Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2019 Dec 1.
Published in final edited form as: J Pers. 2018 Feb 14;86(6):1003–1016. doi: 10.1111/jopy.12371

Alcohol use and personality change in middle and older adulthood: Findings from the Health and Retirement Study

Martina Luchetti 1,, Antonio Terracciano 2, Yannick Stephan 3, Angelina R Sutin 4
PMCID: PMC6054906  NIHMSID: NIHMS935972  PMID: 29357105

Abstract

Objective

Personality is known to predict alcohol consumption but how alcohol use is related to personality change is less clear, especially at older ages. The present study examined the effects of level of alcohol consumption and history of dependence on change in the five-factor model personality traits in a national cohort of Americans aged over 50.

Method

Over 10,000 adults who participated in 2006-08 waves of the Health and Retirement Study reported on personality and alcohol use and were followed over 4 years.

Results

Latent difference score models indicated decreases in extraversion to be attenuated for individuals categorized as light-to-moderate drinkers at baseline, while decreases in conscientiousness were accentuated by having experienced alcohol dependence symptoms. Moreover, personality difference scores correlated with changes in the amount of alcohol consumed at follow-up.

Conclusions

The findings suggest that patterns of alcohol consumption are associated with changes in personality across the second half of the lifespan.

Keywords: alcohol consumption, alcohol use disorders, middle and older adulthood, five-factor model, personality change


The five-factor model of personality operationalizes traits as relatively stable patterns of thoughts, feelings and behaviors that distinguish individuals from one another (McCrae & Costa, 1999). Although traits are relatively stable, there are also predictable changes in personality over the lifespan (Terracciano, McCrea, Brant, & Costa, 2005), even into old age (see Roberts & Mroczek, 2008, Roberts, Walton, & Viechtbauer, 2006, Wrzus & Roberts, 2017, for reviews). For instance, conscientiousness, the tendency to be self-controlled and disciplined, generally increases over time and peaks in middle life (e.g., Donnellan & Lucas, 2008; Roberts et al., 2006; Wortman, Lucas, & Donnellan, 2012) before declining in old age (e.g., Kandler, Kornadt, Hagemeyer, & Neyer, 2015; Mõttus, Johnson, & Deary, 2012). Neuroticism, the tendency to be prone to anxiety and depression, declines with age (e.g., Roberts et al., 2006; see also Terracciano et al., 2005) but may increase in older adulthood (e.g., Kandler et al., 2015).

In addition to normative developmental changes, there is appreciable individual variability in personality trajectories with aging (e.g., Specht, Egloff, & Schmukle, 2011). Increasing attention has been directed toward better understanding this variability because traits like (low) conscientiousness and (high) neuroticism are associated with a variety of negative outcomes, such as cognitive decline and dementia in older adulthood (e.g., Luchetti, Terracciano, Stephan, & Sutin, 2016; Terracciano et al., 2014), poor health (e.g., Löckenhoff, Terracciano, Ferrucci, & Costa, 2012; Turiano, Pitzer, Armour, Karlamangla, Ryff, & Mroczek, 2012), and mortality (e.g., Jokela et al., 2013). The association between personality and health, however, is unlikely to be unidirectional (see Jokela, Hakulinen, Singh-Manoux, & Kivimaki, 2014; Stephan, Sutin, Luchetti, & Terracciano, 2016; Sutin, Zonderman, Ferrucci, & Terracciano, 2013). For instance, the presence of chronic disease has been associated with accelerated declines in conscientiousness, extraversion, openness, and lower emotional stability over time (Jokela et al., 2014; Sutin et al., 2013). Individuals’ lifestyle also appear to contribute to personality functioning and its changes over time (e.g., Allen, Magee, Vella, & Laborde, 2017; Allen, Vella, & Laborde, 2015). In a study of two cohorts of Americans, for instance, Stephan, Sutin and Terracciano (2014) found physical inactivity to be associated with maladaptive personality changes, i.e. decreases in conscientiousness. Allen and colleagues (2015) also found physical inactivity and other unhealthy behaviors (i.e. cigarettes smoking, alcohol drinking and poor diet) to predict trait mean changes at the sample level and within individuals over time. In accordance with the corresponsive principle of personality development (Roberts & Wood, 2006), life experiences or health/body changes deepen the traits that led individuals to those experiences and changes. This assumes that personality traits that predispose to (un)healthy lifestyle may also change in response to the adoption to certain behaviors (e.g., exercise more, drink less alcohol, etc.). For example, the traits that motivate alcohol use/abuse, such as low conscientiousness or high neuroticism (Hakulinen et al., 2015; Shin, Hong, & Jeon, 2012) may be fostered by this behavior.

The present study focuses on a specific behavior, i.e. alcohol use, and its relation with personality change in adults 50 years and older. Alcohol consumption is common and dependence and abuse (i.e. Alcohol Use Disorders, AUDs; American Psychiatric Association, 2013) are increasing among older adults in the United States (DiBartolo & Jarosinski, 2017; Han, Moore, Sherman, Keyes, & Palamar, 2017). The effect of alcohol is not limited to physical health outcomes (Lim et al., 2012; Mostofsky et al., 2016) but extends to emotional and psychological functioning (Immonen, Valvanne, & Pitkala, 2011). Both beneficial and detrimental effects are observed, which depend on the level of alcohol consumed. For example, light-to-moderate consumption has been linked to better cognitive functioning, higher well-being and fewer depressive symptoms, compared to abstinence (e.g., Lang, Wallace, Huppert, & Melzer, 2007). In contrast, excessive consumption is associated with risks for physical health (e.g., cancer, diabetes, liver diseases, etc.) and neuropsychiatric disorders (Rehm, 2011). Although engagement in alcohol consumption is associated with change in personality in adolescence and young adulthood (e.g., Littlefield, Sher, & Wood, 2009; White et al., 2011), little is known about the association between alcohol and personality trajectories at older ages. To that end, this study examines how alcohol use, both current consumption and past dependence and/or abuse, is associated with change in personality traits, and how personality changes are related to change in alcohol drinking (i.e., number of drinks/week), using a large, longitudinal sample of middle-aged and older adults.

Alcohol use, personality and its change

Personality has long been associated with use of alcohol and other substances (Tarter, 1988; e.g., Terracciano, Löckenhoff, Crum, Bienvenu, & Costa, 2008; Turiano, Whiteman, Hampson, Roberts, & Mroczek, 2012). Of the five-factor personality traits, low conscientiousness, low agreeableness, and high neuroticism, have significant cross-sectional and longitudinal associations with different types of alcohol outcomes, including consumption, alcohol-related problems and/or disorders (Malouff et al., 2007 and Kotov, Gamez, Schmidt, & Watson, 2010 for meta-analyses; see also Hakulinen, et al., 2015; Martin & Sher, 1994; Ruiz, Pincus, & Dickinson, 2003; Turiano, Whiteman, et al., 2012). Individuals who lack of self-control and discipline tend to drink more alcohol (Bogg & Roberts, 2004; see e.g., Atherton, Robins, Rentfrow, & Lamb, 2014), and traits related to disinhibition, such as low conscientiousness, low agreeableness and impulsivity, are often implicated in alcohol use and abuse (Ruiz et al., 2003; see also Sher, Grekin, & Williams, 2005). High sociability and extraversion also predict excessive (binge) drinking (e.g., Cheng & Furnham, 2013; Hong & Paunonen, 2009) and increase in the amount of alcohol consumption over time (Hakulinen et al., 2015). For openness, the link with alcohol use is less clear, with studies reporting positive (e.g., Hakulinen et al., 2015), negative (e.g., Mezquita et al., 2015) and non-significant associations (e.g., Atherton et al., 2014; McAdams & Donnellan, 2009). Notably, findings vary based on the type of alcohol outcomes considered (see Malouff et al., 2007; e.g., Mezquita et al., 2015). Traits like neuroticism are more relevant in the context of clinical symptoms of alcohol dependence and abuse (Martin & Sher, 1994; Ruiz et al., 2003), as individuals with high neuroticism/emotional negativity may abuse of alcohol particularly while experiencing tension, depression, or loneliness (Immonen et al., 2011).

