Abstract
Objective:
To determine whether perceived cooking skills in emerging adulthood predicts better nutrition a decade later.
Methods:
Data were collected as part of the Project Eating and Activity in Teens and Young Adults longitudinal study. Participants reported on adequacy of cooking skills in 2002–2003 (age 18–23 years) and subsequently reported on nutrition-related outcomes in 2015–2016 (age 30–35 years) (n = 1,158). Separate regression models were used to examine associations between cooking skills at age 18–23 years and each subsequent outcome.
Results:
One fourth of participants described their cooking skills as very adequate at 18–23 years, with no statistically significant differences by sociodemographic characteristics. Reports of very adequate cooking skills at age 18–23 years predicted better nutrition-related outcomes 10 years later, such as more frequent preparation of meals including vegetables (P < .001) and less frequent fast food consumption (P < .001).
Conclusions and Implications:
Developing adequate cooking skills by emerging adulthood may have long-term benefits for nutrition over a decade later. Ongoing and new interventions to enhance cooking skills during adolescence and emerging adulthood are warranted but require strong evaluation designs that observe young people over a number of years.
Keywords: cooking, eating, longitudinal, nutrition
INTRODUCTION
An emerging body of evidence suggests that developing cooking and food preparation skills is important for nutritional well-being. Involvement in cooking has been associated with healthier diets and eating behaviors among adults1 and adolescents.2–5 Moreover, involvement in meal preparation during the adolescent transition to young adulthood has been associated with better nutrition indicators later in life.6 Yet the practice of home cooking is declining7 and there are growing concerns that the skill of cooking may be lost in future generations.8 Previously, these skills were transmitted intergenerationally or through formal school curriculum.8,9 However, recent surveys found that few adolescents and adults reported that they learned to cook from school.10,11
Over the past decade, numerous interventions were designed with the aim of developing cooking skills and confidence among children and adolescents12 as well as adults.13 Many of these programs reported short-term benefits, particularly with participant confidence in cooking, knowledge of cooking techniques, and attitudes toward eating new foods, including vegetables.12,13 Robustly measuring the long-term impact of these types of programs remains challenging. In short, it is unknown whether developing cooking skills and confidence early in life makes a meaningful difference to nutrition and healthy eating throughout adulthood. Thus, by drawing on longitudinal data, the current study aimed to address this gap by determining whether adequate cooking skill perceptions in emerging adulthood were associated with better eating behaviors and weight status a decade later.
METHODS
Study Design and Population
Data were collected as part of the population-based Project Eating and Activity in Teens and Young Adults (EAT) longitudinal study of weight-related behaviors, weight status, and factors associated with these outcomes among young people. For the original assessment (EAT-I) in 1998–1999, adolescents enrolled at 31 public middle schools and senior high schools in the Minneapolis–St Paul metropolitan area of Minnesota completed surveys and anthropometric measures in school classrooms.14,15 Follow-up assessments were conducted using a combination of mailed and online surveys in 2003–2004 (EAT-II) and 2015–2016 (EAT-IV) to examine changes in the weight-related outcomes of the original participants as they progressed through emerging adulthood and entered their third decade of life.16–18
Approximately 22.6% of the original study population was lost to follow-up at EAT-II, primarily owing to missing contact information at EAT-I (n = 411) and no address found at follow-up (n = 591). At EAT-IV, survey invitations were sent only to participants who had responded to at least 1 previous follow-up survey (EAT-II or EAT-III) and for whom current contact information was available (n = 2,770). Among those who could be contacted, the response rate at EAT-II was 68.5%, and at EAT-IV the response rate was 66.1%. To capture the unique and dynamic life changes between early and later adulthood, the current analysis includes only the 1,158 participants who responded at both of these waves and were in the emerging adult life stage (age 18–23 years) at EAT-II (2002–2003).
The University of Minnesota’s Institutional Review Board Human Subjects Committee approved all protocols used in Project EAT at each time point. Parental consent and written assent from participants was obtained in 1998–1999. For Projects EAT-II and EAT-IV, participants were mailed a consent form with their paper survey or reviewed a consent form as part of the online survey.
