Skip to main content
European Journal of Psychotraumatology logoLink to European Journal of Psychotraumatology
. 2018 Aug 10;9(1):1500821. doi: 10.1080/20008198.2018.1500821

Association between feeling threatened by a terrorist attack and subjective health: a web survey a week after the attacks of 22 March 2016 in Belgium

恐怖袭击威胁与主观健康的关联:2016年3月22日比利时袭击发生一周后的一项网络调查

Asociación entre sentirse amenazado por un acto terrorista y salud subjetiva: Una encuesta vía web una semana después del ataque del 22 de marzo del 2016 en Bélgica.

Reginald Deschepper a,, Stefaan Six a, Yori Gidron b, Anne-Marie Depoorter a, Marie Vandekerckhove a, Nancy Gheysens a, Roel Van Overmeire a, Johan Bilsen a
PMCID: PMC6095037  PMID: 30128083

ABSTRACT

Background: The wave of terrorist attacks over the past years in Europe and other regions may cause problems such as anxiety and depressive symptoms. Some studies suggest that perceived threat might also trigger physical health problems.

Objective: To investigate the association between feeling threatened and subjective health during the week following a terrorist attack.

Method: Online survey with a self-selected sample in the Belgian population one week after the terrorist attacks in 2016. Participants were invited through the Belgian media to fill in a questionnaire in Dutch, French or English on a website. The main outcomes were the association between ‘feeling threatened’ and subjective health problems. Perceived threat was measured with the question ‘During the week after the attacks … Did you feel threatened?’ Subjective health was measured by using standardized scales (ACSA, PHQ-4, PHQ-15).

Results: A total of 2620 respondents completed the questionnaire, of whom 69.8% were female, 27.7% lived and 43.1% worked in Brussels. Gender, age, place of living and working, media exposure, religiousness and religious affiliation were associated significantly with higher perceived threat. A total of 21% of the respondents felt much or very much threatened during the week after the attacks. They reported significantly higher levels of mental and physical health problems. The most frequently reported problems were anxiety and depressive symptoms. The health problems that differentiated most markedly between those with low and high levels of perceived threat were fainting spells, chest pain and shortness of breath.

Conclusion: In a self-selected sample of respondents, ‘feeling threatened’ was strongly associated with lower level of wellbeing and higher levels of mental and physical health problems. The most prevalent health problems were mental health problems but the most pronounced differences between people with low versus high levels of perceived threat were physical health problems.

KEYWORDS: Terrorist attacks, perceived threat, Belgium, health problems, symptom monitoring

HIGHLIGHTS: • In a self-selected sample of respondents, one week after a terrorist attack, ‘feeling threatened’ was strongly associated with higher levels of mental and physical health problems.• Three-quarters of the people who felt threatened reported at least a moderate level of mental or physical health problems.• The most pronounced differences between people with low versus high levels of perceived threat were some physical health problems.

1. Background

Terrorist attacks are fundamentally different from other catastrophes (Stein et al., 2004). A review of 160 catastrophes demonstrated more negative mental health responses following terrorist disasters than following natural or technical disasters (Norris, Friedman, & Watson, 2002). Emotional distress, anxiety and depressive symptoms are common reactions in populations directly affected by terrorist attacks (Chen, Chung, Chen, Fang, & Chen, 2003; Ford, Adams, & Dailey, 2007). In the three to five days after the attacks on 11 September 2001 in the US, 44% experienced substantial stress reactions, 90% experienced at least low levels of stress and 47% reported increased anxiety and fear (DiMaggio & Galea, 2006; Schuster et al., 2001). Vulnerable persons may develop more serious psychological consequences. Of the people living in the regions of the attacks, 9.4% met criteria for depression and 7.5% for post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) (Galea et al., 2002). Furthermore, when people are anticipating disasters, their fears can worsen existing symptoms (Schuster et al., 2001). In contrast, some studies suggest that habituation may occur, due to continuous exposure to threat, as in Israel (Bleich, Gelkopf, & Solomon, 2003).

Several studies indicate that the effects are not limited to mental health problems. The Worcester Heart Attack study, conducted in a region more than 300 km from New York, suggests that the attacks in New York and on the Pentagon resulted in a significant increase in fatal acute myocardial infarctions (Goldberg et al., 2005; Qureshi, Merla, Steinberg, & Rozanski, 2003). A plausible mechanism linking acute stress responses to cardiac events is that the perceived stress evokes an acute and non-regulated fight-or-flight response that decreases parasympathetic (vagal) tone (Lampert, Baron, McPherson, & Lee, 2002). This may eventually result in detrimental biological effects (Blackburn & Epel, 2012; Gidron, Gilutz, Berger, & Huleihel, 2002).

In the past years, Europe has also been confronted with a wave of attacks. Although the risk of being directly hit by such attacks is quite limited, the perception that anyone can be hit at any moment may engender feelings of threat. Rather than objective factors, such as proximity to the terrorist attack, the perception of threat has a psychological effect (Ford et al., 2007; Hansen, Nissen, & Heir, 2013; Nissen, Birkeland Nielsen, Solberg, Bang Hansen, & Heir, 2015). Although several studies have found an association between exposure and post-traumatic symptoms (Hansen et al., 2013; Heir, Blix, & Knatten, 2016), other studies did not find such association (Bleich et al., 2003; Ford et al., 2007). Subjective perception of threat is a good predictor of probable PTSD (Cukor et al., 2011). These findings point towards the importance of the subjective perception of threat by such attacks. Feeling threatened by terrorist attacks may also induce behavioural changes that in turn can lead to increased use of cigarettes, alcohol and illegal drugs (Vlahov, Galea, Ahern, Resnick, & Kilpatrick, 2004).

