Abstract
Traditionally, screening research tests how well a given symptom inventory can identify a concurrent depressive episode. Although developmental psychopathology could inform screening protocols for a myriad of depression outcomes (e.g., prospective depressive episodes), approaches typically used in research make it difficult to translate these findings. Using a translational analytic approach and multiwave longitudinal study design, we examined how screening for cognitive vulnerabilities (rumination, dysfunctional attitudes, and attributional style) may improve our ability to identify concurrent depressive episodes, prospective depressive episodes, first lifetime episodes of depression, and recurrent major depressive episodes. There were 473 sixth-grade (early adolescents) and ninth-grade (middle adolescents; AgeM = 13.15, AgeSD = 1.62) students who completed baseline self-report cognitive vulnerability and depressive symptom measures. At baseline and every 6 months for 3 years, pediatric depression interviews were completed by the caregiver and youth. A receiver operating characteristic (ROC) approach was utilized to test our aims. Distinct algorithms best forecasted our depression outcomes. Rumination and attributional style emerged as unique and incrementally valid predictors for prospective episodes after controlling for baseline depressive symptoms. Rumination was the only unique predictor for first lifetime depressive episodes. For recurrent major depression, rumination in early adolescence and attributional style in middle adolescence served as incremental predictors beyond baseline depressive symptoms. Proposed cutoffs and diagnostic likelihood ratios are offered for algorithms for each depression outcome. Assessing cognitive vulnerability represents a feasible method to improve depression screening initiatives. Using an ROC-informed approach can help prevention initiatives better leverage the considerable gains made within developmental psychopathology research.
Developmental psychopathology aims to understand the multifaceted processes that contribute to the onset and maintenance of psychological distress (Cicchetti & Toth, 2009). The discipline plays a key role in the formation of evidence-based mental health services by providing an empirical road map to the processes that need to be targeted by clinical protocols (Garber, Korelitz, & Samanez-Larkin, 2012; Ialongo et al., 2006). Yet, despite this translational promise, most screening protocols do not assess vulnerabilities for psychological distress and predominantly focus on mental health symptoms (Lavigne, Meyers, & Feldman, 2016; Wissow et al., 2013). Broadening our protocols to include risk factors for psychopathology could improve prognostic models for pediatric mental health issues.
The goal of the present study was to help develop a feasible, multi-indicator approach to adolescent depression screening. Because of the prevalence, chronicity, and consequences of pediatric depression, routine depression screening is recommended by age 12 (Forman-Hoffman et al., 2016). These screening programs are tasked with not only identifying concurrent distress and functioning but also forecasting future depression risk. However, a paucity of applied studies assess prospective outcomes (Wissow et al., 2013), and basic research studies tend to use methods and analytical plans that make their findings challenging to translate into clinical settings (Youngstrom, 2014; Youngstrom et al., 2017). We sought to address this gap in the literature by explicitly examining the clinical utility and incremental validity of cognitive vulnerability measures, beyond traditional depressive symptom inventories, when forecasting depression outcomes. Specifically we examined how well our screening solutions performed when estimating risk for concurrent and prospective episode onset, as well as first lifetime episodes of depression and recurrent major depressive episodes, two clinically relevant depression outcomes (Monroe & Harkness, 2011; Petit, Hartley, Lewinsohn, Seeley, & Klein, 2013). To our knowledge, this represents the first study to model an approach for how cognitive vulnerabilities can be used in an applied context to simultaneously target multiple concurrent and prospective depression outcomes.
A Risk Factor Approach to Depression Screening
Multi-indicator screening approaches, comprising presenting symptoms, psychosocial correlates, and underlying vulnerabilities, exist for a myriad of chronic, pediatric conditions. For forecasting pediatric obesity, screening protocols often depend on multiple sources of data including body mass index, blood pressure, lipids, and self-reported physical activity (Smith, Skow, Bodurtha, & Kinra, 2013). Evidence-based screening protocols for dental cavities involve a physical exam, along with familial and environmental predictors of oral health care (Divaris, 2016). More proximal to mental health, large screening initiatives (i.e., the PROMIS initiative; Irwin et al., 2010) assess social health in youth. These measures were originally used to identify youth with social impairment secondary to a chronic disease (Varni et al., 2014); however, the measure can also help identify youth at risk for psychological distress (DeWalt et al., 2015). We therefore situate our study’s aims within the context of a larger health movement that simultaneously examines manifest symptoms and underlying risk at the screening stage.
Multiple informants or inventories (De Los Reyes et al., 2015; Lavigne et al., 2016; Mash & Hunsley, 2005) have typically been used to improve pediatric mental health screening and assessment initiatives. Although these approaches better discriminate between positive and negative diagnoses by providing multiple perspectives on depression presentations, they are still largely dependent on the manifestation of concurrent symptoms and may be limited in predicting prospective depression episodes. To date, few studies have explicitly examined incremental validity of multi-informant depression assessment procedures (Johnston & Murray, 2003), let alone investigated vulnerabilities within a translational science context. Seely, Stice and Rohde (2009) found that poor academic functioning improved our ability to predict prospective depressive episodes in female adolescents beyond self-reported depressive symptoms. Meanwhile, Cohen and colleagues (Cohen et al., 2016) found that predisaster mental health symptoms and trauma exposure best predicted postdisaster adolescent depression. We build on these studies by focusing on cognitive vulnerabilities, as they (a) are valid risk factors for adolescent depression (Cohen et al., 2017; Hankin, Snyder, & Gulley, 2016; Ingram, Miranda, & Segal, 2006), (b) can easily be screened for in applied settings, and (c) are targeted by cognitive-behavioral preventative interventions (Garber et al., 2012).
Cognitive Vulnerability for Adolescent Depression
Depressogenic cognitive vulnerabilities are defined as a stylistic, enduring way of thinking that precede the emergence of depression (Ingram et al., 2006). Early adolescence represents a critical period for these vulnerabilities to develop (Jacobs, Reinecke, Gollan, & Kane, 2008), which helps explain the heightened sensitivity to depression during this developmental epoch, especially for girls (Hankin & Abramson, 2001). Over the years, several theoretical models have articulated specific examples of these cognitive styles. The hopelessness theory of depression posits that stable and global depressogenic inferences about the cause of a negative event (i.e., attributional style), consequences of a negative event, and the implications of the event for oneself lead to the development of hopelessness and subsequent depression (Abramson, Metalsky, & Alloy, 1989). Although certain depressogenic inferential styles begin forming in childhood (e.g., depressogenic inferential styles about consequences; Cohen, Young, & Abela, 2012), evidence shows that these cognitive risks stabilize into relatively traitlike vulnerabilities and reliably predict later depression by early adolescence (Carter-Smith & Garber, 2011; Cole et al., 2008). Another cognitive vulnerability model is Beck’s (1983) theory of depression, which posits the role of a negative cognitive triad consisting of dysfunctional attitudes concerning the self, the world, and the future. Elevated levels of dysfunctional attitudes predict prospective depressive symptoms in adolescent samples (Abela & Sullivan, 2003). Finally, the response styles theory (Nolen-Hoeksema, 1991) highlights the influence of rumination in contributing to depression. Rumination involves dwelling on the potential meaning, causes, and consequences of one’s problems, concerns, or symptoms of distress. In adolescence, rumination is associated with depression episode onset and longer depressive episodes (Abela & Hankin, 2011).
Applied Developmental Psychopathology
The aforementioned studies were based on multiwave, prospective studies, the ideal approach for demonstrating the longitudinal associations between vulnerabilities and prospective outcomes. Accompanying these study designs were complex analytic plans, which utilized hierarchical linear (Singer & Willett, 2003) or structural equation modeling (Cheong, MacKinnon, & Khoo, 2003) to test the depressogenic influence of these vulnerabilities over time. Although these statistical methods are important to adequately test developmental theories of psychopathology, the intention of these analyses is not to aid clinical decision making (Hunsley & Meyer, 2003; Youngstrom, 2014). For these findings to reach their translational promise, a receiver operating characteristic (ROC) approach may be necessary.
The ROC curve is a representative plot of the true positive rate against the false positive rate across a continuum of scores, allowing one to calculate the sensitivity (i.e., the ability to correctly identify a true positive) and specificity (i.e., the ability to correctly identify a true negative) for specific cutoff points on an index test. In addition to the ROC curve, diagnostic likelihood ratios (Straus, Richardson, Glasziou, & Haynes, 2011) can help estimate the likelihood of whether an individual is presenting with, or will develop, the target disorder based on his or her scoring profile. Knowing the posterior probability of developing a target disorder allows providers and institutions to conduct their own cost–benefit analysis when making referral decisions based on screening profiles. Utilizing the ROC curve together with diagnostic likelihood ratios is viewed as a “best practice” for determining the clinical utility of a potential index test for a pediatric mental health disorder (Youngstrom, 2014; Youngstrom et al., 2017). To date, ROC approaches have been used to evaluate assessments for pediatric anxiety disorders (Van Meter et al., 2014), bipolar disorder (Youngstrom, Genzlinger et al., 2015), attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (Jarrett, Van Meter, Youngstrom, Hilton, & Ollendick, 2016) and posttraumatic stress disorder (You, Youngstrom, Feeny, Youngstrom, & Findling, 2017). However, few studies have explicitly used this approach to assess the incremental validity of including risk factors, as well as symptom measures, for predicting pediatric mental health disorders (see Cohen et al., 2016; Danielson et al., 2017, for exceptions).