Although research has focused primarily on personality traits as predictors of alcohol use, changes in personality may occur as a consequence of alcohol consumption and dependence. Much of what is known comes from studies of adolescents and young adults (e.g., Blonigen et al., 2015; Littlefield, Sher, & Steinley, 2010; Samek et al., in press; White et al., 2011). For example, Littlefield and colleagues (2009) found that decreases in dependence symptoms and alcohol-related problems were associated with decreases in impulsivity and neuroticism from ages 18 to 35 and that this relation held even after controlling for acquisition of adult roles (e.g., marriage). Hicks, Durbin, Blonigen, Iacono, and McGue (2012) also found that resolution (vs. persistence) of AUDs was associated with “recovery” in terms of personality functioning (i.e. reduced negative emotionality) during emerging adulthood. Less is known about how alcohol consumption and dependence is associated with personality change in middle-aged and older adults. Chronic, excessive alcohol drinking is known to alters multiple physiological systems (Juster, Russell, Almeida, & Picard, 2016), to increase the risk of chronic conditions, including cardiovascular and respiratory diseases (Djoussé & Gaziano, 2008; Simet & Sisson, 2015). Higher disease burden and biological dysfunction have been linked to maladaptive personality change, such as lower conscientiousness, agreeableness, extraversion, openness, and higher neuroticism, in middle-aged and older adults (Jokela et al., 2014; Stephan et al., 2016). In addition, excessive alcohol consumption predict faster cognitive decline (Sabia et al., 2014), which is likely to foster personality change (Terracciano, Stephan, Luchetti, & Sutin, in press). Even after a full-remission, individuals with a history of AUDs are at greater risk to report metabolic abnormalities, cardiovascular and gastrointestinal medical conditions, especially in the elderly (Udo, Vásquez, & Shaw, 2015), which may foster maladaptive personality profiles, such as higher emotional instability (higher neuroticism), restriction of social activities and isolation, incapability of long term-planning and disorganization (lower conscientiousness). Using data from a longitudinal cohort (average age ~43), Allen and colleagues (2015) found that increases in alcohol consumption were associated with increases in neuroticism. This study assessed alcohol use on a scale from 1 to 8 (1 = ‘I no longer drink or have never drank alcohol’, 2 = ‘1 to 2 standard drinks’, … 8 = ‘13 or more standard drinks’). However, it is important to differentiate abstinence from light-to-moderate and heavy consumption, and consider symptoms of dependence and/or abuse. As far as we know, there is no evidence on the effect of normative levels of alcohol consumption on personality development in older adulthood. Older adults tend to drink more frequently but moderately compared to young adults (Britton, Ben-Shlomo, Benzeval, Kuh, & Bell, 2015). Based on the literature that link moderate consumption to a variety of positive outcomes (e.g., less depressive symptoms and better cognitive health; Lang et al., 2007), as opposite to abstinence or heavy alcohol consumption, it is possible to hypothesize moderate alcohol use to be associated with the maintenance (or retaining) of desirable personality traits (i.e. higher conscientiousness). While changes in personality have been linked to changes in alcohol drinking during adolescence and early adulthood (e.g., Littlefield et al., 2010; Riley, Rukavina, & Smith, 2016), the relation between changes in these two constructs has not been examined in older adults, even though of clinical interest.

The present study

The present study is innovative in examining how both levels of alcohol consumption and a history of AUDs are associated with personality development focusing on the latter part of adulthood. By adopting a structural equation model framework, this study estimates latent difference score models to assess (a) intra-individual change—i.e. systematic change (increase or decrease)—and intra-individual variability (fluctuation) within each personality dimension over time, and (b) examine individual differences in within-participants change. Based on the rational described above, we hypothesized light-to-moderate alcohol consumption at baseline to be associated with more adaptive personality profiles—in particular, with maintaining higher levels of conscientiousness over time, relative to abstinence or excessive alcohol drinking. In contrast, the presence (vs. absence) of past dependence and/or abuse was expected to increment within-person change in neuroticism, and decreases in the other traits. We additionally examined cross-lagged paths from alcohol consumption (number of drinks/week) to changes in personality and personality to change in the amount of alcohol consumed over the follow-up period, and tested whether latent difference scores of these two constructs were correlated in the current sample.

Methods

Participants

Data were drawn from the Health and Retirement Study (HRS), an ongoing longitudinal study of Americans aged 50 years and older and their spouses. The study is sponsored by the National Institute on Aging and the data are publicly available at http://hrsonline.isr.umich.edu/. HRS participants are interviewed every two years on a range of health and psychological measures. Personality traits were first assessed in 2006 as part of a psychosocial questionnaire. Half of the sample completed the questionnaire in 2006 and again in 2010; the other half completed it in 2008 and again in 2012. The current study used the combined 2006-08 samples as baseline and the 2010-12 data as follow-up (4-year interval).

A total of 14,302 participants had data on personality at baseline and 14,274 reported on alcohol consumption (N = 11,470 reported on symptoms of dependence and/or abuse). Of these, 10,094 respondents (>70%) had scores on at least one personality trait at the follow-up (see Table 1 for descriptive statistics and intercorrelations among variables). Those with follow-up data were younger (d = .45), more educated (d = .28) and wealthy (d = .11), more likely female and white, and scored higher on extraversion (d = .12), openness (d = .14), agreeableness (d = .07), and conscientiousness (d = .26), and lower on neuroticism (d = .12). Further, respondents were more likely to drink alcohol (d = .06) but less likely reported past dependence and/or alcohol abuse (17.4% vs. 19.6% for those without follow-up).

Table 1.

