Abstract
Functional circuit assembly is thought to require coordinated development of excitation and inhibition, but whether they are co-regulated cell-autonomously remains unclear. We investigate effects of decreased glutamatergic synaptic input on inhibitory synapses by expressing AMPAR subunit, GluA1 and GluA2, C-terminal peptides (GluA1CTP and GluA2CTP) in developing Xenopus tectal neurons. GluACTPs decrease excitatory synaptic inputs and cell-autonomously decreases inhibitory synaptic inputs in excitatory and inhibitory neurons. Visually evoked excitatory and inhibitory currents decrease proportionately, maintaining excitation/inhibition. GluACTPs affect dendrite structure and visual experience-dependent structural plasticity differently in excitatory and inhibitory neurons. Deficits in excitatory and inhibitory synaptic transmission and experience-dependent plasticity manifest in altered visual receptive field properties. Both visual avoidance behavior and learning-induced behavioral plasticity are impaired, suggesting that maintaining excitation/inhibition alone is insufficient to preserve circuit function. We demonstrate that excitatory synaptic dysfunction in individual neurons cell-autonomously decreases inhibitory inputs and disrupts neuronal and circuit plasticity, information processing and learning.
Both inhibitory and excitatory input development are shaped by activity, but one may be dependent on the other. Here, the authors examine plasticity of inhibitory inputs in vivo, as well as behavioral consequences in tadpoles where excitatory transmission has been impaired.
Introduction
Activity plays a critical role in the refinement and maintenance of functional neural circuits, which are thought to require coordinated development of two principle components: excitatory and inhibitory neurons1. Although proportional co-regulation of excitation and inhibition and a constant excitation/inhibition ratio have been widely observed during circuit development1, our understanding of how glutamatergic excitatory inputs affect the development of inhibition at synaptic and neuronal levels remains incomplete. Mounting evidence from different brain regions and species suggests that perturbing activity or sensory experience delays development of inhibition and disrupts the maturation and specification of inhibitory neurons and circuits2–7, however most of these studies perturbed activity broadly and were unable to resolve cell-autonomous and circuit-based outcomes. Direct evidence that glutamatergic synaptic inputs drive the cell autonomous development of inhibitory input in individual neurons remains elusive8.
As the predominant mediator of fast excitatory synaptic transmission, AMPARs provide the initial depolarization that is essential for the activation of NMDARs and subsequent secondary signal transduction and synaptic plasticity mechanisms. Four types of AMPAR subunit (GluA1–4) form different hetero- and homo-dimers of AMARs, with GluA1 and GluA2 being the major AMPAR subunits. Regulation of the trafficking of postsynaptic AMPAR underlies activity-dependent plasticity of synaptic strength9–11. Regulatory sites within the C-terminal region of GluA1 and GluA2 subunits are required for synaptic trafficking of AMPARs9,12. Expression of peptides corresponding to the GluA C-terminal peptides (GluA1CTP or GluA2CTP) impairs AMPAR trafficking, decreases excitatory synaptic transmission, and disrupts experience-dependent synaptic plasticity13–15. GluACTPs are therefore effective tools to disrupt AMPAR-mediated excitatory transmission in individual neurons, permitting study of outstanding questions concerning the role of excitatory synaptic inputs in structural and functional development of neurons and circuits.
Here, we express GluA1CTP or GluA2CTP, referred to collectively as GluACTPs, in individual tectal neurons to assess the effects of impaired excitatory synaptic transmission on inhibitory synaptic inputs and the development of structural and functional properties in excitatory and inhibitory neurons in vivo. We show that GluACTP expression proportionally decreases excitatory and inhibitory synaptic inputs, resulting in a constant balance of excitation to inhibition in both inhibitory and excitatory neurons. In vivo time-lapse imaging demonstrates that deficits in excitatory synaptic inputs have distinct effects on dendritic arbor development and experience-dependent structural plasticity in excitatory and inhibitory neurons. GluACTP-mediated decreases in excitatory and inhibitory transmission also manifest in deficits in visual information processing, recorded as impaired spatial and temporal receptive field properties, as well as visuomotor behavior. Finally, GluACTP expression blocks learning-induced behavioral plasticity. Our results demonstrate that excitatory synaptic dysfunction leads to cell-autonomous inhibitory synaptic dysfunction, which then ramifies to impair neuronal and circuit properties and degrade behavioral performance.
Results
GluACTP expression reduces E and I synaptic transmission
To test whether decreasing glutamatergic synaptic inputs in individual neurons affects GABAergic synaptic transmission, we sparsely transfected tectal neurons with constructs co-expressing GFP and GluA1CTP or GluA2CTP, and recorded mEPSCs and mIPSCs from GFP+ neurons 5–8 days later (Fig. 1a). mEPSC frequency was significantly reduced in both GluA1CTP and GluA2CTP-expressing neurons, with no significant change in mEPSC amplitudes (Fig. 1b, c). The decrease in mEPSC frequency likely reflects loss of synapses over several days of GluACTP expression. Interestingly, both the frequency and amplitude of mIPSCs were significantly reduced in GluACTP-expressing neurons (Fig. 1d, e), suggesting that excitatory synaptic inputs govern the development of inhibitory synaptic inputs in a cell-autonomous manner. By contrast, disrupting inhibitory synaptic inputs by interfering with GABAAR trafficking does not affect excitatory input onto the same neurons16. Paired pulse ratios of excitatory synaptic currents were comparable in neurons expressing GluA1CTP (n = 5), GluA2CTP (n = 5), and controls (n = 7) (Fig. 1f, g), consistent with a deficit in AMPAR trafficking into postsynaptic sites13–15,17.
Tectal neurons receive direct excitatory retinal inputs as well as feed forward and feedback inhibitory inputs within tectal circuits11,18,19. To determine if the decreased excitatory synaptic transmission affected the E/I balance of evoked synaptic responses, we recorded excitatory excitatory compound synaptic currents (eCSCs) and inhibitory compound synaptic currents (iCSCs) from transfected tectal neurons evoked by full-field visual stimulation in intact animals (Fig. 1h). Visually evoked eCSCs and iCSCs recorded from either GluA1CTP- or GluA2CTP-expressing neurons were significantly smaller than controls (Fig. 1i, j), however, the ratio of total integrated charge transfer between iCSCs and eCSCs remained unchanged between GluACTP-expressing and control neurons (Fig. 1k). These data further demonstrate that interfering with GluA1- and GluA2-containing AMPAR trafficking not only decreases excitatory synaptic transmission onto the transfected cells, but also induces a proportional decrease in the inhibitory synaptic transmission onto the same neurons.
