpected in the context (a lawyer in the context of a farm with a haystack and a tractor). Another 10 were congruent, meaning the person was expected given the context (a farmer in a farm). The final 10 events included objects with low contextual value (Bar and Aminoff 2003), meaning they can be found in in a variety of contexts. The allocation of events to conditions was counterbalanced across participants. Images were obtained from freely available online resources labeled with a Creative Commons License.
Procedure
The experiment consisted of three phases: encoding, distraction and retrieval (see Fig. 1). The encoding phase was interleaved and took place over four blocks, one block for each pairwise association (A–B, B–C, C–D, D–A). Critically, A–B and D–A pairs are not directly associated with the C component (which defines whether an event is assigned to a congruent or incongruent condition). These pairs are congruent in both congruent and incongruent conditions, thus providing an opportunity for testing the effect of incongruence on the rest of the event. Each block consisted of 30 randomized-order trials, resulting in a total of 120 trials in the encoding phase. Each trial began with a one-second fixation cross, followed by presentation of one pair of labeled images for 3 sec. Participants were instructed to imagine the components interacting together, as vividly as they could, while being aware of their respective location on the screen (right and left).
To prevent participants from actively rehearsing the encoded information, a distractor task involving solving arithmetic problems was used for 5 min. Participants were instructed to be as accurate as possible and were informed that if they failed to reach a certain performance threshold their data would be excluded from further analysis. Following this task, the retrieval phase began, where items were presented in a pseudorandomized order, based on 10 premade lists. Two retrieval tasks were used, a recognition task for each component, followed by a cued-recall task only for the recognized components.
Participants were first presented with the yes/no recognition task. They had a maximum of 10 sec to complete this task. If they responded “yes,” a second cued-recall task took place immediately. For this task, the recognized component was coded as “cue” and the recalled component as “target.” Participants were asked to recall which item appeared with the previously recognized item (a source recall task, retrieve the item in the location indicated on the screen). Each pairwise association from each event was tested in both directions in a randomized order (for example, forward A–? and backward ?–B). A cued-recall answer was scored as correct if it was identical to the item presented at encoding, if it was a specific case of the same category (for example, “car mechanic” instead of “mechanic”) or semantically similar (“gymnast” instead of “acrobat”). If the participant failed to give an answer within 20 sec of the cue being presented, the trial was scored as incorrect. In addition to the 120 items that were encoded, 30 additional items, making up six events, were used as foils.
Experiment 2
Experiment 2 was identical to Experiment 1, with the following modifications:
Participants
Thirty-five participants (9 males) gave informed consent to take part in the experiment. Data from seven participants were removed from any further analysis due to technical failure of the eye-tracker (one participant), poor eye-tracking data (three participants; inclusion of these participants in the behavioral analysis did not change the results) and poor memory performance, above or below three times the IQR (three participants). Thus, data from 28 participants between the ages 18–28 (M = 20.8, SD = 2.78) were analyzed.
Materials and apparatus
The materials were identical to those used in Experiment 1. To examine fixation patterns during encoding, eye movements were recorded during encoding, using an ASL infrared eye tracker (Eye-Trac 6000, Applied Science Laboratories) at a sampling rate of 60 Hz. The desktop-mounted camera was placed under the presentation screen, 70 cm away from the participant. A chin-rest was used to minimize participants’ movement.
Procedure
Before the experiment started, eye calibration was performed using a nine-point matrix. During the encoding phase in Experiment 2, events were presented sequentially, trial-by-trial, rather than across blocks as was done in Experiment 1. Each of the four pairs comprising an event was presented for three seconds, with a 1 sec fixation cross between them. Following the last pair of the event, a fixation cross was displayed for 2 sec, until a new event had started. To examine whether reduced performance for the incongruent C components was due to a difficulty in integrating them as part of the events, an additional retrieval inference task was used. Following the cued-recall task, participants were presented with a location or a person (A or C components) for a maximum of 10 sec and were asked to recall its counterpart person or location from the same event, respectively.
