Abstract
Meta-analyses techniques using 11 independent datasets were used to determine correlations between pork loin quality evaluated at 1-d postmortem and quality evaluated at 12- to 28-d postmortem. Datasets encompassed approximately 3,957 loins. The effects of aging on ventral loin surface quality were determined using a paired-T test. Ventral loin surfaces become 8% lighter (P < 0.0001), 44.5% redder (P < 0.0001), and 46% more yellow (P < 0.0001) during the aging period. Therefore, it is apparent that loin quality changes during postmortem aging. Because of this, it becomes necessary to determine the correlation between early and aged pork quality parameters. Pearson correlation coefficients within original datasets were calculated, and then sample-weighted mean correlations and variances [Var(r)] were calculated across datasets. Early postmortem ventral instrumental lightness (L*) was moderately correlated with aged ventral L* = 0.50), aged ventral visual color = −0.38), aged chop face (freshly cut) L* = 0.44), and aged chop face visual color = −0.38). Early postmortem ventral instrumental redness (a*) was moderately correlated with aged ventral a* = 0.49) and aged chop face a* = 0.46). Early postmortem ventral visual color was moderately correlated with aged ventral L* = −0.51), aged ventral color ( = 0.50), aged chop face L* = −0.43), and aged chop face visual color = 0.43). However, no instrumental or visual color parameters were moderately or strongly correlated with instrumental tenderness or sensory panel ratings of tenderness and juiciness (|| ≤ 0.36). Early postmortem ventral visual marbling was moderately correlated with aged ventral marbling = 0.63) and aged chop face visual marbling = 0.56). Visual marbling was not (|| ≤ 0.12) correlated with instrumental tenderness or sensory panel ratings of tenderness and juiciness. Stepwise regression, using a holo-analysis approach, was used to determine the predictive ability of early postmortem ventral color, marbling, firmness, and pH on sensory panels ratings of tenderness (R2 = 0.13), juiciness (R2 = 0.09), and flavor (R2 = 0.28). Early postmortem color and marbling are important pork quality traits in consumers purchasing decisions, but are poorly related to traits associated with eating experience. Still, they may be predicative of traits associated with purchasing intent.
Keywords: correlation, holo-analysis, meta-analysis, pork, quality, sensory
INTRODUCTION
Predicting and improving quality is an important goal for the pork industry. The adoption of a U.S. pork quality grading system, based on early postmortem visual color and marbling, has been proposed as a potential strategy to improve pork quality (USDA, 2017). Previously, consumers rated darker-colored pork chops as juicier than lighter-colored pork chops (Norman et al., 2003), and darker color and greater marbling were positively correlated with sensory panel ratings of tenderness of pork loins (Huff-Lonergan et al., 2002). A challenge with the proposed grading system is that, commercially, loins are evaluated on the ventral surface of the loin at 1-d postmortem, but consumers use the transverse section (chop face) of the loin after a period of aging to make purchasing decisions and determine eating experience. For a pork quality grading system to be successful, quality estimates determined at the production facility on the ventral surface of the loin (1-d postmortem) must be correlated with aged quality estimates determined on the chop face and sensory characteristics. Although factors like the evaluation of quality attributes during fabrication have been addressed, what is less understood is the correlation between quality attributes measured at the time of grading and quality traits observed by the consumer after aging. Therefore, the objective was to calculate those correlations using meta-analyses techniques. A meta-analysis provides a quantitative, statistical approach to the summarization of previous literature and scientific findings and is a review of the literature, both published and unpublished, using a structured statistical approach (Schmidt and Hunter, 2014). By compiling results of multiple studies, statistical power is increased enabling greater precision in determining true relationships (Sauvant et al., 2008).