Survey Development
The Project EAT survey was tailored at each assessment wave to reflect age-appropriate topics and areas of evolving interest. Perceived adequacy of cooking skills was assessed in emerging adulthood (age 18–23 years) and several other food preparation and meal behaviors were assessed in later adulthood (age 30–35 years). The item on adequacy of cooking skills was adapted from a 10-state survey of young adult food habits19 and was pretested along with other new survey items in focus groups with 20 young adults before they were added to the Project EAT-II survey. Similarly, for EAT-IV, 2 focus groups were conducted to pretest an initial draft of the survey with a community-based sample of 35 young adults. For pretesting at both waves, young adults individually completed a draft version of the survey and then provided oral feedback as a group on the content of the survey, the wording of items, and the response options provided for each item.20,21 Psychometric properties of measures are reported when available based on data collected for EAT-IV. Scale psychometric properties were examined in the full sample of responders to the EAT-IV survey and estimates of item test-retest reliability were determined in a subgroup of 103 participants who completed the EAT-IV survey twice within 1–4 weeks. All test-retest correlations had P values < .001.
The independent variable of perceived adequacy of cooking skills was assessed by asking How adequate are your cooking skills? Participants could reply with 4 options: very adequate, adequate, inadequate, or very inadequate. The very inadequate and inadequate groups were combined for analyses because of the smaller numbers in those groups.
Frequency of having prepared a meal with vegetables was assessed by asking During the past month, how often have you prepared a meal that included vegetables? Participants could select 1 of 6 options ranging in frequency from never to most days of the week (test-retest r = .84). Based on the distribution, responses were dichotomized to represent most days of the week and a few times a week or less. Whether participants were usually involved in household food preparation was assessed by asking participants to select who was involved, from a list of their family members. Participants who replied me were considered a usual food preparer (test-retest agreement for selecting self = 91%).
Family meals, fast food for family meals, and barriers to food preparation were assessed among participants who reported being a parent to ≥1 children at the time of the EAT-IV survey. Frequency of family meals was assessed by asking During the past 7 days, how many times did all or most of the people living in your household eat a meal together? with 6 responses ranging from never to ≥7 times (test-retest r = .64). Responses were dichotomized at ≥7 times or less to create 2 groups of similar sizes. Fast food for family meals was assessed by asking During the past week, how many times was a family meal purchased from a fast-food restaurant and eaten together at the restaurant or at home? with 4 responses ranging from never to ≥3 times (test-retest r = .54). Responses were dichotomized at ≥1 time to capture weekly purchases. Barriers to food preparation were assessed with a 5-item scale asking about having time and energy for meal preparation, meal planning, and feeding children right. The scale was adapted from Storfer-Isser and Musher-Eizenman22 and was found to have good internal consistency and reliability (Cronbach α= .74; test-retest r = .73) in the EAT sample. Possible scores ranged from 5 to 25, with higher scores indicating greater barriers to food preparation.
Fast-food restaurant frequency was assessed with the item In the past week, how often did you eat something from a fast-food restaurant? with 6 response options ranging from never to >7 times. Responses were dichotomized at 1–2 times or more often to create 2 groups of similar size (test-retest r = .54).
Daily servings of fruit, vegetables, whole grains, and sugar-sweetened beverages were assessed using a semiquantitative food-frequency questionnaire that was administered at the same time as the Project EAT-IV survey.23 A daily serving was defined as the equivalent of 0.5 cup of fruits and vegetables and 16 g of whole grains. For sugar-sweetened beverages, a serving was defined as the equivalent of 1 glass, bottle, or can. Previous studies examined and reported on the reliability and validity of intake estimates.24,25 Responses to the food-frequency questionnaire were excluded if participants reported a biologically implausible level of total energy intake (<500 or >5,000 kcal/d) or left ≥20 items blank.26 Daily servings of fruits, vegetables, whole grains, and sugar-sweetened soda were assessed at EAT-II following the same method as described previously, but with the youth version of the same food-frequency questionnaire.27
Weight status of overweight (body mass index [BMI], 25–29.9) or obesity (BMI ≥30) was determined by self-reported height and weight. In a validation study among a subsample of 127 Project EAT-III young adult participants, the correlation between measured and self-reported BMI values was r = .95.28
Unhealthy food at home was assessed with a 3-item scale developed for the Project EAT surveys. Participants were asked to report on the frequency of home availability of potato chips or salty snacks, chocolate or other candy, and soda (test-retest reliability r = .70, .66, and .72, respectively). Healthy food at home was assessed with a 5-item scale developed for the Project EAT surveys. Participants reported on the frequency of home availability of fruits and vegetables and whole-wheat bread and on the frequency of fruits, vegetables, and milk being served at meals (test-retest reliability all >.68).