In addition to the analytic plans used in developmental psychopathology, the methods typically used in this research may also inhibit the translation of these findings into applied settings. First, the majority of studies examine cognitive vulnerability in isolation (Hankin et al., 2016), preventing inferences to be made about which cognitive style may offer the strongest signal for depression risk. Examining multiple cognitive vulnerabilities simultaneously can help determine which indicators should be prioritized at the screening stage. Second, most studies concerning adolescent cognitive vulnerability have used depression symptom measures as their main outcome (Cohen et al., 2017; Hankin et al., 2016). Although youth depression is dimensionally structured at the latent level (Hankin et al., 2017), depression diagnoses still serve a critical function within the realm of clinical decision making and are recommended to use in tests of incremental validity for clinical screening and assessment protocols (Garb, 2003; Youngstrom, 2014). Important to note, the analytic approach just described can still capture dimensional nuances of depression by estimating both subthreshold and threshold risk outcomes.
Concurrent Depression, Prospective Depression, First Episodes, and Recurrent Depression
All depression outcomes are not created equal. The link between cognitive vulnerability and depression risk can vary based on past and current experiences with depression (Alloy et al., 2006). Relatedly, other findings suggest that risk profiles may differ for specific depression outcomes. For instance, first lifetime episodes of depression (FLED) may be more sensitive to environmental stressors, with subsequent episodes being triggered more by intrapersonal processes (Monroe & Harkness, 2011). To attenuate the elevated prevalence rates in adolescence, greater attention should be paid to FLED in prevention research (Allen, Hetrick, Simmons, & Hickie, 2007). Similarly, unique risk factors for a chronic pattern of depression (i.e., recurrent major depressive disorder [rMDD]) may exist (Monroe & Harkness, 2011; Petit et al., 2013). These findings have led some to suggest that prevention protocols need to develop separate screening solutions to target individuals prone to a persistent depression course (Hill, Yaroslavsky, & Pettit, 2015). Given that the risk profile for depression may differ for these specific depression outcomes, our algorithms for interpreting cognitive vulnerability measures within a screening protocol may vary when attempting to predict a concurrent or prospective episode, FLED, or rMDD. Distinguishing between concurrent and prospective outcomes, as well as more nuanced FLED and rMDD outcomes, can lead to a clearer understanding of the incremental validity of cognitive vulnerability measures by using a more homogeneous depression criterion.
The present study aimed to identify (a) the incremental validity of cognitive vulnerabilities in forecasting depression; (b) delineating which styles confer the greatest risk; and (c) optimizing screening solutions for concurrent episodes, prospective episodes, FLED, and rMDD. Given well-documented sex and age differences for depression onset and risk (Hankin & Abramson, 2001), we examined how these solutions may vary for girls versus boys and for early adolescents versus middle adolescents. By simultaneously comparing different cognitive vulnerability measures, assessing across four clinically significant depression outcomes, and applying a state-of-the-science translational analytical approach, we sought to create cognitive vulnerability-based algorithms that could improve empirically based clinical decision making for depression prevention at the screening stage.
METHOD
Participants and Procedures
A multisite sample drawn from urban and suburban communities was recruited for the present study. Participating families responded to letters sent home by local schools describing a longitudinal study focused on different predictors of emotional and behavioral well-being in youth. At baseline, the sample consisted of 473 youth who were in the sixth grade (AgeM = 11.75, AgeSD = 0.70) and ninth grade (AgeM = 14.65, AgeSD = 0.60). Adolescents were eligible for the study if parents reported during a telephone screen that their child was in the sixth or ninth grade at baseline; was fluent in English; and did not have an autism spectrum diagnosis, psychotic disorder, or intellectual disability. In our study, sixth-grade students were considered early adolescents and ninth-grade students were referred to as middle adolescents. Youth were relatively balanced with regard to sex (female = 57%) and grade (sixth = 52%). The study’s racial/ethnic composition was as follows: White = 61%, African American = 12%, Asian American = 9%, Hispanic = 7%, which is comparable to the ethnic and racial characteristics of the United States, with the exception of fewer Hispanic participants (see Hankin et al., 2015, for further details).
The caretaker and youth visited the laboratory for an in-person, in-depth assessment at baseline. Youth completed measures of cognitive vulnerability and depressive symptoms. Diagnostic interviews were conducted with both the adolescent and caregiver using a semistructured diagnostic interview for youth lifetime and current depressive episodes. Subsequently, for the next 3 years, youth and caregivers were interviewed every 6 months to ascertain whether the adolescent had a depressive episode onset in the preceding 6 months. These frequent assessments limit biases with retrospective recall (Compton & Lopez, 2014; Costello & Erkanli, 2006). In accordance with the Standards for Reporting of Diagnostic Accuracy Group statement (Bossuyt et al., 2003), diagnosticians were not privy to the scores on our indicators. Retention rate from baseline to 36-month follow-up for the study was 93%. Caretakers provided informed written consent for their own and their child’s participation; youth provided written assent. The Institutional Review Boards at both study sites approved all procedures.
Measures
Depression Diagnoses
Trained interviewers administered the Mood Disorders section of the Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for School-Age Children (K-SADS-PL; Kaufman et al., 1997) to youth and caretakers to assess for pediatric depression at baseline and each follow-up. At baseline, youth and caregivers reported lifetime history of depression and current diagnostic status, whereas at each follow-up, families reported on episode occurrence over the preceding 6 months. Licensed clinical psychologists trained the interviewers to conduct the diagnostic interviews. Both interviews informed youths’ diagnostic status using best estimate diagnostic procedures (Klein, Dougherty, & Olino, 2005). Discrepancies between parent and child reports were resolved during weekly supervision meetings with a licensed clinical psychologist based on the quality of the diagnostic report (e.g., behavioral specific examples were provided for a given symptom) and the empirical literature (e.g., self-report may be better for the internalizing aspects of adolescent depression; De Los Reyes et al., 2015). Diagnostic interview interrater reliability was good (K = .91) based on approximately 20% of reviewed interviews. Youth were diagnosed with a depressive episode if they met Diagnostic and Statistical Manual for Mental Disorders (4th ed.; American Psychiatric Association, 1994) criteria for major depressive disorder–definite, major depressive disorder–probable (four depressive symptoms for at least 2 weeks), or minor depressive disorder–definite (two or three threshold depressive symptoms for at least 2 weeks).
In the present study, four binary depression outcomes were calculated from the Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for School-Age Children: (a) concurrent episodes (i.e., presenting with a depressive episode at baseline), (b) prospective episodes (i.e., a depressive episode during the follow-up period), (c) FLED (i.e., at least one episode during the study with no prior lifetime diagnosis reported at baseline), and (d) rMDD (i.e., multiple depressive episodes during the course of the study or a lifetime history of depression reported at baseline and at least one depressive episode reported during the study).
The Children’s Depression Inventory
The Children’s Depression Inventory (CDI; Kovacs, 1992), a self-report 27-item questionnaire, assessed pediatric depressive symptoms. The CDI was chosen because it is the most commonly used measure of youth depression (Myers & Winters, 2002) and a recommended measure for assessing depression in applied settings (Klein et al., 2005). For the present study, the CDI ranged from 0 to 35 (M = 7.08, SD = 5.87 at baseline) and demonstrated adequate reliability (α = 0.84).
Children’s Response Style Questionnaire–Rumination Subscale
The Children’s Response Style Questionnaire–Rumination Subscale (CRSQ-R; Abela, Vanderbilt, & Rochon, 2004) is modeled after Nolen-Hoeksema’s Response Style Questionnaire (Nolen-Hoeksema & Morrow, 1991). The CRSQ-R is a 13-item self-report measure that assesses one’s tendency to ruminate or focus on negative aspects of oneself. For each item, youth indicate how often they respond in a ruminative way when feeling sad, with higher scores indicating a greater tendency to ruminate. The CRSQ-R is a reliable and valid measure of rumination in youth samples (Abela & Hankin, 2011). The CRSQ-R had a Cronbach’s alpha level of 0.79 in the present study.