Inhibition decreases cell-autonomously in E and I neurons
Excitatory and inhibitory tectal neurons demonstrate different visual experience-dependent structural and functional plasticity20. To test whether disrupting excitatory synaptic transmission affects excitatory and inhibitory neurons differentially, we combined immunohistochemical labeling of the excitatory and inhibitory synaptic markers PSD95 and gephyrin, with GABA immunolabeling to examine the density of excitatory and inhibitory postsynaptic puncta in dendrites of GluACTP-expressing neurons.
In the optic tectum, individual tectal neurons express a mixture of AMPARs with different subunit compositions (GluA2-lacking or GluA2-containing). The proportion of GluA2-lacking to GluA2-containing AMPARs varies among individual tectal neurons such that more immature neurons show higher content of calcium permeable GluA2-lacking AMPARs21. Both excitatory and inhibitory neurons express GluA1 and GluA2 in the developing optic tectum, as shown by double immunolabeling with GABA and GluA1 or GluA2 antibodies (Fig. 2a, b).
We examined the effects of GluACTP expression on the density of PSD95 and gephyrin puncta in dendrites of sparsely transfected excitatory or inhibitory tectal neurons. Excitatory and inhibitory tectal neurons receive excitatory and inhibitory synaptic inputs (Fig. 2c). Both PSD95 and gephyrin immunolabeling are highly punctate, with higher puncta density in the neuropil than the somatic region (Fig. 2d). GFP+ dendritic segments were identified as inhibitory or excitatory by GABA immunolabeling. In both excitatory and inhibitory neurons, GluA2CTP expression reduced the density of both PSD95 and gephyrin puncta (Fig. 2e–h), indicating a decrease in the number of both excitatory and inhibitory synapses onto transfected neurons. GluA1CTP expression decreased both PSD95 and gephyrin puncta in excitatory neurons but not in inhibitory neurons. Given that over 70% of tectal neurons are excitatory20, these results are consistent with decreased mEPSC and mIPSC frequency seen in electrophysiological recordings from randomly recorded neurons, and demonstrate cell-autonomous loss of inhibitory synaptic inputs induced by decreased excitatory input in both excitatory and inhibitory neurons.
Structural plasticity differently altered in E and I neurons
To assess effects of disrupted excitatory synaptic transmission on dendritic arbor development and experience-dependent structural plasticity in excitatory and inhibitory neurons, we performed in vivo time-lapse imaging of GluACTP and GFP-coexpressing individual neurons. Excitatory and inhibitory neurons were identified by post hoc GABA immunolabeling (Fig. 3a, b). Total dendritic branch length (TDBL) and total branch tip number (TBTN) from three-dimensional (3D) reconstructions of the imaged neurons indicated that GluA1CTP and GluA2CTP expression in excitatory neurons significantly decreased branch density, without affecting dendritic arbor branching pattern (Fig. 3c–e). GluA2CTP expression also significantly decreased TBTN in excitatory neurons (Fig. 3c). In inhibitory neurons, GluA2CTP expression significantly increased TDBL, without affecting TBTN or branch density (Fig. 3d). Interestingly, GluA2CTP expression also changed the branching pattern of inhibitory neurons, causing neurons to branch significantly farther from the soma (Fig. 3f), possibly reflecting a compensatory response to decreased excitatory inputs, consistent with observations of activity-dependent redistribution of synapse in the absence of normal activity22.
Previous studies showed that 4 h of short term enhanced visual experience (STVE) increased dendritic arbor growth rate in tectal neurons compared to 4 h in dark, and that GluACTP expression blocks this visual experience-dependent dendritic arbor structural plasticity14. Since these previous studies imaged randomly sampled tectal neurons, the results likely reflect plasticity in excitatory neurons, the majority of tectal neurons. Inhibitory tectal neurons, on the other hand, demonstrate a bimodal experience-dependent plasticity, with an inverse correlation between the valence of plasticity in response to dark and STVE in individual neurons20. Furthermore, inhibitory neurons cluster into two functional groups, one group increases dendritic arbor growth rate in response to STVE and decreases arbor growth rate in the dark, while the other decreases arbor growth rate in STVE and increases it in dark.
To test if the bimodal experience-dependent structural plasticity in inhibitory neurons is affected by disrupting excitatory synaptic inputs, we collected time-lapse images of individual tectal neurons coexpressing GFP and GluA1CTP or GluA2CTP in animals exposed to 4 h of dark followed by 4 h of STVE (Fig. 4a). In excitatory neurons, GluACTP blocked the STVE-induced dendritic arbor plasticity (Control: ΔTDBL, p < 0.01, ΔTBTN, p < 0.05. GluA1CTP:ΔTDBL, p = 0.52, ΔTBTN, p = 0.83. GluA2CTP:ΔTDBL, p = 0.94, ΔTBTN, p = 0.82. Wilcoxon test). Comparing dendritic arbor growth rates over 4 h in STVE and 4 h in dark for individual neurons demonstrated that GluACTP selectively blocked the structural plasticity of excitatory neurons in response to STVE but not in dark (Fig. 4b, c). The pooled population of inhibitory neurons showed no difference in dendritic arbor plasticity between dark and STVE in controls (ΔTDBL, p = 0.38, ΔTBTN, p = 0.59) or GluACTP-expressing neurons (GluA1CTP:ΔTDBL, p = 0.06, ΔTBTN, p = 0.48. GluA2CTP:ΔTDBL, p = 0.96, ΔTBTN, p = 0.92, Wilcoxon test). The magnitude of structural responses to either dark or STVE was not different between GluACTP-expressing and control neurons (Fig. 4d). By contrast, plotting dendritic arbor growth rates over 4 h in STVE versus dark for individual neurons demonstrates that GluACTP expression disrupted the inverse correlation between the valence of structural plasticity in dark and STVE observed in control inhibitory neurons (Fig. 4e).