Descriptive statistics for the variables under study

Intercorrelations

Variables M SD N N1 N2 E1 E2 O1 O2 A1 A2 C1 C2
Personality
  N1 2.05 0.62 14,226 1
  N2 1.99 0.60 10,007 .63* 1
  E1 3.19 0.56 14,289 −.22* −.20* 1
  E2 3.16 0.57 10,102 −.19* −.25* .68* 1
  O1 2.93 0.56 14,249 −.20* −.21* .54* .39* 1
  O2 2.88 0.58 10,072 −.17* −.24* .38* .55* .66* 1
  A1 3.52 0.48 14,293 −.13* −.10* .57* .34* .42* .27* 1
  A2 3.51 0.50 10,018 −.11* −.14* .40* .57* .30 * .45* .62* 1
  C1 3.35 0.49 14,275 −.25* −.22* .41* .29* .47* .34* .45* .32* 1
  C2 3.36 0.50 10,092 −.19* −.27* .27* .43* .32* .49* .30 * .48* .62* 1
Socio-demographics
  Age 68.85 9.97 14,302 −.10* −.06* −.04* −.04* −.12* −.12* −.02* −.05* −.10* −.10*
  Sex (female) 58.8% 14,302 .08* .10* .09* .09* −.00 .01 .26* .25* .10* .10*
  Race 1 (black) 12.8% 14,301 −.07* −.08* .06* .04* .02 .01 .01 −.01 −.04* −.04*
  Race 2 (other) 4.3% 14,301 .03* .04* −.01 .00 −.01 .01 −.00 −.01 −.01 −.01
  Education 12.56 3.12 14,284 −.13* −.12* .07* .072* .28* .27* .06* .07* .18* .19*
  Income 64467.97 162253.38 14,302 −.03* −.03* .03* .04* .08* .08* .00 .01 .07* .06*
  Wealth 527928.99 1265057.77 14,302 −.04* −.05* .03* .06* .09* .11* −.02 .00 .09* .09*
Alcohol Variables
History of AUDs
  CAGE Score ≥1 18.0% 11,470 .09* .06* −.04* −.05* −.02 −.02 −.08* −.09* −.11* −.11*
  CAGE Interval 12.46 4.75 11,470 −.07* −.05* .03* .02 −.07* −.08* .01 .01 −.03* −.04*
Drinking Categories
  Non-drinkers 67.3% 14,274 .04* .04* −.04* −.05* −.11* −.12* .06* .04* −.05* −.05*
  Light-to-moderate drinkers 27.0% 14,274 −.05* −.05* .04* .05* .10* .11* −.05* −.03* .06* .06*
  Moderate-to-heavy drinkers 5.7% 14,274 .02 .01 .01 .01 .03* .04* −.03* −.02 .00 −.01
Drinks/week at baseline 2.50 6.23 14,274 −.03* −.03* .03* .04* .10* .11* −.07* −.05* .04* .04*
Drinks/week at follow-up 2.34 5.77 11,855 −.02* −.05* .03* .06* .12* .13* −.06* −.04* .06* .06*

Note. Means and standard deviations are reported if not otherwise specified. Of 14,302 respondents with personality data, 14,274 also reported on alcohol consumption at baseline (N = 11,470 reported on alcohol dependence/abuse; of these, 10,094 scored at least on one personality dimension at the follow-up. N1 / N2: Neuroticism at baseline / follow-up; E1 / E2: Extraversion at baseline / follow-up; O1 / O2: Openness at baseline / follow-up; A1 / A2: Agreeableness at baseline / follow-up; C1 / C2: Conscientiousness at baseline / follow-up. Drinking Categories: no alcohol consumption, light-to-moderate (≥1 to 7 drinks/week for women and ≥1 to 14 drinks/week for men), and moderate-to-heavy consumption (>7 drinks/week for women and >14 drinks/week for men). Number of drinks/week at baseline and follow-up were natural log transformed for the analyses. CAGE Score ≥1 identify past alcohol dependence and/or abuse.

*

p ≤ .01.

Missing data at follow-up were handled using full information maximum likelihood estimation. This approach provides more accurate parameter estimates than other common methods (i.e. listwise and pairwise deletion) and yields low convergence failures and near optimal Type I error rates in structural equation models (Enders & Bandalos, 2001).

Measures

Personality Traits

Participants completed the Midlife Development Inventory (MIDI) Personality Scales (Lachman & Weaver, 1997), a brief self-report measure of the five factor personality dimensions: neuroticism, extraversion, openness, agreeableness, and conscientiousness. Participants rated how much each of 26 adjectives described themselves on a scale ranging from 1 (not at all) to 4 (a lot). The adjectives were: nervous, calm, moody, and worrying for neuroticism; outgoing, lively, friendly, active, and talkative for extraversion; creative, curious, broad minded, sophisticated, imaginative, intelligent, and adventurous for openness; helpful, warm, caring, softhearted, and sympathetic for agreeableness; and organized, hardworking, thorough, responsible, and careless for conscientiousness. MIDI has good construct validity and its five-factor structure holds across the adult age span (see Mroczek & Kolarz, 1998, and Zimprich, Allemand, & Lachman, 2012). In the current sample, alphas reliabilities ranged from .66 (conscientiousness) to .79 (openness) at baseline and from .68 (conscientiousness) to .79 (openness and agreeableness) at follow-up. Measurement invariance has been tested in previous works using the HRS (see Stephan, Sutin, Bosselut, & Terracciano, 2017) and further documented in the online supplementary material for the current longitudinal sample.

Alcohol Variables

Alcohol Consumption

At the baseline and follow-up assessments, participants were asked “Do you ever drink any alcoholic beverages such as beer, wine, or liquor?” If they responded yes, they were then asked how many days per week they drank alcohol and on average how much they consumed on days they drank over the previous 3 months. Weekly alcohol consumption was calculated by multiplying the number of drinks a day by the number of days per week participants reported having consumed alcohol. Based on recommendations from the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism (NIAAA, 2017), respondents at baseline were categorized into “non-drinkers” (i.e. those who responded to not drink alcoholic beverage or had 0 drinks/week), “light-to-moderate drinkers” (≥1 to 7 drink/week for women and (≥1 to 14 drink/week for men), and “moderate-to-heavy drinkers” (>7 drink/week for women and >14 drink/week for men). The total number of drinks/week (continuous variable) was calculated for both 2006-08 baseline and 2010-12 follow-up, which paralleled the personality assessments.

History of AUDs

Consistent with previous research on AUDs in the HRS (Kuźma, Llewellyn, Langa, Wallace, & Lang, 2014), we used the 3-item version of the CAGE questionnaire (Hinkin et al., 2001) to identify a history of alcohol dependence and/or abuse: (1) Have people ever annoyed you by criticizing your drinking? (2) Have you ever felt bad or guilty about drinking? (3) Have you ever taken a drink first thing in the morning to steady your nerves or get rid of a hangover? Participants were considered to have a history of problematic drinking if they endorsed at least one of these items. We considered the first assessment available since the first wave of HRS in 1992 through the 2006–2008 baseline personality assessment. The time elapsed from CAGE assessment to the personality baseline was up to 16 years.

Covariates

Age, sex (coded as 0 for men and 1 for women), race/ethnicity (coded as 1 for black and 0 for white and other ethnic groups, and 1 others and 0 for black/white), level of education (in years), total household income and wealth were included as covariates. Age squared was also included to account for potential non-linear associations between age and personality. All covariates were from the baseline personality assessment.

Analytic Plan

We conducted our analyses using Mplus 8 (Muthén & Muthén, 1998–2017). Specifically, we constructed latent difference score models based on the latent factors of each personality trait at baseline and follow-up. This approach has several advantages, including the examination of intra-individual change and potential individual differences in within-individual change (see e.g., Allen et al., 2017, Paleari & Fincham, 2015, and Selig & Preacher, 2009), as well as the minimization of measurement error by fixing latent factor loadings and item intercepts to be invariant over time (Meredith, 1993).

