Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2019 Oct 1.
Published in final edited form as: Curr Opin Behav Sci. 2018 May 7;23:92–97. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2018.04.005

Focus on females: A less biased approach for studying strategies and mechanisms of memory

Natalie C Tronson
PMCID: PMC6075684  NIHMSID: NIHMS961985  PMID: 30083579

Abstract

Recent work on sex differences in learning and memory has demonstrated that females and males differ in cognitive and behavioral strategies, as well as neural mechanisms required to learn, retrieve and express memory. Although our understanding of the mechanisms of memory is highly sophisticated, this work is based on male animals. As such, the study of female memory is narrowed to a comparison with behavior and mechanisms defined in males, resulting in findings of male-specific mechanisms but little understanding of how females learn and store information. In this paper, we discuss a female-focused framework and experimental approaches to deepen our understanding of the strategies and neural mechanisms engaged by females (and males) in learning, consolidation, and retrieval of memory.


Despite the large number of studies on learning and memory and underlying neural mechanisms, a surprisingly small proportion of these focus on how females learn and remember information. Given the fundamental role of neuronal plasticity in the brain, together with the conservation of these mechanisms across species [1], the overwhelming assumption for many years has been that the mechanisms underlying memory formation would be largely independent of sex. However, chromosomal and hormonal influences during development result in divergent neural circuits and structures [2,3] that together with hormonal milieu during adulthood, drive sex differences in a wide range of behaviors and neural functions [4,5]. Sex differences are evident in a number of memory tasks in performance (e.g., verbal memory), cognitive strategy (e.g., spatial memory), and/or circuit and molecular mechanisms (e.g., fear conditioning) (for review see [••6,7]). More importantly, women and men are differentially vulnerable to disorders of memory including post-traumatic stress disorder [8], Alzheimer’s Disease and other dementias [9], and learned aspects of addiction [10]. Understanding the neural mechanisms underlying learning and memory in both females and males is critical for identifying risk factors and developing sex-specific interventions and treatments for these disorders.

The goal of studying sex differences in memory is to identify what information females and males are learning, the strategies by which each sex solves memory tasks, and the similar and distinct neural (circuit, systems, and molecular) mechanisms underlying acquisition, consolidation, retrieval and post retrieval memory processes. However, our extensive knowledge of basic mechanisms of memory garnered over decades of research in male animals has narrowed the question of sex differences to a comparison of behavior and mechanisms in females with those defined in males, thereby constraining our understanding of memory in females [5,11]. In this paper, we discuss recent findings from fear conditioning and approaches to advance the study of learning and memory in both females and males.

Studying sex differences in memory

There are two interrelated approaches that dominate the study of sex differences in memory: (1) Selection of memory tasks that show clear sex differences in performance, and comparing the strategies, circuits and molecular mechanisms engaged by each sex; and (2) Identifying neural circuits and molecular mechanisms well known for their role in memory in males and examining the role of these mechanisms in females.

(1) Searching for sex-specific mechanisms where there are sex differences in memory performance

Utilizing sex differences in performance on memory tasks to examine sex differences in underlying mechanisms of learning and memory is an appealing place to start. Under parameters where males form robust context fear memories and females do not, only males show increased activation of memory-related signaling pathways during consolidation, including the extracellular regulated protein kinase 1/2 (ERK1/2) [12]. Even when both sexes acquire context fear conditioning, females do not engage the same signaling pathways as males [1316]. Although these studies identify differences in behavior and signaling in the hippocampus, they fail to identify what information females are learning and the mechanisms by which females store memory.

Where sex differences in memory performance are more nuanced, studies of neural mechanisms are more informative. For example, females show more generalization between fear-associated contexts [••17,18] and differential activation of amygdala and hippocampus during retrieval [••17]. Together these findings suggest that females are not only using different neural circuit and/or molecular mechanisms during memory processes, but that different mechanisms are engaged because what information is retrieved is somewhat different in females compared with males. Accordingly, in cued fear discrimination tasks females initially show better discrimination between shock-paired and unpaired cues [19,20] but greater generalization after extended training [21]. This suggests that how these fear and “safety” memories are encoded, and what information is retrieved fundamentally differs between females and males.