Application of unsupervised cluster analysis based on ΔTBTN in response to STVE versus dark clustered control inhibitory neurons into two evenly sized subpopulations, called Group I and Group II neurons (Fig. 5a, Group I: n = 14; Group II: n = 16). Group I and Group II neurons displayed experience-dependent structural plasticity with opposite valence, accounting for the lack of experience-dependent plasticity in the pooled population (Fig. 4c). The plasticity profile of Group I inhibitory neurons was similar to excitatory neurons, retracting dendrites in the dark and extending them in STVE. The plasticity profile of Group II inhibitory neurons was the opposite, extending dendrites in the dark and retracting them in STVE (Fig. 5a). We applied the same cluster analysis to GluACTP-expressing inhibitory neurons and assigned the clustered subgroups by the difference between average ΔTBTN in dark and STVE as in control neurons (Group I: ΔTBTN in STVE > ΔTBTN in dark; Group II: ΔTBTN in STVE < ΔTBTN in dark). GluA1CTP-expressing inhibitory neurons clustered into two groups with similar structural plasticity profiles as control neurons: Group I neurons retracted dendrites in dark and extended them in STVE. Group II neurons had opposite plasticity profiles. Both groups showed significant differences between plasticity in dark and STVE (Fig. 5b, Group I: n = 11; Group II: n = 6). By contrast, GluA2CTP-expressing inhibitory neurons clustered into groups that displayed distinct experience-dependent structural plasticity profiles from control neurons: Group I neurons extended dendrites and Group II neurons retracted them in both dark and STVE. Growth rates were not significantly different between dark and STVE in either subgroup (Fig. 5c, Group I: n = 5, p = 0.44; Group II: n = 12, p = 0.41, Wilcoxon sign rank test), and the experience-dependent bimodal structural plasticity in dark and STVE seen in control inhibitory neurons was abolished. In addition, comparison of the structural plasticity in response to dark or STVE between GluACTP-expressing and control inhibitory neurons showed that within both Group I and Group II neurons, GluA2CTP expression significantly altered the plasticity in response to dark, not STVE (Fig. 5d–g). By contrast, the plasticity in response to either dark or STVE in GluA1CTP-expressing inhibitory neurons was not different from controls in either group. These results indicate that GluA2 is of particular importance for the bimodal plasticity in inhibitory neurons. Considering that only GluA2CTP expression significantly decreased PSD95 and gephyrin puncta in inhibitory neurons (Fig. 2g, h), these results again suggest that, unlike excitatory neurons, inhibitory tectal neurons are more sensitive to the disruption of GluA2-mediated AMPAR trafficking. The loss of the bimodal plasticity response indicates that disrupting excitatory synaptic inputs to inhibitory neurons changed their circuit connectivity20.
Using unsupervised cluster analysis, control excitatory neurons cluster into two groups distinguished by their plasticity in the dark (Fig. 6a, Group I: n = 13; Group II: n = 10): Group I excitatory neuron dendrites retract in dark and extend in STVE (n = 13, p < 0.001, Wilcoxon signed rank test). Group II excitatory neuron dendrites typically grow in both dark and STVE and grow significantly more in dark than in STVE (n = 10, p < 0.05). GluA1CTP expression significantly changed the plasticity profile in excitatory neurons: Group I neurons retract dendrites in both dark and STVE and Group II neurons extend dendrites in both dark and STVE. Responses to dark and STVE were not different within each group (Fig. 6b. Group I: n = 4, p = 0.75. Group II: n = 8, p = 0.46, Wilcoxon signed rank test). Interestingly, GluA2CTP expression changed the experience-dependent structural plasticity profile of excitatory neurons to a bimodal pattern resembling that of control inhibitory neurons: half retract dendrites in dark and extend them in STVE, and half have the opposite plasticity profile (Fig. 6c, Group I: n = 8. Group II: n = 8.). Comparison of the structural plasticity between GluACTP-expressing and control excitatory neurons within each group showed that GluA1CTP and GluA2CTP significantly affected the plasticity in response to STVE but not dark (Fig. 6d–g). The observation that decreased excitatory inputs significantly affected the STVE response in excitatory neurons, and the dark response in inhibitory neurons provides further evidence that the plasticity of inhibitory tectal neurons is actively regulated in dark20,23.
Decreased E and I synaptic inputs disrupt RF properties
Interaction of excitatory and inhibitory inputs is thought to be essential for the developmental refinement of visual receptive field (RF) properties1,24,25. Here, we showed that interfering with glutamatergic inputs compromises both excitatory and inhibitory inputs yet E/I remains constant. To test whether RF properties are affected by GluACTP expression, we measured the spatial and temporal RF properties in transfected neurons. We recorded spatial RFs in both cell-attached mode, to measure the spiking receptive fields (sRF), and whole-cell mode, to measure excitatory receptive fields (eRF) and inhibitory receptive fields (iRF) respectively in response to light off visual stimuli (Fig. 7a). sRFs, eRFs, and iRFs were all significantly smaller in GluA1CTP- and GluA2CTP-expressing neurons than controls (Fig. 7b–d). GluACTP expression disrupted the convergence of eRF and iRF as shown by significantly greater distance between the eRFs and iRFs centers (Fig. 7e). We generated temporal receptive field (tRF) maps by binning the number of spikes in 100 ms intervals over the 700 ms recording period following the OFF stimulus (Fig. 7f). The tRFs in control GFP+ neurons were confined to 200 ms following the stimulus (Fig. 7g). By contrast, the tRF in GluA1CTP- and GluA2CTP-expressing neurons extended up to 600 ms after the stimulus. Consequently, the average spike latency and the full width at the half maximal response (FWHM) of the tRFs in GluA1CTP- and GluA2CTP-expressing neurons were significantly greater than controls (Fig. 7h, i). The decreased convergence of eRF and iRF and the increased temporal span of the visually evoked spikes are consistent with a decreased inhibition16,19,24,25. Together, these data indicate that decreasing excitatory synaptic inputs and the subsequent cell autonomous decrease in inhibitory synaptic inputs in tectal neurons disrupted the tectal circuits underlying visual information processing and impaired both spatial and temporal RF properties.