Adolescent Cognitive Style Questionnaire
The Adolescent Cognitive Style Questionnaire (ACSQ; Hankin & Abramson, 2002) is a self-report inventory that measures inferences about cause, consequence, and oneself, as featured in hopelessness theory. The ACSQ presents the adolescent with negative hypothetical events in achievement and interpersonal domains and asks the youth to make inferences about the causes (internal–external, stable–unstable, and global–specific) and consequences of the event and the characteristics about the self based on the hypothetical event. Each item dimension is rated from 1 to 7, with higher scores indicating a more negative cognitive style. The ACSQ has demonstrated excellent internal consistency, reliability, good test–retest reliability, and a factor structure consistent with hopelessness theory (Hankin & Abramson, 2002). Internal reliability in this sample was 0.92.
Children’s Dysfunctional Attitude Scale
The Children’s Dysfunctional Attitude Scale (CDAS; Abela & Sullivan, 2003) is a questionnaire designed to assess dysfunctional attitudes in youth. For each item (e.g., “I should be good at everything I try”), participants are asked to rate how much each statement applies to them (i.e., never true, sometimes true, most of the time true, and always true). In the present study, we utilized a short form (20 items) of the CDAS (Flouri & Panourgia, 2014). Total scores on the measure range between 0 to 60, with higher scores indicating increased dysfunctional attitudes. The CDAS has adequate reliability and predictive validity of concurrent and prospective adolescent depressive symptoms (Flouri & Panourgia, 2014; McWhinnie, Abela, Knäuper, & Zhang, 2009). Internal reliability in this sample was 0.81.
Data Analytic Strategy
Baseline scores on the CDI and all cognitive vulnerabilities (CRSQ, ACSQ, and CDAS) represented our main predictors for our four binary outcomes: (a) a concurrent episode at baseline, (b) a prospective episode during the follow-up period, (c) FLED, and (d) rMDD. Initially, two-way interactions were created in hierarchical logistic regression models to examine if cognitive vulnerabilities’ association with depression varied as a function of sex and/or grade. If significant, area under the curves (AUCs) were computed for these subpopulations separately. Next, we used the “best approach” ROC steps outlined by Youngstrom (2014) to determine the validity of an index test. For ROC analyses, significance is determined if the AUC does not include 0.50 in the confidence interval; however, higher cutoffs for clinical utility have been recommended. In the present study, an AUC greater than 0.56 conferred a significant, albeit small effect (Rice & Harris, 2005), whereas an AUC of 0.70 (Swets, 1988) was prioritized. Hanley and McNeil’s (1983) method was used to examine significant differences between the AUCs. Finally, diagnostic likelihood ratios (DLRs; Straus et al., 2011) were calculated for each inventory. DLRs were based on informative tertiles with the cutoff for the subthreshold group placed at 70% sensitivity and the high-risk group being formed at 90% specificity for forecasting prospective episodes of depression. These cutoffs were based on the approximate cutoffs of current screening initiatives using symptom-based measures for pediatric mental health conditions (Lavigne et al., 2016).
We repeated variants of these steps to test for incremental validity. First, we used CDI scores to predict each cognitive vulnerability and saved the residuals. These residual scores represent the independent variance of the cognitive vulnerability beyond depressive symptoms. We then examined if the AUC for the residual score remained significant (see Edens, Skeem, & Douglas, 2006; Hastings, Krishnan, Tangney, & Stuewig, 2011, for examples of using this residual approach to calculate an adjusted ROC curve). If multiple cognitive vulnerabilities remained significant within this approach, we examined the residuals from these cognitive vulnerabilities to ensure their effect was unique (e.g., we examined the difference between the observed and predicted scores for rumination based on the attributional style score). Finally, DLRs for informative tertile scores across indicators were summed to determine which combination of risk factor, and at what level, conferred the greatest risk. This allowed us to calculate the DLR for both convergent (e.g., high depressive symptoms and high rumination) and divergent (e.g., high depressive symptoms and low rumination) profiles.
RESULTS
Preliminary Analyses
Table 1 presents the correlations and descriptive statistics for our baseline predictors. With regard to our criterion variables, all of our outcomes exceeded the minimum number of cases needed to conduct ROC (N = 20; Kraemer, 1992): (a) 4.5% (N = 21) had a concurrent depressive episode at baseline, (b) 30% (N = 137) of our sample had a prospective depressive episode during the follow-up stage, (c) 22.6% (N = 77) had a FLED, and (d) 18.9% (N = 84) had rMDD. Regression analyses suggested that the relation between attributional style and rMDD varied as a function of age (B = .22, SE = .10, Wald = 4.54, p = .03), such that attributional style forecasted rMDD in ninth grade (p < .01) but not sixth grade students (p > .05). Thus, we calculated AUCs separately for sixth- and ninth-grade students with models that included attributional style forecasting rMDD. All other associations between cognitive vulnerability and depression were invariant to sex and age (p > .05).
TABLE 1.
Descriptive Statistics and Bivariate Correlations between Baseline Predictors, Age, and Sex
1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1. Rumination (M = 26.39, SD = 7.87) | ||||||
2. Dysfunctional Attitudes (M = 33.25, SD = 7.43) | .44** | |||||
3. Attributional Style (M = 89.11, SD = 26.27) | .49** | .46** | ||||
4. Depressive Symptoms (M = 7.29, SD = 5.90) | .56** | .43** | .47** | |||
5. Sex | .12** | −.07 | .02 | .05 | ||
6. Age | .13** | .19** | .24** | .22** | −.02 |
Note: Correlations with sex are point biserial correlation coefficients, while all other correlations are Pearson correlation coefficients. Rumination = Total score on the Children’s Response Style Questionnaire–Rumination subscale (Abela et al., 2004); Dysfunctional Attitudes = total score on the Children’s Dysfunctional Attitude Scale (Abela & Sullivan, 2003); Attributional Style = total score on the Adolescent Cognitive Style Questionnaire (Hankin & Abramson, 2002); Depressive Symptoms = total score on the Children’s Depression Inventory (Kovacs, 1992); Sex = parent-reported child sex (male = 0, female = 1); Age = parent-reported child’s chronological age.
p < .01.
Area Under the Curve
AUCs for our depression outcomes are presented in Table 2. For concurrent episodes at baseline, only rumination and depressive symptoms conferred diagnostic status. For prospective episodes, AUCs for all indicators were significant and exerted a medium effect. We found a similar effect for all predictors of rMDD, with the exception of attributional style not being significant in early adolescents. As for FLED, only rumination and dysfunctional attitudes were significant. Pairwise comparisons using Hanley and McNeil’s (1983) method suggested that across analyses, significant indicators forecasted diagnostic patterns similarly (p > .05; e.g., the AUCs for rumination and depressive symptoms predicting concurrent episodes, prospective episodes, and rMDD were not significantly different).
TABLE 2.
AUC for Individual Predictors for Depression Outcomes
AUC | SE | Cohen’s d (Effect Size) | |
---|---|---|---|
Concurrent Episodes | |||
Rumination | 0.69* | 0.07 | 0.70 (Medium) |
Dysfunctional Attitudes | 0.56 | 0.07 | 0.21 (Small) |
Attributional Style | 0.56 | 0.07 | 0.21 (Small) |
Depressive Symptoms | 0.70* | 0.06 | 0.74 (Medium) |
Prospective Episodes | |||
Rumination | 0.67* (unique predictor) | 0.03 | 0.62 (Medium) |
Dysfunctional Attitudes | 0.62* | 0.03 | 0.43 (Medium) |
Attributional Style | 0.64* (unique predictor) | 0.03 | 0.51 (Medium) |
Depressive Symptoms | 0.63* (unique predictor) | 0.03 | 0.47 (Medium) |
FLED | |||
Rumination | 0.63* (unique predictor) | 0.03 | 0.47 (Medium) |
Dysfunctional Attitudes | 0.58* | 0.04 | 0.29 (Small) |
Attributional Style | 0.56 | 0.04 | 0.21 (Small) |
Depressive Symptoms | 0.56 | 0.06 | 0.21 (Small) |
rMDD | |||
Rumination | 0.69* (unique for 6th grade only) | 0.03 | 0.70 (Medium) |
Dysfunctional Attitudes | 0.63* | 0.04 | 0.47 (Medium) |
Attributional Style | |||
(6th grade/9th grade) | 0.54/0.67* (unique predictor) | 0.06/0.05 | 0.14/0.62 (Medium) |
Depressive Symptoms | 0.66* (unique predictor) | 0.04 | 0.58 (Medium) |
Note: For rumination forecasting rMDD, the AUC reflects the curve for the total sample but is only a unique predictor in the sixth grade. AUC = area under the curve; SE = standard error for AUC; Cohen’s d = effect size; Small, Medium, and Large = effect size for predictor (Rice & Harris, 2005); Concurrent episodes = meeting criteria for an episode of depression at baseline; Prospective episodes = meeting criteria for an episode of depression during the follow-up period; FLED = first lifetime episode of depression (i.e., episode of depression during 36 months with no lifetime history); rMDD = recurrent major depression disorder (i.e., more than one episode of depression over the lifetime); Rumination = total score on the Children’s Response Style Questionnaire–Rumination subscale (Abela et al., 2004); Dysfunctional attitudes = total score on the Children’s Dysfunctional Attitude Scale (Abela & Sullivan, 2003); Attributional style = total score on the Adolescent Cognitive Style Questionnaire (Hankin & Abramson, 2002); Depressive symptoms = total score on the Children’s Depression Inventory (Kovacs, 1992); Unique predictor = whether the residuals were significant once covarying out other significant predictors for that criterion (first depressive symptoms, and then other significant predictors).
p < .05.