The covariates age, sex, race/ethnicity, and socioeconomic status (SES) were determined by self-report on the EAT-I survey. Socioeconomic status (SES) was primarily based on adolescent reports of parental educational level, but also included reports of family eligibility of public assistance and parental employment level during the Project EAT-I survey.15 Household educational attainment was based on report of the highest level of education that the participant or his or her spouse or partner had completed at the time of response to the EAT-IV survey.
Analysis
All data were analyzed using the SURVEY procedures in the SAS software package (version 9.3, SAS, Cary, NC, 2011) to account for the weighting of the dataset. The analyses were weighted to correct for nonresponse by participants and approximately reflects the original population-based cohort. Prevalence estimates were derived using bivariate analyses to examine the simple association between the variables of interest (eg, to describe the relationship between sociodemographic variables and perceived cooking skills). Multiple regression models were conducted to determine the relationship between perceived cooking skills at age 18–23 years and food preparation behaviors, nutrition and weight indicators, and aspects of the home food environment at age 30–35 years. All analyses controlled for age, sex, ethnicity, SES at EAT-I, and educational attainment at EAT-IV. A separate set of regression models was generated to control for the dependent variable as measured in EAT-II if the measure was available (fast-food restaurant frequency; consumption of fruits, vegetables, sugar-sweetened beverages, and whole grains; body size; and availability of healthy and unhealthy food at home). Associations were considered to be statistically significant at P < .05 or where 95% confidence levels were nonoverlapping.
RESULTS
Most participants perceived their cooking skills to be adequate at age 18–23 years (Table 1). Approximately one quarter of adults reported their cooking skills to be very adequate, and 56% to be adequate. There were no differences in perceived cooking skills by sex, race or ethnicity, SES (measured at EAT-I) or educational attainment (measured at EAT-IV), or age.
Table 1.
Perceived adequacy of Cooking skills | |||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Very inadequate, inadequate | Adequate | Very adequate | |||||||
Characteristics | n | % or Meanb | CIc | n | % or Meanb | CIc | n | % or Meanb | CIc |
Total | 211 | 19.1 | 16.1–22.1 | 656 | 56.0 | 52.4–59.7 | 275 | 24.9 | 21.7–28.0 |
Sex | |||||||||
Male | 92 | 20.1 | 15.3–24.8 | 269 | 56.3 | 50.6–62.3 | 117 | 23.6 | 18.9–28.3 |
Female | 119 | 18.1 | 14.5–21.7 | 387 | 55.7 | 51.1–60.3 | 158 | 26.2 | 22.1–30.3 |
Race/ ethnicity | |||||||||
White | 166 | 20 | 16.7–22.4 | 508 | 57 | 53.6–60.7 | 199 | 23 | 20.2–26.4 |
Non-white | 44 | 19 | 12.6–24.5 | 145 | 55 | 47.3–61.7 | 75 | 27% | 20.8–33.1 |
SES (measured at Project EAT-I) | |||||||||
Low | 21 | 19.2 | 9.6–28.8 | 63 | 53.3 | 42.1–64.4 | 30 | 27.5 | 17.8–37.3 |
Low–middle | 36 | 20.2 | 13.4–27.1 | 96 | 53.2 | 44.1–62.4 | 41 | 26.5 | 18.2–34.9 |
Middle | 44 | 19.0 | 12.5–25.6 | 148 | 57.4 | 49.7–65.0 | 64 | 23.6 | 17.5–29.7 |
High–middle | 76 | 18.9 | 14.7–23.1 | 222 | 58.5 | 53.0–64.0 | 84 | 22.6 | 17.9–27.2 |
High | 32 | 15.6 | 10.4–20.8 | 124 | 57.5 | 50.4–64.5 | 55 | 27.0 | 20.6–33.4 |
Educational attainment (measured at Project EAT-IV) | |||||||||
High school graduate or equivalent | 26 | 20.6 | 11.7–29.6 | 84 | 60.2 | 49.9–70.5 | 31 | 19.2 | 11.4–26.9 |
Some university | 46 | 16.8 | 10.7–22.8 | 143 | 53.2 | 45.5–60.9 | 68 | 30.0 | 22.9–37.1 |
Four-year university degree | 87 | 19.7 | 15.6–23.9 | 256 | 57.0 | 51.5–62.4 | 100 | 23.3 | 18.6–28.0 |
Graduate or professional degree | 51 | 18.7 | 13.3–24.2 | 170 | 56.4 | 49.7–63.2 | 74 | 24.8 | 19.3–30.4 |
Age, y (mean) | 211 | 20.5 | 20.3–20.6 | 656.0 | 20.4 | 20.3–20.5 | 275.0 | 20.4 | 20.3–20.5 |
CI indicates confidence interval; Project EAT, Project Eating and Activity in Teens and Young Adults; SES, Socioeconomic status.