Not everybody is equally at risk of feeling threatened and of the negative consequences this entails. Some categories of people such as women (Bleich et al., 2003; Schuster et al., 2001; Stein et al., 2004), ethnic minorities (Schuster et al., 2001; Stein et al., 2004), people of lower socioeconomic status (Rubin, Brewin, Greenberg, Simpson, & Wessely, 2005), children and people already suffering from chronic disease or pre-existing psychiatric disorders (Schuster et al., 2001) proved to be more vulnerable. A meta-analysis showed that PTSD due to terrorist attacks was more prevalent in Western Europe than in North America (DiMaggio & Galea, 2006). Protective factors that have been reported are older age, social support, being married (Ford et al., 2007) and community characteristics, such as availability of resources, the general sense of support and solidarity (Stein et al., 2013).

Notwithstanding the evidence of the impact of terroristic attacks, most studies are conducted in the US and only a small number of European studies deal with the more recent wave of terrorist attacks (Goodwin, Kaniasty, Sun, & Ben-Ezra, 2017; Hansen et al., 2017, 2013; Heir et al., 2016). As this particular wave of terrorism in Europe is rather recent, the evidence of the impact of terrorism on the population is not so well documented. More specifically, the role of perceived threat on subjective health is less clear. Subjective health is an important indicator because it is strongly associated with morbidity (Goldberg P, 2001) and functional impairment (Kroenke, Spitzer, Williams, & Löwe, 2010). It can reflect the objective health status and serve as a global measure of health status in the general population (Benjamins, Hummer, Eberstein, & Nam, 2004).

The aim of this study was to investigate the association between feeling threatened and subjective health during the week following a terrorist attack.

2. Methods

2.1. Design, participants and measures

We conducted a cross-sectional study using a web survey among a self-selected sample one week after the terrorist attacks in Belgium on 22 March 2016. Participants were invited through the Belgian media (some regional TV stations, radio and some widespread newspapers) to take part in a study using questionnaires in Dutch, French or English. The study was approved by the Medical Ethics Committee of UZ Brussels/VUB (B.U.N. 143,201,526,618).

Threat was measured with the single question ‘During the week after the attacks … did you feel threatened?’ and a 5-point answer scale (ranging from ‘not at all’ to ‘very much’).

Subjective wellbeing was measured by means of the Anamnestic Comparative Self-Assessment (ACSA) scale, where the + 5 and −5 scale anchors respectively reflected respondents’ memories of the best and worst period in their whole life (Bernheim, 1999).

Self-reported subjective health problems were measured with two Patient Health Questionnaires. Mental health problems were assessed with the Patient Health Questionnaire-4 (PHQ-4) (Kroenke et al., 2010), a validated tool for detecting anxiety and depression, the two most common mental disorders for which the scores are strongly associated with functional impairment and healthcare use (Cronbach’s alpha = .878). Respondents were asked to indicate how often they have been bothered by each symptom in the week following the attacks, on a scale ranging from ‘Never’ = 0, ‘One or a few days’ = 1, ‘More than half of the week’ = 2 to ‘Every day’ = 3. The total score ranges from 0 to 12 with cut off points of 3, 6 and 9 representing thresholds for mild, moderate and severe symptom levels, respectively.

Self-reported physical health problems were assessed by the Patient Health Questionnaire-15 (PHQ-15) (Kroenke, Spitzer, & Williams, 2002). The PHQ-15 includes 15 symptoms that account for more than 90% of symptoms seen in primary care (exclusive of upper respiratory symptoms) (Cronbach’s alpha = .835). Respondents were asked to rate how much they have been bothered by each symptom in the week following the attacks. This was rated as ‘Not bothered’ = 0, ‘Bothered a little’ = 1 or ‘Bothered a lot’ = 2. The total score ranges from 0 to 30 with cut off points of 5, 10 and 15 representing thresholds for mild, moderate and severe symptom levels, respectively.

Attribution of reported subjective wellbeing was measured with the question: ‘Have the attacks in Brussels affected your answer to the above question (best – worst time of my life)?’ We used a 5-point scale ranging from −5 ‘I felt much worse’ to + 5 ‘I felt much better by the threat of terrorism’.

Attribution of health problems was measured with the question: ‘Do you think the physical/mental problems you listed in the previous question are independent of the attacks and subsequent threat?’ We used a 4-point scale ranging from ‘completely independent of the attacks and subsequent terrorist threat’ to ‘fully coherent with the attacks and subsequent terrorist threat’.

In addition, background information (age, gender, working place, place of residence, living situation, education, professional status, occupation, religiousness, religious affiliation, media exposure) was requested. Media exposure was measured by average number of hours a day viewing information during the week after the terrorist attacks (on the TV, radio, internet, in the newspaper, etc.). Respondents could express their thoughts with the final open question ‘Do you have any comments?’

2.2. Statistical analysis

We calculated frequencies of respondents’ characteristics and their relationship with the level of feeling threatened, as well as relationships between feeling threatened and wellbeing outcomes. For these analyses, the level of threat was coded as ‘high’ (‘much’/’very much’) and ‘low’ (‘none at all’, ‘a little’, ‘moderate’) threat. For ACSA, PHQ-4 and PHQ-15, we calculated mean scores to test for significant differences according to the level of perceived threat. ACSA was further recoded into five subgroups ranging from worst (−5 or −4) to best (+ 4 or + 5) period of the respondent’s life. PHQ-4 and PHQ-15 were recoded into four categories (‘none’, ‘mild’, ‘moderate’ and ‘severe’ psychological distress and ‘minimal’, ‘low’, ‘medium’ and ‘high’ level of physical health problems) (Kroenke et al., 2002, 2010). We tested difference in the level of threat by characteristics of the respondents with Chi-square and association between feeling threatened and subjective health with independent t-tests. The scores of respondents who did not fill out one or more questions of the scales were treated as missing values. Finally, to test the independent role of perceived threat in health outcomes, we conducted a hierarchical multiple regression, where we controlled for effects of age, gender, education and residence place (block 1), and then entered perceived threat (block 2) in relation to the outcome. All statistical analyses were performed using SPSS version 24.0.

3. Results

3.1. Characteristics of the population

In total, N = 2620 respondents completed the online survey between 29 and 31 March 2016 (Table 1).

Table 1.

Characteristic of respondents (N = 2620).

  N %
Gender    
 Female 1830 69.8
 Male 790 30.2
Age    
 18–25 290 11.1
 26–45 1297 49.5
 46–65 888 33.9
 65+ 135 5.1
 Missing 10 0.4
Living situation    
 Alone 536 20.5
 With partner 1320 50.4
 With another person 417 15.9
 Other 341 13.0
 Missing 6 0.2
Place of living    
 Brussels 725 27.7
 Not-Brussels 1887 72.0
 Missing 8 0.3
Place of working    
 Brussels 1128 43.1
 Not-Brussels 1487 56.7
 Missing 5 0.2
Education    
 Lower secondary or less 240 9.2
 Upper secondary 841 32.1
 College/university 1539 58.7
Religious affiliation    
 Christian 1103 42.1
 Liberals 898 34.3
 Muslim 41 1.6
 Other 467 17.8
 Multiple 103 3.9
 Missing 8 0.3
Religiousness    
 Non-religious 1346 51.4
 A little faith 808 30.8
 Faithful 378 14.4
 Very religious 88 3.4
Media use    
 < 1 hour/day 234 8.9
 1–2 hours/day 673 25.7
 2–3 hours/day 616 23.5
 > 3 hours/day 1097 41.9

Respondents felt very much (5.2%), much (15.8%), moderately (24.4%), a little (31.1%) or not at all (23.4%) threatened during the week after the attacks (not in Table 1).

3.2. Factors associated with feeling threatened

Gender, place of living and working, age, religiousness and media exposure were associated with a high level of threat. There was also a significant difference in threat level as a function of religious affiliation, with larger proportions of Muslims perceiving the highest threat (Table 2).

Table 2.

Level of feeling threatened according to characteristics of respondents.

  N= 2620 % Low Threat
79
% High Threat
21
p-value
Gender        
 Female 1830 76.1 23.9 < 0.001
 Male 790 85.7 14.3
Age       < 0.001
 18–25 290 81.0 19.0
 26–45 1297 75.8 24.2
 46–65 888 80.7 19.3
 65+ 135 92.6 7.4  
Living situation       0.069
 Alone 536 79.3 20.7
 With partner 1320 79.9 20.1
 With another person 417 80.3 19.7
 Other 341 73.6 26.4
Place of living        
 Brussels 725 72.4 27.6 < 0.001
 Not-Brussels 1887 81.7 18.3
 Place of working        
 Brussels 1128 74.7 25.3 < 0.001
 Not-Brussels 1485 82.3 17.7
Education        
 Lower secondary or less 240 73.8 26.3 0.111
 Upper secondary 841 79.8 20.2
 College/university 1539 79.3 20.7
Religious affiliation        
 Christian 1103 75.4 24.6  
 Liberals 898 85.7 14.3 < 0.001
 Muslim 41 65.9 34.1
 Multiple 103 81.6 18.4
 Other 467 75.2 24.8
Religiousness        
 Non-religious 1346 82.5 17.5  
 A little faith 808 76.4 23.6 < 0.001
 Faithful 378 73.5 26.5
 Very religious 88 72.7 27.3
Media exposure        
 < 1 hour/day 234 90.2 9.8  
 1–2 hours/day 673 87.2 12.8 < 0.001
 2–3 hours/day 616 81.8 18.2
 > 3 hours/day 1097 69.9 30.1

3.3. Association between feeling threatened and subjective health

Firstly, respondents with high levels of perceived threat reported lower subjective wellbeing. Among respondents with high levels of perceived threat, 36.8% considered the period after the attacks as the worst period of their life (Table 3). Almost three-quarters (72.1%) of respondents in the ‘high threat’ group attributed their low subjective wellbeing to the threat of terrorism.

Table 3.

Association between feeling threatened and subjective health.

  Total
N = 2620
Low Threat
N = 2069
High Threat
N = 551
Signifi-cance*
Subjective wellbeing
Mean score on ACSA (SD) −1.41 (2.02) −1.03 (1.91) −2.81 (1.76) < 0.001
– Worst period of my life (−5 or −4) 362 (13.8%) 159 (7.7%) 203 (36.8%) < 0.001
– More like the worst period (−3 or −2) 983 (37.5%) 740 (35.8%) 243 (44.1%)
– Not really like the worst or best period (−1 to 1) 1035 (39.5%) 942 (45.5%) 93 (16.9%)
– More like the best period (2 or 3) 196 (7.5%) 185 (8.9%) 11 (2.0%)
– Best period of my life (4 or 5) 44 (1.7%) 43 (2.1%) 1 (0.2%)
Felt much worse by the threat of terrorism 868 (33.1%) 471 (22.8%) 397 (72.1%)  
Mental health problems
Mean score on PHQ_4 (SD) 4.72 (3.57) 3.86 (3.16) 7.99 (3.15) < 0.001
Problems are strongly associated or fully coherent with the attacks and subsequent terrorist threat 1315 (50.2%) 875 (42.3%) 440 (79.9%)  
Categories of psychological distress        
– None (score 0–2) 825 (31.5%) 799 (38.6%) 26 (4.7%) < 0.001
– Mild (score 3–5) 763 (29.1%) 660 (31.9%) 103 (18.7%)
– Moderate (score 6–8) 467 (17.8%) 322 (15.6%) 145 (26.3%)
– Severe (score 9–12) 454 (17.3%) 207 (10.0%) 247 (44.8%)
Physical health problems
Mean score on PHQ-15 (SD) 5.92 (5.22) 4.99 (4.59) 9.50 (5.94) < 0.001
Problems are strongly associated or fully coherent with the attacks and subsequent terrorist threat 652 (24.9%) 376 (18.2%) 276 (50.1%)  
Levels of Physical symptoms
Minimal (0–4) 1226 (46.8%) 1116 (53.9%) 110 (20.0%) < 0.001
Low (5–9) 757 (28.9%) 574 (27.7%) 183 (33.2%)
Medium (10–14) 328 (12.5%) 206 (10.0%) 122 (22.1%)
High (15 −30) 203 (7.7%) 101 (4.9%) 102 (18.5%)

* Tested using independent t-tests

Secondly, respondents with high levels of perceived threat also reported substantially more mental health problems (mean score on PHQ-4 scale: 7.99 versus 3.86 compared to those with lower threat levels), and 44.8% of them reported severe symptoms of psychological distress. Of the people perceiving a high threat, 79.9% reported that their symptoms were strongly associated or fully coherent with the attacks.

Thirdly, physical health problems also occurred more often in this group of respondents (mean score on PHQ-15 scale: 9.50 for high perceived threat versus 4.99 for low perceived threat). Almost one in five (18.5%) respondents with high levels of perceived threat reported a high level of physical health problems and half of them indicated that their physical health problems were strongly associated or fully coherent with the threat. Three-quarters (75.5%) of the respondents who experienced high levels of perceived threat reported at least a moderate level of psychological distress (score ≥ 6) or medium level of physical health problems (score ≥ 10).

All of the subjective health problems were more frequent in the group with high levels of perceived threat. The most frequently reported problems were mental health problems (anxiety and depressive symptoms), lack of energy and sleeping problems. However, the subjective health problems that differentiated most markedly between those with high and low perceived threat were fainting spells (3.6 times higher), chest pain (2.4 times higher) and shortness of breath (2.4 times higher) (Table 4).

Table 4.

Differences in subjective health according to the level of perceived threat.

  Total
N = 2620 (%)
Low Threat
N = 2069 (%)
High Threat
N = 551 (%)
Ratio Signifi-cance
Mental health problems
Feeling nervous or on the edge 2149 (82.0) 1619 (78.3) 530 (96.2) 1.2 < 0.001
Not being able to stop or control worrying 1647 (62.9) 1148 (55.5) 499 (90.6) 1.6 < 0.001
Feeling down, depressed or hopelessness 1638 (62.5) 1155 (55.8) 483 (87.7) 1.6 < 0.001
Little interest or pleasure in doing things 1571 (60.0) 1107 (53.5) 464 (84.2) 1.6 < 0.001
Physical health problems
Feeling tired or having little energy 1876 (71.6) 1393 (67.3) 483 (87.7) 1.3 < 0.001
Trouble falling or staying asleep, or sleeping too much 1729 (66.0) 1256 (60.7) 473 (85.8) 1.4 < 0.001
Headaches 1092 (41.7) 760 (36.7) 332 (60.3) 1.6 < 0.001
Feeling your heart pound or race 855 (32.6) 566 (27.4) 289 (52.5) 1.9 < 0.001
Stomach pain 733 (28.0) 477 (23.1) 256 (46.5) 2.0 < 0.001
Back pain 817 (31.2) 585 (28.3) 232 (42.1) 1.5 < 0.001
Nausea, gas or indigestion 659 (25.2) 449 (21.7) 210 (38.1) 1.8 < 0.001
Pain in your arms, legs or joints (knees, hips, etc.) 739 (28.2) 545 (26.3) 194 (35.2) 1.3 < 0.001
Dizziness 502 (19.2) 316 (15.3) 186 (33.8) 2.2 < 0.001
Shortness of breath 470 (17.9) 287 (13.9) 183 (33.2) 2.4 < 0.001
Chest pain 442 (16.9) 268 (13.0) 174 (31.6) 2.4 < 0.001
Constipation or diarrhoea 526 (20.1) 361 (17.4) 165 (29.9) 1.7 < 0.001
Menstrual cramps or other problems with your periods (female only N = 1830) 206 (11.3) 147 (10.6) 59 (13.5) 1.3 0.013
Pain or problems during sex 188 (7.2) 119 (5.8) 69 (12.5) 2.2 < 0.001
Fainting spells 62 (2.4) 32 (1.5) 30 (5.4) 3.6 < 0.001

3.4. Association of perceived threat with subjective wellbeing, mental and physical health problems

We then examined the unique contribution of perceived threat to subjective health outcomes, beyond the role of three confounders, using hierarchical multiple regressions. The latter included age, gender and place of residence (Brussels, elsewhere). Age and gender reflected background information known to affect health outcomes. Place of residence reflected an objective estimate of proximity and exposure to the terrorist attacks and this was also significantly related to subjective wellbeing and both health outcomes.

After statistically controlling for the effects of age, gender and place of residence, perceived threat accounted for an additional and significant 20% of the variance in wellbeing, 30% of the variance in mental health problems and 17% of the variance in physical health problems (Table 5).

Table 5.

Hierarchical multiple regressions, testing the unique association of perceived threat with subjective wellbeing, mental and physical health problems, beyond background variables (N = 2620).

Model and variables B 95% CI Sig. R2
Subjective wellbeing as outcome        
1 Age 0.00 −0.01, 0.00 0.33  
Gender 0.57 0.40, 0.74 < 0.001  
Place of residence (Brussels/not) 0.35 0.18, 0.52 < 0.001 0.02
2. Age        
Gender        
Place of residence (Brussels/not)        
Perceived threat −0.81 −0.87, −0.74 < 0.001 0.22
Mental health problems as outcome        
1 Age 0.02 0.01, 0.03 < 0.001  
Gender −1.53 −1.83, −1.23 < 0.001  
Place of residence (Brussels/not) −0.90 −1.20, −0.59 < 0.001 0.05
2. Age        
Gender        
Place of residence (Brussels/not)        
Perceived threat 1.76 1.66, 1.86 < 0.001 0.36
Physical health problems as outcome        
1 Age 0.01 −0.01, 0.02 0.41  
Gender −2.74 −3.18, −2.31 < 0.001  
Place of residence (Brussels/not) −0.53 −0.98, −0.09 0.02 0.06
2. Age        
Gender        
Place of residence (Brussels/not)        
Perceived threat .94 1.77–2.10 < 0.001 0.23

4. Discussion

In a self-selected sample of respondents, ‘feeling threatened’ was strongly associated with lower level of wellbeing and higher level of mental and physical health problems. The most prevalent health problems were mental health problems, but the most pronounced differences between people with low versus high levels of perceived threat were physical health problems.

A main finding is that three-quarters of the people in this study who self-identified as feeling much or very much threatened in the week following the terrorist attacks in Brussels felt bad and reported moderate to severe levels of mental or physical health problems (Kroenke et al., 2002, 2010). Some subgroups reported more negative effects: people working or living in the region of the attacks, women and religious people, especially Muslims. Some symptoms, such as fainting spells and chest pain, were more prevalent in the respondents who experienced high levels of threat. Perceived threat was significantly associated with wellbeing as well as mental and physical health, even after statistically controlling for the effects of age, gender and place of residence.

As far as we know, this is the first study exploring self-reported subjective health problems shortly after one of the more recent terrorist attacks in Europe and especially on the role of feeling threatened. By collecting the data shortly after the attacks, recall bias was limited. By using a self-selected online sample, we were able to recruit a large number of respondents who were willing to provide us a thorough insight into how they felt and what they experienced after these tragic attacks.

An obvious limitation of this study is that the sample was not representative. We must be very prudent about generalizing the results. Women and highly educated persons are more highly represented than in the population, for example. It is also unclear whether people who worried most were overrepresented among our respondents. Furthermore, people with less access to or with fewer skills in using the Internet were probably underrepresented. Another limitation is that the reported symptoms were based entirely on self-assessment and that we had no baseline pre-stressor measurement. The cross-sectional design does not enable conclusions about the directionality of observed associations or about cause and effect and neither can we make statements about the evolution of health problems. PHQ measures are often used for research but, to our knowledge, there is limited or no research on the usefulness and validity of the PHQ-4 and PHQ-15 shortly after terrorist attacks.

Although the risk of being directly affected by a terrorist attack might be very low, our findings show that the perceived threat is a major factor associated with how people react to such traumatic events. This finding is in line with previous research indicating that perception of threat, rather than real risk, is the most important predictor of self-reported health problems and that people perceiving high threat are therefore a vulnerable group for reporting and possibly even for developing certain health problems (Blackburn & Epel, 2012; Heir et al., 2016; Nissen et al., 2015).

The most prevalent problems reported by people who felt highly threatened were anxiety and stress-related mental health problems, lack of energy and sleeping problems. These problems were also found in studies conducted after other terroristic attacks. However, the most pronounced differences were related to some less prevalent but more alarming health problems: fainting spells, chest pain and shortness of breath. This poses the question of whether such health problems might be related to a rise in myocardial infarctions shortly after terroristic attacks in high-risk individuals, as reported in other studies (Goldberg et al., 2005; Qureshi et al., 2003).

The finding that women are more prone to threat and its effect is well known (Schuster et al., 2001; Stein et al., 2004). People working or living in the region of the attacks felt more threatened, which is in line with most other studies (Hansen et al., 2013). However, some studies only found a weak association between perceived threat and level of exposure (Bleich et al., 2003; Cohen et al., 2006).

The finding that middle-aged people feel more threatened than other age groups is consistent with a study by Chen et al. (2003) after the attacks in the US on 9/11. A possible explanation might be that feelings of threat in this age group may be extended to their close significant others. Hence, subsidiary feelings of threat, especially with regard to one’s children, might augment one’s perception of threat (Mawson, 2005).

Muslims reported the highest levels of threat. Although they were only a very small subgroup in our study, this finding is in line with other studies after terrorist attacks (Rubin et al., 2005; Schuster et al., 2001; Stein et al., 2004). Studies in Israel showed that populations associated with the offenders do not suffer less than the populations that are the explicit target of terrorists (Bleich et al., 2003; Cohen & Eid, 2007). It is however remarkable that violence ‘in the name of religion’ affects religious people the most. An alternative explanation might be that these people felt more vulnerable because of their limited income or because of threat from the terrorist attacks and from being mistakenly affiliated with its origin (Rubin et al., 2005).

People who spend a lot of time viewing information about the terrorist threat reported significantly higher levels of perceived threat. We cannot derive from our data whether people who felt threatened sought out more information or whether the opposite was true: people felt more threatened because of seeing more information about the attacks.

Our findings suggest that terrorist attacks do not only result in victims directly affected by the explosions but that many others may experience subjective health problems. After all, this is one of the main aims of terrorism. It can be therefore hypothesized that the months preceding the attacks, during which Belgium was in a state of highest alert after the attacks in Paris in November 2015, could already have primed a negative impact on the population. Other studies also found that by anticipating disasters, peoples’ fears can worsen existing symptoms (Schuster et al., 2001) and that an on-going perceived threat may engender health problems (Heir et al., 2016; Nissen et al., 2015).

Our study reports on subjective health specifically one week after the attacks and cannot make predictions about how this will evolve in the future. Many people are resilient and recover soon after such events (DiMaggio & Galea, 2006). However, a study by Stein et al. (2004) after the 9/11 attacks found that a significant number of adults continued to experience terrorism-related distress and disruption of their daily lives two months after the attacks (Stein et al., 2004). In a longitudinal study among police responders enrolled in the World Trade Center Health Registry, the prevalence of PTSD doubled between 2003–2004 and 2006–2007, suggesting that certain responses may even worsen with time (Bowler et al., 2012).

Three-quarters of the people who felt threatened reported at least a moderate level of mental or physical health problems. Longitudinal studies are needed to investigate if these subjective health problems will abate spontaneously or persevere and might require treatment by a professional caregiver. Self-administrated scales have been suggested as an efficient method for stratifying people into screen-positive and screen-negative groups, and might therefore be helpful by allowing clinicians to prioritize their limited time in favour of a smaller group with high scores (Kroenke et al., 2010). However, no systematic attempts have been made to identify populations at risk early after the attacks (Gruebner et al., 2016).

Our finding that ‘feeling threatened’ might serve as a red flag for people being at higher risk of developing health problems and might lead to the development of a practical tool for detecting people who might need greater attention by caregivers. However, the sensitivity and specificity of such a one-item measure might be low and probably more elaborate computerized questionnaires are required for prediction and follow-up of people at risk of developing health problems after traumatic events like terrorist attacks (Bourla, Mouchabac, El Hage, & Ferreri, 2018). Web-based intervention of this kind may be a useful tool to reach people with stress-related health problems and to conduct a first kind of ‘automatic’ triage by guiding them through a step-by-step process resulting in personalized advice such as to contact a suggested health care service or an invitation for another assessment of health problems after some time. This is especially useful because other studies have shown that a substantial number of people with high levels of symptoms do not seek professional care, and thus might develop problems that remain under the radar (Chang et al., 2017; Dyb, Jensen, Glad, Nygaard, & Thoresen, 2014)). It has also been shown that self-reported symptoms are highly associated with clinician-rated somatoform disorder symptom counts and that high scores are strongly associated with worsening function, increased disability days and health care utilization (Kroenke et al., 2010).

In addition to this, policymakers and caregivers should be prepared to deal with an increase in health problems in the days after the attacks, mainly for stress-related problems (Vandentorren, Paty, Baffert, Chansard, & Caserio-Schönemann, 2016). Especially for people with pre-existing problems, the terrorist threat might be a trigger causing severe problems such as major depression (Neria et al., 2013) and cardiac problems, including myocardial infarction (Goldberg et al., 2005; Qureshi et al., 2003).

The finding that certain populations are particularly vulnerable and that they may present typical health problems is also a reason to be prepared for providing adequate care tailored to the high-risk groups (Neria et al., 2013). Syndromic surveillance might be a useful measure for early detection at population level and to monitor the effects of terrorist attacks over time (Vandentorren et al., 2016). In addition, prevention strategies are needed that should be applied to all at risk, including those not yet showing PTSD symptoms shortly after the traumatic events.

Furthermore, not only survivors of terrorist attacks and their relatives are confronted with major stressors. Pre-hospital responders and health care services workers in general are professionally exposed to traumatic events. This is also the case for those providing psychosocial support to survivors and other people affected. Here too, the psychological problems are often underestimated (Bowler et al., 2012).

Further research is needed to check if physical health problems, such as fainting spells and chest pain, may be engendered by terroristic attacks in people who felt threatened. Longitudinal research is also needed to address the evolution of terrorism-related health problems and to shed light on cause–effect relationships, the role of religion and other factors, and the best ways to deal with (future) terrorist threats, should they happen.

5. Conclusion

In a self-selected sample of respondents, ‘feeling threatened’ was strongly associated with a lower level of subjective health. The most prevalent symptoms were mental health problems but the most pronounced differences between people with low versus high levels of perceived threat were physical health problems.

Funding Statement

This work was supported by Innoviris [BRGPRO1].

Disclosure statement

No potential conflict of interest was reported by the authors.

References

  1. Benjamins M. R., Hummer R. A., Eberstein I. W., & Nam C. B. (2004). Self-reported health and adult mortality risk: An analysis of cause-specific mortality. Social Science & Medicine (1982), 59(6), 1297–10. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Bernheim J. L. (1999). How to get serious answers to the serious question: ‘how have you been?’: subjective quality of life (QOL) as an individual experiential emergent construct. Bioethics, 13(3–4), 272–287. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Blackburn E. H., & Epel E. S. (2012). Telomeres and adversity: Too toxic to ignore. Nature, 490(7419), 169–171. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bleich A., Gelkopf M., & Solomon Z. (2003). Exposure to terrorism, stress-related mental health symptoms, and coping behaviors among a nationally representative sample in Israel. JAMA, 290(5), 612–620. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bourla A., Mouchabac S., El Hage W., & Ferreri F. (2018). e-PTSD: An overview on how new technologies can improve prediction and assessment of Posttraumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD). Feb 6, 9(sup1), 1424448. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bowler R. M., Harris M., Li J., Gocheva V., Stellman S. D., Wilson K., … Cone J. E. (2012). Longitudinal mental health impact among police responders to the 9/11 terrorist attack. American Journal of Industrial Medicine, 55(4), 297–312. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Chang J.-C., Yen A. M.-F., Chen -H.-H., Chen -S. L.-S., Chiu S. Y.-H., Fann J. C.-Y., & Lee C.-S. (2017). Comorbid diseases as risk factors for incident posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) in a large community cohort (KCIS no.PSY4). Scientific Reports, 7, 41276. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Chen H., Chung H., Chen T., Fang L., & Chen J.-P. (2003). The emotional distress in a community after the terrorist attack on the World Trade Center. Community Mental Health Journal, 39(2), 157–165. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Cohen M., & Eid J. (2007). The effect of constant threat of terror on Israeli Jewish and Arab adolescents. Anxiety, Stress, and Coping, 20(1), 47–60. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Cohen P., Kasen S., Chen H., Gordon K., Berenson K., Brook J., & White T. (2006). Current affairs and the public psyche: American anxiety in the post 9/11 world. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 41(4), 251–260. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Cukor J., Wyka K., Jayasinghe N., Weathers F., Giosan C., Leck P., … Difede J. (2011). Prevalence and predictors of posttraumatic stress symptoms in utility workers deployed to the World Trade Center following the attacks of September 11, 2001. Depression and Anxiety, 28(3), 210–217. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. DiMaggio C., & Galea S. (2006). The behavioral consequences of terrorism: A meta-analysis. Academic Emergency Medicine: Official Journal of the Society for Academic Emergency Medicine, 13(5), 559–566. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Dyb G., Jensen T., Glad K. A., Nygaard E., & Thoresen S. (2014). Early outreach to survivors of the shootings in Norway on the 22nd of July 2011. European Journal of Psychotraumatology, Jul 2, 5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Ford J. D., Adams M. L., & Dailey W. F. (2007). Psychological and health problems in a geographically proximate population time-sampled continuously for three months after the September 11th, 2001 terrorist incidents. Anxiety, Stress, and Coping, 20(2), 129–146. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Galea S., Ahern J., Resnick H., Kilpatrick D., Bucuvalas M., Gold J., & Vlahov D. (2002). Psychological sequelae of the September 11 terrorist attacks in New York City. The New England Journal of Medicine, 346(13), 982–987. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Gidron Y., Gilutz H., Berger R., & Huleihel M. (2002). Molecular and cellular interface between behavior and acute coronary syndromes. Cardiovascular Research, 56(1), 15–21. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Goldberg P., Guéguen A., & Schmaus A., et al. (2001). Longitudinal study of associations between perceived health status and self reported diseases in the french gazel cohort. Journal of Epidemiology & Community Health, 55(4), 233–238. doi: 10.1136/jech.55.4.233 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Goldberg R. J., Spencer F., Lessard D., Yarzebski J., Lareau C., & Gore J. M. (2005). Occurrence of acute myocardial infarction in Worcester, Massachusetts, before, during, and after the terrorists attacks in New York City and Washington, DC, on 11 September 2001. The American Journal of Cardiology, 95(2), 258–260. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Goodwin R., Kaniasty K., Sun S., & Ben-Ezra M. (2017). Psychological Distress and Prejudice Following Terror Attacks in France. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 91(August), 111–115. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Gruebner O., Sykora M., Lowe S. R., Shankardass K., Trinquart L., Jackson T., Galea S. (2016). Mental health surveillance after the terrorist attacks in Paris. Lancet (London, England), 387(10034), 2195–2196. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Hansen M. B., Birkeland M. S., Nissen A., Blix I., Solberg Ø., & Heir T. (2017). Prevalence and Course of Symptom-Defined PTSD in Individuals Directly or Indirectly Exposed to Terror: A Longitudinal Study. Psychiatry, 80(2), 171–183. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Hansen M. B., Nissen A., & Heir T. (2013). Proximity to terror and post-traumatic stress: A follow-up survey of governmental employees after the 2011 Oslo bombing attack. BMJ Open, 3, 7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Heir T., Blix I., & Knatten C. K. (2016). Thinking that one’s life was in danger: Perceived life threat in individuals directly or indirectly exposed to terror. The British Journal of Psychiatry: the Journal of Mental Science, 209(4), 306–310. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Kroenke K., Spitzer R. L., & Williams J. B. W. (2002). The PHQ-15: Validity of a new measure for evaluating the severity of somatic symptoms. Psychosomatic Medicine, 64(2), 258–266. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Kroenke K., Spitzer R. L., Williams J. B. W., & Löwe B. (2010). The patient health questionnaire somatic, anxiety, and depressive symptom scales: a systematic review. General Hospital Psychiatry, 32(4), 345–359. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Lampert R., Baron S. J., McPherson C. A., & Lee F. A. (2002). Heart rate variability during the week of September 11, 2001. JAMA, 288(5), 575. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Mawson A. R. (2005). Understanding mass panic and other collective responses to threat and disaster. Psychiatry, 68(2), 95–113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Neria Y., Wickramaratne P., Olfson M., Gameroff M. J., Pilowsky D. J., Lantigua R., … Weissman M. M. (2013). Mental and physical health consequences of the September 11, 2001 (9/11) attacks in primary care: A longitudinal study. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 26(1), 45–55. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Nissen A., Birkeland Nielsen M., Solberg Ø., Bang Hansen M., & Heir T. (2015). Perception of threat and safety at work among employees in the Norwegian ministries after the 2011 Oslo bombing. Anxiety, Stress, and Coping, 28(6), 650–662. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Norris F. H., Friedman M. J., & Watson P. J. (2002). 60,000 disaster victims speak: Part II. summary and implications of the disaster mental health research. Psychiatry, 65(3), 240–260. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Qureshi E. A., Merla V., Steinberg J., & Rozanski A. (2003). Terrorism and the heart: Implications for arrhythmogenesis and coronary artery disease. Cardiac Electrophysiology Review, 7(1), 80–84. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Rubin G. J., Brewin C. R., Greenberg N., Simpson J., & Wessely S. (2005). Psychological and behavioural reactions to the bombings in London on 7 July 2005: Cross sectional survey of a representative sample of Londoners. BMJ (Clinical Research Ed.), 331(7517), 606. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Schuster M. A., Stein B. D., Jaycox L., Collins R. L., Marshall G. N., Elliott M. N., Berry S. H. (2001). A national survey of stress reactions after the September 11, 2001, terrorist attacks. The New England Journal of Medicine, 345(20), 1507–1512. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Stein B. D., Elliott M. N., Jaycox L. H., Collins R. L., Berry S. H., Klein D. J., & Schuster M. A. (2004). A national longitudinal study of the psychological consequences of the September 11, 2001 terrorist attacks: Reactions, impairment, and help-seeking. Psychiatry, 67(2), 105–117. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Stein N. R., Schorr Y., Krantz L., Dickstein B. D., Solomon Z., Horesh D., & Litz B. T. (2013). The differential impact of terrorism on two Israeli communities. The American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 83(4), 528–535. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Vandentorren S., Paty A.-C., Baffert E., Chansard P., & Caserio-Schönemann C. (2016). Syndromic surveillance during the Paris terrorist attacks. Lancet (London, England), 387(10021), 846–847. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Vlahov D., Galea S., Ahern J., Resnick H., & Kilpatrick D. (2004). Sustained increased consumption of cigarettes, alcohol, and marijuana among Manhattan residents after September 11, 2001. American Journal of Public Health, 94(2), 253–254. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from European Journal of Psychotraumatology are provided here courtesy of Taylor & Francis

RESOURCES