Focusing on tasks that have clear sex differences in memory performance has had some success in identifying sex differences mechanisms of memory, but there are major limitations to this approach. First, sex differences in behavior do not necessarily represent differences in underlying neural mechanisms, and the lack of differential behavioral outcomes doesn’t rule out differences in mechanism [11,22]. Thus focusing on memory tasks that show striking differences in performance on memory tasks will fail to identify sex differences in neural mechanisms that mediate converging memory processes and behavioral responses. For example, both men and women show strong acquisition of fear conditioning, and activation of the canonical fear circuit (notably hippocampus and amygdala) [23,24], but women show greater activation of amygdala, medial prefrontal cortex, dorsal anterior cingulate cortex, and less retrosplenial cortex activation compared with men [25]. Second, the reliance of exploiting differences in behavioral performance assumes that females have the same conditioned responses (CR) as males. Yet recent work demonstrates sex differences in fear CRs. For example, in cued fear conditioning both male and female rats show freezing behavior, but females also show more rapid, active, “darting” responses to a conditioned fear stimulus [••26]. In fear potentiated startle, in which mice show an exaggerated startle response in the presence of a cue paired with footshock, females show slower extinction and less retention compared with males, in direct contrast to comparison experiments using a freezing CR that show faster extinction and greater retention by females [27,28]. Thus despite the apparent advantages of directly comparing females to males on memory performance, this approach fails to identify what females are learning, and how females are storing memory.

(2) Focusing on “known” memory-related mechanisms

The second approach to studying sex differences in mechanisms of memory has focused on brain regions and molecular mechanisms of memory that are well established in males, and testing whether these circuits and mechanisms are similarly required for memory in females. For example, in studies of fear conditioning, the canonical neural circuits including hippocampus and amygdala are well established in males [23,24]. Nevertheless, we have recently shown that although cFos activation is similar between the sexes during consolidation, during retrieval of context fear conditioning, females show more cFos activation in basal amygdala, whereas males show more activation in dorsal hippocampus [17]. Differential activation of amygdala and hippocampus between females and males have also been demonstrated in higher firing rates, number of spines, excitatory input, and LTP in amygdala [29,30], and weaker LTP in the hippocampus [3133] of females.

At a molecular level, using signaling pathways known for their role in male memory as a starting point has identified both molecular mechanisms that are common to both sexes (e.g. PKA and Akt) as well as male-specific mechanisms (e.g., including CaMKKα/β, cfos, bdnf), even under conditions of context fear conditioning where both sexes acquire robust memory [1316]. This suggests that both females and males recruit a set of basic memory mechanisms during consolidation, but with differential activation of a subset of sex-specific pathways and processes. Studies on transgenic animals generated based on mechanisms established males have identified both similarities and differences in mechanisms required for memory in females. For example, in GluA1 knockout mice, both sexes show deficits in spatial memory, but only males exhibit impairments of context fear conditioning [34]. Given the importance of AMPA receptor activation and trafficking to memory formation, and the importance of GluA1 in particular [35], this is a surprising and striking finding, yet it only tells us another male-specific mechanism. In mice with CREB mutations, both females and males show deficits in long-term memory for tone- and context fear conditioning, and for Morris Water Maze [36]. Nevertheless, sex differences in the role of hippocampal CREB in memory are observed in other studies, where males show reduced activation of CREB [37] and females show greater susceptibility to disruptive effects of more subtle manipulations of CREB [38]. That cyclic AMP (cAMP)-related signaling may be differentially required for memory in females compared with males is supported by the finding that mutations of PDE4 (3′,5′-cAMP-specific phosphodiesterases) result in enhanced LTP in male, but not female mice [39]. Targeting mechanisms required for memory in male animals has successfully identified mechanisms that are required for memory in both sexes, as well as male-specific mechanisms. This approach, however, fails to identify mechanisms recruited by females but not males.

Starting with a male-centric hypothesis and targeting known sex differences in performance or mechanisms of memory established in males can never identify female-specific mechanisms of learning and memory. More insidious, this male-centric approach comes with the central assumption that females are doing fundamentally the same thing as males, just more- or less- well. Questions of sex differences commonly framed as “(how) are females are different from males?” assume males are “normal” and females as somehow “less than normal”. In order to move forward, we need to reframe this question from a direct comparison between the sexes towards a female-centered view in order to identify what females are learning, how they are storing and retrieving memories, and thus determine where they differ from males.