Eye-tracking analysis
Eyeneal software (Applied Science Laboratories) was used to convert the raw gaze coordinates to fixation points. The start of a fixation point was defined as six sequential gaze points with a standard deviation smaller than 0.5 visual degrees. The end of a fixation was marked when three consecutive gaze points were at least one visual degree away from the initial fixation location. The fixation points reported below are the average point of the start and end fixation locations. Two areas of interest (AOI) were defined in the Fixplot software (Applied Science Laboratory), one for each of the images displayed on the screen. Statistical analyses were conducted on the number of fixations and fixation duration for each AOI.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgments
The authors thank L. Bailey for help with data collection for Experiment 2. D.F. is supported by a PDS award from the University of Manchester.
Author contributions: D.F., D.M., B.W., and D.T. designed the experiment, D.F collected and analysed data, D.F., D.M., B.W. and D.T. wrote the manuscript.
Competing interest statement: The author(s) declare no competing financial interests.
Footnotes
[Supplemental material is available for this article.]
Article is online at http://www.learnmem.org/cgi/doi/10.1101/lm.047738.118.
References
- Anderson JR. 1981. Effects of prior knowledge on memory for new information. Mem Cogn 9: 237–246. [Google Scholar]
- Atienza M, Crespo-Garcia M, Cantero JL. 2010. Semantic congruence enhances memory of episodic associations: role of theta oscillations. J Cogn Neurosci 23: 75–90. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bar M, Aminoff E. 2003. Cortical analysis of visual context. Neuron 38: 347–358. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bartlett FC. 1932. Remembering: an experimental and social study. Cambridge University, Cambridge. [Google Scholar]
- Bayen UJ, Kuhlmann BG. 2011. Influences of source-item contingency and schematic knowledge on source monitoring: tests of the probability-matching account. J Mem Lang 64: 1–17. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bein O, Reggev N, Maril A. 2014. Prior knowledge influences on hippocampus and medial prefrontal cortex interactions in subsequent memory. Neuropsychologia 64: 320–330. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bein O, Livneh N, Reggev N, Gilead M, Goshen-Gottstein Y, Maril A. 2015. Delineating the effect of semantic congruency on episodic memory: the role of integration and relatedness. PLoS One 10: e0115624. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brod G, Werkle-Bergner M, Shing YL. 2013. The influence of prior knowledge on memory: a developmental cognitive neuroscience perspective. Front Behav Neurosci 7: 1–13. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burton RL, Lek I, Caplan JB. 2017. Associative independence revisited: competition between conflicting associations can be resolved or even reversed in one trial. Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) 70: 832–857. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Caplan JB, Rehani M, Andrews JC. 2014. Associations compete directly in memory. Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) 67: 955–978. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Craik FIM, Tulving E. 1975. Depth of processing and the retention of words in episodic memory. J Exp Psychol Gen 104: 268–294. [Google Scholar]
- Geiselman RE, Callot R. 1990. Reverse versus forward recall of script-based texts. Appl Cogn Psychol 4: 141–144. [Google Scholar]
- Ghosh VE, Gilboa A. 2014. What is a memory schema? A historical perspective on current neuroscience literature. Neuropsychologia 53: 104–114. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gilboa A, Marlatte H. 2017. Neurobiology of schemas and schema-mediated memory. Trends Cogn Sci 21: 618–631. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Giovanello KS, Schnyer D, Verfaellie M. 2009. Distinct hippocampal regions make unique contributions to relational memory. Hippocampus 19: 111–117. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Greene RL, Tussing AA. 2001. Similarity and associative recognition. J Mem Lang 45: 573–584. [Google Scholar]
- Greve A, Cooper E, Kaula A, Anderson MC, Henson RN. 2017. Does prediction error drive one-shot declarative learning? J Mem Lang 94: 149–165. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hemmer P, Steyvers M. 2009. Integrating episodic and semantic information in memory for natural scenes. Proceedings of the 31st Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, 1557–1562. Retrieved from http://csjarchive.cogsci.rpi.edu/proceedings/2009/papers/337/paper337.pdf. [Google Scholar]