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Study Selection Criteria
Eleven independent experiments were selected for inclusion in the analysis. Inclusion criteria included studies where pigs were slaughtered at a commercial facility, loins were evaluated on the ventral surface of the loins on day-1 postmortem and then again after an aging period that ranged from 12 to 28 d. More importantly, exclusion criteria for studies in the meta-analyses were those where pigs were raised or slaughtered in academic settings and where the pigs used in studies were considered no longer contemporaries to current U.S. pigs (i.e., historical studies were excluded). Using these specific predefined inclusion and exclusion criteria allows for appropriate industry application of the results. Original experimental objectives of the parent studies were not considered and often times multiple treatments (e.g., dietary, genotype, or slaughter day) were represented within a dataset. Data from published and unpublished experiments were used in the analysis because a meta-analysis is a review of all available information that meet the selection criteria, and are based on a structured statistical approach (Schmidt and Hunter, 2014).
Independent Variables
Eight independent variables were identified as potential indicators of aged quality and sensory traits. All independent variables were traits that could be measured at a processing facility in real time using noninvasive methods (methodology that would not decrease the total value of the loin). All 8 variables were measured on the ventral surface of the loin and included instrumental lightness (L*), redness (a*), yellowness (b*), visual color, visual marbling, subjective firmness, and early postmortem ultimate pH (>16-h post exsanguination). Extractable lipid, of a pork chop trimmed of subcutaneous fat, was also included as an independent variable because extractable lipid should not change during postmortem aging. However, the visual detection of marbling may be influenced by things such as lighting or temperature of the loin (AMSA, 2012). Using extractable lipid as an independent variable provided an objective measurement for marbling.
Instrumental color values were measured with either a Konica Minolta colorimeter (Minolta Camera Company, Osaka, Japan) or a HunterLab Miniscan spectrophotometer (HunterLab Associates, Reston, VA) and used either D65 (noon daylight, 6500 K) or C (average north sky daylight, 6774 K) illuminant (AMSA, 2012). Because the objective of the analysis was to determine the correlation between early and postmortem loin quality traits and because correlations are unitless, data from both machines and both illuminants were pooled. Oxygenation of myoglobin was not controlled and likely differed among studies. However, all data were collected as dictated by the constraints of fresh pork data collection at a processing facility.
Visual color and marbling used the National Pork Producers Council standards for evaluation of fresh pork loins (NPPC, 1999). Likewise, subjective firmness scores were determined using the National Pork Producers Council standards for firmness (NPPC, 1991).
Extractable lipid data were collected on chops trimmed of all subcutaneous fat and accessory muscles. Lipid was then extracted using petroleum ether or a mixture of chloroform and methanol.
Statistical Analyses
There were 3 underlying objectives. Those were as follows: 1) evaluate the change in pork quality parameters during postmortem aging using a paired-T analysis; 2) correlate early postmortem loin quality parameters (color, marbling, firmness, and muscle pH), with aged loin quality parameters; and 3) determine the predictive ability of early postmortem loin quality parameters for traits associated with purchasing intent (aged color and marbling) and eating experience (sensory tenderness, juiciness, and flavor with a holo-analysis.
Paired T-test.
Individual loin served as the experimental unit for determination of changes in quality estimates between early and aged traits. Differences in early and aged loin quality variables within a loin were compared using the paired option of the PROC T-Test in SAS. Means was accepted as statistically different at P ≤ 0.05.
Meta-analysis.