Behavioral impairments caused by GluACTP expression
We next addressed whether GluACTP expression affects visual avoidance behavior and behavioral plasticity. The results described above predict two outcomes of GluACTP expression: on one hand, if constant E/I is sufficient for circuit function underlying behavior, then the co-regulation of inhibitory inputs in response to GluACTP expression and the resultant constant E/I, predicts that visual avoidance behavior would be intact. By contrast, GluACTP expression impaired visual information processing and RF properties, predicting that circuit function and plasticity would be impaired. We bulk-electroporated tecta with GluA1CTP, GluA2CTP, or GFP expression constructs and evaluated visual avoidance behavior and avoidance behavior plasticity. We first tested whether changes in excitatory and inhibitory synaptic inputs occur after bulk electroporation, which results in ~15–30% transfection rate16. We recorded visually evoked eCSCs and iCSCs from GFP+ neurons expressing either GluA1CTP or GluA2CTP and compared them to control neurons expressing GFP alone. As seen above, expressing GluA1CTP or GluA2CTP more broadly in tectal neurons significantly reduced both the eCSC and iCSC in transfected neurons, so E/I remains comparable to controls (Fig. 8a). The visual avoidance behavior is a tectally mediated behavior in which an animal changes swim trajectory in response to an approaching visual stimulus16. Animals improve their avoidance behavior following a visual-conditioning training protocol26. Control tadpoles avoided dots with diameters ranging from 0.2 to 0.6 cm, with the peak avoidance response to dots of 0.4 cm diameters (Fig. 8b, c). GluA1CTP- and GluA2CTP-expressing tadpoles had significantly lower avoidance indices to stimuli of 0.4 cm and 0.2 cm, and GluA2CTP-expressing animals also showed a significantly decreased response to 0.6 cm dots (Fig. 8c), indicating impaired visual behavior in these animals despite of balanced E/I. GluA1CTP and GluA2CTP expression also blocked visual conditioning-mediated plasticity of the behavior (Fig. 8d). This learning deficit is consistent with the compromised experience-dependent structural plasticity and visual information processing observed in individual neurons expressing GluACTPs.
Discussion
Genetic variants of proteins associated with glutamatergic synaptic function, such as CTNAP2 and SHANK3, have been implicated in the etiology of neuropsychiatric disorders, placing excitatory synapse dysfunction in the spotlight as a candidate mechanism underlying pathogenesis of these disorders27. Animal models with these genetic deficits also show reduced inhibitory tone, recapitulating human patient studies28. Some of these neurological disorders are thought to have neurodevelopmental origins, such as ASD and schizophrenia, raising the intriguing questions: Is there a causal link between defective excitatory synaptic function during development and reduced inhibition? And how do deficits in excitatory synaptic function lead to deficits in inhibitory function?
Here we examined the role of glutamatergic excitatory synaptic transmission in the development of inhibition and excitation at synaptic, neuronal, circuit and behavioral levels, focusing on inhibitory synapses and neurons. Very little is known about whether and how E/I is maintained in inhibitory neurons. Interfering with AMPAR-mediated inputs specifically in parvalbumin neurons, either with GluA1 or GluA4 knockout, or by manipulating neuronal pentraxins, delayed inhibitory circuit maturation, blocked cortical plasticity, and resulted in behavioral deficits29–31. We report a coordinated cell-autonomous reduction in synaptic inhibition in response to decreasing glutamatergic transmission by GluACTP expression in individual neurons. The decreased inhibitory input was not only observed in spontaneous activity but also in visually evoked inhibitory synaptic responses, which drastically altered the spatial and temporal visual receptive field properties in transfected tectal neurons. Disrupting excitatory transmission also blocked experience-dependent structural plasticity in both inhibitory and excitatory neurons. Interestingly, the structural plasticity deficit in inhibitory neurons occurred in the response to dark, whereas in excitatory neurons the deficit occurred in response to visual stimulation. These synaptic and cellular defects translated into behavioral deficits when GluACTPs were expressed more extensively in tectal neurons.
GluACTPs contain core regulatory sites for AMPAR trafficking but lack other functional domains. They compete with endogenous AMPARs for binding partners, thereby interfering with AMPAR trafficking into synapses and decreasing excitatory synaptic transmission. Different mechanisms may underlie the disruption of excitatory synaptic inputs by the expression of GluA1CTP and GluA2CTP due to different synaptic delivery mechanisms of GluA1- and GluA2-containing AMPARs12,13. GluA1CTP does not affect basal AMPAR-mediated currents but abolishes activity-dependent synaptic potentiation. GluA2CTP significantly decreases basal AMPAR-mediated synaptic transmission, which produces greater synaptic potentiation in response to LTP-inducing protocols, but due to impaired synaptic delivery of GluA2-containing receptors, this increased synaptic strength is not maintained10. AMPAR trafficking also affects homeostatic plasticity where GluA2CTP blocks inactivation-induced synaptic scaling of excitatory synaptic inputs32. Our data demonstrate cell-type specific differences in the effects of GluA1- and GluA2-CTP. While GluA1- and GluA2-CTP affected excitatory neurons similarly, only GluA2-CTP significantly affected inhibitory neurons, both in terms of synaptic inputs, as well as dendritic morphology and structural plasticity. The differential effects of GluA1- and GluA2-CTP in inhibitory neurons are unlikely to result from their differential expression, because both subunits have equally wide expression in all tectal neurons. Inhibitory neurons may employ a CaMK pathway with much slower kinetics than excitatory neurons in coupling excitatory inputs to downstream regulatory pathways33. Considering the difference in GluA1 and GluA2 gating kinetics34, this may contribute to the different sensitivity of inhibitory neurons to GluA1 expression and GluA2CTP expression. Further investigation will clarify the downstream cell type-specific mechanisms affected by manipulating excitatory input.
Although many studies have examined circuit-wide regulation of E/I balance1,2,4,35,36, whether inhibitory synaptic inputs are regulated cell-autonomously remains unclear37. Knocking down all AMPAR-mediated glutamatergic transmission in a small subset of neurons reduced both mIPSC and evoked IPSCs in hippocampal slice culture, suggesting cell-autonomous regulation of E/I38. Decreasing excitability by potassium channel expression in cortical pyramidal neurons resulted in postsynaptic neuron-specific decrease in inhibitory synaptic inputs from the same presynaptic parvalbumin neurons without affecting excitatory inputs, suggesting cell-autonomous regulation36. However, conflicting results had been reported in cultured neurons that similar potassium channel over-expression induced a homeostatic increase in excitatory synaptic inputs without changing inhibitory synaptic inputs2,39. Our results showing the proportional decrease in excitatory and inhibitory inputs in sparsely transfected GluACTP-expressing neurons provide in vivo evidence that cell-autonomous regulation of inhibitory inputs underlies the maintenance of E/I. We previously reported that decreasing inhibition by expressing a peptide interfering with GABAA receptor trafficking does not affect glutamatergic synaptic inputs onto the same neuron, thereby disrupting E/I and causing dysfunction of the tectal circuit16. These results suggest that cell-autonomous modification of E/I is triggered by a direct change in excitatory synaptic input and not by the net change of excitatory inputs relative to inhibitory inputs.