We next examined the unique variance of each indicator. For concurrent episodes, neither rumination (AUC = .59, p > .10) nor depressive symptoms (AUC = .60, p > .10) were significant. This suggests that the unique variance associated with either predictor is not enough to indicate a concurrent episode. For predicting prospective episodes, rumination (AUC = 0.62, p < .001), dysfunctional attitudes (AUC = .59, p < .01), and attributional style (AUC = 0.58, p <.01), each had a unique effect beyond depressive symptoms. Dysfunctional attitudes, however, were not unique after accounting for rumination (AUC = .54, p > .10), but the residuals associated with attributional style secondary to dysfunctional attitudes and rumination were still significant (AUC = .62, p = .03). As for FLED, residuals associated with dysfunctional attitudes were not significant once accounting for rumination (AUC = .50, p > .50). For rMDD in early adolescents, only residuals associated with rumination were significant (AUC = .69, p = .001). In middle adolescents, only the effect of attributional style was independent of depressive symptoms (AUC = .60, p = .03).1
Diagnostic Likelihood Ratios
DLRs for each unique, significant predictor can be found in the top panel of Table 3. For rumination, low scores ranged from 0 to 19, medium scores ranged from 20 to 35, and high scores were 36 and above. For attributional scores, low scores were 61 or lower, medium scores were 62 to 114, and high scores were 115 and above. Finally, for depressive symptoms, 0 to 3 corresponded to low scores, 4 to 14 were medium scores, and high scores were 15 and above. Of note, a score of 15 falls within the published recommendations range for clinical cutoffs (13–20) published by Kovacs (1992). Despite neither depressive symptoms nor rumination conferring unique variance for concurrent episodes, we still calculated DLRs for both indicators to examine the incremental contribution of rumination symptoms above and beyond depressive symptoms alone. With regard to individual measures, each significant cognitive vulnerability conferred an approximate twofold to threefold increase in risk, and depressive symptoms corresponded to a twofold to fourfold increase in risk across our depression outcomes.2
TABLE 3.
Diagnostic Likelihood Ratios for Significant and Unique Individual Predictors and Combined Models
Low | Medium | High | |
---|---|---|---|
Independent Models | |||
Concurrent Episodes | |||
Rumination | 0.44 | 0.57 | 3.97 |
Depressive Symptoms | 0.29 | 1.09 | 2.60 |
Prospective Episodes | |||
Rumination | 0.58 | 0.91 | 1.94 |
Attributional Style | 0.43 | 0.83 | 2.79 |
Depressive Symptoms | 0.73 | 0.87 | 4.30 |
FLED | |||
Rumination | 0.40 | 1.26 | 0.94 |
rMDD | |||
Rumination (6th Grade) | 0.29 | 1.09 | 2.12 |
Attributional Style 9th Grade) | 0.22 | 0.76 | 2.89 |
Depressive Symptoms (6th/9th) | 0.81/0.50 | 0.95/1.01 | 2.17/3.48 |
Combined Models | |||
Concurrent Episodes (Rumination and Depressive Symptoms) | |||
0.47 | 0.66 | 3.34 | |
Prospective Episodes (Depressive Symptoms, Rumination, and Attributional Style) | |||
0.59 | 0.88 | 6.67 | |
FLED (Rumination and Depressive Symptoms) | |||
0.49 | 1.00 | 1.85 | |
rMDD (Rumination/Depressive Symptoms–6th Grade/Attributional Style/Depressive Symptoms–9th Grade). | |||
0.40/0.00 | 0.95/0.64 | 2.36/2.98 |
Note: The following are cutoff scores on each individual measure as well as for the combined models. Low: Rumination (0–19), Attributional Style (29–61), Depressive Symptoms (0–3), Combined Model = two low scores on individual inventories. Medium: Rumination (20–35), Attributional Style (62–114), Depressive Symptoms (4–14). Combined Model = two medium scores or one low, one medium, and one high score or two high scores and one low score. High: Rumination (36 and up), Attributional Style (115 and up), Depressive Symptoms (15 and up), Combined Model = all high scores or two high scores and one moderate score or one moderate score and one high score. For individuals strictly interested in FLED, only the rumination measure should be used. Table 3 presents the diagnostic likelihood ratios (DLRs) for different tertiles of each screening inventory, as well as the DLRs for different combinations of these tertiles. DLRs represent the ratio of target disorders present within a specific scoring range out of the total number of target disorders divided by the number of target disorders absent within that scoring range divided by the total number of target disorders absent (see Straus et al., 2011). Concurrent episodes = depression episode at baseline; Prospective episodes = a depression episode during the follow-up assessment; FLED = first lifetime episode of depression (i.e., episode of depression during 36 months with no lifetime history); rMDD = recurrent major depression disorder (i.e., more than one episode of depression over the lifetime); Rumination = total score on the Children’s Response Style Questionnaire–Rumination subscale (Abela et al., 2004); Attributional style = total score on the Adolescent Cognitive Style Questionnaire (Hankin & Abramson, 2002); Depressive symptoms = total score on the Children’s Depression Inventory (Kovacs, 1992).
DLRs for all combined models are in the bottom of Table 3. As we did not find any significant differences between predictors, we did not distinguish profiles based on whether a particular indicator was high, medium, or low (i.e., high depressive symptoms and low rumination levels would be represented the same as someone with high rumination and low depressive symptoms). For concurrent episodes, being above threshold in the combined model corresponded to a 28% higher DLR than the DLR for high depressive symptoms. In other words, having moderate or high rumination together with high depressive symptoms led to a 28% increase in likelihood of being currently depressed. For prospective episodes, adolescents with high attributional style, rumination, and depressive symptoms (or even just two out of the three and a moderate score for the third indicator) were 6 times as likely to develop a depressive episode during the follow-up period than not. Presenting above threshold in the combined model represents an approximate 50% increase for depression risk compared to assessing depressive symptoms alone. For FLED, presenting above threshold in the combined model led to a 46% to 96% increase in DLRs above having moderate-elevated rumination scores alone.3 Finally, DLRs for rMDD models are presented separately for early and middle adolescents. DLRs associated with threshold scores on the combined model were approximately the same as those found for depressive symptoms alone. However, combined model DLRs seem to be more specific (i.e., correctly identify a nonpositive case), as low scores in the combined models led to dramatically lower DLRs compared to assessing depressive symptoms alone. In other words, the combined models for forecasting rMDD specifically improved our ability to forecast those not at risk for developing a recurrent depressive course.
DISCUSSION
The goal of the present study was to leverage the considerable amount of basic research on cognitive vulnerabilities for depression (Cohen et al., 2017; Hankin et al., 2016; Jacobs et al., 2008) into improved methods for predicting adolescent depression outcomes. Depression screening protocols have two main objectives: to identify concurrent functional impairment/distress and to forecast prospective depression risk (Forman-Hoffman et al., 2016). To date, the majority of applied research focuses on improving strategies for identifying concurrent depressive episodes with a paucity of studies investigating the prediction of prospective outcomes. We sought to address this gap in the literature by examining the incremental validity of including cognitive vulnerabilities when screening for both concurrent and prospective adolescent depression. Furthermore, we tested prediction algorithms for FLED and recurrent episodes (rMDD) separately as these convey important, significant information that has not previously been examined. Using a translational analytic approach, we found that assessing rumination and attributional style at the screening stage incrementally improves identification of depression during adolescence, especially prospective and persistent patterns of depression. Next we discuss the implications of our findings and how to integrate our results into practice.
To date, the few studies that have examined cognitive styles using an ROC approach have shown inconsistent support for their ability to identify a concurrent depression diagnosis (e.g., Shapero et al., 2015; Young & Dietrich, 2014). This pattern of findings mirrors the current study, as we received mixed support for cognitive vulnerability measures’ ability to improve identification of concurrent episodes. To add a novel indicator to a screening battery, the inventory must demonstrate added, unique variance in predicting the target disorder beyond the existing protocol (Garb, 2003; Hunsley & Meyer, 2003). In our study, the AUCs for attributional style and dysfunctional attitudes were not significant for current depression identification. The AUC for rumination, although statistically significant, did not differ from depressive symptoms and did not demonstrate unique variance when predicting concurrent depressive episodes. At the same time, a 28% increase in DLRs for those with elevated rumination and depressive symptoms, compared to the DLR for only depressive symptoms, suggests a potential meaningful difference in one’s likelihood for presenting with a current depression diagnosis. Some argue that standard AUC benchmarks for incremental validity may underestimate the impact of a novel indicator (Pencina, Steyerberg, & D’Agostino, 2011), so alternative approaches may be necessary to determine whether rumination should ultimately be used in screening and assessment models for identifying concurrent depression.