Data from original datasets (experiments) were previously analyzed based on the objectives of the respective original experiment. Therefore, each dataset was first independently analyzed to determine the correlations between 8 independent variables (early postmortem traits) and 17 dependent variables (aged postmortem traits). Pearson correlation coefficients for original datasets were calculated using the CORR procedure of SAS. A dataset consisting of the study name, number of observations, and the correlation coefficient was generated for each of the 136 combinations of independent and dependent variables. Then, the sample-weighted mean correlation and sample-weighted variance [Var(r)] were calculated such that studies with larger samples sizes were given more weight than studies with smaller sample sizes. These 2 statistics are sample-weighted because studies with a greater number of observations have less sample error than studies with fewer observations, and thus should be assigned more weight. The sample-weighted mean correlation was calculated using the following equation: where Ni is the sample size for each study and ri is the observed correlation coefficient. Sample-weighted variance was calculated using the following equation: Because the greatest source of variability across original studies is sampling error (Arthur et al., 2001), the next step in the process was calculating sampling error variance. The effect of sampling error is that study coefficients will vary randomly from the population value. The meta-analysis procedures remove the variability across study coefficients that can be attributed to sampling error. This, in turn, results in a more accurate estimate of the true variance across the observed study coefficients. Sampling error variance was calculated using the following equation: , where is the sample-weighted mean correlation and is the average sample size of the original studies. From there, 95% confidence intervals were calculated based on the sample-weighted mean correlation (rho). Lower confidence limits were calculated as follows: , where N equals the total number of observations across all studies and K equals the number of studies included in the meta-analysis. Upper confidence limits were calculated as follows: . Sample-weighted mean correlations were considered different from zero when the 95% confidence interval did not include zero. Correlations were considered low/weak when || ≤ 0.35, moderately correlated when || = 0.36 to 0.67, and strongly correlated when || = 0.68 to 1.0, similar to Taylor (1990).
Holo-analysis.
Unlike a meta-analysis that requires qualification for data to be included in the analysis, a holo-analysis is defined as the integration of all available data on a specific topic (Rosen, 2006). Therefore, entire raw datasets rather than means from analyzed datasets were used to conduct a holo-analysis of the ability of noninvasive early postmortem loin quality traits (e.g., visual color, visual marbling, subjective firmness, and pH) collected on the ventral surface of the loin to predict traits associated with purchasing intent and eating experience. Because variables such as instrument selection, illuminant selection, degree of observer selection, and machine aperture size are all known to influence instrumental color, CIE lightness (L*), redness (a*), and yellowness (b*) were not included in the list of predictor (independent variables). Likewise, because slice shear force and Warner–Bratzler shear force estimate tenderness in different magnitudes, instrumental tenderness was not included in the list of dependent variables. Data were analyzed using the REG procedure in SAS. Dependent variables included sensory tenderness, juiciness, flavor, and visual color and marbling of the cut surface of aged pork chops. Independent variables included visual color, visual marbling, and subjective firmness on the ventral surface of a 1-day postmortem boneless pork loin. Also included were pH and extractable lipid. Extractable lipid was included in the list of independent variables because intramuscular lipid should not change during aging. However, the ability to assess visual marbling is largely a function of the skill level of the technician. Extractable lipid provided a quantifiable measure of marbling. A linear regression equation was developed using the independent candidate variables to predict all dependent variables. The initial regression model included each of the 5 independent variables. Multicollinearity among independent variables was assessed using a variance inflation factor (VIF) statistic. However, no parameters exceeded VIF values of 2; therefore, all independent variables remained as candidate variables for selection in the model. Influence of individual observations on the estimated dependent variable was determined using the difference of fit (DFITTS) statistic. Observations were considered to have excessive influence on the estimation of the regression parameters when DFITTS 2, where p was the number of parameters considered and n is the total number of observations. Using the stepwise selection method, independent variables were required to have a significant F statistic at the SLENTRY and SLSTAY level = 0.15 to be included and remain in the final model.
RESULTS
Changes in Pork Loin Quality During Aging
Using T-tests to determine the differences between early and aged postmortem traits of the same loin allows for an estimate of the magnitudes of the changes expected in these traits with aging. Because of the very large sample size (>3,950) used in this analysis, this T-test indicates statistical differences (P < 0.05) even with very small differences between early and aged quality estimates. Therefore, the magnitude of these changes is likely more useful for interpretation than the statement of statistical significance.
Ventral loin surfaces become 8% lighter (P < 0.0001), 44.5% redder (P < 0.0001), and 46% more yellow (P < 0.0001) during the aging period (Table 1). Instrumental lightness changed from 47.55 at 1-day postmortem to 51.53 after the aging period. Instrumental redness changed from 6.51 at 1-day postmortem to 10.24 after the aging period. Instrumental yellowness changed from 4.78 at 1-day postmortem to 7.64 after the aging period. Visual color also changed during the aging period, but the magnitude of the change was only 0.21 units (P < 0.0001). Therefore, it is apparent that color of the ventral surface changes during the postmortem aging period. although the differences represent a substantial change from an instrumental perspective, the visual change in color will likely not influence consumer purchasing decisions as consumers likely will not perceive this small difference. Likewise, early and aged estimates of visual marbling and subjective firmness scores were statistically different (P < 0.0001), but each changed less than a half of one respective NPPC unit.
Table 1.
Item | Loins, n | Days postmortem | Difference | SED2 | P-value | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Early | Aged | |||||
Lightness3, L* | 3956 | 47.55 | 51.53 | −3.99 | 0.05 | <0.0001 |
Redness4, a* | 3957 | 6.51 | 10.24 | −3.72 | 0.05 | <0.0001 |
Yellowness5, b* | 3957 | 4.78 | 7.64 | −2.86 | 2.41 | <0.0001 |
Visual color6 | 4259 | 2.9 | 3.1 | −0.2 | 0.01 | <0.0001 |
Visual marbling7 | 4253 | 2.4 | 2.0 | 0.4 | 0.01 | <0.0001 |
Subjective firmness8 | 4257 | 2.9 | 2.6 | 0.3 | 0.01 | <0.0001 |
1Early and aged postmortem quality data are compared using a paired-T test on the same loin.
2Standard error of the difference of the mean.
3L* measures darkness to lightness (greater L* indicates a lighter color).
4a* measures redness (greater a* indicates a redder color).
5b* measures yellowness (greater b* indicates a more yellow color).
6NPPC color based on the 1999 standards measured in half point increments where 1 = palest, 6 = darkest.
7NPPC marbling based on the 1999 standards measured in half point increments where 1 = least amount of marbling, 6 = greatest amount of marbling.
8NPPC firmness based on the 1991 scale measured in half point increments where 1 = softest, 5 = firmest.
Correlations Between Early and Aged Loin Quality
Color.
Early postmortem ventral instrumental lightness (L*) was moderately correlated with aged ventral lightness = 0.50), aged ventral subjective color = −0.38), aged instrumental lightness of the chop face = 0.44), and aged subjective color = −0.38) of the chop face (Table 2). Early postmortem ventral instrumental lightness was not strongly correlated (|| ≥ 0.68) with any instrumental color or subjective color, marbling or firmness trait on the aged ventral surface of the loin or the aged but freshly cut chop face.
Table 2.
Item | Total observations | Number of studies | Rho1 | 95% Confidence interval | Sampling variance | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Lower limit | Upper limit | |||||
Aged ventral surface | ||||||
Lightness, L* | 3,957 | 8 | 0.50 | 0.48 | 0.53 | 0.0011 |
Redness, a* | 3,957 | 8 | −0.09 | −0.12 | −0.06 | 0.0020 |
Yellowness, b* | 3,957 | 8 | 0.32 | 0.29 | 0.35 | 0.0016 |
Visual color | 4,108 | 9 | −0.38 | −0.40 | −0.35 | 0.0016 |
Visual marbling | 4,108 | 9 | 0.04 | 0.01 | 0.08 | 0.0022 |
Subjective firmness | 4,108 | 9 | −0.08 | −0.11 | −0.05 | 0.0022 |
Aged chop face | ||||||
Lightness, L* | 4,213 | 11 | 0.44 | 0.41 | 0.46 | 0.0017 |
Redness, a* | 4,213 | 11 | −0.05 | −0.08 | −0.02 | 0.0026 |
Yellowness, b* | 4,213 | 11 | 0.31 | 0.28 | 0.33 | 0.0021 |
Visual color | 4,108 | 9 | −0.38 | −0.40 | −0.35 | 0.0016 |
Visual marbling | 3,889 | 9 | 0.07 | 0.04 | 0.10 | 0.0023 |
Subjective firmness | 3,180 | 7 | −0.02 | −0.06 | 0.01 | 0.0022 |
Sensory estimates | ||||||
Instrumental tenderness2 | 5,075 | 11 | −0.16 | −0.19 | −0.13 | 0.0021 |
Cook loss | 5,073 | 11 | 0.20 | 0.17 | 0.22 | 0.0020 |
Sensory tenderness | 1,556 | 7 | −0.06 | −0.11 | −0.01 | 0.0045 |
Sensory juiciness | 1,556 | 7 | −0.07 | −0.12 | −0.02 | 0.0045 |
Sensory flavor | 1,405 | 6 | 0.01 | −0.04 | 0.06 | 0.0043 |
1Sample-weighted mean correlation.