The cell-autonomous downregulation of inhibition following disruption of excitatory inputs may have important implications for the etiology of some neurological disorders. Loss of function of the autism-related cell adhesion molecule CNTNAP2 in cultured cortical neurons caused a cell-autonomous decrease in both excitatory and inhibitory synaptic input40. CNTNAP2 preferentially co-localizes with GluA1 and knocking it down in cultured neurons led to abnormal cytoplasmic aggregation of GluA1, suggesting a role in AMPAR trafficking41. The dysfunction of inhibitory synaptic transmission observed with CNTNAP2 knockdown may be a secondary consequence of disrupted excitatory transmission due to defective AMPAR trafficking, as we observed here. Another consequence of the coordinated decrease in inhibition induced by decreased excitation is that E/I remained relatively stable. E/I is thought to be critical for neural circuit stability and normal brain function1. Disrupted E/I is associated with several neurological diseases, including epilepsy, schizophrenia, and autism spectrum disorders42–44. Here we showed that even though E/I was resilient to disruption of excitatory inputs, nervous system function was still significantly compromised at both neuronal and circuit levels, shown by abnormal dendritic morphology, altered experience-dependent plasticity, and defective receptive field properties, suggesting that balanced E/I is not sufficient to maintain normal brain function. Mice with MECP2 knockout in forebrain excitatory neurons provide another example of simultaneous reduction in excitation and inhibition resulting in functional deficits despite relatively balanced E/I45. As we showed here, some of the defects in RF properties resulted from an insufficient excitatory drive, such as the decreased RF size and delayed peak response to visual stimulation. Others are typical results of decreased inhibitory inputs, e.g., decreased temporal precision. Both would contribute to the defective spatial and temporal tuning of visual responses and subsequent impairment of experience-dependent plasticity and animal behavior.
Specific cellular mechanisms underlying the cell-autonomous downregulation of inhibition by decreased excitatory inputs are still unclear. One potential mechanism is retrograde signaling through BDNF, which has been shown to regulate formation and maintenance of inhibitory inputs in response to excitatory inputs46. NO has also been proposed as a retrograde messenger that mediates heterosynaptic potentiation of GABAergic synapses47. Previous studies indicate that cell-autonomous regulation of inhibitory synaptic inputs is independent of postsynaptic spiking2. Manipulation of neuronal intrinsic excitability with potassium channels expression suppresses neuronal spiking activity but does not prevent postsynaptic depolarization evoked by excitatory synaptic inputs, which allows local Ca2+ influx and can trigger downstream signaling pathways controlling the formation and maintenance of inhibitory synapses48. When GABAergic currents are hyperpolarizing and AMPARs are the major source of synaptic depolarization, as is the case in our experiments, disrupting AMPAR-mediated excitatory synaptic inputs reduces the depolarization that is crucial for the activation of NMDARs, which has been shown to be obligatory for the development of inhibitory synaptic transmission47,49. Our data provide direct evidence for an essential role for glutamatergic excitatory transmission in the cell autonomous development of GABAergic inhibition and distinguished effects of excitatory inputs on excitatory and inhibitory neurons.
Methods
Animals and transfection
Albino Xenopus laevis embryos were obtained from either in-house mating or from Xen Express (Brooksville, FL), and reared at 22–23 °C with 12 h dark/12 h light cycle in 0.1× Steinberg’s solution (in mM: 58.0 NaCl, 0.67 KCl, 0.34 Ca(NO3)2, 0.83 MgSO4, 3.0 HEPES, pH 7.2). Animals were fed from stage 47. All animal protocols were approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee of the Scripps Research Institute and the local ethics committee of the Hangzhou Normal University. Stage 46–48 animals were anesthetized in 0.02% MS-222 (Tricane methanesulfonate, Sigma, St. Louis, MO), and the tectum was co-electroporated with pGal4 and either UAS::GluA1CTP-T2A-GFP or UAS::GluA2CTP-T2A-GFP. Control animals were transfected with pGal4 and UAS::GFP constructs. Animals from the same batches were randomly assigned to each experimental group. All animals were screened 5–8 days post electroporation for proper level of expression before experiments.
Electrophysiology
All recordings were performed at room temperature (20–22 °C). During the recordings, brains were perfused with extracellular saline containing (in mM: 115 NaCl, 2 KCl, 3 CaCl2, 1.5 MgCl2, 5 HEPES, 10 glucose, 0.01 glycine and 0.05 Tubocurarine, pH 7.2, osmolality 255 mOsm). Visually evoked synaptic currents were recorded from tectal neurons in the middle of the tectum in whole-cell mode using a K+-based pipette solution (in mM: 110 K-gluconate, 8 KCl, 5 NaCl, 1.5 MgCl2, 20 HEPES, 0.5 EGTA, 2 ATP, and 0.3 GTP). Action potentials were recorded in cell-attached mode. Recording micropipettes were pulled from borosilicate glass capillaries and had resistances in the range of 7–9 MΩ. Liquid junction potential was adjusted during recording. Whole-cell recordings were accepted for analysis from cells in which the series resistance did not change over 10% and input resistance (0.7–2 GΩ) remained relatively constant. Signals were filtered at 2 kHz with a Multiclamp 700A amplifier (Molecular Devices, Palo Alto, CA). Data were sampled at 10 kHz using ClampFit 10 (Molecular Devices). Responses, including spikes, eCSCs, and iCSCs to light off stimuli were analyzed by Matlab (The MathWorks, Psychophysics Toolbox extensions).
For whole-cell recordings, tadpoles were anesthetized and tectal lobes were cut along the dorsal midline with a sharp needle. Live tadpoles were immobilized on a sylgard cushion in the recording chamber with one eye facing the center of a back-projection screen. Full-field visual stimuli were generated in Matlab with Physics Toolbox and presented from lowest to highest luminance (10, 20, and 250 cd m−2) from a projector (Samsung, sp-p310ME LED projector) to the back-projection screen. Each stimulus was presented ten times (Frequency: 0.1 Hz, interval: 0.05 Hz). For recording of visually evoked synaptic compound currents in bulk-electroporated animals, tadpoles were transfected with DNA constructs expressing GFP, or GFP with GluR1CTP or GluR2CTP respectively. Animals were screened to select those with dense GFP expression for recordings.
