Skip to main content
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America logoLink to Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America
letter
. 2018 Jul 10;115(29):E6676–E6677. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1714497115

Misunderstandings regarding the application of Granger causality in neuroscience

Lionel Barnett a,1, Adam B Barrett a, Anil K Seth a
PMCID: PMC6055162  PMID: 29991604

Stokes and Purdon (1) raise several concerns about the use of Granger–Geweke causality (GGC) analysis in neuroscience. They make two primary claims: (i) that GGC estimates may be severely biased or of high variance and (ii) that GGC fails to reveal the full structural/causal mechanisms of a system.

Unfortunately, these claims rest, respectively, on an incomplete evaluation of the literature and a misconception about what GGC can be said to measure.

Stokes and Purdon (1) explain how bias and variance in GGC estimation arise from the use of separate, independent full and reduced regressions. However, this problem has long been recognized (2, 3) and, moreover, has already been solved by methods which derive GGC from a single full regression. These methods effectively calculate reduced model parameters from the full model via factorization of the spectral density matrix. Published approaches (also implemented in freely available software) include Wilson’s frequency-domain algorithm (4), Whittle’s time-domain algorithm (3), and a state-space method involving solution of a discrete-time algebraic Riccati equation (5). Thus, the source of bias and variance discussed in ref. 1 has already been resolved (see also ref. 6). We note that Stokes and Purdon (1) erroneously state that “Barnett and Seth … have proposed fitting the reduced model and using it to directly compute the spectral components,” whereas, as mentioned, we derive GGC from a single full regression (3).

Stokes and Purdon (1) then note that GGC reflects a combination of “transmitter” and “channel” dynamics and is independent of “receiver” dynamics. This independence has also been previously identified; it follows directly from the invariance of GGC under certain affine transformations (7). Stokes and Purdon (1) argue that this runs “counter to intuitive notions of causality intended to explain observed effects,” since, as they put it, “neuroscientists seek to determine the mechanisms that produce ‘effects’ within a neural system or circuit as a function of inputs or ‘causes’ observed at other locations.” However, this perspective is more closely aligned with approaches such as dynamic causal modeling (DCM)—usually characterized as effective connectivity—which attempt to find the optimal mechanistic (circuit level) description that explains observed data. GGC, by contrast, models statistical dependencies among observed responses and is therefore a measure of (directed) functional connectivity (8). Essentially, the distinction is between making inferences about an underlying physical causal mechanism (DCM) and making inferences about directed information flow (GGC; ref. 9). Both address valid questions.

Our view is that the real problems associated with GGC analysis of neurophysiological data reside elsewhere: with issues of stationarity, linearity, and exogenous influences, as noted in ref. 1, but also with the noise, sampling rates, and temporal/spatial aggregation engendered by neural data acquisition (10).

Footnotes

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  • 1.Stokes PA, Purdon PL. A study of problems encountered in Granger causality analysis from a neuroscience perspective. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2017;114:E7063–E7072. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1704663114. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Chen Y, Bressler SL, Ding M. Frequency decomposition of conditional Granger causality and application to multivariate neural field potential data. J Neurosci Methods. 2006;150:228–237. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2005.06.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Barnett L, Seth AK. The MVGC multivariate Granger causality toolbox: A new approach to Granger-causal inference. J Neurosci Methods. 2014;223:50–68. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2013.10.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Dhamala M, Rangarajan G, Ding M. Estimating Granger causality from Fourier and wavelet transforms of time series data. Phys Rev Lett. 2008;100:018701. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevLett.100.018701. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Barnett L, Seth AK. Granger causality for state-space models. Phys. Rev. E (Rapid Commun) 2015;91:040101(R). doi: 10.1103/PhysRevE.91.040101. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Faes L, Stramaglia S, Marinazzo D. 2017. On the interpretability and computational reliability of frequency-domain Granger causality. F1000Res 6:1710.
  • 7.Barrett AB, Barnett L, Seth AK. Multivariate Granger causality and generalized variance. Phys Rev E Stat Nonlin Soft Matter Phys. 2010;81:041907. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevE.81.041907. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Seth AK, Barrett AB, Barnett LC. Granger causality analysis in neuroscience and neuroimaging. J Neurosci. 2015;35:3293–3297. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4399-14.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Barnett L, Barrett AB, Seth AK. Granger causality and transfer entropy are equivalent for Gaussian variables. Phys Rev Lett. 2009;103:0238701. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevLett.103.238701. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Barnett L, Seth AK. Detectability of Granger causality for subsampled continuous-time neurophysiological processes. J Neurosci Methods. 2017;275:93–121. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2016.10.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America are provided here courtesy of National Academy of Sciences

RESOURCES