The value of using a multi-indicator approach was best exemplified when predicting prospective depressive episodes. Rumination and attributional style both improved our ability to predict prospective depressive episodes in early and middle adolescents. DLRs for the at-risk category in the combined model were more than 50% greater than the DLRs associated with using depressive symptoms alone. The challenges of using single inventories to determine concurrent depression diagnostic status (Fristad, Emery, & Beck, 1997; Klein et al., 2005; Matthey & Petrovski, 2002) seem to be exacerbated when examining future distress. In our study no single indicator, including depressive symptoms, for prospective episodes reached the AUC benchmark of 0.70 (Swets, 1988). Others have addressed this issue for concurrent depression by querying multiple informants (De Los Reyes et al., 2015) or utilizing multiple mental health screening inventories (Lavigne et al., 2016). For prospective episodes, using a multi-informant or multimental health screening approach may be limited, as the algorithms are largely dependent on current or recent symptoms. Instead, others have recommended incorporating risk factors into our evidence based assessment approaches (Youngstrom et al., 2017). Although these risk factors tend to focus on immutable demographic factors (e.g., sex), we demonstrated that the assessment of cognitive vulnerabilities led to a significant improvement in our ability to forecast prospective depression episodes when compared to using depression symptoms as the sole indicator.
Our study represented one of the first applications of a translational analytic approach to develop screening solutions for FLED and rMDD in adolescence. Prevention research has recommended an increased focus on both of these specific depression outcomes (Allen et al., 2007; Petit et al., 2013). For FLED, rumination was the only significant indicator; however, its translation into a screening framework is not straightforward. Deflated DLRs, compared to our other outcomes, suggests that our algorithm did not calibrate as well for FLED (Straus et al., 2011). Therefore, it may be advantageous to consider other risk factors beyond depressive symptoms and cognitive vulnerability when creating protocols for this depression outcome. For instance, major stressful life events may play a larger role in FLED compared to recurrent episodes (Monroe & Harkness, 2011), and more proximal indicators of our stress response symptom (e.g., HPA-Axis) may be instrumental in forecasting an initial depressive episode (Mazurka, Wynne-Edwards, & Harkness, 2016). Thus, future research may need a multi-indicator protocol that combines rumination, stressors, and biological indicators to adequately predict FLED.
Finally, for rMDD, we found that pairing rumination with depressive symptoms best forecasted this persistent course in early adolescence, and attributional style and depressive symptoms best identified rMDD in middle adolescence. Interestingly, the value of a multi-indicator approach within the context of rMDD seems to be that it is more specific, as opposed to more sensitive, for predicting positive cases. DLRs in the lowest risk group either exceeded or came close to the 0.25 threshold posited by Straus and colleagues (2011) in which one could be “moderately certain” of a negative outcome (i.e., no diagnosis). These estimates were dramatically lower than the DLRs reported for the lowest tertile of depressive symptoms alone. Cognitive vulnerability measures therefore may best be used in models for persistent adolescent depression (Hill et al., 2015) to help rule out individuals not at risk for rMDD so that resources can be better prioritized for those who are most vulnerable to this chronic depression course.
The present study should be viewed in light of several limitations. First, it is important to acknowledge that although cognitive vulnerabilities and depressive symptoms are conceptually distinct at the latent level (Hankin et al., 2016; Jacobs et al., 2008), in the present study they were both assessed via self-report. It is critical for future research to use a multimethod approach when attempting to identify novel predictors to reduce the shared method variance between screening indicators (see De Los Reyes & Aldao, 2015, for a discussion). Second, diagnostic outcomes were based on clinician “best estimates” from multi-informant diagnostic interviews. Although this approach reflects best practice (Klein et al., 2005), clinicians may be more swayed by parental compared to youth report (Youngstrom et al., 2011) leading some to suggest that diagnostic interviews should be viewed as a “fuzzy” gold standard (Zhou, Obuchowski, & McClish, 2002). Third, cutoff scores on dimensional measures have inherent limitations, especially for those at or near the threshold who may be improperly classified (Sheldrick et al., 2015). Although we did our best to mitigate this limitation by using multilevel diagnostic likelihood ratios (Straus et al., 2011), we acknowledge that our multiple cutoff scores are vulnerable to misclassification. Fourth, we only assessed for mood disorders and did not include other pediatric diagnoses. Given high rates of comorbidity between depression and other pediatric disorders (e.g., anxiety; Avenevoli, Swendsen, He, Burstein, & Merikangas, 2015; Cohen, Young, Gibb, Hankin, & Abela, 2014), as well as the association between cognitive vulnerabilities and discrete forms of psychological distress (Hankin et al., 2016), future research should examine the sensitivity of our findings with regard to depression. Finally, our study was conducted within the context of a research study. It is important that future studies replicate these findings in applied settings, as demand characteristics may influence how adolescents respond within a research study compared to a clinical context (Krosnick, 1999). Under a recent rubric put forth to evaluate empirically based assessment protocols the current study qualifies as “adequate” for demonstrating clinical utility of an index test for pediatric mental health but would need to be replicated in an applied setting to merit a higher rating (Youngstrom et al., 2017).
Clinical Implications
Across medicine (Bossuyt et al., 2003), and within pediatric mental health (Youngstrom et al., 2017), there is a movement to make basic research more accessible to clinical decision making. Table 4 provides four example screening profiles and how the approach modeled in the present study can be used to improve empirically based referral decisions. It is our hope that modeling these examples will not only facilitate the use of cognitive vulnerability measures into practice but also show the potential for applying this translational analytic plan to other risk factors for adolescent mental health.
TABLE 4.
Stoplight Model for Examples of Screening Cases
Exemplars | Pretest Prob | Score Profile | DLR | Posttest Prob |
---|---|---|---|---|
6th-Grade Boy | Concurrent: 2.0% | CDI: 15 (Hi) | Concurrent: 3.34 | Concurrent: 6.4% |
Prospective: 16.0% | CRSQ: 29 (Med) | Prospective: 0.88 | Prospective: 14.4% | |
FLED: 10.3% | ACSQ: 98 (Med) | FLED: 1.85 | FLED: 17.1% | |
rMDD: 9.4% | rMDD: 2.36 | rMDD: 18.9% | ||
6th-Grade Girl | Concurrent: 3.4% | CDI: 15 (Hi) | Concurrent: 3.34 | Concurrent: 9.4% |
Prospective: 26.6% | CRSQ: 26 (Med) | Prospective: 6.67 | Prospective: 70.1% | |
FLED: 26.2% | ACSQ: 124 (Hi) | FLED: 1.85 | FLED: 40.0% | |
rMDD: 15.0% | rMDD: 2.36 | rMDD: 29.4% | ||
9th-Grade Boy | Concurrent: 6.3% | CDI: 15 (Hi) | Concurrent: 3.34 | Concurrent: 17.6% |
Prospective: 30.1% | CRSQ: 29 (Hi) | Prospective: 6.67 | Prospective: 74.1% | |
FLED: 16.9% | ACSQ: 92 (Med) | FLED: 1.85 | FLED: 27.5% | |
rMDD: 16.1% | rMDD: 2.98 | rMDD: 56.8% | ||
9th-Grade Girl | Concurrent: 6.6% | CDI: 14 (Med) | Concurrent: 0.66 | Concurrent: 4.7% |
Prospective: 45.5% | CRSQ: 23 (Med) | Prospective: 0.88 | Prospective: 42.8% | |
FLED: 33.8% | ACSQ: 131 (Hi) | FLED: 1.00 | FLED: 33.8% | |
rMDD: 33.3% | rMDD: 2.98 | rMDD: 59.8% | ||
Interpretation | ||||
6th-Grade Boy | Yellow Zone: Despite an elevated DLR for a concurrent episode, the likelihood of presenting with a current episode are still quite low based on the post-test probability. Absent any critical symptoms, an immediate referral for a full assessment is not necessary due to the low risk of a current or future episode. At the same time, the elevated risk for rMDD and FLED warrants routine monitoring, and potentially even increased monitoring in the coming years. | |||
6th-grade Girl | Red Zone: An assessment for a full mental health assessment is warranted and any available preventative services should be initiated immediately based on these screening scores. It is particularly alarming that early adolescent girls with this scoring profile have an over 70% likelihood of developing a depressive episode in the upcoming 3 years despite the fact that less than 10% with this profile will be currently depressed. | |||
9th-Grade Boy | Red Zone: Middle adolescent boys with this scoring profile should immediately be referred for a mental health assessment and preventative services should be delivered. These youth are nearly three times more likely to be experiencing a concurrent depressive episode than the base rate, and are at significant risk for a prospective episode. The heightened risk for rMDD is another reason that an immediate referral is warranted. | |||
9th-Grade Girl | Yellow Zone: Middle adolescent girls with this profile should be closely monitored, but a referral for a full assessment may not be imminent. Compared to their peers, youth with this scoring profile are at decreased or equivalent risk for current, future, and FLED depression outcomes. Yet, increased monitoring is warranted despite a subthreshold CDI score (14) due to these youth’s heightened risk for rMDD. Increased monitoring may help identify risk for this pattern earlier allowing for an opportunity to prevent a chronic depression course. |
Note: Exemplars = examples of scoring profiles based on demographic data and the scoring profile; Pretest Prob = percentage chance of each depression outcome based on sex and age; Score Profile = sample scores on significant, unique indicators; DLR = diagnostic likelihood ratio; Posttest Prob = (prevalence/ (1-prevelance) × DLR) / ((prevalence/(1 – prevelance)) + 1) (Straus et al., 2011); Concurrent = current depression diagnosis at baseline; Prospective = experiencing a depressive episode during the follow-up period; FLED = first lifetime episode of depression (i.e., episode of depression during 36 months with no lifetime history); rMDD = recurrent major depression disorder (i.e., more than one episode of depression over the lifetime); Rumination = total score on the Children’s Response Style Questionnaire–Rumination subscale (Abela et al., 2004); Attributional style = total score on the Adolescent Cognitive Style Questionnaire (Hankin & Abramson, 2002); Depressive symptoms = total score on the Children’s Depression Inventory (Kovacs, 1992); Hi = high tertile; Med = medium tertile; Yellow Zone = increased monitoring; Red Zone = refer for mental health services.