Five models were tested, one for each personality trait. Figure 1 provides a conceptual representation of the model examining conscientiousness latent change over the 4-year follow-up. The model included fixed-unit value coefficients (=1), so that the second latent factor (i.e., C2) is defined by the sum of C1 and ΔC. As proposed by McArdle (2009), and Selig and Preacher (2009), the latent difference score was controlled for the intercept/initial level of conscientiousness (see Figure 1a). The ΔC mean captures the mean-level change; that is, whether individuals have increased (if the Δ mean is positive) or decreased (if negative) over time. The ΔC variance captures inter-individual differences around the mean change. To examine whether alcohol use/dependence influenced individual difference in within-individual change, we regressed each alcohol variables, respectively, on the initial level and change of each trait (as shown in Figure 1b). Mplus syntax examples are provided in the online supplemental material. All conditional models controlled for age, age squared, sex, race, education, income and wealth. Significance was set at p ≤.01.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Conceptual model exploring conscientiousness latent difference score. Item loadings and intercepts were set to be equal across time points. Socio-demographic covariates (age, age squared, sex, race, education, income and wealth) and alcohol variables were entered as predictors of C1 and ΔC in the conditional latent difference score model.

To examine cross-lagged paths and correlated change in alcohol consumption and personality, we extended the previous difference score models using parallel assessments of alcohol use and personality traits at baseline and follow-up. For these models, we treated the total number of drinks/week reported by participants (continuous variable) as single indicator for alcohol consumption; to reduce skewness in the distribution, this variable was natural log-transformed. Latent difference scores were estimated for both alcohol drinking and each personality traits, using the same strategy outlined above. Figure 2 illustrates the paths from alcohol consumption at baseline to change in personality, from personality to change in alcohol consumption, and the correlated change between the two variables.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Conceptual model exploring correlated difference scores of alcohol consumption and conscientiousness. Number of drinks/week was used as single indicator of alcohol consumption at baseline and follow-up; this variable were natural log transformed to reduce the skewness of the distribution. Item loadings and intercepts of personality indicators were set to be equal across time points. Model controlled for socio-demographic covariates.

Results

Descriptive statistics and interrelations among all variables are shown in Table 1. For each personality dimension, we estimated latent difference scores as depicted in Figure 1. In the unconditional models (Figure 1a), significant negative mean change and variances were observed for all traits (median standardized Δ estimate = −.13, and all Δ variances ps ≤.01), indicating a certain degree of variability in within-person decrease across traits over time. All model showed adequate fit to the data (see Table 2 for details), with a comparative fit index (CFI) > .90, and a root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) and standardized root mean square residual (SRMR) <.08 (Hu & Bentler, 1999; Marsh, Hau, & Grayson, 2005).

Table 2.

Levels of alcohol consumption and history of AUDs as predictors of latent difference scores of personality traits

Standardized estimates (99% Confidence Interval)

Models/Paths N E O A C
Unconditional Model
Δ Means/Intercepts −.13* (−17, −.10) −.13* (−.17, −.10) −.14* (−.18, −.11) −.07* (−.10, −.03) −.12* (−.16, −.09)
Δ Variances .86* (.84, .89) .92* (.90, .94) .91* (.90, .93) .91* (.89, .93) .95* (.93, .97)
Personality baseline → Δ Personality −.37* (−.41, −.34) −.29* (−.32, −.25) −.30* (−.33, −.27) −.30* (−.34, −.27) −.23* (−.27, −.19)
Model fit indices
RMSEA .04 .06 .06 .04 .04
SRMR .03 .05 .05 .03 .03
CFI .98 .95 .91 .98 .97

Conditional Model 1: Levels of Alcohol Consumption
Δ Means/Intercepts −.19* (−.36, −.02) −.24* (−.41, −.07) −.27* (−.43, −.12) −.24* (−.40, −.08) −.35* (−.53, −.16)
Δ Variances .85* (.82, .88) .91* (.88, .93) .89* (.87, .91) .88* (.86, .91) .92* (.90, .95)
Personality baseline → Δ Personality −.39* (−.42, −.35) −.30* (−.33, −.26) −.34* (−.37, −.30) −.33* (−.37, −.30) −.27* (−.31, −.22)
Light-to-moderate → Δ Personality −.01 (−.08, .06) .10* (.02, .18) .06 (−.01, .13) .06 (−.01, .14) .06 (−.02, .14)
Moderate-to-heavy → Δ Personality −.01 (−.15, .12) .09 (−.05, .23) .06 (−.06, .19) .07 (−.07, .20) −.01 (−.16, .14)
Model fit indices
RMSEA .04 .05 .05 .04 .03
SRMR .03 .04 .04 .03 .02
CFI .94 .91 .88 .94 .94

Conditional Model 2: History of AUDs
Δ Means/Intercepts −.12 (−.39, .14) −.14 (−.40, .13) −.28* (−54, −.03) −.26* (−.52, −.00) −.32* (−.62, −.02)
Δ Variances .85* (.82, .87) .90* (.88, .93) .89* (.87, .91) .89* (.86, .91) .92* (.89, .95)
Personality baseline → Δ Personality −.39* (−.43, −.36) −.30* (−.34, −.26) −.34* (−.38, −.30) −.34* (−.38, −.30) −.28* (−.33, −.23)
AUDs → Δ Personality .03 (−.06, .13) −.07 (−.16, .02) −.06 (−.15, .03) −.05 (−.14, .04) −.10* (−.21, −.00)
Model fit indices
RMSEA .04 .05 .05 .04 .03
SRMR .03 .03 .04 .03 .02
CFI .95 .91 .88 .95 .94

Note. The conditional latent difference score models were adjusted for age, age squared, sex, race, education, income and wealth; models with AUDs as predictor of change further adjusted for CAGE interval (years). Levels of alcohol consumption were entered as dummy coded variables in the models (reference category: no consumption). Number of observations ranged from 14,265 to 14,294 across personality dimensions for the unconditional models, from 14,219 to 14,247 for the models assessing the effect of alcohol consumption, and from 11,428 to 14,450 for the models assessing history of AUDs.

N = Neuroticism; E = Extraversion; O = Openness; A = Agreeableness; C = Conscientiousness. RMSEA = root mean square error of approximation; SRMR = standardized root mean square residual; CFI = comparative fit index.

*

p ≤ .01.

We then examined levels of alcohol consumption at baseline (dummy coded; reference: no consumption) and history of alcohol dependence (CAGE ≥1) as possible sources of inter-individual differences in intra-individual change of personality, net of other socio-demographic factors (i.e. conditional latent difference model, Figure 1b). Each alcohol variable was entered separately as a predictor of initial level and change of each personality traits, accounting for age, age squared, sex, race, education, income and wealth, and CAGE interval (in years) for the models estimating the effect of dependence. As shown in Table 2, the models estimating the effect of alcohol consumption on personality change confirmed a decrease for all five personality traits over the 4-year follow-up (median standardized Δ estimate = −.24, and all Δ variances ps ≤.01); for extraversion but not the other traits, this decrease was less pronounced for those categorized as light-to-moderate drinkers vs. non-drinkers (β = .10)1. For the models estimating the effect of alcohol dependence (see Table 2), within-person decreases were accentuated for conscientiousness (mean Δ = −.32) among individuals with a history of AUDs (β = −.10); no other significant effects were observed.

We also tested cross-lagged paths from alcohol consumption (i.e. number of drinks/week) to change in personality traits and from personality to change in the amount of alcohol consumed, and examined the correlation between latent differences in these variables. Alcohol consumption decreased over time, though there was significant variability in within-person decrease in drinks/week (Δ estimate = −.03, p >.01; variance = .84, p ≤.01). As shown in Table 3, the cross-lagged associations between initial alcohol drinking and personality with changes in these variables were not significant, except for openness (Openness baseline → Δ Drinks/week, β = .03) and conscientiousness (Conscientiousness baseline → Δ Drinks/week, β = .04), which were linked to attenuated decreases in number of drinks/week. Nonetheless, change in the amount of alcohol consumed correlated positively with change in extraversion and negatively with change in neuroticism.