For receptive field mapping, white squares on a dark background were presented in an 8 × 8 grid of 0.5 × 0.5 cm2 non-overlapping squares covering a 4 × 4 cm2 area in the visual field. The entire visual field was mapped by randomly presenting white squares for 1.5 s with 5 s intervals. For spike-receptive field mapping, using cell-attached recordings, the response within 700 ms after the onset of the off stimulus at each grid position was determined as the average number of total spikes per stimulus from two or three repeats. After cell-attached recording, whole-cell voltage clamp recording was accomplished by applying negative pressure. The same visual stimuli were used to measure inhibitory spatial receptive fields and excitatory spatial receptive fields, by holding membrane potential at 0 mV and −60 mV respectively. Total synaptic charge transfer over 700 ms from the onset of stimulus was normalized to the peak response and computed by Matlab to show spatial receptive field size. All values larger than three times the standard deviation of spontaneous activity were included in the measurements of spatial receptive fields. The center of the spatial receptive field was defined as the center of the square that elicited the highest responses (maximum number of spikes).
Time-lapse imaging of dendritic arbor and data analysis
Animals were electroporated with DNA constructs at stage of 45–46 and screened for those with sparsely transfected and well-isolated cells. For imaging, animals were anesthetized with 0.01% MS-222 (Sigma) and were placed in a Sylgard chamber covered by a glass coverslip. Images were collected every 4 h before and after each visual experience session (dark or STVE). Two-photon z-series were collected at 1 μm steps with a 20× water immersion objective (Olympus XLUMPlanFL 0.95NA) at 3–4× scan zoom using a custom-built microscope modified from an Olympus FV300 system20.
Complete dendritic arbors of each neuron were reconstructed using a semi-manual function in the Filament module of Imaris (Bitplane, US). Total dendritic length and TBTN were automatically calculated by the software. 3D Sholl analysis calculated the number of branches that intersect concentric circles at increasing distances from the cell soma, using a customized Matlab program with reconstructed filament data exported from Imaris. The dendritic structural data of the control group (Figs. 3–6) was a subset of a previously reported data set[20]. These neurons were collected and processed in parallel with the GluACTP-expressing experimental groups.
Immunohistochemistry and image data analysis
For post hoc analysis of neurons imaged for in vivo time-lapse experiments, animals were fixed at the end of the imaging experiment with freshly made 4% paraformaldehyde and 2% glutaraldehyde (Electron Microscopy Sciences, Fort Washington, PA) in 1× PBS (pH 7.4) using a Pelco BioWave Pro microwave (Model 36500, Ted Pella, Redding, CA. 350 mV on 20 s, off 20 s, on 20 s, followed by 150 mV on 1 min, off 1 min, on 1 min). The animals were then post fixed at 4 °C overnight and washed in 1× PBS using the microwave (150 mV on–off–on, 1 min each). In total, 30 µm vibratome sections of the dissected brains were cut for free-floating immunofluorescence labeling. Sections were incubated in 1% sodium borohydride (Sigma) in 1× PBS for 15 min to quench autofluorescence, blocked in 10% normal goat serum (Jackson Lab, ME) in PBS with 2% Triton X-100 (PBST) for 1 h in room temperature, followed by incubation in rabbit anti GABA polyclonal antibody (Sigma A2052, RRID: AB_477652, 1:2000 in PBST with 1% normal goat serum) for 48 h at 4 °C. Secondary antibody (goat anti rabbit Alexa Fluor 633, Thermo Fisher Scientific, Cat# A-21070, RRID: AB_2535731) was diluted 1:500 in PBST and incubated for an hour at room temperature. After 3 × 15 min rinses with PBS, sections were mounted on slides in Vectashield Mounting Medium with DAPI (Vector Laboratories, Burlingame, CA). For immunolabeling with GluA antibodies, stage 47 animals were fixed in 4% PFA and 0.5% glutaraldehyde. Primary antibodies used include mouse anti GluA2 (N) antibody (1:200, Millipore, Cat# MAB397, RRID: AB_2113875) and mouse anti GluA1-N antibody (1:500, generous gifts from Dr. R. Huganir, Johns Hopkins University Medical School). For PSD95 and gephyrin puncta analysis, 7 days after electroporation of the corresponding DNA constructs, animals were fixed with 4% PFA with 1 h post fixation at room temperature. Free-floating vibratome sections were quenched and blocked as mentioned above, followed by incubation in the primary antibodies including rabbit-anti-PSD95 polyclonal antibody (1:50, Thermo Fisher Scientific, Cat# 51-6900, RRID: AB_2533914), mouse-anti-gephyrin monoclonal antibody (1:50, Synaptic Systems, Cat# 147021, RRID: AB_2232546), guinea pig-anti-GABA polyclonal antibody (1:1500, Millipore, Cat# AB175, RRID: AB_91011). Secondary antibodies used include goat-anti-guinea pig Alexa Fluor 568 (Thermo Fisher Scientific, Cat# A-11075, RRID: AB_141954), goat-anti-rabbit BV421 (BD Biosciences Pharmingen, Cat# 565014, RRID: AB_2716308), and donkey-anti-mouse Alexa Fluor 647 (Thermo Fisher Scientific Cat# A-31571, RRID: AB_162542). Brains from control and experimental groups were embedded in the same blocks and processed under exactly same conditions throughout the experiments.
Images of immunolabeled sections for post hoc identification of GABAergic neurons for the in vivo time-lapse imaging experiments were collected on an Olympus Fluoview 500 confocal microscope or a Nikon C2 confocal microscope. GFP fluorescence signal was well preserved in the fixed tissue, thus no antibody was needed for visualization. The GFP+ imaged neurons were located using a 20× air objective and confirmed by location and dendritic arbor branching patterns. Higher magnification images were then taken at >3 different z-depth through the soma to examine GABA immunoreactivity. Samples with poor GABA immunostaining were not included in the analysis.
Images of PSD95 and Gephyrin immunolabeling were acquired on a Nikon C2 confocal microscope with a 40× PlanFluor Oil objective (N.A. 1.3) at 2048 × 2048 to achieve a final resolution of 0.15 μm pixel−1. Analysis of dendritic puncta density was performed using SynPAnal program50. Transfected dendritic segments of control or experimental groups were defined solely based on the GFP signal. The dendritic segment was deemed inhibitory if the average GABA immunolabeling intensity within the segment was above a threshold set by mean + 2 SD of GABA immunolabeling intensity of three randomly drawn GFP-segments of similar length and width in the neuropil region within the same section adjacent to the transfected GFP+ dendritic segment. In a subset of dendritic segments (n = 496), for which the soma could be identified, 82.5% showed GABA immunoreactivity consistent with their soma, confirming the reliability of identifying GABAergic inhibitory dendritic segments based on GABA immunoreactivity within the segments. Puncta density values (per unit length of dendrite) of PSD95 and Gephyrin puncta within the transfected dendritic segments were automatically taken by the program and were normalized to the average puncta density of the neuropil regions within the same section to control for immunostaining variability across sections. All image analysis was done blind to experimental conditions.