Understanding the base rate for the target disorder is important for protecting against the base rate fallacy (Gigerenzer, Gaissmaier, Kurz-Milcke, Schwartz, & Woloshin, 2007) and allowing protocols to develop their own decision rules based on objectives and resources (Sheldrick et al., 2015; Youngstrom, 2014). Due to the increased risk for depression in girls compared to boys and middle adolescents compared to early adolescents (Avenevoli et al., 2015; Hankin & Abramson, 2001), we calculated our sample’s base rate for our four main depression outcomes based on these demographic characteristics (Columns 1 and 2). Although we decided to use base rates from our own study for these examples, epidemiological studies (see Schaefer et al., 2017) can also be used to determine pretest probabilities. In the third column in Table 4, we selected scores at or approaching the cutoff (15) for the CDI identified in the current study. These CDI scores were selected due to scores at the threshold being especially challenging from a referral perspective (Sheldrick et al., 2015). Use of additional indicators of depression may be particularly beneficial in these situations.
The fourth column represents the tailored DLRs for each depression outcome derived from Table 3, and the posttest prevalence is presented in the fifth column (see Straus et al., 2011, for calculation details). Within an empirically based assessment approach, referral decisions may ultimately be based on the posttest odds/prevalence for developing the target disorder (Youngstrom, 2014). For the present study, we used an adapted version of Youngstrom, Choukas-Bradley, Calhoun, and Jensen-Doss’s (2015) stoplight model for the assessment context. For our study, the “yellow zone” represents increased monitoring and the “red zone” reflects the need to refer for a full assessment and initiation of available preventative services. We note the differences between the yellow zone and red zone are highly context dependent and ultimately depend on the objectives of the screening program and the resources available for follow-up services (Youngstrom et al., 2017).
Interpretations for these four example cases are detailed in the bottom half of Table 4. These examples collectively illustrate the potential importance of accounting for cognitive vulnerabilities when making referral decisions and assessing multiple depression outcomes. For instance, in the first two examples, an early adolescent boy and early adolescent girl have identical depression scores. Even after accounting for differences in pretest probability, and using the higher DLR derived from the combined model (3.34) compared to using depressive symptoms alone (2.60), one would still probably decide to handle referral decisions the same when only using concurrent episodes as the criterion. However, examining the contents of Table 4 it becomes clear that concern should be heightened in the sixth-grade girl, particularly in relation to prospective diagnoses. Specifically, her probability of having a depressive episode over the next 3 years is over 70%, approximately 5 times greater than her male counterpart due in large part to her elevated attributional style score. Recognition of these differing recommendations for these two screening exemplars is possible only by distinguishing between depression outcomes and including cognitive vulnerability measures at the screening stage.
Finally, in addition to making individual patient decisions, our findings can have practical consequences in the designs of emerging preventative models for depression. Using an evidence-based medicine approach to depression screening can advance stepped-care depression prevention approaches in unselected adolescent samples (Van Straten, Hill, Richards, & Cuijpers, 2015). Stepped-care models are guided by the principles that patients may respond to lower levels of intervention prior to being sent for therapy and that attempting these low-cost interventions (e.g., online resources; Cuijpers, Van Straten, & Andersson, 2008) may be sufficient. Stepped-care and associated multistage screening protocols (Lavigne et al., 2016; Morey, Arora, & Stark, 2015) are also advantageous due to their inherent, dimensional approach to operationalizing psychopathology. To date, most stepped-care models have focused on adults, have used depression symptoms as the screen at each stage, and have not focused on prospective risk (Rohde, 2015; Van Straten et al., 2015). Implementing our findings into screening protocols for stepped-care models for adolescent depression can help allocate resources based on prospective risk and specific depression outcomes (e.g., rMDD) as one enters the vulnerable adolescent years.
Acknowledgments
FUNDING
This research was supported by the National Institute of Mental Health Grants 5R01MH077195 and 5R01MH077178 awarded to Benjamin Hankin and Jami Young.
Footnotes
We also used a more traditional hierarchical logistic regression approach to test our assumptions concerning incremental validity (Hunsley & Meyer, 2003). The pattern of findings across cognitive vulnerabilities was identical.
For FLED, a degenerate pattern for rumination emerged, as moderate scores corresponded to a higher DLR (i.e., increased FLED risk) than elevated rumination scores. Smoothing techniques using the k-nearest neighbors algorithm were unable to fix the degenerate pattern of data. Inspection of quintiles revealed that using a lower score for our threshold (23 instead of 36) and subthreshold (19 instead of 20) led to a monotonic trend in our tertiles. In favor of parsimony, however, we decided to keep uniform cutoffs for our depression outcomes. Prevention programs specifically focused on FLED should consider using these lower cutoff scores within screening protocols.
The impact of degeneracy noted in Table 2 for rumination is partially mitigated in the combined model as moderate and high levels of rumination are treated the same within the context of elevated depression scores.