Strategies to move beyond female vs male comparisons in learning and memory research

1. Identification of behavioral and cognitive strategies used by females (and males) in memory tasks

It is becoming apparent that behavioral and cognitive strategies preferentially engaged during memory tasks differ between the sexes. In addition to the well-known sex differences in spatial strategies [••40,41], recent work has highlighted that females and males differ in how they solve a variety of tasks [••17, ••26, ••42], suggesting that what information is learned and retrieved, and the behavioral response to fear-associated memory is not identical. To understand sex differences in learning and memory, we need to determine what CRs are most appropriate, and systematically assess the cognitive strategies used by both sexes — even when using the same CR — before we can adequately interpret sex differences in circuits and mechanisms underlying memory.

We have many tools to look beyond the level of responding in order to determine how animals are learning or remembering a given task. For example, in context fear conditioning, the concept that male rodents are learning a conjunctive context representation that gets associated with the aversive footshock, rather than associations between individual contextual elements with footshock was empirically established [4347]. Similarly, role of hippocampus in context fear conditioning, and the conditions under which context fear conditioning can be hippocampus-independent in male rodents was established over many years [43,4850]. Applying a similar rigorous and systematic experimental approach to learning and memory in females is essential for understanding what females are learning, what information is preferentially retrieved, and what behavioral responses to measure.

2. Female-focused approach

In order to validate tasks for females, identify whether the same CRs are appropriate measures of memory, and determine how females are solving memory tasks, there needs to be a shift away from a purely male-centric lens, towards a focus on females. This approach has some notable successes in identifying female-specific mechanisms of memory. For example, a body of work has examined the roles and molecular mechanisms of gonadal hormones on memory function in females ([5, ••51,52], and reviewed in this issue [53]). Importantly, these findings have profound insights for mechanisms of memory and synaptic plasticity in females and in males [••54].

Another female-focused approach is targeting circuits, pathways, and genes identified as having sex-specific activation or expression in human studies. In women, but not men, a polymorphism of pituitary adenylate-cyclase activating polypeptide receptor type 1 (PAC1R), increases risk for some symptoms of post-traumatic stress disorder [55]. Following these findings, recent work has identified PAC1R in prefrontal cortex as a female-specific mechanism for trace fear conditioning [••56]. Similarly, by focusing on sex differences in cholinergic signaling [57], a recent study has identified a differential role of cholinergic signaling in retrieval of memory in females and males, with inhibition of nicotinic acetylcholine receptors resulting in decreased freezing in males and increased freezing in females [58]. Targeting genes, neurotransmitter systems, and neural circuits known to exhibit sex differences in non-mnemonic tasks will be a powerful way to study sex differences, and specifically identify female-specific mechanisms underlying memory formation, consolidation, and retrieval.

3. Exploratory research and screening tools

The lack of molecular and neural targets identified as potentially important for memory in females is a current limitation to applying a female-focused approach. In order to effectively move away from the male-centric approach to studying memory, we need to use exploratory approaches to identify female-specific mechanisms. Screening of molecular pathways and neural circuits has had success in males, identifying distinct temporal and transcriptional patterns and epigenetic modifications between tasks [5961] and brain regions activated during retrieval of a recent versus a remote fear memory [62]. Exploratory approaches used together with brain regions previously identified as critical for memory will serve as a way to rapidly advance the identification of novel female-specific mechanisms.

There are some successes in females using an exploratory approach: by systematically examining neuronal activity within the basolateral and lateral amygdala — nuclei known for their critical role in fear conditioning — one recent study demonstrated higher baseline firing rate and more dendritic spines in the basal and lateral nuclei of the amygdala of females, associated with greater cue-dependent fear in females compared with males [29]. Another study demonstrated that female mice with the Vall66Met BNDF polymorphism show impaired CA3-dependent object place memory, whereas males exhibited intact memory [63]. These findings demonstrate a differential role of function and gene expression in females and males thus highlighting the importance of exploratory research in both sexes.

Exploratory research to determine what transcriptional, signaling, and neural loci are activated in females during learning, memory consolidation, and memory retrieval will be critical for identifying mechanisms and circuits that are not engaged by males. Recent advances in sequencing [64] and technologies for quantifying brain-wide activation (e.g., CLARITY; [65,66]) provide powerful, broad-based screening methods. Applying these tools in both sexes will identify novel mechanisms of learning and memory processes, additional targets for female-centric approaches to studying sex differences in memory, and insights into possible cognitive strategies engaged by females in learning and memory tasks.