Visual avoidance assay and visual conditioning
The visual avoidance assay was conducted as previously described16. Tadpoles were placed in an 8 × 3 cm tank filled to a depth of ~1 cm with Steinberg’s rearing solution. Visual stimuli were presented to a back-projection screen on the bottom of the chamber using a microprojector (3 M, MPro110). Videos of tadpoles illuminated by IR LEDs were recorded with a Hamamatsu ORCA-ER digital camera. Visual stimuli were generated and presented by MATLAB. Randomly positioned moving spots of 0.04, 0.2, 0.4, and 0.6 cm diameter were presented in random order for 60 s. Visual avoidance behavior was scored as a change in the swimming trajectory in the first ten encounters between each tadpole and moving spots (the percentage of avoidance responses out of 10 encounters, plotted as avoidance index). For visual conditioning, animals were exposed to moving bars (1 cm width; 0.3 Hz; Luminance: 25 cd m−2) continuously for 2 or 4 h, or for three repeats of 5 min of moving bars interleaved by 5-min blank, for a total of 30 min, as described26.
Cluster analysis and statistical tests
Custer analysis was performed based on ΔTBTN over STVE versus dark of individual neurons using an unsupervised agglomerative hierarchical tree method in MATLAB (linkage.m) based upon their pair-wise vectorial distance in the constructed 2D space (pdist.m)20.
All data are presented as mean ± s.e.m. Data are considered significantly different when p values are less than 0.05. Where noted, either two-tailed Student’s t test or nonparametric Wilcoxon sign rank test was performed for within-cell comparison. For comparisons of multiple groups, either ANOVA with Newman–Keuls test or Kruskal–Wallis test with post hoc Mann–Whitney U test were performed. The statistical test used for each experiment is specified in the results. Sample size for each experiment was confirmed with power test using the program G*power.
Experiments and analysis were performed blind to the experimental conditions.
Data availability
The data sets generated during and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding authors.
Acknowledgements
We thank Dr. Richard Huganir for sharing the GluA1 antibody and Dr. Eric Danielson for providing the SynPAnal puncta analysis software. We thank Dr. Daniel Choquet, Dr. Anton Maximov, Dr. Richard Huganir and members of the Cline lab for critical discussions. This work was supported by the NEI (EY011261 and EY027437) and the Hahn Family Foundation to H.T.C., and National Nature Sciences Foundation of China (NSFC 31271176) and Zhejiang Provincial Natural Science Foundation of China (LY17C090007) to W.S.
Author contributions
Designed research: H.-y.H., W.S. and H.T.C., Performed research: H.-y.H., W.S., L.Z. and X.G., Contributed analytic tools: H.-y.H. and W.S., Analyzed data: H.-y.H., W.S. and H.T.C., Wrote the paper: H.-y.H., W.S. and H.T.C.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
These authors contributed equally: Hai-yan He, Wanhua Shen.
Publisher's note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Contributor Information
Wanhua Shen, Email: shen@idrbio.org.
Hollis T. Cline, Email: cline@scripps.edu
References
- 1.Froemke RC. Plasticity of cortical excitatory-inhibitory balance. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2015;38:195–219. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-071714-034002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Hartman KN, Pal SK, Burrone J, Murthy VN. Activity-dependent regulation of inhibitory synaptic transmission in hippocampal neurons. Nat. Neurosci. 2006;9:642–649. doi: 10.1038/nn1677. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Huang ZJ, Di Cristo G, Ango F. Development of GABA innervation in the cerebral and cerebellar cortices. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2007;8:673–686. doi: 10.1038/nrn2188. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Jiang B, Huang ZJ, Morales B, Kirkwood A. Maturation of GABAergic transmission and the timing of plasticity in visual cortex. Brain Res. Brain Res. Rev. 2005;50:126–133. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2005.05.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Liu Y, Zhang LI, Tao HW. Heterosynaptic scaling of developing GABAergic synapses: dependence on glutamatergic input and developmental stage. J. Neurosci. 2007;27:5301–5312. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0376-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Pieraut S, et al. Experience-dependent remodeling of basket cell networks in the dentate gyrus. Neuron. 2014;84:107–122. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.09.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Wamsley B, Fishell G. Genetic and activity-dependent mechanisms underlying interneuron diversity. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2017;18:299–309. doi: 10.1038/nrn.2017.30. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Akgul G, McBain CJ. Diverse roles for ionotropic glutamate receptors on inhibitory interneurons in developing and adult brain. J. Physiol. 2016;594:5471–5490. doi: 10.1113/JP271764. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Bredt DS, Nicoll RA. AMPA receptor trafficking at excitatory synapses. Neuron. 2003;40:361–379. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00640-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Kessels HW, Malinow R. Synaptic AMPA receptor plasticity and behavior. Neuron. 2009;61:340–350. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.01.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Wu G, Malinow R, Cline HT. Maturation of a central glutamatergic synapse. Science. 1996;274:972–976. doi: 10.1126/science.274.5289.972. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Zhou Z, et al. The C-terminal tails of endogenous GluA1 and GluA2 differentially contribute to hippocampal synaptic plasticity and learning. Nat. Neurosci. 2018;21:50–62. doi: 10.1038/s41593-017-0030-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Shi S, Hayashi Y, Esteban JA, Malinow R. Subunit-specific rules governing AMPA receptor trafficking to synapses in hippocampal pyramidal neurons. Cell. 2001;105:331–343. doi: 10.1016/S0092-8674(01)00321-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Haas K, Li J, Cline HT. AMPA receptors regulate experience-dependent dendritic arbor growth in vivo. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2006;103:12127–12131. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0602670103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Yoon BJ, Smith GB, Heynen AJ, Neve RL, Bear MF. Essential role for a long-term depression mechanism in ocular dominance plasticity. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2009;106:9860–9865. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0901305106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Shen W, McKeown CR, Demas JA, Cline HT. Inhibition to excitation ratio regulates visual system responses and behavior in vivo. J. Neurophysiol. 2011;106:2285–2302. doi: 10.1152/jn.00641.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Rumpel S, LeDoux J, Zador A, Malinow R. Postsynaptic receptor trafficking underlying a form of associative learning. Science. 2005;308:83–88. doi: 10.1126/science.1103944. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Akerman CJ, Cline HT. Refining the roles of GABAergic signaling during neural circuit formation. Trends Neurosci. 2007;30:382–389. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2007.06.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Khakhalin AS, Koren D, Gu J, Xu H, Aizenman CD. Excitation and inhibition in recurrent networks mediate collision avoidance in Xenopus tadpoles. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2014;40:2948–2962. doi: 10.1111/ejn.12664. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.He HY, Shen W, Hiramoto M, Cline HT. Experience-dependent bimodal plasticity of inhibitory neurons in early development. Neuron. 2016;90:1203–1214. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.04.044. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Aizenman CD, Munoz-Elias G, Cline HT. Visually driven modulation of glutamatergic synaptic transmission is mediated by the regulation of intracellular polyamines. Neuron. 2002;34:623–634. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(02)00674-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Chen Y, et al. Activity-induced Nr4a1 regulates spine density and distribution pattern of excitatory synapses in pyramidal neurons. Neuron. 2014;83:431–443. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.05.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Gambrill AC, Faulkner R, Cline HT. Experience-dependent plasticity of excitatory and inhibitory intertectal inputs in Xenopus tadpoles. J. Neurophysiol. 2016;00611:02016. doi: 10.1152/jn.00611.2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Richards BA, Voss OP, Akerman CJ. GABAergic circuits control stimulus-instructed receptive field development in the optic tectum. Nat. Neurosci. 2010;13:1098–1106. doi: 10.1038/nn.2612. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Tao HW, Poo MM. Activity-dependent matching of excitatory and inhibitory inputs during refinement of visual receptive fields. Neuron. 2005;45:829–836. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.01.046. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Shen W, et al. Acute synthesis of CPEB is required for plasticity of visual avoidance behavior in Xenopus. Cell Rep. 2014;6:737–747. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2014.01.024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Volk L, Chiu SL, Sharma K, Huganir RL. Glutamate synapses in human cognitive disorders. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2015;38:127–149. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-071714-033821. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Rapanelli M, Frick LR, Pittenger C. The role of interneurons in autism and tourette syndrome. Trends Neurosci. 2017;40:397–407. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2017.05.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Fuchs EC, et al. Recruitment of parvalbumin-positive interneurons determines hippocampal function and associated behavior. Neuron. 2007;53:591–604. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.01.031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Gu Y, et al. Obligatory role for the immediate early gene NARP in critical period plasticity. Neuron. 2013;79:335–346. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.05.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Pelkey KA, et al. Pentraxins coordinate excitatory synapse maturation and circuit integration of parvalbumin interneurons. Neuron. 2015;85:1257–1272. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.02.020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Gainey MA, Hurvitz-Wolff JR, Lambo ME, Turrigiano GG. Synaptic scaling requires the GluR2 subunit of the AMPA receptor. J. Neurosci. 2009;29:6479–6489. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3753-08.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Cohen SM, et al. Excitation-transcription coupling in parvalbumin-positive interneurons employs a novel CaM Kinase-dependent pathway distinct from excitatory neurons. Neuron. 2016;90:292–307. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.03.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Geiger JR, et al. Relative abundance of subunit mRNAs determines gating and Ca2+permeability of AMPA receptors in principal neurons and interneurons in rat CNS. Neuron. 1995;15:193–204. doi: 10.1016/0896-6273(95)90076-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Chen JL, et al. Clustered dynamics of inhibitory synapses and dendritic spines in the adult neocortex. Neuron. 2012;74:361–373. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.02.030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Xue M, Atallah BV, Scanziani M. Equalizing excitation-inhibition ratios across visual cortical neurons. Nature. 2014;511:596–600. doi: 10.1038/nature13321. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Joseph A, Turrigiano GG. All for one but not one for all: excitatory synaptic scaling and intrinsic excitability are coregulated by CaMKIV, whereas inhibitory synaptic scaling is under independent control. J. Neurosci. 2017;37:6778–6785. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0618-17.2017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Lu W, Bushong EA, Shih TP, Ellisman MH, Nicoll RA. The cell-autonomous role of excitatory synaptic transmission in the regulation of neuronal structure and function. Neuron. 2013;78:433–439. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.02.030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Peng YR, et al. Postsynaptic spiking homeostatically induces cell-autonomous regulation of inhibitory inputs via retrograde signaling. J. Neurosci. 2010;30:16220–16231. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3085-10.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Anderson GR, et al. Candidate autism gene screen identifies critical role for cell-adhesion molecule CASPR2 in dendritic arborization and spine development. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2012;109:18120–18125. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1216398109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Varea O, et al. Synaptic abnormalities and cytoplasmic glutamate receptor aggregates in contactin associated protein-like 2/Caspr2 knockout neurons. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2015;112:6176–6181. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1423205112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Fritschy JM. Epilepsy E/I balance and GABA(A) receptor plasticity. Front. Mol. Neurosci. 2008;1:5. doi: 10.3389/neuro.02.005.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Lewis DA, Hashimoto T, Volk DW. Cortical inhibitory neurons and schizophrenia. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2005;6:312–324. doi: 10.1038/nrn1648. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Nelson SB, Valakh V. Excitatory/inhibitory balance and circuit homeostasis in autism spectrum disorders. Neuron. 2015;87:684–698. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.07.033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Banerjee A, et al. Jointly reduced inhibition and excitation underlies circuit-wide changes in cortical processing in Rett syndrome. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2016;113:E7287–E7296. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1615330113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Lu B, Wang KH, Nose A. Molecular mechanisms underlying neural circuit formation. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2009;19:162–167. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2009.04.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Nugent FS, Penick EC, Kauer JA. Opioids block long-term potentiation of inhibitory synapses. Nature. 2007;446:1086–1090. doi: 10.1038/nature05726. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Bloodgood BL, Sabatini BL. Ca(2+) signaling in dendritic spines. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2007;17:345–351. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2007.04.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Gu X, Zhou L, Lu W. An NMDA receptor-dependent mechanism underlies inhibitory synapse development. Cell Rep. 2016;14:471–478. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2015.12.061. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Danielson E, Lee SH. SynPAnal: software for rapid quantification of the density and intensity of protein puncta from fluorescence microscopy images of neurons. PLoS ONE. 2014;9:e115298. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0115298. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
The data sets generated during and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding authors.