Table 3.

Correlated latent difference scores of alcohol consumption and personality traits

Standardized estimates (99% Confidence Interval)

Models/Paths N E O A C
Means/Intercepts
  Δ Drinks/week −.03 (−.13, .07) −.03 (−.13, .07) −.03 (−.13, .07) −.03 (−.13, .07) −.03 (−.13, .07)
  Δ Personality Trait −.19* (−.35, −.02) −.25* (−.42, −.08) −.29* (−.45, −.13) −.24* (−.40, −.08) −.35* (−.54, −.17)
Variances
  Δ Drinks/week .84* (.82, .85) .84* (.82, .85) .84* (.82, .85) .84* (.82, 85) .84* (.82, .85)
  Δ Personality Trait .85* (.82, .88) .91* (.88, .92) .89* (.87, .91) .89* (.86, .91) .92** (.90, .95)
Personality ↔ Drinks/week at baseline −.01 (−.04, .01) .04* (.01, .06) .04* (.02, .06) −.03* (−.05, −.01) .02* (.00, .05)
Personality baseline → Δ Personality −.39* (−.42, −.35) −.30* (−.33, −.26) −.34* (−.38, −.31) −.34* (−.37, −.30) −.27* (−.31, −.22)
Personality baseline → Δ Drinks/week −.00 (−.03, .02) .02 (−.00, .05) .03* (.00, .06) .00 (−.02, .03) .04* (.02, .07)
Drinks/week baseline → Δ Drinks/week −.45* (−.48, −.43) −.45* (−.48, −.43) −.46* (−.48, −.43) −.45* (−.48, .43) −.45* (−.48, −.43)
Drinks/week baseline → Δ Personality −.00 (−.04, .03) .04 (−.00, .07) .02 (−.01, .06) .01 (−.02, .05) .01 (−.03, .05)
Δ Drinks/week ↔ Δ Personality −.04* (−.08, −.01) .05* (.01, .08) .03 (−.00, .06) .01 (−.02, .04) .01 (−.03, .04)
Model fit indices
RMSEA .04 .05 .05 .04 .03
SRMR .03 .04 .04 .03 .02
CFI .96 .93 .90 .95 .96

Note. All cross-lagged difference score models were adjusted for age, age squared, sex, race, education, income and wealth. Number of drinks/week at baseline and follow-up were natural log transformed. Number of observations was 14,283 for each cross-lagged difference score models.

N = Neuroticism; E = Extraversion; O = Openness; A = Agreeableness; C = Conscientiousness. RMSEA = root mean square error of approximation; SRMR = standardized root mean square residual; CFI = comparative fit index.

*

p ≤ .01.

Discussion

Using a large, longitudinal sample of middle aged and older adults, the present study examined how alcohol use, both current consumption and a history of dependence, contributes to personality change over a 4-year interval. Latent difference score models indicated a decrease for all five personality traits with significant variability in intra-individual change. In line with our expectations, moderate levels of alcohol consumption tended to be associated with the maintenance or retaining of desirable personality traits; specifically, light-to-moderate drinking predicted less of a decrease in extraversion, though this trend did not reach significance for the other traits. By contrast, a history of alcohol dependence was found to accentuate decrease in conscientiousness over time, but not decreases in the other traits. This study adds to prior work on the importance of health-related behaviors and lifestyle factors for personality development in adulthood. It also extends the findings on the effect of alcohol on personality change beyond adolescence and early adulthood to a significant part of later adulthood. When using parallel assessments of personality and alcohol drinking (drinks/week), we also observed correlated changes in these variables over time.

In literature, there is evidence that link light-to-moderate alcohol consumption to a variety of positive outcomes, from stress reduction to better cognitive functioning, well-being and health (e.g., González-Rubio et al., 2016; Lang et al., 2007; Paulson et al., 2018; Reas, Laughlin, Kritz-Silverstein, Barrett-Connor, & McEvoy, 2016), as opposite to abstinence or heavy drinking. In their paper, Allen and colleagues (2015) suggested that desirable personality traits (i.e. higher extraversion, openness or conscientiousness) might be maintained through lifestyle improvements or healthy choices. In this study, we found preliminary evidence that supports this hypothesis. In particular, light-to-moderate drinkers showed an attenuated decrease in extraversion over time. Individual who drink alcohol are likely to be healthier, more outgoing and socially engaged than abstainers, and derive more reward (i.e. mood enhancement) from drinking alcohol in social contexts (see Fairbairn et al., 2015). It is thus possible that alcohol foster extraversion over time through social factors connected with drinking. Moreover, alcohol drinkers relative to those who never drank or stop drinking tend to report a better health status (Frisher et al., 2015; see also Green & Polen, 2001), which in turn helps to maintain a more (socially) active lifestyle. Contrary to our expectation, however, moderate alcohol drinking was not significantly associate with change in conscientiousness nor openness.

Alcohol dependence also predicted personality change in later life. In particular, decreases in conscientiousness were accentuated for individuals with a history of AUDs (CAGE ≤1). Alcohol dependence is accompanied by brain damages and neurotransmitter imbalances (Bühler & Mann, 2011; Oscar-Berman & Marinković, 2007), which are likely to manifest in change of behavior and personality over time. It is also associated with increased risk for of chronic diseases, including severe cognitive impairment and dementia (e.g., Kuźma et al., 2014), which are related to decrease in traits like conscientiousness (Pocnet, Rossier, Antonietti, & Von Gunten, 2013; Terracciano et al., in press). Even after remission of clinical symptoms, it may be possible for individuals who experienced alcohol dependence to continue to be disorganized and incapable of long-term planning. Surprisingly, CAGE was not associated with change in neuroticism, though neuroticism has been identified as a significant predictor of alcohol-related problems and disorders in past studies (Martin & Sher, 1994; Ruiz et al., 2003).

As noted above, alcohol may influence personality development in several ways. For instance, the amount of alcohol consumed or the decision to abstain from alcohol may depend on individuals’ health, especially at older ages. Also, genetic influences may determine variation in the development of dependence symptoms and personality. Twin studies have demonstrated significant overlap between the genetic diathesis for AUDs and genetic variation in the five-factor personality traits (de Moor et al., 2011; Littlefield et al. 2011). Future studies need to focus on possible mediators and sequential pathways through with alcohol use and personality are related. Notably, our study provided preliminary evidence of correlated changes between these two constructs at later stages of life, as emerged in cross-lagged difference score models. Even though directional causality cannot be inferred from the data, it is clinically relevant to know whether personality change occur with modification in life-styles and health behaviors.