Contributor Information
Joseph R. Cohen, Department of Psychology, University of Illinois at Urbana Champaign
Felix K. So, Department of Psychology, University of Illinois at Urbana Champaign
Benjamin L. Hankin, Department of Psychology, University of Illinois at Urbana Champaign
Jami F. Young, Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Children’s Hospital of Philadelphia
References
- Abela J, Vanderbilt E, Rochon A. A test of the integration of the response styles and social support theories of depression in third and seventh grade children. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology. 2004;23(5):653–674. doi: 10.1521/jscp.23.5.653.50752. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Abela JR, Sullivan C. A test of Beck’s cognitive diathesis-stress theory of depression in early adolescents. The Journal of Early Adolescence. 2003;23(4):384–404. doi: 10.1177/0272431603258345. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Abela JRZ, Hankin BL. Rumination as a vulnerability factor to depression during the transition from early to middle adolescence: A multiwave longitudinal study. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2011;120(1):259–271. doi: 10.1037/a0022796. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Abramson L, Metalsky G, Alloy L. Hopelessness depression: A theory-based subtype of depression. Psychological Review. 1989;96(2):358. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.96.2.358. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Allen N, Hetrick S, Simmons J, Hickie I. Early intervention for depressive disorders in young people: The opportunity and the (lack of) evidence. Medical Journal of Australia. 2007;187(7):215–217. doi: 10.5694/j.1326-5377.2007.tb01329.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Alloy LB, Abramson LY, Whitehouse WG, Hogan ME, Panzarella C, Rose DT. Prospective incidence of first onsets and recurrences of depression in individuals at high and low cognitive risk for depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2006;115(1):145. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.115.1.145. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual for mental disorders. 4. Washington, DC: Author; 1994. [Google Scholar]
- Avenevoli S, Swendsen J, He JP, Burstein M, Merikangas KR. Major depression in the national comorbidity survey–adolescent supplement: Prevalence, correlates, and treatment. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 2015;54(1):37–44. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2014.10.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beck AT. Cognitive therapy of depression: New perspectives. In: Clayton PJ, Barrett JE, editors. Treatment of depression: Old controversies and new approaches. New York, NY: Raven Press; 1983. pp. 265–290. [Google Scholar]
- Bossuyt PM, Reitsma JB, Bruns ED, Gatsonis CA, Glasziou PP, Irwig LM, … De Vet HCW. Towards complete and accurate reporting of studies of diagnostic accuracy: The STARD initiative. Clinical Chemistry and Laboratory Medicine. 2003;41(1):68–73. doi: 10.1515/CCLM.2003.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carter-Smith JS, Garber J. Predictors of the first onset of a major depressive episode and changes in depressive symptoms across adolescence: Stress and negative cognitions. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2011;120(4):779–796. doi: 10.1037/a0025441. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cheong J, MacKinnon DP, Khoo ST. Investigation of mediational processes using parallel process latent growth curve modeling. Structural Equation Modeling. 2003;10(2):238–262. doi: 10.1207/S15328007SEM1002_5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cicchetti D, Toth S. The past achievements and future promises of developmental psychopathology: The coming of age of a discipline. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2009;50(1/2):16–25. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.01979.x/full. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen JR, Adams ZW, Menon SV, Youngstrom EA, Bunnell BE, Acierno R, … Danielson CK. How should we screen for depression following a natural disaster? An ROC approach to post-disaster screening in adolescents and adults. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2016;202:102–109. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2016.05.034. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen JR, Tengshe C, Sheshko DM, Chan RCK, Hankin BL, Abela JRZ. Hopelessness. In: Levesque RJR, editor. Encyclopedia of adolescence. Vol. 1. New York, NY: Springer; 2017. pp. 1329–1334. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen JR, Young JF, Abela JR. Cognitive vulnerability to depression in children: An idiographic, longitudinal examination of inferential styles. Cognitive Therapy and Research. 2012;36(6):643–654. doi: 10.1007/s10608-011-9431-6. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen JR, Young JF, Gibb BE, Hankin BL, Abela JRZ. Why are anxiety and depressive symptoms comorbid in youth? A multi-wave, longitudinal examination of competing etiological models. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2014;161:21–29. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2014.02.042. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cole DA, Ciesla JA, Dallaire DH, Jacquez FM, Pineda AQ, LaGrange B, … Felton JW. Emergence of attributional style and its relation to depressive symptoms. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2008;117(1):16–31. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.117.1.16. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Compton WM, Lopez MF. Accuracy in reporting past psychiatric symptoms: The role of cross-sectional studies in psychiatric research. JAMA Psychiatry. 2014;71(3):233–234. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.4111. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Costello EJ, Erkanli A. Is there an epidemic of child or adolescent depression? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2006;47(12):1263–1271. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01682.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cuijpers P, Van Straten A, Andersson G. Internet-administered cognitive behavior therapy for health problems: A systematic review. Journal of Behavioral Medicine. 2008;31(2):169–177. doi: 10.1007/s10865-007-9144-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Danielson CK, Cohen JR, Adams ZW, Youngstrom EA, Soltis K, Amstadter AB, Ruggiero KJ. Clinical decision-making following disasters: Efficient identification of PTSD risk in adolescents. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2017;45(1):117–129. doi: 10.1007/s10802-016-0159-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- De Los Reyes A, Aldao A. Introduction to the special issue: Toward implementing physiological measures in clinical child and adolescent assessments. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology. 2015;44(2):221–237. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2014.891227. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- De Los Reyes A, Augenstein TM, Wang M, Thomas SA, Drabick DAG, Burgers DE, Rabinowitz J. The validity of the multi-informant approach to assessing child and adolescent mental health. Psychological Bulletin. 2015;141(4):858–900. doi: 10.1037/a0038498. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- DeWalt DA, Gross HE, Gipson DS, Selewski DT, DeWitt EM, Dampier CD, … Varni JW. PROMIS® pediatric self-report scales distinguish subgroups of children within and across six common pediatric chronic health conditions. Quality of Life Research. 2015;24(9):2195–2208. doi: 10.1007/s11136-015-0953-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Divaris K. Predicting dental caries outcomes in children: A “risky” concept. Journal of Dental Research. 2016;95(3):248–254. doi: 10.1177/0022034515. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Edens JF, Skeem JL, Douglas KS. Incremental validity analyses of the Violence Risk Appraisal Guide and the Psychopathy Checklist: Screening Version in a civil psychiatric sample. Assessment. 2006;13(3):368–374. doi: 10.1177/1073191105284001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Flouri E, Panourgia C. Negative automatic thoughts and emotional and behavioural problems in adolescence. Child and Adolescent Mental Health. 2014;19(1):46–51. doi: 10.1111/camh.12004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Forman-Hoffman V, McClure E, McKeeman J, Wood CT, Middleton JC, Skinner AC, Viswanathan M. Screening for major depressive disorder in children and adolescents: A systematic review for the US Preventive Services Task Force. Annals of Internal Medicine. 2016;164(5):342–349. doi: 10.7326/M15-2259. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fristad MA, Emery BL, Beck SJ. Use and abuse of the children’s depression inventory. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1997;65(4):699–702. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.65.4.699. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Garb HN. Incremental validity and the assessment of psychopathology in adults. Psychological Assessment. 2003;15(4):508–520. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.15.4.508. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Garber J, Korelitz K, Samanez-Larkin S. Translating basic psychopathology research to preventive interventions: A tribute to John RZ Abela. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology. 2012;41(5):666–681. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2012.710161. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gigerenzer G, Gaissmaier W, Kurz-Milcke E, Schwartz LM, Woloshin S. Helping doctors and patients make sense of health statistics. Psychological Science in the Public Interest. 2007;8(2):53–96. doi: 10.1111/j.1539-6053.2008.00033.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hankin BL, Abramson LY. Development of gender differences in depression: An elaborated cognitive vulnerability–transactional stress theory. Psychological Bulletin. 2001;127(6):773–796. doi: 10.1037//0033-2909.127.6.773. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hankin BL, Abramson LY. Measuring cognitive vulnerability to depression in adolescence: Reliability, validity, and gender differences. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology. 2002;31(4):491–504. doi: 10.1207/S15374424JCCP3104_8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hankin BL, Davis EP, Snyder H, Young JF, Glynn LM, Sandman CA. Temperament factors and dimensional, latent bifactor models of child psychopathology: Transdiagnostic and specific associations in two youth samples. Psychiatry Research. 2017;252:139–146. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2017.02.061. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hankin BL, Snyder HR, Gulley LD. Cognitive risks in developmental psychopathology. In: Cicchetti D, editor. Developmental psychopathology. 3. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley; 2016. pp. 312–385. [Google Scholar]
- Hankin BL, Young JF, Abela JR, Smolen A, Jenness JL, Gulley LD, … Oppenheimer CW. Depression from childhood into late adolescence: Influence of gender, development, genetic susceptibility, and peer stress. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2015;124(4):803–816. doi: 10.1037/abn0000089. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hanley JA, McNeil BJ. A method of comparing the areas under receiver operating characteristic curves derived from the same cases. Radiology. 1983;148(3):839–843. doi: 10.1148/radiology.148.3.6878708. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hastings ME, Krishnan S, Tangney JP, Stuewig J. Predictive and incremental validity of the violence risk appraisal guide scores with male and female jail inmates. Psychological Assessment. 2011;23(1):174–183. doi: 10.1037/a0021290. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hill RM, Yaroslavsky I, Pettit JW. Enhancing depression screening to identify college students at risk for persistent depressive symptoms. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2015;174:1–6. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2014.11.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hunsley J, Meyer GJ. The incremental validity of psychological testing and assessment: Conceptual, methodological, and statistical issues. Psychological Assessment. 2003;15(4):446–455. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.15.4.446. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ialongo NS, Rogosch FA, Cicchetti D, Toth SL, Buckley J, Petras H, Neiderhiser J. A developmental psychopathology approach to the prevention of mental health disorders. In: Cicchetti D, Cohen D, editors. Developmental psychopathology. New York, NY: Wiley; 2006. pp. 968–1018. [Google Scholar]