Conclusion

Given sex differences in susceptibility to memory-related disorders including post-traumatic stress disorder, Alzheimer’s Disease and other dementias, understanding how both females and males learn, store, and retrieve memories is critical for developing novel treatments, preventive strategies, and identifying at-risk individuals. However, with the influx of new data on females and memory, in part as a consequence of NIH Sex as a Biological Variable initiative, it is clear a simple comparison of females to males on behavioral response or neural mechanism is not sufficient to identify and interpret how females and males solve problems of memory. Shifting toward a female-focused study of memory and its mechanisms, validating behavioral tasks in females, and utilizing exploratory approaches will be critical for reframing the question from “how are females different from males?” to “how are females (and males) solving this task?” and towards a new conceptual framework for understanding of how both sexes learn, encode, and retrieve memories.

HIGHLIGHTS.

  • Direct comparisons between sexes fail to detect female-specific memory mechanisms

  • Females and males differ in behavioral and cognitive strategies in memory tasks

  • Appropriate behavioral measures are key for studying what females are learning

  • Exploratory approaches are necessary to identify female-specific memory mechanisms

  • A female-focused approach is necessary for novel insights into learning and memory

Acknowledgments

Many thanks to A.A. Keiser for her helpful discussions of content and comments on the manuscript.

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

References

  • 1.Barco A, Bailey CH, Kandel ER. Common molecular mechanisms in explicit and implicit memory. J Neurochem. 2006;97:1520–1533. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-4159.2006.03870.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Jazin E, Cahill L. Sex differences in molecular neuroscience: from fruit flies to humans. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2010;11:9–17. doi: 10.1038/nrn2754. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.McCarthy MM. Sex differences in the developing brain as a source of inherent risk. Dialogues Clin Neurosci. 2016;18:361–372. doi: 10.31887/DCNS.2016.18.4/mmccarthy. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Koss WA, Frick KM. Sex differences in hippocampal function. J Neurosci Res. 2017;95:539–562. doi: 10.1002/jnr.23864. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Choleris E, Galea LAM, Sohrabji F, Frick KM. Sex differences in the brain: Implications for behavioral and biomedical research. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2018;85:126–145. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.07.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6••.Keiser AA, Tronson NC. Molecular mechanisms of memory in males and females. In: Shansky RM, editor. Sex Differences in the Central Nervous System. Elsevier Inc; 2015. p. 27. This recent review details mechanisms of memory in males, and the identified similarities and differences between the sexes. Notably, although a number of male-specific molecular mechanisms of memory have been identified, few female-specific mechanisms are known. [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Dalla C, Shors TJ. Sex differences in learning processes of classical and operant conditioning. Physiol Behav. 2009;97:229–238. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2009.02.035. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Kessler RC, Petukhova M, Sampson NA, Zaslavsky AM, Wittchen H-U. Twelve-month and lifetime prevalence and lifetime morbid risk of anxiety and mood disorders in the United States. Anxiety mood Disord United States. 2012;21:169–184. doi: 10.1002/mpr.1359. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Podcasy JL, Epperson CN. Considering sex and gender in Alzheimer disease and other dementias. Dialogues Clin Neurosci. 2016;18:437–446. doi: 10.31887/DCNS.2016.18.4/cepperson. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Quinn JJ, Hitchcott PK, Umeda EA, Arnold AP, Taylor JR. Sex chromosome complement regulates habit formation. Nat Publ Gr. 2007;10:1398–1400. doi: 10.1038/nn1994. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.de Vries GJ, Södersten P. Sex differences in the brain: The relation between structure and function. Horm Behav. 2009;55:589–596. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2009.03.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Gresack JE, Schafe GE, Orr PT, Frick KM. Sex differences in contextual fear conditioning are associated with differential ventral hippocampal extracellular signal-regulated kinase activation. Neuroscience. 2009;159:451–467. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2009.01.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Mizuno K, Giese KP. Towards a molecular understanding of sex differences in memory formation. Trends Neurosci. 2010;33:285–291. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2010.03.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Mizuno K, Antunes-Martins A, Ris L, Peters M, Godaux E, Giese KPP. Calcium/calmodulin kinase kinase β has a male-specific role in memory formation. Neuroscience. 2007;145:393–402. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2006.11.056. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Antunes-Martins A, Mizuno K, Irvine EE, Antunes-Martins A, Mizuno K, Irvine EE, Lepicard EM, Giese KP. Sex-dependent up-regulation of two splicing factors, Psf and Srp20, during hippocampal memory formation. Learn Mem. 2007;14:693–702. doi: 10.1101/lm.640307. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Mizuno K, Dempster E, Mill J, Giese KP. Longlasting regulation of hippocampal Bdnf gene transcription after contextual fear conditioning. Genes, Brain Behav. 2012;11:651–659. doi: 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2012.00805.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17••.Keiser AA, Turnbull LM, Darian MA, Feldman DE, Song I, Tronson NC. Sex Differences in Context Fear Generalization and Recruitment of Hippocampus and Amygdala during Retrieval. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2017;42:397–407. doi: 10.1038/npp.2016.174. Keiser et al. demonstrate that females show greater generalization of context fear and a bias towards basal amygdala activation during memory retrieval in females, compared with hippocampus activation in males. Together these findings suggest sex differences in what information is preferentially retrieved and in the neural mechanisms activated during memory recall. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Lynch JF, Winiecki P, Vanderhoof T, Riccio DC, Jasnow AM. Hippocampal cytosolic estrogen receptors regulate fear generalization in females. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2016;130:83–92. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2016.01.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Day HLLL, Reed MM, Stevenson CW. Sex differences in discriminating between cues predicting threat and safety. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2016;133:196–203. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2016.07.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Foilb AR, Bals J, Sarlitto MC, Christianson JP. Sex differences in fear discrimination do not manifest as differences in conditioned inhibition. Learn Mem. 2018;25:49–53. doi: 10.1101/lm.045500.117. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Lonsdorf TB, Haaker J, Schümann D, Sommer T, Bayer J, Brassen S, Bunzeck N, Gamer M, Kalisc R. Sex differences in conditioned stimulus discrimination during context-dependent fear learning and its retrieval in humans: the role of biological sex, contraceptives and menstrual cycle phases. J Psychiatry Neurosci. 2015;40:368–375. doi: 10.1503/140336. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.De Vries GJ. Minireview: Sex differences in adult and developing brains: compensation, compensation, compensation. Endocrinology. 2004;145:1063–1068. doi: 10.1210/en.2003-1504. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Maren S. Neurobiology of Pavlovian fear conditioning. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2001;24:897–931. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.24.1.897. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Izquierdo I, Furini CRG, Myskiw JC. Fear Memory. Physiol Rev. 2016;96:695–750. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00018.2015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Lebron-Milad K, Milad MR. Sex differences, gonadal hormones and the fear extinction network: Implications for anxiety disorders. BiolMmood Anxiety Disord. 2012;2:3. doi: 10.1186/2045-5380-2-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26••.Gruene TM, Flick K, Stefano A, Shea SD, Shansky RM. Sexually divergent expression of active and passive conditioned fear responses in rats. Elife. 2015:4. doi: 10.7554/eLife.11352. This paper describes sex differences in active (darting) and passive (freezing) CRs in females and males, highlighting the need for validating the appropriate conditioned response(s) to measure in females in fear-associated fear tasks. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Voulo ME, Parsons RG. Response-specific sex difference in the retention of fear extinction. Learn Mem. 2017;24:245–251. doi: 10.1101/lm.045641.117. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Gruene TM, Roberts E, Thomas V, Ronzio A, Shansky RM. Sex-specific neuroanatomical correlates of fear expression in prefrontal-amygdala circuits. Biol Psychiatry. 2015;78:186–193. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.11.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Blume SR, Freedberg M, Vantrease JE, Chan R, Padival M, Record MJ, DeJoseph MR, Urban JH, Rosenkranz JA. Sex- and estrus-dependent differences in rat basolateral amygdala. J Neurosci. 2017;37:758–17. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0758-17.2017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Chen L-SS, Tzeng W-YY, Chuang J-YY, Cherng CG, Gean P-WW, Yu L. Roles of testosterone and amygdaloid LTP induction in determining sex differences in fear memory magnitude. Horm Behav. 2014;66:498–508. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2014.07.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Maren S, De Oca B, Fanselow MS. Sex differences in hippocampal long-term potentiation (LTP) and Pavlovian fear conditioning in rats: positive correlation between LTP and contextual learning. Brain Res. 1994;661:25–34. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(94)91176-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Monfort P, Gomez-Gimenez B, Llansola M, Felipo V. Gender Differences in Spatial Learning, Synaptic Activity, and Long-Term Potentiation in the Hippocampus in Rats: Molecular Mechanisms. ACS Chem Neurosci. 2015;6:1420–1427. doi: 10.1021/acschemneuro.5b00096. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Yang D-W, Pan B, Han T-Z, Xie W. Sexual dimorphism in the induction of LTP: Critical role of tetanizing stimulation. Life Sci. 2004;75:119–127. doi: 10.1016/j.lfs.2003.12.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Dachtler J, Fox KD, Good MA. Gender specific requirement of GluR1 receptors in contextual conditioning but not spatial learning. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2011;96:461–467. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2011.07.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Rumpel S, LeDoux J, Zador A, Malinow R. Postsynaptic receptor trafficking underlying a form of associative learning. Science (80-) 2005;308:83–88. doi: 10.1126/science.1103944. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Bourtchuladze R, Frengueili B, Blendy J, Cioffi D, Schutz G, Silva AJ. Deficient Long-Term Memory in Mice with a Targeted Mutation of the CAMP-Responsive Element-Binding Protein. Cell. 1994;79:59–68. doi: 10.1016/0092-8674(94)90400-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Kudo K, Qiao C-X, Kanba S, Arita J. A selective increase in phosphorylation of cyclic AMP response element-binding protein in hippocampal CA1 region of male, but not female, rats following contextual fear and passive avoidance conditioning. Brain Res. 2004;1024:233–243. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2004.08.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Hebda-Bauer EK, Luo J, Watson SJ, Akil H. Female CREBαδ-deficient mice show earlier age-related cognitive deficits than males. Neuroscience. 2007;150:260–272. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2007.09.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Campbell SL, van Groen T, Kadish I, Smoot LHM, Bolger GB. Altered phosphorylation, electrophysiology, and behavior on attenuation of PDE4B action in hippocampus. BMC Neurosci. 2017;18:77. doi: 10.1186/s12868-017-0396-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40••.Yagi S, Chow C, Lieblich SE, Galea LAM. Sex and strategy use matters for pattern separation, adult neurogenesis, and immediate early gene expression in the hippocampus. Hippocampus. 2016;26:87–101. doi: 10.1002/hipo.22493. This paper finds sex differences in mechanism of discrimination between spatial memories even when males and females use the same strategy for the task. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Bettis TJ, Jacobs LF. Sex differences in memory for landmark arrays in C57BL/J6 mice. Anim Cogn. 2013;16:873–882. doi: 10.1007/s10071-013-0619-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42••.Shansky RM. Sex differences in behavioral strategies: avoiding interpretational pitfalls. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2018;49:95–98. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2018.01.007. This exceptionally important new review discusses the need to assess whether behavioral responses differ between males and females before jumping to the interpretation that sex differences in performance reflect better/worse ability in a variety of tasks. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Rudy JW. Contextual conditioning and auditory cue conditioning dissociate during development. Behav Neurosci. 1993;107:887–891. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.107.5.887. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Rudy JW, Barrientos RM, O’Reilly RC. Hippocampal formation supports conditioning to memory of a context. Behav Neurosci. 2002;116:530–538. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.116.4.530. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Rudy JW, O’Reilly RC. Contextual fear conditioning, conjunctive representations, pattern completion, and the hippocampus. Behav Neurosci. 1999;113:867. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.113.5.867. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Barrientos RM, O’Reilly RC, Rudy JW. Memory for context is impaired by injecting anisomycin into dorsal hippocampus following context exploration. Behav Brain Res. 2002;134:299–306. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(02)00045-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Matus-Amat P, Higgins EA, Barrientos RM, Rudy JW. The Role of the Dorsal Hippocampus in the Acquisition and Retrieval of Context Memory Representations. J Neurosci. 2004;24:2431–2439. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1598-03.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Phillips RG, LeDoux JE. Differential contribution of amygdala and hippocampus to cued and contextual fear conditioning. Behav Neurosci. 1992;106:274. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.106.2.274. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Maren S, Aharonov G, Fanselow MS. Neurotoxic lesions of the dorsal hippocampus and Pavlovian fear conditioning in rats. Behav Brain Res. 1997;88:261–274. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(97)00088-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Zelikowsky M, Hersman S, Chawla MK, Barnes CA, Fanselow MS. Neuronal Ensembles in Amygdala, Hippocampus, and Prefrontal Cortex Track Differential Components of Contextual Fear. J Neurosci. 2014;34:8462–8466. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3624-13.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51••.Frick KM, Tuscher JJ, Koss WA, Kim J, Taxier LR. Estrogenic regulation of memory consolidation: A look beyond the hippocampus, ovaries, and females. Physiol Behav. 2018;87:57–66. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2017.07.028. This review details the important role of locally synthesized and circulating estrogens on memory in both females and males and across the adult lifespan. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Huang GZ, Woolley CS. Estradiol acutely suppresses inhibition in the hippocampus through a sex-specific endocannabinoid and mGluR-dependent mechanism. Neuron. 2012;74:801–8. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.03.035. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Frick KM, Kim J, Koss WA. Estradiol and hippocampal memory in female and male rodents. Curr Opin Behav Sci. 2018 doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2018.03.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54••.Oberlander JG, Woolley CS. 17β-Estradiol Acutely Potentiates Glutamatergic Synaptic Transmission in the Hippocampus through Distinct Mechanisms in Males and Females. J Neurosci. 2016;36:2677–90. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4437-15.2016. This study found diverging molecular mechanisms mediating estradiol effects at the synapse in females and males, emphasizing the need to study both sexes. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Ressler KJ, Mercer KB, Bradley B, Jovanovic T, Mahan A, Kerley K, Norrholm SD, Kilaru V, Smith AK, Myers AJ, et al. Post-traumatic stress disorder is associated with PACAP and the PAC1 receptor. Nature. 2011;470:492–497. doi: 10.1038/nature09856. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56••.Kirry AJ, Herbst MR, Poirier SE, Maskeri MM, Rothwell AC, Twining RC, Gilmartin MR. Pituitary adenylate-cyclase activating-polypeptide (PACAP) signaling in the prefrontal cortex modulates cued fear learning, but not spatial working memory, in female rats. Neuropharmacology. 2018;133:145–154. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2018.01.010. This paper identifies PAC1 in medial prefrontal cortex as a female-specific mechanism of trace fear conditioning. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Mitsushima D. Sex Differences in the Septo-Hippocampal Cholinergic System in Rats: Behavioral Consequences. Springer; Berlin, Heidelberg: 2010. pp. 57–71. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Rashid H, Mahboob A, Ahmed T. Role of cholinergic receptors in memory retrieval depends on gender and age of memory. Behav Brain Res. 2017;331:233–240. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2017.05.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Poplawski SG, Schoch H, Wimmer ME, Hawk JD, Walsh JL, Giese KP, Abel T. Object-location training elicits an overlapping but temporally distinct transcriptional profile from contextual fear conditioning. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2014;116:90–95. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.09.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Peixoto LL, Wimmer ME, Poplawski SG, Tudor JC, Kenworthy CA, Liu S, Mizuno K, Garcia BA, Zhang NR, Giese K, et al. Memory acquisition and retrieval impact different epigenetic processes that regulate gene expression. BMC Genomics. 2015;16(Suppl 5):S5. doi: 10.1186/1471-2164-16-S5-S5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Poplawski SG, Peixoto L, Porcari GS, Wimmer ME, McNally AG, Mizuno K, Giese KP, Chatterjee S, Koberstein JN, Risso D, et al. Contextual fear conditioning induces differential alternative splicing. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2016;134(Pt B):221–235. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2016.07.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Wheeler AL, Teixeira CM, Wang AH, Xiong X, Kovacevic N, Lerch JP, McIntosh AR, Parkinson J, Frankland PW. Identification of a Functional Connectome for Long-Term Fear Memory in Mice. PLoS Comp Biol. 2013;9:e1002853. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1002853. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Marrocco J, Petty GH, Ríos MB, Gray JD, Kogan JF, Waters EM, Schmidt EF, Lee FS, McEwen BS. A sexually dimorphic pre-stressed translational signature in CA3 pyramidal neurons of BDNF Val66Met mice. Nat Commun. 2017;8:808. doi: 10.1038/s41467-017-01014-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Rubin TG, Gray JD, McEwen BS. Experience and the ever-changing brain: what the transcriptome can reveal. Bioessays. 2014;36:1072–1081. doi: 10.1002/bies.201400095. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Chung K, Deisseroth K. CLARITY for mapping the nervous system. Nat Methods. 2013;10:508–513. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.2481. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Deisseroth K. Form Meets Function in the Brain: Observing the Activity and Structure of Specific Neural Connections. Springer; 2016. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES