Abstract
Atypical habituation and aberrant exploration of novel stimuli have been related to the severity of autism spectrum disorders (ASDs), but the underlying neuronal circuits are unknown. Here we show that chemogenetic inhibition of dopamine (DA) neurons of the ventral tegmental area (VTA) attenuates exploration toward nonfamiliar conspecifics and interferes with the reinforcing properties of nonfamiliar conspecific interaction in mice. Exploration of nonfamiliar stimuli is associated with the insertion of GluA2-lacking AMPA receptors at excitatory synapses on VTA DA neurons. These synaptic adaptations persist upon repeated exposure to social stimuli and sustain conspecific interaction. Global or VTA DA neuron-specific loss of the ASD-associated synaptic adhesion molecule neuroligin 3 alters the behavioral response toward nonfamiliar conspecifics and the reinforcing properties of conspecific interaction. These behavioral deficits are accompanied by an aberrant expression of AMPA receptors and an occlusion of synaptic plasticity. Altogether, these findings link impaired exploration of nonfamiliar conspecifics to VTA DA neuron dysfunction in mice.
Individuals with autism spectrum disorder have alteration in social and novelty behaviors. Here, Bellone and colleagues show that chemogenetic inhibition of mouse dopamine neurons in the ventral tegmental area can blunt exploration towards unfamiliar conspecifics, and that these behavioral deficits are recapitulated in mice lacking neuroligin3 gene product.
Introduction
From infancy, we encounter an array of diverse stimuli from the environment. Repeated exposure to a stimulus can result in habituation whereas nonfamiliar stimuli usually increases exploratory behavior. Habituation and novelty recognition allow us focusing attention on what is unknown, promote exploratory behavior, facilitate learning, and are predictive of cognitive function later in life1. Several neuropsychiatric disorders are characterized by deficits in habituation and novelty exploration. In autism spectrum disorder (ASD), young patients show prolonged attention to depictions of objects, but reduced attention to social stimuli2. Moreover, ASD patients are hyporesponsive to novel visual stimuli and exhibit slowed habituation to faces3,4. Such alterations are observed in a significant number of individuals with ASD, as they have been reported in clinical studies using diverse stimuli and read-outs5–7. However, the circuits and neuronal mechanisms underlying this specific aspect of the ASD phenotype remain largely unknown.
Dopamine (DA) neurons in the ventral tegmental area (VTA) and substantia nigra pars compacta (SNc) may contribute to the habituation to familiar stimuli and to the exploration of nonfamiliar stimuli. DA neurons increase their activity in response to novel environments8, to stimuli of positive or negative value9, and to natural rewards10. Interestingly, these neurons also respond to nonrewarding novel stimuli and their responses habituate when the stimulus becomes familiar11,12. This has led to the proposal that novelty by itself may be rewarding. In rodents, nonfamiliar conspecifics or nonfamiliar objects increase Ca2+-transients in VTA DA neurons and this activity is necessary to promote social, but not object exploration13. Glutamatergic synapses onto DA neurons undergo several forms of synaptic plasticity that may contribute to the modification of social interactions in response to experience. Specific synaptic adaptations have been described during development, after drug exposure, cue-reward learning, reciprocal social interactions, and after repeated burst stimulation of DA neurons14–18. Furthermore, glutamatergic transmission is altered in several ASD animal models19, and we have recently shown that deficits in the postnatal development of excitatory transmission onto VTA DA neurons lead to sociability deficits20. Notably, several studies highlight decreased social reward processing in patients with ASD21,22, and these alterations have been hypothesized to precipitate further developmental consequences in social cognition and communication23. Whether specific forms of synaptic plasticity in the VTA are induced by exposure to nonfamiliar stimuli (novelty-induced synaptic plasticity), and whether aberrant plasticity associated with exploration of nonfamiliar conspecific in the VTA is related to the maladaptive responses in ASD mouse models is still largely unknown.
In this study, we parse the response to and the preference for nonfamiliar conspecifics as specific aspects of sociability controlled by DA neurons. We demonstrate that intact VTA DA neuron excitability is necessary to express a preference for nonfamiliar conspecifics but not for nonfamiliar objects. Additionally, we adopt a conditioned place preference protocol, based on interaction with familiar or nonfamiliar conspecific, to demonstrate that VTA DA neuron function underlies the reinforcing properties of social interaction. Mice lacking the ASD-associated synaptic adhesion molecule neuroligin 3 (Nlgn3) exhibit aberrant exploration of nonfamiliar conspecifics as well as deficit in habituation processing. These phenotypes are recapitulated by VTA DA neuron-specific down-regulation of Nlgn3. Finally, we discovered a form of novelty-induced synaptic plasticity at glutamatergic inputs onto VTA DA neurons that sustains conspecific interactions and is impaired in Nlgn3 KO and Nlgn3 VTA DA knockdown mice.
Results
VTA DA neurons and exploration of nonfamiliar conspecifics
Mice have been reported to interact with their conspecifics, to habituate upon repeated contact with the same subject, and to exhibit increased exploration when subsequently brought into contact with a nonfamiliar mouse24. To examine whether VTA DA neurons regulate exploration of nonfamiliar conspecifics, we examined the behavior of mice in which the inhibitory DREADD (hM4Di)25 or mCherry were virally expressed in DA neurons of the VTA (VTA::DAhM4Di: AAV5-hSyn-DIO-hM4Di-mCherry or VTA::DAmCherry: AAV5-hSyn-DIO-mCherry injected into DAT-Cre mice, Fig. 1a). Virus infusions led to mCherry expression in 50% of TH+ (tyrosine hydroxylase, an enzyme necessary for DA synthesis) VTA neurons and in few (2%) of TH+ cells in the neighboring substantia nigra pars compacta (SNc; Supplementary Fig. 1a), confirming preferential targeting of the VTA. Application of the hM4Di ligand clozapine-n-oxide (CNO) decreased the neuronal excitability of VTA::DAhM4Di neurons compared to VTA::DAmCherry ex vivo (Supplementary Fig. 1b) and decreases DA release in striatal regions in vivo26.
We then assessed the time spent in social interaction upon repeated exposure to the same mouse (habituation) and the subsequent response to a nonfamiliar conspecific (Fig. 1b). To compare between social and nonsocial stimuli, we also examined the behavioral responses to familiar and nonfamiliar objects (Supplementary Fig. 1c–f). When repeatedly exposed to the same mouse (Fig. 1c) or object stimulus (Supplementary Fig. 1c; s1 and o1, respectively), VTA::DAhM4Di animals injected with vehicle show progressive reduction in the interaction with the stimuli over days. This process has been referred to as habituation27. After four habituation days, the animals increased their exploratory behavior toward either a nonfamiliar social (s2; Fig. 1d) or a nonfamiliar object stimulus (o2, Supplementary Fig. 1d) at day 5.
To study the role of VTA DA neurons in this behavioral trait, DA neuron excitability was decreased by intra-peritoneal (i.p.) injection of CNO in VTA::DAhM4Di before the exposure to the nonfamiliar stimulus at day 5. VTA::DAhM4Di animals decreased their exploratory behavior toward the nonfamiliar conspecific whereas control VTA::DAmCherry mice treated with CNO showed unaltered stimulus exploration (s2, Fig. 1e, f). Interestingly, when exposed to a nonfamiliar object (o2), both VTA::DAhM4Di and VTA::DAmCherry animals treated with CNO exhibited an increased exploration (Supplementary Fig. 1e, f). Thus, reducing VTA DA neuron excitability specifically alters the exploration of a nonfamiliar conspecific, but not of a nonfamiliar object, suggesting a differential requirement of DA neuron activity for driving exploration of social and inanimate stimuli.
VTA DA neurons and preference for nonfamiliar conspecifics
To assess the role of VTA DA neuron excitability in mediating the exploration of a nonfamiliar conspecific over an inanimate object or a familiar conspecific stimulus, VTA::DAhM4Di and VTA::DAmCherry mice were subject to the three-chamber test28 under vehicle and CNO conditions. The test was performed twice: first, animals received either vehicle or CNO and, after 1 week of washout, the test was repeated and the pharmacological treatment was counterbalanced (Fig. 2a). To monitor potential off target effects of CNO, we also included VTA::DAmCherry mice treated with CNO as controls. During the task, test mice were given a choice between an object (o1) versus a nonfamiliar mouse (s1 or s3) and subsequently a choice between a familiar (second exposure to s1 or s3) versus a nonfamiliar conspecific (s2 or s4).
Previous studies define sociability in this assay as longer time spent in the chamber with the same-sex nonfamiliar mouse rather than in the chamber with the object, and more time spent sniffing the same-sex mouse rather than sniffing the object29,30. According to these criteria, VTA::DAhM4Di mice treated with vehicle (Supplementary Fig. 2a, d), VTA::DAmCherry mice treated with CNO (Supplementary Fig. 2b, e) as well as VTA::DAhM4Di mice treated with CNO (Supplementary Fig. 2c, f) exhibited sociability. We observed a decreased distance moved upon CNO-mediated reduction of DA neuron excitability (Supplementary Fig. 2g). However, despite the reduced locomotion, mice still expressed social preference.
In the second phase of the task, we assessed preference for social novelty that was defined as follows: longer time spent in the chamber with the same-sex nonfamiliar rather than in the chamber with the familiar mouse and more time spent sniffing the same-sex nonfamiliar conspecific rather than sniffing the familiar mouse, particularly during the first 5 min of the test28. While preference for social novelty was exhibited by VTA::DAhM4Di mice treated with vehicle (Fig. 2b, e) and by VTA::DAmCherry mice treated with CNO (Fig. 2c, f), it was absent in VTA::DAhM4Di mice treated with CNO (Fig. 2d, g). CNO treated VTA::DAhM4Di mice displayed a reduction in distance moved (Fig. 2h). Additionally, to compare preference for social novelty across groups, we calculated a “social novelty index”, as time spent sniffing the nonfamiliar stimulus minus time spent exploring the familiar target, in the first and last 5 min of the assay. We found that the social novelty index was reduced by CNO injections in VTA::DAhM4Di mice compared to both CNO treated VTA::DAmCherry and vehicle treated VTA::DAhM4Di (Fig. 2i). Altogether, these findings indicate that reducing the excitability of DA neurons decreases the exploration of novel social stimuli, when given a choice between nonfamiliar and familiar conspecifics.
VTA DA neurons and nonfamiliar conspecific reinforcement
To investigate whether nonfamiliar conspecific interactions are reinforcing in mice, we performed a conditioned place preference (CPP) task (modified from 31, 32). Briefly, test mice are housed with familiar mice throughout the protocol. After the Pre-TEST, we performed 4 days of repeated conditioning where wild-type (WT) mice learn to associate one compartment of the apparatus with the presence of either a familiar conspecific (familiar, f1), a nonfamiliar conspecific (s1) or a nonfamiliar object (o1) stimulus, while the other compartment is left empty (Fig. 3a, b). At day 5 (Post-TEST) the preference to explore the two compartments, in absence of any stimulus, was quantified and compared to Pre-TEST. While no significant preference was developed for the familiar conspecifics (Fig. 3c and Supplementary Fig. 3a), mice exhibited preference for the compartment associated with the nonfamiliar conspecifics (Fig. 3d and Supplementary Fig. 3b), and an avoidance for the novel object stimulus associated chamber (Fig. 3e and Supplementary Fig. 3c). Interestingly, across conditioning sessions, we observed habituation to all the stimuli (Fig. 3f–h). However, when the time of interaction with the stimulus during the first and the last day of conditioning was analyzed, we observed a longer interaction with the nonfamiliar conspecifics compared to the other stimuli at either time point (Fig. 3i). These data suggest that a nonfamiliar conspecific remains salient over days and promotes contextual associative learning.
To assess the role of VTA DA neuron excitability in mediating the reinforcing properties of nonfamiliar conspecific interactions, both control VTA::DAmCherry and VTA::DAhM4Di received injections of CNO before each conditioning session and were treated with vehicle before the Post-TEST (Fig. 3j). Control VTA::DAmCherry but not VTA::DAhM4Di mice developed a preference for the compartment associated with the nonfamiliar conspecifics (Fig. 3k and Supplementary Fig. 3d, e). These observations suggest that the excitability of DA neurons mediates both the interaction with nonfamiliar conspecifics as well as the acquisition of nonfamiliar conspecific-induced contextual associations.
Altered conspecific interactions in Nlgn3KO mice
Patients with ASD exhibit slowed habituation to faces4 and are less responsive to social reward22. Thus, we tested whether a deletion of Nlgn3 in mice, a category 2 (strong candidate) classified ASD-linked gene (http://gene.sfari.org)33–35 encoding a postsynaptic adhesion molecule36, might result in deficits in exploration of nonfamiliar conspecifics and in the reinforcing properties of conspecific interaction. Global Nlgn3KO mice37 exhibit reduced ultrasonic vocalization and social memory in male–female interactions as well as altered motor behaviors and olfaction38–41. We examined the interaction time upon repeated exposure to a familiar mouse (habituation) and the subsequent response to a nonfamiliar conspecific (Fig. 4a). Nlgn3KO mice exhibited overall lower interaction times, no significant habituation, and lacked the increased response to nonfamiliar conspecifics seen in Wild Type (WT) littermates (Fig. 4b, c and Supplementary Fig. 4a–d). However, Nlgn3KO mice showed habituation, increased exploration of nonfamiliar objects (Fig. 4d, e) and preference for nonfamiliar objects in a novel object recognition task (Fig. 4f–h). This indicates that both novelty preference and memory for objects are unaltered. In addition to impaired response to nonfamiliar conspecifics, Nlgn3KO mutants exhibit alterations in motor activity (Fig. 4i) and marble burying (Fig. 4j). In an olfactory discrimination test42, Nlgn3KO male mice showed normal response and habituation to a social odor (Supplementary Fig. 4e). However, the mutant mice had a significantly decreased response when subsequently presented to a second (novel) social odor (Supplementary Fig. 4e). To further examine conspecific interaction in Nlgn3KO mice, we tested the reinforcing properties of social interaction31,32. When mice are conditioned in a conditioned place preference paradigm with familiar mice, Nlgn3KO mice did not develop a preference for the social compartments, whereas WT mice did (Fig. 4k, l, and Supplementary Fig. 4f, g). These findings suggest that Nlgn3KO mice exhibit altered social interactions and defects in social reward behaviors.
Nlgn3 loss-of-function in VTA DA neurons alters sociability
The diverse alterations in social but also nonsocial behaviors in Nlgn3KO mice, indicate that multiple different systems might contribute to their phenotype. To test whether any alterations are due to Nlgn3 functions in VTA DA neurons we generated microRNA-based knock-down vectors for conditional suppression of Nlgn3 expression (Supplementary Fig. 5a, b). Cre-dependent AAV-based vectors were injected into the developing VTA of DAT-Cre mice at postnatal days 5–6 and mice were analyzed using a battery of behavioral tests (AAV2-DIO-miRNlgn3 in DAT-Cre mice: VTA::DANL3KD, Fig. 5a, b, and see Supplementary Fig. 5c for off-target areas affected and Supplementary Fig. 5d, e for further controls). Notably, VTA::DANL3KD mice exhibited a similar impairment in reinforcing properties of conspecific interaction as the global Nlgn3KO mice in the conditioned place preference paradigm (Fig. 5c, d, and Supplementary Fig. 5f, g) indicating that Nlgn3 downregulation in VTA DA neurons is sufficient to mimic this aspect of the global Nlgn3KO phenotype. Furthermore, when repeatedly exposed to the same and subsequently to a nonfamiliar conspecific, VTA::DANL3KD mice showed an overall reduction in social exploration and a blunted response to novel conspecific stimuli (Fig. 5e–g, and Supplementary Fig. 5h–k). At the same time, VTA::DANL3KD mice showed preference for novel objects in the novel object recognition task (Fig. 5h–j). Thus, there is a specific requirement for Nlgn3 in VTA DA neurons for appropriate exploration of nonfamiliar conspecifics and for the reinforcing properties of social interaction. By contrast, motor activity, marble burying, and social olfaction that are altered in global Nlgn3KO mice were not modified in the VTA::DANL3KD mutants (Fig. 5k, l, Supplementary Fig. 5l). Interestingly, we observed that knock-down of Nlgn3 in VTA-DA neurons of adult mice produced a similar but less pronounced social interaction phenotype as in developing animals, with reduced habituation and reduced response to nonfamiliar conspecifics (Supplementary Fig. 6). Thus, Nlgn3 expression, in both developing and mature VTA DA circuits, is required for habituation and nonfamiliar conspecific exploration.
A synaptic signature of saliency detection in VTA DA neurons
Several experiences strengthen synaptic transmission at excitatory inputs onto DA neurons and drive the insertion of GluA2-lacking AMPARs, which can be assessed by calculating a rectification index (RI)17. We tested whether nonfamiliar exploration induced specific forms of long-lasting synaptic plasticity at excitatory inputs onto DA neurons in the VTA (novelty-induced synaptic plasticity). In WT mice, the RI increased at synapses 24 h after the exploration of either a nonfamiliar mouse or a nonfamiliar object when compared to RI calculated from home caged mice (Fig. 6a). By contrast, the RI was unchanged after the exposure to a new context and AMPA/NMDA ratios were unchanged for any of the above conditions (Fig. 6b). When AMPAR EPSCs were recorded after repeated exposure (over 4 days) to object stimuli, the RI was normalized to control condition (Fig. 6c). A subsequent exposure to a new object (o2) increased the RI (Supplementary Fig. 7a). By contrast, GluA2-lacking AMPARs were detected in mice repeatedly exposed to a nonfamiliar conspecific stimulus (s1) over a 4-day period and were still present at these synapses after 10 days of repeated exposure (Fig. 6c). Remarkably, the AMPA/NMDA ratio was significantly elevated after 4 days of social (s1) repeated exposure relative to baseline but was normalized after 10 days of repeated exposure (Fig. 6d), while the paired-pulse ratio (PPR) remained unchanged throughout (Supplementary Fig. 7b). Taken together, these data indicate that repeated exposure to a nonfamiliar conspecific stimulus, but not an object stimulus, transiently increases synaptic strength (AMPA/NMDA ratio) and produces a stable insertion of GluA2-lacking AMPARs at VTA DA neuron excitatory inputs.
To understand the functional role of noncanonical AMPARs inserted during nonfamiliar conspecific exposure, we infused the GluA2-lacking AMPAR blocker NASPM into the VTA starting from the second day of interaction with either social or object stimuli (Fig. 7a, b). NASPM infused mice reduced the interaction with a conspecific stimulus upon repeated exposure (Fig. 7c); by contrast, the infusions did not alter long-term habituation to an object (Fig. 7d), interaction in the home cage between two familiar mice or distance moved in an open field (Supplementary Fig. 7c–e). To further understand the impact of GluA2-lacking AMPARs at VTA DA neuron inputs on conspecific repeated exposure, we promoted the insertion of GluA2-lacking AMPARs via blue-light illumination of ChR2 or eYFP expressing VTA DA neurons18 of DAT-Cre mice (VTA::DAChR2: AAV5-Ef1α-DIO-ChR2(H134R)-eYFP, VTA::DAeYFP: AAV5-Ef1α-DIO-eYFP). DA neuron stimulation consisted in 15-minute long ChR2-mediated bursts of action potentials18 delivered the day before each conspecific exposure (Fig. 7e, f). This noncontingent burst activation increased RI in photocurrent positive neurons (IChR2+; Fig. 7g) and blocked habituation to social stimuli (Fig. 7h). Altogether, these data indicate that GluA2-lacking AMPARs might represent a synaptic signature of conspecific saliency and, once inserted, their activity counteracts habituation.
Nlgn3 loss-of-function impairs novelty-induced plasticity
Nlgn3 has been implicated in the regulation of AMPARs at glutamatergic synapses39. We therefore hypothesized that defects in DA neuron synaptic function could represent the mechanism underlying the aberrant habituation to familiar conspecifics and response to nonfamiliar conspecifics in VTA::DANL3KD mice. We explored glutamate receptor function in VTA DA neurons of global Nlgn3KO and conditional VTA::DANL3KD mice. Notably, we observed increased RI of AMPAR-mediated currents indicating the aberrant presence of GluA2-lacking AMPARs at excitatory inputs onto VTA DA neurons in both Nlgn3 loss-of-function models (Fig. 8a). Given the abnormal elevation of GluA2-lacking AMPARs in naïve VTA::DANL3KD mice, we hypothesized that in these mice synaptic plasticity induced by exposure to nonfamiliar conspecifics might be occluded. Indeed, GluA2-lacking AMPARs in VTA DA neurons were not further increased 24 h after nonfamiliar social stimulus exposure in VTA::DANL3KD mice (Fig. 8b). Thus, aberrant plasticity of GluA2-lacking AMPARs in VTA DA neurons is associated with an impaired response to a social novel stimulus.
Discussion
In this study, we establish that intact VTA DA neuron excitability is necessary for (1) the exploration of nonfamiliar social stimuli, (2) the preference for nonfamiliar versus familiar conspecifics, and (3) the acquisition of nonfamiliar conspecific-induced contextual associations. Novel stimuli, independent of their nature, leave a plasticity trace at glutamatergic synapses in the VTA, which persists upon repeated exposure to social stimuli and supports sustained conspecific interactions. We use a deletion of the ASD-associated gene Nlgn3 and demonstrate that global Nlgn3 knock-out results in an impaired habituation and an aberrant exploration of nonfamiliar conspecifics. Furthermore, selective inactivation of Nlgn3 in VTA DA neurons disrupts novelty-induced plasticity at glutamatergic synapses in the VTA, alters exploration of nonfamiliar conspecific, and the reinforcing properties of conspecific interactions while having no detectable effect on motor behaviors or olfaction.
Global loss of Nlgn3 is also accompanied by a broad spectrum of additional phenotypes, including changes in olfaction and in motor-related behaviors38–41. Thus, the origin of social behavior alterations in these mice was unclear. Previous studies explored phenotypes in mice carrying a point mutation in Nlgn3 that reduces (but does not abolish) Nlgn3 expression and has been observed in 2 patients from one family43. For this model, it was concluded that behavioral phenotypes are significantly dependent on the genetic context with significant phenotypes reported for some genetic backgrounds but not others44–46. Our study demonstrates that VTA DA neuron specific Nlgn3 loss of function is sufficient to recapitulate sociability deficits reported in global KO mice.
Although several studies have provided instrumental information about the neuronal circuits, within the reward system, that control social behavior in rodents13,47–49, the synaptic adaptations occurring at VTA DA neurons during interactions with nonfamiliar conspecifics remained largely unknown. Here, we show that while reduced excitability or conditional suppression of Nlgn3 in VTA DA neurons affect the exploration of nonfamiliar conspecifics, they both fail to modify responses to novel objects, presumably because of the higher intrinsic salience of social stimuli. Consistent with this hypothesis, we observe that while both nonfamiliar object and conspecific exploration trigger the insertion of GluA2-lacking AMPARs, only the repeated exposure to the same nonfamiliar mouse results in the maintenance of GluA2-lacking AMPARs at glutamatergic synapses of VTA DA neurons. Therefore, we hypothesize that while the insertion of non-canonical AMPARs reflects the novelty associated to the stimulus, their persistence signals the higher salience of the conspecific over the object stimulus. The insertion and the expression of noncanonical AMPARs has been previously associated with nonsocial and highly salient experiences, such as cocaine exposure17. However, a causal relationship between behavioral responses to salient stimuli and GluA2-lacking AMPAR expression at VTA DA neurons has not been reported. Here, we show that the insertion of noncanonical AMPARs at VTA DA neurons contributes to behavioral responses to social stimuli suggesting that these receptors could also represent a functionally-relevant synaptic signature responsible for the behavioral responses associated with other salient stimuli.
Changes in AMPA/NMDA ratio occur in response to both rewarding and aversive processes50, and synaptic strengthening is transiently expressed and necessary for associative learning15. Consistent with previous findings16, we report an increased AMPA/NMDA ratio at VTA DA neuron excitatory inputs in response to social interaction, which is transiently expressed upon repeated exposure to nonfamiliar conspecifics, but not object stimuli. However, although the increased synaptic strength might represent an additional signature related to the saliency of social interaction, its role in habituation processing and, possibly contextual learning, warrants further investigation.
In recent years, accumulating evidences indicate that synaptic adaptations associated to reward and aversion occur at projection-specific subclasses of VTA DA neurons51,52. An anatomic-functional segregation of reward circuitry is also emerging in respect of social behavior: while VTA DA neurons projecting to the nucleus accumbens (NAc), but not prefrontal cortex (PFC), promote conspecific interaction13, DA neuron projections to the interpenduncular nucleus control familiarity signaling47. Thus, the specific synaptic signatures observed in response to nonfamiliar conspecific exposure might also occur in dedicated VTA circuits. At the same time, given the intrinsic diversity of sensory and emotional information provided by social vs. inanimate stimuli, it is conceivable that synaptic plasticity occurs at specific inputs to defined subclasses of VTA DA neurons. Additional investigations of synaptic properties of defined inputs to projection-specific DA neuron subclasses is needed to further understand the circuits and the synaptic mechanisms underlying both novelty and saliency processing associated with conspecific and inanimate stimuli.
Altered social interactions and communication are defining aspects of the autism phenotype. However, such alterations may arise from a plethora of neuronal processing defects, ranging from alterations in perception, sensory processing, multisensory integration, or positive and negative valence assigned to conspecific stimuli23. In this work, we specifically explore neuronal circuitry relevant for the exploration to and the preference for nonfamiliar conspecifics. We chose this domain, as studies in children with ASD demonstrated altered habituation and responses to novel stimuli7,53. Notably, in toddlers, a slowed habituation to faces but normal habituation to repeatedly viewed objects has been reported to coincide with more severe ASD symptoms54. Several rodent models of ASD exhibit altered social novelty responses24,55, and such alterations have been suggested to reflect changes in social memory or discrimination. However, brain areas and circuit elements contributing to these changes in habituation and social novelty responses in mice and humans are largely unknown. Our rodent work not only highlights a contribution of VTA DA neurons to this process but also takes steps toward identification of the synaptic basis of social novelty responses and habituation. Considering the complexity of ASD behavioral dysfunctions, we propose that fractionating the autism phenotype according to specific behavioral domains based on neuronal circuit elements will provide a productive stratification criterion for patient populations. Thus, we speculate that in a subpopulation of individuals with ASD alterations in VTA DA function might contribute to the social interaction phenotype whereas in other subgroups of patients alterations in social interaction may arise for different reasons. A prediction from this hypothesis is that stratification of patient populations based on an assessment of novelty responses, habituation, and social reward may help to identify subgroups of patients that would particularly benefit from interventions targeting function and plasticity of the VTA-DA circuit elements.
Methods
Animals
The study was conducted with WT and transgenic mice in C57BL/6J background. WT mice were obtained from Charles River. For DA neuron-specific manipulations DAT-iresCre (Slc6a3tm1.1(cre)Bkmn)56 and DAT-Cre BAC transgenic mice57 were employed. Nlgn3KO mice were previously described37. Male and female mice were housed in groups (weaning at P21 – P23) under a 12 h light – dark cycle (7:00 a.m.–7:00 p.m.). All physiology and behavior experiments were performed during the light cycle. For Nlgn3KO and WT mice, multiple behavioral tests were performed with the same group of animals, with a minimum of 3 days in-between tests. VTA::DANL3KD and VTA::GFP participated in one behavioral test prior to the start of social CPP. A minimum of two independent cohorts were used for the behavioral experiments. Embryos for cortical cultures were obtained from NMRI mice (Janvier). All the procedures performed at UNIGE and Biozentrum complied with the Swiss National Institutional Guidelines on Animal Experimentation and were approved by the respective Swiss Cantonal Veterinary Office Committees for Animal Experimentation.
Surgery
Injections of rAAV5-hSyn-DIO-hM4D(Gi)-mCherry and rAAV5-hSyn-DIO-mCherry were performed in DAT-Cre mice at 4–7 weeks. For additional information on chemogenetic viral vectors, see Supplementary Methods. Mice were anesthetized with a mixture of oxygen (1 L/min) and isoflurane 3% (Baxter AG, Vienna, Austria) and placed in a stereotactic frame (Angle One; Leica, Germany). The skin was shaved, locally anesthetized with 40–50 µL lidocaine 0.5% and disinfected. Bilateral craniotomy (1 mm in diameter) was then performed over the VTA at following stereotactic coordinates: ML ± 0.5 mm, AP −3.2 mm, DV −4.20 ± 0.05 mm from Bregma. The virus was injected via a glass micropipette (Drummond Scientific Company, Broomall, PA) into the VTA at the rate of 100 nl/min for a total volume of 200 nL in each side. The virus was incubated for 3–8 weeks prior to perform the behavioral tasks or electrophysiological recordings.
Injections of purified AAV2-DIO-miRNlgn3-GFP, AAV2-Synaptophysin-GFP and AAV2-DIO-miR-GFP were done at P5–P6 for developmental knockdown and at 4–7 weeks for adult knockdown. For additional information on viral vectors and validation of construct for Neuroligin 3 downregulation, see Supplementary Methods. Injections were performed under a mixture of oxygen and isoflurane anesthesia (Baxter AG, Vienna, Austria) as previously described. The animals were placed in a stereotaxic frame (Kopf Instrument) and a single craniotomy was made over the VTA at the following stereotaxic coordinates: ML + 0.15 mm, AP + 0.2 mm, DV −4.2 mm from lambda for P5-P6, and for 4–7 weeks: ML ± 0.4 mm, AP -3.2 mm, DV −4.4 mm from Bregma. Injections were made with a 33-G Hamilton needle (Hamilton, 65460-02) for a total volume of 200 nL. Injections sites were confirmed post hoc by immunostaining on VTA. The virus was incubated for 3–4 weeks prior to perform the behavioral tasks or immunostaining. Mice were excluded from the study if the body weight was less than 75% of the mean body weight at the start of behavior trials.
Injections of rAAV5-Ef1α-DIO-hChR2(H134R)-eYFP and rAAV5-Ef1α-DIO-eYFP were performed in DAT-Cre mice at 4–5 weeks. For additional information on viral vectors, see Supplementary Methods. Mice were anesthetized and placed in a stereotactic frame (Angle One; Leica, Germany) as previously described. The skin was shaved, locally anesthetized with 40–50 µL of lidocaine 0.5% and disinfected. Unilateral craniotomy (1 mm in diameter) was then performed to reach the VTA with a 10° angle, at following stereotactic coordinates: ML ± 0.9 mm, AP −3.2 mm, DV −4.20 ± 0.05 mm from Bregma (Paxinos). The virus was injected via a glass micropipette (Drummond Scientific Company, Broomall, PA) into the VTA at the rate of 100 nl/min for a total volume of 500 nL. The virus was incubated for 3–4 weeks and subsequently, mice were implanted with optic fibers above the VTA. The animals were anesthetized, placed in a stereotactic frame, the skin was shaved and a unilateral craniotomy was performed as previously described. The optic fiber was implanted with a 10° angle at the following coordinates: ML ± 0.9 mm, AP −3.2 mm, DV −3.95 ± 0.05 mm from Bregma above the VTA and fixed to the skull with dental acrylic.
Implantations of stainless steel 26-gauge cannula (PlasticsOne, Virginia, USA) were performed on WT mice at 8–10 weeks. Mice were anesthetized and placed in a stereotactic frame as previously described. Unilateral craniotomy (1 mm in diameter) was then performed over the VTA at following stereotactic coordinates: ML ± 0.9 mm, AP −3.2 mm, DV −3.95 ± 0.05 mm from Bregma. The cannula was implanted with a 10° angle, placed above the VTA and fixed on the skull with dental acrylic. Between experiments, the cannula was protected by a removable cap. All animals underwent behavioral experiments 1–2 weeks after surgery.
Three-chamber test
The three-chambered social preference test was performed in a rectangular Plexiglas arena (60 × 40 × 22 cm) (Ugo Basile, Varese, Italy) divided into three chambers (each 20 × 40 × 22 cm) that communicate by removable doors situated on the walls of the center chamber. Three to eight weeks after virus infusions, VTA::DAhM4Di and VTA::DAmCherry mice were randomly assigned to two batches that received intraperitoneal injections of either saline (vehicle) or Clozapine N-oxide (5 mg kg−1; for further details about drug preparation and injection see Supplementary Methods). All injections were done 30 min before starting the experiment. One to two weeks after, the experimental subjects treated first with CNO received vehicle and vice versa, thus performing the task in both conditions. The habituation phase consisted in 10 min of free exploration of the empty arena. Subsequently, the mouse was temporarily kept in the center chamber by closing the removable doors. Two enclosures were placed in the centers of the side chambers. One enclosure was left empty (inanimate object, o1) and the other one contained a nonfamiliar social stimulus (novel juvenile mice C57BL/6J, 3–4 weeks, s1/s3 in vehicle or CNO condition). The doors were then removed and the experimental mouse freely explored the arena and the two enclosures for 10 min. The walls of the enclosures, consisting of vertical metal bars, allowed visual, auditory, olfactory and tactile contact between the experimental mouse and the stimulus mouse. The stimuli mice were habituated to the enclosures during three sessions of 20 min the 3 days before the experiment. The position of the stimuli was randomly assigned and counterbalanced.
The mice were then restrained a second time in the center chamber by closing the removable doors. The enclosures were held in position and a nonfamiliar conspecific (s2/s4 in vehicle or CNO condition) was placed in the empty one. In this phase, the prior nonfamiliar conspecific is considered as familiar (social familiar, s1/s3). The doors were opened and the experimental mouse explored the arena for 10 min, with the two enclosures containing the familiar and the nonfamiliar social stimuli. At the end of the 10 min, the experimental and stimuli mice returned to their home cage.
Every session was video-tracked and recorded using Ethovision XT (Noldus, Wageningen, the Netherlands), which provided the time in the different chambers and the distance moved during the test. An experimenter blind to the treatment of animals also manually scored behavior. The stimulus interaction was scored when the nose of the experimental subject was oriented toward the enclosures at a distance approximately less than 2 cm. The time interaction was used to calculate the Social Novelty Index as: Interactionnonfamiliar−Interactionfamiliar. The arena was cleaned with 5% ethanol solution and dried between trials.
Long-term habituation/nonfamiliar exploration task
An experimental cage similar to the animal’s home cage was used for this task. The bedding was replaced after each trial and water and food were available. During the habituation phase (4 days, day 1–4), all VTA::DAhM4Di experimental mice received an intraperitoneal injection of saline 30 min before the task. The experimental VTA::DAhM4Di mouse was placed in the cage with a nonfamiliar conspecific (novel juvenile mouse, C57Bl/6J, 3–4 weeks old, s1). The animals were let free to explore the cage and to interact with each other for 15 min. At the end of the trial, the experimental and stimulus mice were returned to their homecage. For four consecutive days the experimental mouse was exposed to the same conspecific (s1) and habituated to the environment and the social stimulus. Day 5 consisted in the novelty phase. The VTA::DAhM4Di experimental mice were split in two batches and were injected with either saline or CNO (5 mg kg−1) 30 min before the trial. A nonfamiliar conspecific (s2) was placed with the experimental mouse in the cage for 15 min to allow direct interaction. In total, the experimental mice were exposed to two different conspecifics: one social stimulus repeatedly presented from day 1–4 (habituation phase, s1) and a second mouse at day 5 (novelty phase, s2) The same protocol as described above was used for object habituation/nonfamiliar exploration task. The VTA::DAhM4Di experimental mice received injection of saline and were exposed to the same object (der klein kaufman tanner; Germany, o1) from day 1–4 (habituation phase). On day 5 the animals were injected with either saline or CNO (5 mg kg−1), and were exposed to a novel object stimulus (novelty phase, o2). VTA::DAmCherry mice underwent the habituation/nonfamiliar exploration task and received an intraperitoneal injection of saline from day 1–4 and CNO (5 mg kg−1) on day 5. The social and object habituation/nonfamiliar exploration task performed with Nlgn3KO and VTA::DANL3KD was performed as described above. The test was done in a cage similar to the mice home cage containing food and water; the same cage was used for the duration of the trial. Three to four weeks old C57Bl/6J male mice were used as stimulus mice, lego blocks and a small plastic toy were used as object. The animals were left to freely interact with the stimulus mouse or object for 15 min. For four consecutive days the experimental mouse was exposed to the same stimulus (s1 or o1). Day 5 consisted of the novelty phase (s2 or o2). At the end of each trial, the experimental and stimulus mice were returned to their home cage.
During the social habituation/nonfamiliar exploration task, nonaggressive interaction was scored (experimenter blind to genotype and treatment group) when the experimental mouse initiated the action and when the nose of the animal was oriented toward the social stimulus mouse only. During the object habituation/nonfamiliar exploration task, the interaction was scored when the nose of the animal was oriented toward the object stimulus. The time interaction was used to calculate the Novelty Index as: InteractionDay 5−InteractionDay 4, both for social and object habituation/nonfamiliar exploration task.
The experimental cage was cleaned with 5% ethanol solution and the bedding was changed between sessions.
For the experiments with pharmacological agents, mice were cannulated to allow the infusion of either saline or 1-Naphthylacetyl spermine trihydrochloride (NASPM), directly in the VTA. The habituation task was performed as previously described. NASPM or saline were infused using a Minipump injector (pump Elite 11, Harvard apparatus, US) with 500 nL of saline (2 min of active injection at 250 nL min−1 rate, and 1 min at rest), 10 min before each trial. At day 1, mice received saline. From day 2–4 of the habituation phase, mice received either 4 µg of NASPM dissolved in 500 nL of saline or 500 nL of saline only (at 250 nL min−1) before each trial. This dose has been previously used to obtain GluA2-lacking AMPARs block in vivo58. After at least 1 week, the animals were re-tested to habituation/nonfamiliar exploration task and the pharmacological treatment was counterbalanced. The scoring of the social or object interaction was made as previously described. The experimental cage was cleaned with 5% ethanol solution and the bedding was changed after every session. To assess the cannula placement, experimental subjects were infused using Chicago Sky Blue 6B (1 mg mL−1), sacrificed 1–2 h later and transcardially perfused as previously described. For detailed experimental procedures about NASPM infusions during open field and familiar conspecific interaction and VTA DA neuron optogenetic stimulation during habituation/nonfamiliar exploration task, see Supplementary Material and Methods.
Nonfamiliar, familiar, and novel object CPP
Conditioned place preference experiments for examining reinforcing properties of nonfamiliar conspecific, familiar conspecific or nonfamiliar object interactions were conducted in an apparatus (spatial place preference; BioSEB) consisting of two adjacent chambers (20 × 20 × 25 cm) with dot (black) or stripe (gray) wall patterns, connected by a lateral corridor (7 × 20 × 25 cm) with transparent walls and floor. The dot chamber was always associated to rough floor, while the stripe chamber with smooth floor. The illumination level was uniform between the two chambers and set at 10–13 lux. ANY-Maze behavior tracking software was used to track animal’s movements within the apparatus and to manually score the time spent in nonaggressive interaction with the stimulus.
At day 0, experimental mice (male C57Bl6/J; group-housed; 8–16 weeks) freely explored the CPP apparatus for 15 min to determine Pre-TEST preference for one or the other chamber. After the Pre-TEST, experimental mice returned to their home cage with their cage-mates. The preference score was calculated as time spent in stimulus chamber (US+) divided by the sum of the time spent in stimulus chamber (US+) and the time spent in the empty chamber. No animals were excluded from the analysis based on preference score and US+ pairings were randomly assigned to dot or stripe chamber. At day 0, nonfamiliar conspecific (male C57Bl/6J; single-housed; 3–4 weeks) or familiar stimuli mice (male C57Bl/6J; cohoused with experimental mice during the conditioning) were habituated to the US+ chamber for 15–30 min. The novel object stimulus was the same used in the object habituation/nonfamiliar exploration task.
From day 1–4, experimental mice underwent a conditioning schedule consisting of 30 min-long sessions (1 per day). Each session was subdivided in six blocks of 5 min during which the animals alternated between US+ and US− chamber, in presence (of either familiar mouse, f1, nonfamiliar mouse, s1 or novel object, o1) or absence (empty) of the stimulus, respectively. Experimental mice were guided through the corridor during the alternations and returned to their home cage with their cage-mates at the end of the conditioning session. Groups were counterbalanced for US+/US− sequences and for dot or stripe wall pattern. VTA::DAhM4Di and VTA::DAmCherry mice received an intraperitoneal injection of CNO (5 mg kg−1) 30–90 min prior each conditioning session. At day 5, during the Post-TEST, experimental mice freely explored the CPP apparatus, without any stimulus for 15 min and the preference score was measured. The CPP apparatus was cleaned with 1% acetic acid, rinsed with distilled water and dried between each experimental subject.
Social conditioned place preference
Mice were tested at P30–P45 and were group housed before the test. The test apparatus was a custom-built cage measuring 46 × 24 × 22 cm divided into three chambers. The two outer chambers (23 × 18 × 22 cm) had vertical or horizontal striped pattern on the walls and flooring consisting or black rubber mats with different patterns (stripes vs squares). The outer chambers were joined together by a smaller chamber (23 × 10 cm) with white walls and floor with a 7 × 7 cm opening at the base to the outer chambers that can be closed. The cage was cleaned with 70% ethanol between each trial. During the pretrial, mice were left to freely explore the cage for 30 min. After the pretrial, all mice were single housed for the remainder of the test and one chamber was assigned the social chamber and one the isolation chamber. All mice received one social and one isolation condition session (30 min each) per day for 4 days, with a two-day rest between the second and third conditioning day. Mice were socially conditioned for 30 min together with their cage-mates followed by conditioning in the isolation chamber for 30 min. After the fourth conditioning day, mice were tested in a 30 min postconditioning trial. The time spent freely exploring the chambers for 30 min was manually scored by an investigator blinded to the genotype. The preference score was calculated as the time spent in the social chamber divided by the combined time spent in the social and isolation chamber. Animals were excluded by pre-established criteria if they exhibited a strong preference for one chamber (more than 2 reference for one chamber). For detailed description of additional behavioral tasks, see Supplementary Methods.
Ex vivo electrophysiology
In all, 200–250 µM thick horizontal midbrain slices were prepared from adolescence/early adulthood C57Bl/6J, VTA::DAhM4Di, VTA::DAmCherry, Nlgn3 KO and VTA::DANL3KD mice. Subjects were anesthetized with isoflurane/O2 and decapitated. Brains were sliced by using a cutting solution containing: 90.89 mM choline chloride, 24.98 mM glucose, 25 mM NaHCO3, 6.98 mM MgCl2, 11.85 mM ascorbic acid, 3.09 mM sodium pyruvate, 2.49 mM KCl, 1.25 mM NaH2PO4, and 0.50 mM CaCl2. Brain slices were incubated in cutting solution for 20–30 min at 35°. Subsequently, slices were transferred in artificial cerebrospinal fluid (aCSF) containing: 119 mM NaCl, 2.5 mM KCl, 1.3 mM MgCl2, 2.5 mM CaCl2, 1.0 mM NaH2PO4, 26.2 mM NaHCO3, and 11 mM glucose, bubbled with 95% O2 and 5% CO2) at room temperature. Whole-cell voltage clamp or current clamp electrophysiological recordings were conducted at 32°–34° in aCSF (2–3 ml/min, submerged slices). Recording pipette contained the following internal solution: 130 mM CsCl, 4 mM NaCl, 2 mM MgCl2, 1.1 mM EGTA, 5 mM HEPES, 2 mM Na2ATP, 5 mM sodium creatine phosphate, 0.6 mM Na3GTP, 0.1 mM spermine and 5 mM lidocaine N-ethyl bromide. Ex vivo CNO validation experiments were conducted in current-clamp configuration with the following internal solution: 140 mM K-Gluconate, 2 mM MgCl2, 5 mM KCl, 0.2 mM EGTA, 10 mM HEPES, 4 mM Na2ATP, 0.3 mM Na3GTP and 10 mM creatine-phosphate. Putative DA neurons of the VTA were identified accordingly to their position (medially to the medial terminal nucleus of the accessory optic tract), morphology, cell capacitance (>28 pF) and low input resistance at positive potentials. Excitatory postsynaptic currents (EPSCs) were recorded in voltage-clamp configuration, elicited by placing a bipolar electrode rostrolaterally to VTA at 0.1 Hz and isolated by application of the GABAAR antagonist picrotoxin (100 µM). Traces were not corrected. Access resistance (10–30 MΩ) was monitored by a hyperpolarizing step of −4 mV at each sweep, every 10 s. Data were excluded when the resistance changed >20%. The AMPA/NMDA ratio was calculated by subtracting to the mixed EPSC (+35 mV), the non-NMDA component isolated by D-APV (50 µM at +35 mV) bath application. The values of the ratio may be underestimated since it was calculated with spermine in the pipette. The rectification index (RI) of AMPARs is the ratio of the chord conductance calculated at negative potential (–60 mV) divided by the chord conductance at positive potential (+40 mV). PPR was measured at −60 mV, with a fixed inter stimulation interval of 50 ms. PPR was calculated by dividing the amplitude of the second EPSC by the amplitude of the first EPSC. To measure the RI from ChR2-expressing VTA DA neurons after in vivo optogenetic stimulation, a 500 ms blue-light pulse was delivered through the microscope objective in voltage-clamp configuration. Neurons were considered photocurrent positive (IChR2+) when they responded with a large depolarizing current in response to optical stimulation, while photocurrent negative (IChR2−) when they did not. Representative example traces are shown as the average of 10–20 consecutives EPSCs typically obtained at each potential. The synaptic responses were collected with a Multiclamp 700B-amplifier (Axon Instruments, Foster City, CA), filtered at 2.2 kHz, digitized at 5 Hz, and analyzed online using Igor Pro software (Wavemetrics, Lake Oswego, OR). Electrophysiology experiments were performed blind to behavioral or genetic manipulation.
Statistical analysis
No statistical methods were used to predetermine the number of animals and cells, but suitable sample sizes were estimated based on previous experience and are similar to those generally employed in the field. The animals were randomly assigned to each group at the moment of viral infections or behavioral tests. Statistical analysis was conducted with GraphPad Prism 6 and 7 (San Diego, CA, USA) and MatLab (The Mathwork). Statistical outliers were identified with the ROUT method (Q = 1) and excluded from the analysis. The normality of sample distributions was assessed with the Shapiro–Wilk criterion and when violated nonparametrical tests were used. When normally distributed, the data were analyzed with independent t test, paired t test, while for multiple comparisons one-way ANOVA and repeated measures (RM) ANOVA were used. When normality was violated, the data were analyzed with Mann–Whitney test, Wilcoxon matched-pairs signed rank test, while for multiple comparisons, Kruskal–Wallis or Friedman test were followed by Dunn’s test. For the analysis of variance with two factors (two-way ANOVA, RM two-way ANOVA and RM two-way ANOVA by both factors), normality of sample distribution was assumed, and followed by Bonferroni post hoc test. All the statistical tests adopted were two-sided. When comparing two samples distributions similarity of variances was assumed, therefore no corrections were adopted. For social behavior experiments, the outlier analysis was conducted on manually scored nonaggressive social interaction. Data are represented as the mean ± s.e.m. and the significance was set at P < 0.05. For further information about the statistical analysis of the three-chamber social interaction task and a summary of the statistical tests adopted for each dataset, see Supplementary Methods and Supplementary Table 1.
Data availability
The data supporting this study are available upon request to the corresponding author.
Electronic supplementary material
Acknowledgments
C.B. is supported by the Swiss National Science Foundation, Pierre Mercier Foundation, and NCCR Synapsy. P.S. is supported by the Swiss National Science Foundation, NCCR Synapsy, and EU-AIMS and the Canton Basel-Stadt. H.H. is supported by the Human Frontier Science Program. We thank Carmen Sandi, Marie Schaer and Alan Carleton for the critical reading of the manuscript. We thank Stamatina Tzanoulinou for insights on behavioral experiments and Lorena Jourdain and Caroline Bornmann for technical support.
Author contributions
Ex vivo electrophysiology experiments were performed by S.B., C.B., and S.M. Behavioral experiments and analysis reported in Figs. 1, 2, 6, 7, Supplementary Figures 1 and 2 were performed by C.P.S. Behavioral experiments and analysis of the Fig. 3 and Supplementary Figure 3 were conducted by S.B. Behavioral experiments and analysis of the Figs. 4, 5, and Supplementary Figures 4–6 were performed by H.H. and L.H.B. S.B. and C.P.S. performed the statistical analyses for the ex vivo electrophysiology and the behavioral experiments in Figs. 1, 2, 3, 6, and Supplementary Figures 1, 2, 3, 7. H.H. performed the statistical analyses for the behavioral experiments in Figs. 4, 5 and Supplementary Figures 4–5. Generation and validation of viral knock-down vectors was performed and analyzed by H.H. Immunohistochemistry in VTA::DAhM4Di and cell counting were performed by C.P.S. and S.B. Immunohistochemistry and cell counting in VTA::DANL3KD mice were performed by H.H. The study was designed and the manuscript written by C.B., P.S., H.H., and S.B., with assistance from C.P.S.
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher's note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
These authors contributed equally: Sebastiano Bariselli, Hanna Hörnberg, Clément Prévost-Solié.
Electronic supplementary material
Supplementary Information accompanies this paper at 10.1038/s41467-018-05382-3.
References
- 1.Turk-Browne NB, Scholl BJ, Chun MM. Babies and brains: habituation in infant cognition and functional neuroimaging. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2008;2:16. doi: 10.3389/neuro.09.016.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Pierce K, Conant D, Hazin R, Stoner R, Desmond J. Preference for geometric patterns early in life as a risk factor for autism. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry. 2011;68:101–109. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Webb SJ, et al. Toddlers with elevated autism symptoms show slowed habituation to faces. Child Neuropsychol. 2010;16:255–278. doi: 10.1080/09297041003601454. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.van Engeland H, Roelofs JW, Verbaten MN, Slangen JL. Abnormal electrodermal reactivity to novel visual stimuli in autistic children. Psychiatry Res. 1991;38:27–38. doi: 10.1016/0165-1781(91)90050-Y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Gomot M, et al. Change detection in children with autism: an auditory event-related fMRI study. Neuroimage. 2006;29:475–484. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.07.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Dawson G, et al. Neural correlates of face and object recognition in young children with autism spectrum disorder, developmental delay, and typical development. Child Dev. 2002;73:700–717. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00433. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Kleinhans NM, et al. Reduced neural habituation in the amygdala and social impairments in autism spectrum disorders. Am. J. Psychiatry. 2009;166:467–475. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.07101681. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Takeuchi T, et al. Locus coeruleus and dopaminergic consolidation of everyday memory. Nature. 2016;537:357–362. doi: 10.1038/nature19325. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Cohen JY, Haesler S, Vong L, Lowell BB, Uchida N. Neuron-type-specific signals for reward and punishment in the ventral tegmental area. Nature. 2012;482:85–88. doi: 10.1038/nature10754. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Schultz W, Dayan P, Montague PR. A neural substrate of prediction and reward. Science. 1997;275:1593–1599. doi: 10.1126/science.275.5306.1593. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Bunzeck N, Düzel E. Absolute coding of stimulus novelty in the human substantia nigra/VTA. Neuron. 2006;51:369–379. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.06.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Schultz W. Predictive reward signal of dopamine neurons. J. Neurophysiol. 1998;80:1–27. doi: 10.1152/jn.1998.80.1.1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Gunaydin LA, et al. Natural neural projection dynamics underlying social behavior. Cell. 2014;157:1535–1551. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2014.05.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Bellone C, Mameli M, Lüscher C. In utero exposure to cocaine delays postnatal synaptic maturation of glutamatergic transmission in the VTA. Nat. Neurosci. 2011;14:1439–1446. doi: 10.1038/nn.2930. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Stuber GD, et al. Reward-predictive cues enhance excitatory synaptic strength onto midbrain dopamine neurons. Science. 2008;321:1690–1692. doi: 10.1126/science.1160873. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Buffington SA, et al. Microbial reconstitution reverses maternal diet-induced social and synaptic deficits in offspring. Cell. 2016;165:1762–1775. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2016.06.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Bellone C, Lüscher C. Cocaine triggered AMPA receptor redistribution is reversed in vivo by mGluR-dependent long-term depression. Nat. Neurosci. 2006;9:636–641. doi: 10.1038/nn1682. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Brown MTC, et al. Drug-driven AMPA receptor redistribution mimicked by selective dopamine neuron stimulation. PLoS. One. 2010;5:e15870. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0015870. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.O’Connor EC, Bariselli S, Bellone C. Synaptic basis of social dysfunction: a focus on postsynaptic proteins linking group-I mGluRs with AMPARs and NMDARs. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2014;39:1114–1129. doi: 10.1111/ejn.12510. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Bariselli S, et al. SHANK3 controls maturation of social reward circuits in the VTA. Nat. Neurosci. 2016;19:926–934. doi: 10.1038/nn.4319. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Scott-Van Zeeland AA, Dapretto M, Ghahremani DG, Poldrack RA, Bookheimer SY. Reward processing in autism. Autism Res. 2010;3:53–67. doi: 10.1002/aur.122. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Demurie E, Roeyers H, Baeyens D, Sonuga-Barke E. Common alterations in sensitivity to type but not amount of reward in ADHD and autism spectrum disorders. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry. 2011;52:1164–1173. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02374.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Chevallier C, Kohls G, Troiani V, Brodkin ES, Schultz RT. The social motivation theory of autism. Trends Cogn. Sci. (Regul. Ed.) 2012;16:231–239. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.02.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Ferguson JN, et al. Social amnesia in mice lacking the oxytocin gene. Nat. Genet. 2000;25:284–288. doi: 10.1038/77040. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Armbruster BN, Li X, Pausch MH, Herlitze S, Roth BL. Evolving the lock to fit the key to create a family of G protein-coupled receptors potently activated by an inert ligand. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2007;104:5163–5168. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0700293104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Ferguson SM, et al. Transient neuronal inhibition reveals opposing roles of indirect and direct pathways in sensitization. Nat. Neurosci. 2011;14:22–24. doi: 10.1038/nn.2703. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Hitti FL, Siegelbaum SA. The hippocampal CA2 region is essential for social memory. Nature. 2014;508:88–92. doi: 10.1038/nature13028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Nadler JJ, et al. Automated apparatus for quantitation of social approach behaviors in mice. Genes Brain Behav. 2004;3:303–314. doi: 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2004.00071.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Moy SS, et al. Sociability and preference for social novelty in five inbred strains: an approach to assess autistic-like behavior in mice. Genes Brain Behav. 2004;3:287–302. doi: 10.1111/j.1601-1848.2004.00076.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Yang M, Silverman JL, Crawley JN. Automated three-chambered social approach task for mice. Curr. Protoc. Neurosci. 2011;Chapter 8:26. doi: 10.1002/0471142301.ns0826s56. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Panksepp JB, Lahvis GP. Social reward among juvenile mice. Genes Brain Behav. 2007;6:661–671. doi: 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2006.00295.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Dölen G, Darvishzadeh A, Huang KW, Malenka RC. Social reward requires coordinated activity of nucleus accumbens oxytocin and serotonin. Nature. 2013;501:179–184. doi: 10.1038/nature12518. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Sanders SJ, et al. Multiple recurrent de novo CNVs, including duplications of the 7q11.23 Williams syndrome region, are strongly associated with autism. Neuron. 2011;70:863–885. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.05.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.C Yuen RK, et al. Whole genome sequencing resource identifies 18 new candidate genes for autism spectrum disorder. Nat. Neurosci. 2017;20:602–611. doi: 10.1038/nn.4524. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Levy D, et al. Rare de novo and transmitted copy-number variation in autistic spectrum disorders. Neuron. 2011;70:886–897. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.05.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Budreck EC, Scheiffele P. Neuroligin-3 is a neuronal adhesion protein at GABAergic and glutamatergic synapses. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2007;26:1738–1748. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2007.05842.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Tanaka KF, et al. Flexible accelerated STOP tetracycline operator-knockin (FAST): a versatile and efficient new gene modulating system. Biol. Psychiatry. 2010;67:770–773. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.12.020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Radyushkin K, et al. Neuroligin-3-deficient mice: model of a monogenic heritable form of autism with an olfactory deficit. Genes Brain Behav. 2009;8:416–425. doi: 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2009.00487.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Baudouin SJ, et al. Shared synaptic pathophysiology in syndromic and nonsyndromic rodent models of autism. Science. 2012;338:128–132. doi: 10.1126/science.1224159. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Rothwell PE, et al. Autism-associated neuroligin-3 mutations commonly impair striatal circuits to boost repetitive behaviors. Cell. 2014;158:198–212. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2014.04.045. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Kalbassi, S., Bachmann, S. O., Cross, E., Roberton, V. H., & Baudouin, S. J. Male and female mice lacking neuroligin-3 modify the behavior of their wild-type littermates. eNeuro4, (2017).ENEURO.0145–17.2017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 42.Yang M, Crawley JN. Simple behavioral assessment of mouse olfaction. Curr. Protoc. Neurosci. 2009;Chapter 8:24–8.24.12. doi: 10.1002/0471142301.ns0824s48. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Jamain S, et al. Mutations of the X-linked genes encoding neuroligins NLGN3 and NLGN4 are associated with autism. Nat. Genet. 2003;34:27–29. doi: 10.1038/ng1136. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Tabuchi K, et al. A neuroligin-3 mutation implicated in autism increases inhibitory synaptic transmission in mice. Science. 2007;318:71–76. doi: 10.1126/science.1146221. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Etherton M, et al. Autism-linked neuroligin-3 R451C mutation differentially alters hippocampal and cortical synaptic function. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2011;108:13764–13769. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1111093108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Jaramillo TC, Liu S, Pettersen A, Birnbaum SG, Powell CM. Autism-related neuroligin-3 mutation alters social behavior and spatial learning. Autism Res. 2014;7:264–272. doi: 10.1002/aur.1362. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Molas S, et al. A circuit-based mechanism underlying familiarity signaling and the preference for novelty. Nat. Neurosci. 2017;20:1260–1268. doi: 10.1038/nn.4607. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.McHenry JA, et al. Hormonal gain control of a medial preoptic area social reward circuit. Nat. Neurosci. 2017;20:449–458. doi: 10.1038/nn.4487. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Hung LW, et al. Gating of social reward by oxytocin in the ventral tegmental area. Science. 2017;357:1406–1411. doi: 10.1126/science.aan4994. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Saal D, Dong Y, Bonci A, Malenka RC. Drugs of abuse and stress trigger a common synaptic adaptation in dopamine neurons. Neuron. 2003;37:577–582. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00021-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Lammel S, Lim BK, Malenka RC. Reward and aversion in a heterogeneous midbrain dopamine system. Neuropharmacology. 2014;76:351–359. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2013.03.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Bariselli, S., Glangetas, C., Tzanoulinou, S. & Bellone, C. Ventral tegmental area subcircuits process rewarding and aversive experiences. J. Neurochem. 10.1111/jnc.13779 (2016). [DOI] [PubMed]
- 53.Vivanti, G. et al. Attention to novelty versus repetition: contrasting habituation profiles in Autism and Williams syndrome. Dev. Cognit. Neurosci. 10.1016/j.dcn.2017.01.006 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 54.Webb SJ, et al. Response to familiar faces, newly familiar faces, and novel faces as assessed by ERPs is intact in adults with autism spectrum disorders. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 2010;77:106–117. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2010.04.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Kwon CH, et al. Pten regulates neuronal arborization and social interaction in mice. Neuron. 2006;50:377–388. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.03.023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Bäckman CM, et al. Characterization of a mouse strain expressing Cre recombinase from the 3’ untranslated region of the dopamine transporter locus. Genesis. 2006;44:383–390. doi: 10.1002/dvg.20228. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Turiault M, et al. Analysis of dopamine transporter gene expression pattern—generation of DAT-iCre transgenic mice. FEBS J. 2007;274:3568–3577. doi: 10.1111/j.1742-4658.2007.05886.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Hong I, et al. AMPA receptor exchange underlies transient memory destabilization on retrieval. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2013;110:8218–8223. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1305235110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The data supporting this study are available upon request to the corresponding author.