A few limitations of the study need to be taken into account. First, this study do not account for early life factors that potentially influence risk taking behaviors and alcohol use in later life. For example, lower scores on IQ tests in childhood/adolescence have shown to predict higher alcohol consumption and occurrence of hangovers in middle adulthood (e.g., Batty, Deary, & Macintyre, 2006; Sjölund, Hemmingsson, & Allebeck, 2015). Similarly, childhood personality (low conscientiousness) have been found to predict levels of alcohol consumption in middle-age (e.g., Hampson, Goldberg, Vogt, & Dubanoski, 2006; Kubicka, Matejcek, Dytrych, & Roth, 2001). Individuals with lower levels of conscientiousness in early adulthood may be more likely to engage in unhealthy patterns of alcohol drinking and develop AUDs in later life, which would consequently influence further decrease on conscientiousness trait over time. Moreover, the present work relayed exclusively on self-report data and a single indicator of alcohol drinking, i.e. count of drinks/week. Future research should consider measures of actual alcohol consumption (i.e. physiological tests) or diagnosis of AUDs, as well as self-report and informant ratings to assess personality.

Despite these limitations, this study suggests that personality traits may change with specific patterns of alcohol consumption beyond early adulthood. The size of the associations also suggests that alcohol may be a distal factor for personality change, perhaps acting through other biological, cognitive and psychological pathways, or in conjunction with other health-related behaviors (see Stephan et al., 2014).

Supplementary Material

Suppl info correction

Acknowledgments

Funding

Research reported in this publication was supported by the the National Institute on Aging (NIA) of the National Institutes of Health under Award Number R01AG053297. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health. Data were drawn from the Health and Retirement Study, a longitudinal project sponsored by NIA U01AG009740 and the Social Security Administration. No direct support was received from NIA U01AG009740 for the analyses reported in this article.

Footnotes

1

In a sensitive analysis, we distinguished those who responded “yes” when asked if they ever drank alcoholic beverages but reported 0 drinks/weeks at the baseline assessment (occasional/infrequent drinkers, 17.7%) from those who reported to had never drank alcohol (abstainers, 49.6%). Results on the effect of light-to moderate alcohol consumption on personality change were the same when excluding occasional/infrequent drinkers from the reference category. Of note, when using moderate-to-light alcohol consumption as reference category, we observed no effect of heavy vs. moderate drinking on latent scores of personality change.

Declaration of Conflicting Interests

The authors declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

Contributor Information

Martina Luchetti, Florida State University College of Medicine, 1115 W. Call Street, Tallahassee, FL, 32306-4300, Phone: +1 850 645 8151.

Antonio Terracciano, Florida State University College of Medicine, 1115 W. Call Street, Tallahassee, FL, 32306-4300, Phone: +1 850-645-0355.

Yannick Stephan, EuroMov, University of Montpellier, 700 Avenue du Pic Saint Loup, 34090 Montpellier, France.

Angelina R. Sutin, Florida State University College of Medicine, 1115 W. Call Street, Tallahassee, FL, 32306-4300, Phone: +1 850 645 0438.

References

  1. Allen MS, Magee CA, Vella SA, Laborde S. Bidirectional associations between personality and physical activity in adulthood. Health Psychology. 2017;36:332–336. doi: 10.1037/hea0000371. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Allen MS, Vella SA, Laborde S. Health-related behaviour and personality trait development in adulthood. Journal of Research in Personality. 2015;59:104–110. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2015.10.005. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  3. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-5) American Psychiatric Pub; 2013. [Google Scholar]
  4. Atherton OE, Robins RW, Rentfrow PJ, Lamb ME. Personality correlates of risky health outcomes: Findings from a large Internet study. Journal of Research in Personality. 2014;50:56–60. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2014.03.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Batty GD, Deary IJ, Macintyre S. Childhood IQ and life course socioeconomic position in relation to alcohol induced hangovers in adulthood: The Aberdeen children of the 1950s study. Journal of Epidemiology & Community Health. 2006;60:872–874. doi: 10.1136/jech.2005.045039. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Blonigen DM, Durbin CE, Hicks BM, Johnson W, McGue M, Iacono WG. Alcohol use initiation is associated with changes in personality trait trajectories from early adolescence to young adulthood. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research. 2015;39:2163–2170. doi: 10.1111/acer.12878. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bogg T, Roberts BW. Conscientiousness and health-related behaviors: A meta-analysis of the leading behavioral contributors to mortality. Psychological Bulletin. 2004;130:887–919. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.130.6.887. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Britton A, Ben-Shlomo Y, Benzeval M, Kuh D, Bell S. Life course trajectories of alcohol consumption in the United Kingdom using longitudinal data from nine cohort studies. BMC Medicine. 2015;13:47. doi: 10.1186/s12916-015-0273-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Bühler M, Mann K. Alcohol and the human brain: a systematic review of different neuroimaging methods. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research. 2011;35:1771–1793. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2011.01540.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Cheng H, Furnham A. Correlates of adult binge drinking: Evidence from a British Cohort. PloS One. 2013;8:e78838. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0078838. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. De Moor MH, Vink JM, van Beek JH, Geels LM, Bartels M, de Geus EJ, Boomsma DI. Heritability of problem drinking and the genetic overlap with personality in a general population sample. Frontiers in Genetics. 2011:2. doi: 10.3389/fgene.2011.00076. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. DiBartolo MC, Jarosinski JM. Alcohol Use Disorder in older adults: Challenges in assessment and treatment. Issues in Mental Health Nursing. 2017;38:25–32. doi: 10.1080/01612840.2016.1257076. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Djoussé L, Gaziano JM. Alcohol consumption and heart failure: A systematic review. Current Atherosclerosis Reports. 2008;10:117–120. doi: 10.1007/s11883-008-0017-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Donnellan MB, Lucas RE. Age differences in the Big Five across the life span: Evidence from two national samples. Psychology and Aging. 2008;23:558–566. doi: 10.1037/a0012897. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Enders CK, Bandalos DL. The relative performance of full information maximum likelihood estimation for missing data in structural equation models. Structural Equation Modeling. 2001;8:430–457. doi: 10.1207/S15328007SEM0803_5. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  16. Fairbairn CE, Sayette MA, Wright AG, Levine JM, Cohn JF, Creswell KG. Extraversion and the rewarding effects of alcohol in a social context. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2015;124:660–673. doi: 10.1037/abn0000024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Frisher M, Mendonça M, Shelton N, Pikhart H, de Oliveira C, Holdsworth C. Is alcohol consumption in older adults associated with poor self-rated health? Cross-sectional and longitudinal analyses from the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing. BMC Public Health. 2015;15:703. doi: 10.1186/s12889-015-1993-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. González-Rubio E, San Mauro I, López-Ruíz C, Díaz-Prieto LE, Marcos A, Nova E. Relationship of moderate alcohol intake and type of beverage with health behaviors and quality of life in elderly subjects. Quality of Life Research. 2016;25:1931–1942. doi: 10.1007/s11136-016-1229-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Green CA, Polen MR. The health and health behaviors of people who do not drink alcohol. American Journal of Preventive Medicine. 2001;21:298–305. doi: 10.1016/S0749-3797(01)00365-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Hakulinen C, Elovainio M, Batty GD, Virtanen M, Kivimäki M, Jokela M. Personality and alcohol consumption: Pooled analysis of 72,949 adults from eight cohort studies. Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 2015;151:110–114. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2015.03.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Hampson SE, Goldberg LR, Vogt TM, Dubanoski JP. Forty years on: Teachers' assessments of children's personality traits predict self-reported health behaviors and outcomes at midlife. Health Psychology. 2006;25:57–64. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.25.1.57. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Han BH, Moore AA, Sherman S, Keyes KM, Palamar JJ. Demographic trends of binge alcohol use and alcohol use disorders among older adults in the United States, 2005–2014. Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 2017;170:198–207. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2016.11.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Hicks BM, Durbin CE, Blonigen DM, Iacono WG, McGue M. Relationship between personality change and the onset and course of alcohol dependence in young adulthood. Addiction. 2012;107:540–548. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2011.03617.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Hinkin CH, Castellon SA, Dickson-Fuhrman E, Daum G, Jaffe J, Jarvik L. Screening for drug and alcohol abuse among older adults using a modified version of the CAGE. The American Journal on Addictions. 2001;10:319–326. doi: 10.1111/j.1521-0391.2001.tb00521.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Hong RY, Paunonen SV. Personality traits and health-risk behaviours in university students. European Journal of Personality. 2009;23:675–696. doi: 10.1002/per.736. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  26. Hu LT, Bentler PM. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal. 1999;6:1–55. doi: 10.1080/10705519909540118. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  27. Immonen S, Valvanne J, Pitkälä KH. Older adults’ own reasoning for their alcohol consumption. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry. 2011;26:1169–1176. doi: 10.1002/gps.2657. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Jokela M, Batty GD, Nyberg ST, Virtanen M, Nabi H, Singh-Manoux A, Kivimäki M. Personality and all-cause mortality: Individual-participant meta-analysis of 3,947 deaths in 76,150 adults. American Journal of Epidemiology, kwt170. 2013 doi: 10.1093/aje/kwt170. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Jokela M, Hakulinen C, Singh-Manoux A, Kivimäki M. Personality change associated with chronic diseases: Pooled analysis of four prospective cohort studies. Psychological Medicine. 2014;44:2629–2640. doi: 10.1017/S0033291714000257. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Juster RP, Russell JJ, Almeida D, Picard M. Allostatic load and comorbidities: A mitochondrial, epigenetic, and evolutionary perspective. Development and Psychopathology. 2016;28:1117–1146. doi: 10.1017/S0954579416000730. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Kandler C, Kornadt AE, Hagemeyer B, Neyer FJ. Patterns and sources of personality development in old age. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2015;109:175–191. doi: 10.1037/pspp0000028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Kotov R, Gamez W, Schmidt F, Watson D. Linking “big” personality traits to anxiety, depressive, and substance use disorders: a meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin. 2010;136:768–821. doi: 10.1037/a0020327. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Kubička L, Matějček Z, Dytrych Z, Roth Z. IQ and personality traits assessed in childhood as predictors of drinking and smoking behaviour in middle-aged adults: a 24-year follow-up study. Addiction. 2001;96:1615–1628. doi: 10.1046/j.1360-0443.2001.961116158.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Kuźma E, Llewellyn DJ, Langa KM, Wallace RB, Lang IA. History of alcohol use disorders and risk of severe cognitive impairment: a 19-year prospective cohort study. The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry. 2014;22:1047–1054. doi: 10.1016/j.jagp.2014.06.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Lachman ME, Weaver SL. The Midlife Development Inventory (MIDI) personality scales: Scale construction and scoring. Technical Report. Waltham, MA: Brandeis University; 1997. [Google Scholar]
  36. Lang I, Wallace RB, Huppert FA, Melzer D. Moderate alcohol consumption in older adults is associated with better cognition and well-being than abstinence. Age and Ageing. 2007;36:256–261. doi: 10.1093/ageing/afm001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Lim SS, Vos T, Flaxman AD, Danaei G, Shibuya K, Adair-Rohani H, Aryee M. A comparative risk assessment of burden of disease and injury attributable to 67 risk factors and risk factor clusters in 21 regions, 1990–2010: A systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2010. The Lancet. 2012;380:2224–2260. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(12)61766-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Littlefield AK, Agrawal A, Ellingson JM, Kristjansson S, Madden PA, Bucholz KK, Sher KJ. Does variance in drinking motives explain the genetic overlap between personality and alcohol use disorder symptoms? A twin study of young women. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research. 2011;35:2242–2250. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2011.01574.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Littlefield AK, Sher KJ, Steinley D. Developmental trajectories of impulsivity and their association with alcohol use and related outcomes during emerging and young adulthood I. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research. 2010;34:1409–1416. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2010.01224.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Littlefield AK, Sher KJ, Wood PK. Is “maturing out” of problematic alcohol involvement related to personality change? Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2009;118:360–374. doi: 10.1037/a0015125. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Löckenhoff CE, Terracciano A, Ferrucci L, Costa PT. Five-factor personality traits and age trajectories of self-rated health: The role of question framing. Journal of Personality. 2012;80:375–401. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2011.00724.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Luchetti M, Terracciano A, Stephan Y, Sutin AR. Personality and cognitive decline in older adults: Data from a longitudinal sample and meta-analysis. The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences. 2016;71:591–601. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbu184. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Malouff JM, Thorsteinsson EB, Rooke SE, Schutte NS. Alcohol involvement and the Five-Factor Model of personality: A meta-analysis. Journal of Drug Education. 2007;37:277–294. doi: 10.2190/DE.37.3.d. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Martin ED, Sher KJ. Family history of alcoholism, alcohol use disorders and the five-factor model of personality. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 1994;55:81–90. doi: 10.15288/jsa.1994.55.81. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Marsh HW, Hau K-T, Grayson D. Goodness of Fit in Structural Equation Models. In: Maydeu-Olivares A, McArdle JJ, editors. Multivariate applications book series. Contemporary psychometrics: A festschrift for Roderick P. McDonald. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 2005. pp. 275–340. [Google Scholar]