- Ingram RE, Miranda J, Segal Z. Cognitive vulnerability to depression. In: Alloy LB, Riskind JH, editors. Cognitive vulnerability to emotional disorders. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum; 2006. pp. 63–91. [Google Scholar]
- Irwin DE, Stucky BD, Thissen D, DeWitt EM, Lai JS, Yeatts K, … DeWalt DA. Sampling plan and patient characteristics of the PROMIS pediatrics large-scale survey. Quality of Life Research. 2010;19(4):585–594. doi: 10.1007/s11136-010-9618-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jacobs RH, Reinecke MA, Gollan JK, Kane P. Empirical evidence of cognitive vulnerability for depression among children and adolescents: A cognitive science and developmental perspective. Clinical Psychology Review. 2008;28(5):759–782. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2007.10.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jarrett MA, Van Meter A, Youngstrom EA, Hilton DC, Ollendick TH. Evidence-Based Assessment of ADHD in Youth Using a Receiver Operating Characteristic Approach. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology. 2016;24:1–13. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2016.1225502. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnston C, Murray C. Incremental validity in the psychological assessment of children and adolescents. Psychological Assessment. 2003;15:496–507. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.15.4.496. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kaufman J, Birmaher B, Brent D, Rao UMA, Flynn C, Moreci P, … Ryan N. Schedule for affective disorders and schizophrenia for school-age children-present and lifetime version (K-SADS-PL): Initial reliability and validity data. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 1997;36(7):980–988. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199707000-00021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Klein DN, Dougherty LR, Olino TM. Toward guidelines for evidence-based assessment of depression in children and adolescents. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology. 2005;34(3):412–432. doi: 10.1207/s15374424jccp3403_3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kovacs M. Children’s Depression Inventory (CDI) manual. New York, NY: Multi-Health Systems; 1992. [Google Scholar]
- Kraemer HC. Evaluating medical tests: Objectives and quantitative guidelines. Newbury Park, CA: Sage; 1992. [Google Scholar]
- Krosnick JA. Survey research. Annual Review of Psychology. 1999;50(1):537–567. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.50.1.537. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lavigne JV, Meyers KM, Feldman M. Systematic review: Classification accuracy of behavioral screening measures for use in integrated primary care settings. Journal of Pediatric Psychology. 2016;41(10):1091–1109. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/jsw049. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mash EJ, Hunsley J. Evidence-based assessment of child and adolescent disorders: Issues and challenges. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology. 2005;34(3):362–379. doi: 10.1207/s15374424jccp3403_1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Matthey S, Petrovski P. The Children’s Depression Inventory: Error in cutoff scores for screening purposes. Psychological Assessment. 2002;14(2):146–149. doi: 10.1037//1040-3590.14.2.146. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mazurka R, Wynne-Edwards KE, Harkness KL. Stressful life events prior to depression onset and the cortisol response to stress in youth with first onset versus recurrent depression. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2016;44(6):1173–1184. doi: 10.1007/s10802-015-0103-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McWhinnie CM, Abela JR, Knäuper B, Zhang C. Development and validation of the revised Children’s Dysfunctional Attitudes Scale. British Journal of Clinical Psychology. 2009;48(3):287–308. doi: 10.1348/014466508X398952. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Monroe SM, Harkness KL. Recurrence in major depression: A conceptual analysis. Psychological Review. 2011;118(4):655–674. doi: 10.1037/a0025190. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morey ME, Arora P, Stark KD. Multiple-stage screening of youth depression in schools. Psychology in the Schools. 2015;52(8):800–814. doi: 10.1002/pits.21860. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Myers K, Winters NC. Ten-year review of rating scales. II: Scales for internalizing disorders. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 2002;41(6):634–659. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200206000-00004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nolen-Hoeksema S. Responses to depression and their effects on the duration of depressive episodes. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1991;100(4):569–582. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.100.4.569. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nolen-Hoeksema S, Morrow J. A prospective study of depression and posttraumatic stress symptoms after a natural disaster: The 1989 Loma Prieta Earthquake. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1991;61(1):115–121. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.61.1.115. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pencina MJ, Steyerberg EW, D’Agostino RB. Extensions of net reclassification improvement calculations to measure usefulness of new biomarkers. Statistics in Medicine. 2011;30(1):11–21. doi: 10.1002/sim.4085. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Petit JW, Hartley C, Lewinsohn PM, Seeley JR, Klein DN. Is liability to recurrent major depressive disorder present before first episode onset in adolescence or acquired after the initial episode? Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2013;122(2):353–358. doi: 10.1037/a0032655. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rice ME, Harris GT. Comparing effect sizes in follow-up studies: ROC Area, Cohen’s d, and r. Law and Human Behavior. 2005;29(5):615–620. doi: 10.1007/s10979-005-6832-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rohde P. Cognitive-behavioral prevention of depression in adolescents. Current Opinion in Psychology. 2015;4:136–141. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2014.12.003. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Schaefer JD, Caspi A, Belsky DW, Harrington H, Houts R, Moffitt TE. Enduring mental health: Prevalence & prediction. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2017;126(2):212–221. doi: 10.1037/abn0000232. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seeley JR, Stice E, Rohde P. Screening for depression prevention: Identifying adolescent girls at high risk for future depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2009;118(1):161–170. doi: 10.1037/a0014741. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shapero BG, Stange JP, Goldstein KE, Black CL, Molz AR, Hamlat EJ, … Alloy LB. Cognitive styles in mood disorders: Discriminative ability of unipolar and bipolar cognitive profiles. International Journal of Cognitive Therapy. 2015;8(1):35–60. doi: 10.1521/ijct.2015.8.1.35. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sheldrick RC, Benneyan JC, Kiss IG, Briggs-Gowan MJ, Copeland W, Carter AS. Thresholds and accuracy in screening tools for early detection of psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2015;56:936– 948. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12442. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Singer JD, Willett JB. Applied longitudinal data analysis: Modeling change and event occurrence. London, UK: Oxford University Press; 2003. [Google Scholar]
- Smith AJ, Skow Á, Bodurtha J, Kinra S. Health information technology in screening and treatment of child obesity: A systematic review. Pediatrics. 2013;131(3):894–902. doi: 10.1542/peds.2012-2011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Straus SE, Richardson S, Glasziou P, Haynes B. Evidence-based medicine: How to practice and teach it. 4. Edinburgh, UK: Elsevier; 2011. [Google Scholar]
- Swets JA. Measuring the accuracy of diagnostic systems. Science. 1988;240:1285–1293. doi: 10.1126/science.3287615. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Meter A, Youngstrom E, Youngstrom JK, Ollendick T, Demeter C, Findling RL. Clinical decision making about child and adolescent anxiety disorders using the Achenbach system of empirically based assessment. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology. 2014;43(4):552–565. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2014.883930. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Straten A, Hill J, Richards DA, Cuijpers P. Stepped care treatment delivery for depression: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychological Medicine. 2015;45(2):231–246. doi: 10.1017/S0033291714000701. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Varni JW, Magnus B, Stucky BD, Liu Y, Quinn H, Thissen D, … DeWalt DA. Psychometric properties of the PROMIS® pediatric scales: precision, stability, and comparison of different scoring and administration options. Quality of Life Research. 2014;23(4):1233–1243. doi: 10.1007/s11136-013-0544-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wissow LS, Brown J, Fothergill KE, Gadomski A, Hacker K, Salmon P, Zelkowitz R. Universal mental health screening in pediatric primary care: A systematic review. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 2013;52(11):1134–1147. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2013.08.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- You DS, Youngstrom EA, Feeny NC, Youngstrom JK, Findling RL. Comparing the diagnostic accuracy of five instruments for detecting posttraumatic stress disorder inyouth. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology. 2017;46(4):511–522. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2015.1030754. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Young CC, Dietrich MS. Screening for rumination and brooding may be a feasible method of identifying adolescents at high risk for depression. Journal of Pediatric Nursing. 2014;29(6):688–695. doi: 10.1016/j.pedn.2014.04.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Youngstrom EA. A primer on receiver operating characteristic analysis and diagnostic efficiency statistics for pediatric psychology: We are ready to ROC. Journal of Pediatric Psychology. 2014;39(2):204–221. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/jst062. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Youngstrom EA, Choukas-Bradley S, Calhoun CD, Jensen-Doss A. Clinical guide to the evidence-based assessment approach to diagnosis and treatment. Cognitive and Behavioral Practice. 2015;22:20–35. doi: 10.1016/j.cbpra.2013.12.005. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Youngstrom EA, Genzlinger JE, Egerton GA, Van Meter AR. Multivariate metaanalysis of the discriminative validity of caregiver, youth, and teacher rating scales for pediatric bipolar disorder: Mother knows best about mania. Archives of Scientific Psychology. 2015;3(1):112–137. doi: 10.1037/arc0000024. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Youngstrom EA, Van Meter A, Frazier TW, Hunsley J, Prinstein MJ, Ong ML, Youngstrom JK. Evidence-based assessment as an integrative model for applying psychological science to guide the voyage of treatment. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice. 2017 doi: 10.1111/cpsp.12207. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Youngstrom EA, Youngstrom JK, Freeman AJ, De Los Reyes A, Feeny NC, Findling RL. Informants are not all equal: Predictors and correlates of clinician judgments about caregiver and youth credibility. Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychopharmacology. 2011;21(5):407–415. doi: 10.1089/cap.2011.0032. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhou X, Obuchowski NA, McClish DK. Statistical methods in diagnostic medicine. New York, NY: Wiley; 2002. [Google Scholar]