Participants include those who participated in the first, second, and fourth waves of the Project EAT survey and were in the emerging adult life stage (age 18–23 years) at EAT-II (2002–2003).
Unadjusted percent or mean.
95% confidence limit for the percentage or mean.
Perceived adequacy of cooking skills in emerging adulthood (age 18–23 years) predicted multiple indicators of nutrition outcomes later in adulthood (age 30–35 years) (Table 2). Specifically, reporting very adequate cooking skills in emerging adulthood was associated with greater odds of preparing a meal with vegetables most days (odds ratio = 3.5; confidence interval, 2.1–5.9) and identifying as a usual food preparer (odds ratio = 2.6; confidence interval, 1.4–4.7) later in adulthood. Similarly, adequate cooking skills in emerging adulthood predicted eating ≥3 servings/d vegetables (P < .001) and less frequent consumption of fast food (P < .001) later in adulthood. Among participants with children (when participants were aged 30–35 years), perceived cooking skills during early adulthood predicted more frequent family meals (P = .02), less frequent fast food for family meals (P < .001), and fewer food preparation barriers (P < .001) a decade later. No other statistically significant relationships between cooking skills and nutrition outcomes were observed.
Table 2.
Adequacy of cooking skills (Age 18–23 y) | |||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Very inadequate, inadequate | Adequate | Very adequate | |||||||||
Nutrition-Related outcomes (Age 30–35 y) | n | %a | ORb | CI4 | % | OR | CI | % | OR | CI | P |
Food preparation behaviors | |||||||||||
Prepared meal with vegetables, most days | 909 | 36 | Reference | – | 48 | 1.8 | 1.2–2.8 | 62 | 3.5 | 2.1–5.9 | < .001 |
Usual food preparer | 1,016 | 63 | Reference | – | 74 | 2.0 | 1.2–3.3 | 79 | 2.6 | 1.4–4.7 | .007 |
Nutrition and weight indicators | |||||||||||
Family meals, ≥7/wke | 614 | 26 | Reference | – | 42 | 2.1 | 1.2–3.6 | 41 | 2.1 | 1.1–3.9 | .02 |
Fast food for family meals, ≥1 timee | 613 | 82 | Reference | – | 85 | 1.2 | 0.6–2.3 | 69 | 0.5 | 0.2–0.9 | <.001 |
Fast food, weekly | 1,141 | 73 | Reference | – | 63 | 0.6 | 0.4–0.9 | 57 | 0.4 | 0.3–0.6 | < .001 |
Fruit, ≥2 servings/d | 1,142 | 34 | Reference | – | 34 | 1.1 | 0.7–1.7 | 35 | 1.2 | 0.8–2.0 | .62 |
Vegetables, ≥3 servings/d | 1,142 | 27 | Reference | – | 36 | 1.7 | 1.1–2.5 | 42 | 2.3 | 1.5–3.6 | <.001 |
Whole grains, ≥3 servings/d | 1,142 | 27 | Reference | – | 18 | 0.5 | 0.3–0.8 | 20 | 0.7 | 0.4–1.2 | .05 |
Sugar-sweetened beverages, ≥1/d | 1,142 | 16 | Reference | 17 | 1.0 | 0.6–1.7 | 20 | 1.3 | 0.7–2.3 | .59 | |
Overweight or obese | 1,073 | 62 | Reference | – | 62 | 0.9 | 0.6–1.4 | 69 | 1.4 | 0.9–2.1 | .18 |
Home environment | LSMeansc | CId | LSMeans | CI | LSMeans | CI | |||||
Unhealthy food at home | 1,123 | 7.2 | 6.8–7.6 | 7.0 | 6.7–7.4 | 6.7 | 6.4–7.1 | .07 | |||
Healthy food at home | 1,127 | 13.6 | 13.0–14.2 | 14.3 | 13.8–14.8 | 14.3 | 13.7–14.8 | .07 | |||
Barriers to food preparatione | 407 | 12.5 | 11.5–13.6 | 11.8 | 11.1–12.4 | 10.4 | 9.7–11.2 | < .001 |
CI indicates confidence interval; OR, odds ratio; EAT, Project Eating and Activity in Teens and Young Adults; LSMeans, least squared means.
Unadjusted percentages.
Analyses controlling for age, sex, race/ethnicity, and socioeconomic status (all measured at EAT-I) and educational attainment (measured at EAT-IV).
Controlling for age, sex, race/ethnicity, and SES (all measured at EAT-I) and educational attainment (measured at EAT-IV).
95% confidence limit for odds ratio or means.
Items asked among only participants with children (54%).
When analyses were repeated to include available measures of the dependent variables (fast-food restaurant frequency; consumption of fruits, vegetables, whole grains, and soda; weight status; and home availability of healthy or unhealthy foods) when participants were aged 18–23 years, the results were unaffected.
DISCUSSION
The aim of the current study was to determine whether reporting adequate cooking skills as a young adult was associated with multiple indicators of healthful nutrition a decade later. The study found that the perception of adequate cooking skills in emerging adulthood predicted food preparation behaviors and healthier eating behaviors more than a decade later.
In the current study, one quarter of emerging adults perceived their cooking skills to be very adequate. The measure of adequacy of cooking skills was self-reported and open to interpretation. In a qualitative study, Wolfson et al29 found that people defined cooking by a broad range of activities, from cooking from scratch to preparing anything at home. Nonetheless, perceived adequacy of cooking skills indicates a marker of self-efficacy regarding cooking and the current findings suggest that this alone may have an enduring impact on diet quality.
Reporting adequate cooking skills at age 18–23 years was associated with usual involvement in meal preparation, having frequent family meals, greater vegetable consumption, and lower consumption of fast food later in life. Findings were consistent with a growing body of literature that suggests learning to cook is associated with better dietary behaviors. In a review of cooking studies among adults, McGowan et al30 found that cooking skills were associated with better dietary behaviors in observational studies, but findings from intervention studies were limited by the few studies and small sample sizes. For example, a Scottish cooking intervention (n = 113) in a high-deprivation area resulted in a small but positive effect on cooking confidence and food choices.31
It is hypothesized that developing cooking skills leads to better dietary behaviors through greater involvement in cooking. Numerous studies demonstrated that involvement in home cooking and food preparation is associated with better dietary indicators for adults, in cross-sectional studies1–3,32 and longitudinal studies.6 That said, a study of Australian adults33 reported only small differences in diet quality for people involved in food preparation, compared with those who were not. Moreover, in a previous cross-sectional analysis of the EAT-II dataset, there were no associations between cooking skills and dietary quality.3 It is possible that the impact of developing cooking skills early in life may not be apparent until later adulthood when individuals have more opportunity and responsibility for meal preparation. The current study found no relationships between perceived cooking skills and later weight status, home food availability, or soda, fruit, or whole-grain consumption. This likely reflects the numerous influences on weight status and food choices, and that consumption of foods such as soda, fruits, and whole grains typically does not require cooking.