  46. McAdams KK, Donnellan MB. Facets of personality and drinking in first-year college students. Personality and Individual Differences. 2009;46:207–212. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2008.09.028. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  47. McArdle JJ. Latent variable modeling of differences and changes with longitudinal data. Annual Review of Psychology. 2009;60:577–605. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.60.110707.163612. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. McCrae RR, Costa PT., Jr . A five-factor theory of personality. In: Pervin LA, John OP, editors. Handbook of personality: Theory and Research. 2. New York: Guilford Press; 1999. pp. 139–153. [Google Scholar]
  49. Meredith W. Measurement invariance, factor analysis and factorial invariance. Psychometrika. 1993;58:525–543. doi: 10.1007/BF02294825. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  50. Mezquita L, Camacho L, Ibá-ez MI, Villa H, Moya-Higueras J, Ortet G. Five-Factor Model and alcohol outcomes: Mediating and moderating role of alcohol expectancies. Personality and Individual Differences. 2015;74:29–34. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2014.10.002. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  51. Mostofsky E, Mukamal KJ, Giovannucci EL, Stampfer MJ, Rimm EB. Key findings on alcohol consumption and a variety of health outcomes from the Nurses’ Health Study. American Journal of Public Health. 2016;106:1586–1591. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2016.303336. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Mõttus R, Johnson W, Deary IJ. Personality traits in old age: Measurement and rank-order stability and some mean-level change. Psychology and Aging. 2012;27:243–249. doi: 10.1037/a0023690. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Mroczek DK, Kolarz CM. The effect of age on positive and negative affect: A developmental perspective on happiness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1998;75:1333–1349. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.75.5.1333. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Muthén LK, Muthén BO. Mplus User’s Guide. Eighth. Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén; 1998–2017. [Google Scholar]
  55. National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism. Alcohol facts and statistics (Fact sheet) 2017 Retrieved from https://pubs.niaaa.nih.gov/publications/AlcoholFacts&Stats/AlcoholFacts&Stats.pdf.
  56. Oscar-Berman M, Marinković K. Alcohol: Effects on neurobehavioral functions and the brain. Neuropsychology review. 2007;17:239–257. doi: 10.1007/s11065-007-9038-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Paleari FG, Fincham FD. The reciprocal relationship between husbands and wives'marital forgivingness: A two-wave cross-lagged latent difference score analysis of ten-year data. TPM: Testing, Psychometrics, Methodology in Applied Psychology. 2015;22:287–308. [Google Scholar]
  58. Paulson D, Shah M, Herring D, Scott R, Herrera M, Brush D, Bassett R. The relationship between moderate alcohol consumption, depressive symptomatology, and C-reactive protein: the Health and Retirement Study. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry. 2018 doi: 10.1002/gps.4746. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Pocnet C, Rossier J, Antonietti JP, von Gunten A. Personality features and cognitive level in patients at an early stage of Alzheimer's disease. Personality and Individual Differences. 2013;54:174–179. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2012.08.035. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  60. Reas ET, Laughlin GA, Kritz-Silverstein D, Barrett-Connor E, McEvoy LK. Moderate, regular alcohol consumption is associated with higher cognitive function in older community-dwelling adults. The Journal of Prevention of Alzheimer's Disease. 2016;3:105–113. doi: 10.14283/jpad.2016.89. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Rehm J. The risks associated with alcohol use and alcoholism. Alcohol research & Health: The Journal of the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism. 2011;34:135–143. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Riley EN, Rukavina M, Smith GT. The reciprocal predictive relationship between high-risk personality and drinking: An 8-wave longitudinal study in early adolescents. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2016;125(6):798–804. doi: 10.1037/abn0000189. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Roberts BW, Mroczek DK. Personality trait change in adulthood. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2008;17:31–35. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2008.00543.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Roberts BW, Walton KE, Viechtbauer W. Patterns of mean-level change in personality traits across the life course: A meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Psychological Bulletin. 2006;132:1–25. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.132.1.1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Roberts BW, Wood D. Personality development in the context of the neo-socioanalytic model of personality. In: Mroczek D, Little T, editors. Handbook of personality development. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 2006. pp. 11–39. [Google Scholar]
  66. Ruiz MA, Pincus AL, Dickinson KA. NEO PI-R predictors of alcohol use and alcohol-related problems. Journal of Personality Assessment. 2003;81:226–236. doi: 10.1207/S15327752JPA8103_05. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Sabia S, Elbaz A, Britton A, Bell S, Dugravot A, Shipley M, Singh-Manoux A. Alcohol consumption and cognitive decline in early old age. Neurology. 2014;82:332–339. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000000063. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Samek DR, Hicks BM, Durbin E, Hinnant JB, Iacono WG, McGue M. Codevelopment between key personality traits and Alcohol Use Disorder from adolescence through young adulthood. Journal of Personality. doi: 10.1111/jopy.12311. (in press) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Selig JP, Preacher KJ. Mediation models for longitudinal data in developmental research. Research in Human Development. 2009;6:144–164. doi: 10.1080/15427600902911247. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  70. Simet SM, Sisson JH. Alcohol's effects on lung health and immunity. Alcohol Research: Current Reviews. 2015;37:199–208. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Sher KJ, Grekin ER, Williams NA. The development of alcohol use disorders. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology. 2005;1:493–523. doi: 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.1.102803.144107. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Shin SH, Hong HG, Jeon SM. Personality and alcohol use: The role of impulsivity. Addictive Behaviors. 2012;37:102–107. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2011.09.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Sjölund S, Hemmingsson T, Allebeck P. IQ and level of alcohol consumption—findings from a national survey of Swedish conscripts. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research. 2015;39:548–555. doi: 10.1111/acer.12656. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Specht J, Egloff B, Schmukle SC. Stability and change of personality across the life course: The impact of age and major life events on mean-level and rank-order stability of the Big Five. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2011;101:862–882. doi: 10.1037/a0024950. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Stephan Y, Sutin AR, Bosselut G, Terracciano A. Sensory functioning and personality development among older adults. Psychology and Aging. 2017;32:139–147. doi: 10.1037/pag0000159. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Stephan Y, Sutin AR, Luchetti M, Terracciano A. Allostatic load and personality: A 4-year longitudinal study. Psychosomatic Medicine. 2016;78:302–310. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0000000000000281. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Stephan Y, Sutin AR, Terracciano A. Physical activity and personality development across adulthood and old age: Evidence from two longitudinal studies. Journal of Research in Personality. 2014;49:1–7. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2013.12.003. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  78. Sutin AR, Zonderman AB, Ferrucci L, Terracciano A. Personality traits and chronic disease: Implications for adult personality development. The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences. 2013;68:912–920. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbt036. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Tarter RE. Are there inherited behavioral traits that predispose to substance abuse? Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1988;56:189–196. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.56.2.189. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Terracciano A, McCrae RR, Brant LJ, Costa PT., Jr Hierarchical linear modeling analyses of the NEO-PI-R scales in the Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging. Psychology and Aging. 2005;20:493–506. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.20.3.493. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Terracciano A, Löckenhoff CE, Crum RM, Bienvenu OJ, Costa PT. Five-Factor Model personality profiles of drug users. BMC Psychiatry. 2008;8 doi: 10.1186/1471-244X-8-22. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Terracciano A, Stephan Y, Luchetti M, Sutin AR. Cognitive impairment, dementia, and personality stability among older adults. Assessment. doi: 10.1177/1073191117691844. (in press) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Terracciano A, Sutin AR, An Y, O’Brien RJ, Ferrucci L, Zonderman AB, Resnick SM. Personality and risk of Alzheimer’s disease: New data and meta-analysis. Alzheimer’s & Dementia: The Journal of the Alzheimer’s Association. 2014;10:179–186. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2013.03.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Turiano NA, Pitzer L, Armour C, Karlamangla A, Ryff CD, Mroczek DK. Personality trait level and change as predictors of health outcomes: Findings from a national study of Americans (MIDUS) The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 67B. 2012:4–12. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbr072. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Turiano NA, Whiteman SD, Hampson SE, Roberts BW, Mroczek DK. Personality and substance use in midlife: Conscientiousness as a moderator and the effects of trait change. Journal of Research in Personality. 2012;46:295–305. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2012.02.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Udo T, Vásquez E, Shaw BA. A lifetime history of alcohol use disorder increases risk for chronic medical conditions after stable remission. Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 2015;157:68–74. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2015.10.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. White HR, Marmorstein NR, Crews FT, Bates ME, Mun EY, Loeber R. Associations between heavy drinking and changes in impulsive behavior among adolescent boys. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research. 2011;35:295–303. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2010.01345.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Wortman J, Lucas RE, Donnellan MB. Stability and change in the Big Five personality domains: Evidence from a longitudinal study of Australians. Psychology and Aging. 2012;27:867–874. doi: 10.1037/a0029322. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Wrzus C, Roberts BW. Processes of personality development in adulthood: The TESSERA Framework. Personality and Social Psychology Review. 2017;21:253–277. doi: 10.1177/1088868316652279. 1088868316652279. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Zimprich D, Allemand M, Lachman ME. Factorial structure and age-related psychometrics of the MIDUS personality adjective items across the life span. Psychological Assessment. 2012;24:173–186. doi: 10.1037/a0025265. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Suppl info correction

RESOURCES