- Horner AJ, Burgess N. 2013. The associative structure of memory for multi-element events. J Exp Psychol Gen 142: 1370–1383. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Horner AJ, Bisby JA, Bush D, Lin W-J, Burgess N. 2015. Evidence for holistic episodic recollection via hippocampal pattern completion. Nat Commun 6: 7462. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Howard MW, Kahana MJ. 2002. When does semantic similarity help episodic retrieval? J Mem Lang 46: 85–98. [Google Scholar]
- Kumaran D. 2013. Schema-driven facilitation of new hierarchy learning in the transitive inference paradigm. Learn Mem 20: 388–394. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kumaran D, Maguire EA. 2007. Which computational mechanisms operate in the hippocampus during novelty detection? Hippocampus 17: 735–748. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kumaran D, Summerfield JJ, Hassabis D, Maguire EA. 2009. Tracking the emergence of conceptual knowledge during human decision making. Neuron 63: 889–901. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li S-C, Lewandowsky S. 1995. Forward and backward recall: different retrieval processes. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 21: 837–847. [Google Scholar]
- McClelland JL. 2013. Incorporating rapid neocortical learning of new schema-consistent information into complementary learning systems theory. J Exp Psychol Gen 142: 1190–1210. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moscovitch M, Craik FIM. 1976. Depth of processing, retrieval cuesm and uniqueness of encoding as factors in recall. J Verbal Learn Verbal Behav 15: 447–458. [Google Scholar]
- Packard PA, Rodríguez-Fornells A, Bunzeck N, Nicolás B, de Diego-Balaguer R, Fuentemilla L. 2017. Semantic congruence accelerates the onset of the neural signals of successful memory encoding. J Neurosci 37: 291–301. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Preston AR, Eichenbaum H. 2013. Interplay of hippocampus and prefrontal cortex in memory. Curr Biol 23: R764–R773. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schlichting ML, Preston AR. 2015. Memory integration: neural mechanisms and implications for behavior. Curr Opin Behav Sci 1: 1–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schlichting ML, Preston AR. 2017. The hippocampus and memory integration: building knowledge to navigate future decisions. In The hippocampus from cells to systems: structure, connectivity, and functional contributions to memory and flexible cognition (ed. Hannula DE, Duff MC), pp. 405–437. Springer International Publishing, Cham. [Google Scholar]
- Schulman AI. 1974. Memory for words recently classified. Mem Cogn 2: 47–52. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Staresina BP, Gray JC, Davachi L. 2009. Event congruency enhances episodic memory encoding through semantic elaboration and relational binding. Cereb Cortex 19: 1198–1207. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Steyvers M, Hemmer P. 2012. Reconstruction from memory in naturalistic environments. In The psychology of learning and motivation (ed. Ross BH), Vol. 56, pp. 125–144. http://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-394393- 4.00004-2. [Google Scholar]
- Tse D, Langston RF, Kakeyama M, Bethus I, Spooner PA, Wood ER, Witter MP, Morris RG. 2007. Schemas and memory consolidation. Science 316: 76–82. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tse D, Takeuchi T, Kakeyama M, Kajii Y, Okuno H, Tohyama C, Bito H, Morris RG. 2011. Schema-dependent gene activation and memory encoding in neocortex. Science 333: 891–895. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van Kesteren MTR, Rijpkema M, Ruiter DJ, Fernandez G. 2010. Retrieval of associative information congruent with prior knowledge is related to increased medial prefrontal activity and connectivity. J Neurosci 30: 15888–15894. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van Kesteren MTR, Ruiter DJ, Fernández G, Henson RN. 2012. How schema and novelty augment memory formation. Trends Neurosci 35: 211–219. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van Kesteren MTR, Beul SF, Takashima A, Henson RN, Ruiter DJ, Fernández G. 2013. Differential roles for medial prefrontal and medial temporal cortices in schema-dependent encoding: from congruent to incongruent. Neuropsychologia 51: 2352–2359. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yang J, Zhao P, Zhu Z, Mecklinger A, Fang Z, Li H. 2013. Memory asymmetry of forward and backward associations in recognition tasks. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 39: 253–269. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zeithamova D, Dominick AL, Preston AR. 2012. Hippocampal and ventral medial prefrontal activation during retrieval-mediated learning supports novel inference. Neuron 75: 168–179. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.