Strengths of the current study include the large, population-based sample size and timeliness of the data. The longitudinal nature of the data, over a long period, adds novelty to this type of research. In addition, the range of food preparation behaviors and diet indicators is valuable. However, there are limitations worth considering when interpreting the findings presented here. First, the data were collected among a cohort of participants who lived in the midwestern region of the US during their adolescence. As such, they may not reflect the greater diversity of the American or international populations. Second, the measure of adequacy of cooking skills was not specific enough to identify the attributes and resources that people need to be able to prepare their own healthy meals. Finally, both the cooking abilities measure and the nutrition-related outcomes were self-reported. As such, it is possible that shared method variance may explain some of these findings, because participants who report their cooking abilities positively may also report their nutrition-related outcomes positively.
IMPLICATIONS FOR RESEARCH AND PRACTICE
Opportunities to develop adequate cooking skills by young adulthood may result in long-term benefits for nutritional well-being, particularly with regard to cooking and eating behaviors. It is striking that simply identifying having adequate cooking skills by emerging adulthood can significantly predict involvement in food preparation and healthier eating over the next decade. Families, health and nutrition professionals, educators, community agencies, and funders can continue to invest in home economics and cooking education despite challenges in evaluating the long-term effects of these programs. Opportunities to develop cooking skills may be particularly important for adolescents and young adults as they develop more autonomy and live independently. Findings presented here provide evidence to justify the implementation of interventions during adolescence and emerging adulthood to enhance cooking skills, with strong evaluation designs that observe young people over a number of years.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
This study was supported by Grant No. R01HL116892 from the National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute (primary investigator: Neumark-Sztainer). The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute or the National Institutes of Health. Dr Winkler’s time was supported by Training Grant T32DK083250 from the National Institute of Diabetes and Digestive and Kidney Diseases (primary investigator: Jeffery).
Footnotes
Conflict of Interest Disclosure: The authors have not stated any conflicts of interest.
REFERENCES
- 1.Wolfson JA, Bleich SN. Is cooking at home associated with better diet quality or weight-loss intention? Public Health Nutr. 2015;18:1397–1406. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Berge JM, MacLehose RF, Larson N, Laska M, Neumark-Sztainer D. Family food preparation and its effects on adolescent dietary quality and eating patterns. J Adolesc Health. 2016;59:530–536. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Larson NI, Perry CL, Story M, Neumark-Sztainer D. Food preparation by young adults is associated with better diet quality. J Am Diet Assoc. 2006; 106:2001–2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Larson NI, Story M, Eisenberg ME, Neumark-Sztainer D. Food preparation and purchasing roles among adolescents: associations with sociodemographic characteristics and diet quality. J Am Diet Assoc. 2006;106:211–218. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Utter J, Denny S, Lucassen M, Dyson B. Adolescent cooking abilities and behaviors: associations with nutrition and emotional well-being. J Nutr Educ Behav. 2016;48:35–41, e31. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Laska MN, Larson NI, Neumark-Sztainer D, Story M. Does involvement in food preparation track from adolescence to young adulthood and is it associated with better dietary quality? Findings from a 10-year longitudinal study. Public Health Nutr. 2012;15:1150–1158. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Smith LP, Ng SW, Popkin BM. Trends in US home food preparation and consumption: analysis of national nutrition surveys and time use studies from 1965–1966 to 2007–2008. Nutr J. 2013;12:45. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Lichtenstein AH, Ludwig DS. Bring back home economics education. JAMA. 2010;303:1857–1858. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Caraher M, Dixon P, Lang T, Carr-Hill R. The state of cooking in England: the relationship of cooking skills to food choice. Br Food J. 1999;101:590–609. [Google Scholar]
- 10.Utter J, Denny S, Lucassen M, Dyson B. Who is teaching the kids to cook? Results from a nationally representative survey of secondary school students in New Zealand [published online ahead of print August 10, 2016]. Int J Adolesc Med Health. 10.1515/ijamh-2016-0064. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Wolfson JA, Frattaroli S, Bleich SN, Smith KC, Teret SP. Perspectives on learning to cook and public support for cooking education policies in the United States: a mixed methods study. Appetite. 2017;108:226–237. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Utter J, Fay AP, Denny S. Child and youth cooking programs: more than good nutrition? J Hung Environ Nutr. 2016;12:1–27. [Google Scholar]
- 13.Reicks M, Trofholz AC, Stang JS, Laska MN. Impact of cooking and home food preparation interventions among adults: outcomes and implications for future programs. J Nutr Educ Behav. 2014;46:259–276. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Neumark-Sztainer D, Story M, Hannan P, Moe J. Overweight status and eating patterns among adolescents: where do youth stand in comparison to the Healthy People 2010 Objectives? Am J Pub Health. 2002;92:844–851. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Neumark-Sztainer D, Croll J, Story M, Hannan PJ, French SA, Perry C. Ethnic/racial differences in weight-related concerns and behaviors among adolescent girls and boys: findings from Project EAT. J Psychosom Res. 2002;53:963–974. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Neumark-Sztainer D, Wall M, Guo J, Story M, Haines J, Eisenberg M. Obesity, disordered eating, and eating disorders in a longitudinal study of adolescents: how do dieters fare 5 years later? J Am Diet Assoc. 2006;106:559–568. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Neumark-Sztainer D, Wall M, Larson N, Eisenberg M, Loth K. Dieting and disordered eating behaviors from adolescence to young adulthood: findings from a 10-year longitudinal study. J Am Diet Assoc. 2011;111:1004–1011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Larson N, Neumark-Sztainer D, Harwood E, Eisenberg M, Wall M, Hannan P. Do young adults participate in surveys that “go green”? Response rates to a web and mailed survey of weight-related health behaviors. Int J Child Health Hum Dev. 2011;4:225–237. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Betts N, Amos R, Keim K, Peters P, Stewart B. Ways young adults view foods. J Nutr Educ. 1997;29:73–79. [Google Scholar]
- 20.Larson N, Neumark-Sztainer D, Story M, van den Berg P, Hannan PJ. Identifying correlates of young adults’ weight behavior: survey development. Am J Health Behav. 2011;35:712–725. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Larson NI, Nelson MC, Neumark-Sztainer D, Story M, Hannan PJ. Making time for meals: meal structure and associations with dietary intake in young adults. J Am Diet Assoc. 2009;109:72–79. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Alberga AS, Sigal RJ, Goldfield G, Prud’homme D, Kenny GP. Overweight and obese teenagers: why is adolescence a critical period? Pediatr Obes. 2012;7:261–273. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Harvard School of Public Health Nutrition Department. https://regepi.bwh harvard.edu/health/nutrition.html Accessed March 8, 2017.
- 24.Feskanich D, Rimm E, Giovannucci E, et al. Reproducibility and validity of food intake measurements from a semiquantitative food frequency questionnaire. J Am Diet Assoc. 1993;93:790–796. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Rimm E, Giovannucci E, Stampfer M, Colditz G, Litin L, Willett W. Reproducibility and validity of an expanded self-administered semiquantitative food frequency questionnaire among male health professionals. Am J Epidemiol. 1992;135:1114–1126, discussion 1127–1136. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Willet W Nutritional Epidemiology. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 1998. [Google Scholar]
- 27.Rockett HR, Wolf AM, Colditz GA. Development and reproducibility of a food frequency questionnaire to assess diets of older children and adolescents. J Am Diet Assoc. 1995;95:336–340. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Quick V, Wall M, Larson N, Haines J, Neumark-Sztainer D. Personal, behavioral and socio-environmental predictors of overweight incidence in young adults: 10-yr longitudinal findings. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2013;10:37. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Wolfson JA, Bleich SN, Smith KC, Frattaroli S. What does cooking mean to you? Perceptions of cooking and factors related to cooking behavior. Appetite. 2016;97:146–154. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.McGowan L, Caraher M, Raats M, et al. Domestic cooking and food skills: a review. Crit Rev Food Sci Nutr. 2017; 57:2412–2431. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Wrieden WL, Anderson AS, Longbottom PJ, et al. The impact of a community-based food skills intervention on cooking confidence, food preparation methods and dietary choices—an exploratory trial. Public Health Nutr. 2007;10:203–211. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Monsivais P, Aggarwal A, Drewnowski A. Time spent on home food preparation and indicators of healthy eating. Am J Prev Med. 2014;47:796–802. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Smith KJ, McNaughton SA, Gall SL, Blizzard L, Dwyer T, Venn AJ. Involvement of young Australian adults in meal preparation: cross-sectional associations with sociodemographic factors and diet quality. J Am Diet Assoc. 2010;110:1363–1367. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]