2Includes WarnerBratzler and slice shear force evaluated chops cooked to 63 °C through 71 °C.
Early postmortem ventral instrumental redness (a*) was moderately correlated with aged ventral redness = 0.49) and aged instrumental redness of the chop face = 0.46, Table 3). Early postmortem ventral instrumental redness was not strongly correlated (|| ≥ 0.68) with any instrumental color or subjective color, marbling or firmness trait on the aged ventral surface of the loin or the aged but freshly cut chop face.
Table 3.
Item | Total observations | Number of studies | Rho1 | 95% Confidence interval | Sampling variance | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Lower limit | Upper limit | |||||
Aged ventral surface | ||||||
Lightness, L* | 3,957 | 8 | −0.06 | −0.09 | −0.03 | 0.0020 |
Redness, a* | 3,957 | 8 | 0.49 | 0.47 | 0.52 | 0.0012 |
Yellowness, b* | 3,957 | 8 | 0.28 | 0.26 | 0.31 | 0.0017 |
Visual color | 4,108 | 9 | 0.11 | 0.08 | 0.14 | 0.0021 |
Visual marbling | 4,108 | 9 | 0.04 | 0.01 | 0.07 | 0.0022 |
Subjective firmness | 4,108 | 9 | 0.06 | 0.03 | 0.09 | 0.0022 |
Aged chop face | ||||||
Lightness, L* | 4,213 | 11 | −0.05 | −0.08 | −0.02 | 0.0026 |
Redness, a* | 4,213 | 11 | 0.46 | 0.43 | 0.48 | 0.0016 |
Yellowness, b* | 4,213 | 11 | 0.21 | 0.18 | 0.24 | 0.0024 |
Visual color | 3,889 | 9 | 0.09 | 0.05 | 0.12 | 0.0023 |
Visual marbling | 3,889 | 9 | 0.11 | 0.07 | 0.14 | 0.0023 |
Subjective firmness | 3,180 | 7 | 0.03 | −0.01 | 0.06 | 0.0022 |
Sensory estimates | ||||||
Instrumental tenderness2 | 5,075 | 11 | −0.07 | −0.10 | −0.04 | 0.0021 |
Cook loss | 5,073 | 11 | 0.05 | 0.02 | 0.07 | 0.0022 |
Sensory tenderness | 1,556 | 7 | −0.07 | −0.12 | −0.02 | 0.0045 |
Sensory juiciness | 1,556 | 7 | −0.05 | −0.10 | 0.00 | 0.0045 |
Sensory flavor | 1,405 | 6 | −0.06 | −0.11 | −0.01 | 0.0043 |
1Sample-weighted mean correlation.
2Includes Warner–Bratzler and slice shear force evaluated chops cooked to 63 °C through 71 °C.
Early postmortem ventral instrumental yellowness (b*) was moderately correlated = 0.37) with aged ventral lightness (Table 4). Early postmortem ventral instrumental yellowness was not strongly correlated (|| ≥ 0.68) with any instrumental color or subjective color, marbling or firmness trait on the aged ventral surface of the loin or the aged but freshly cut chop face.
Table 4.
Item | Total observations | Number of studies | Rho1 | 95% Confidence interval | Sampling variance | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Lower limit | Upper limit | |||||
Aged ventral surface | ||||||
Lightness, L* | 3,957 | 8 | 0.37 | 0.34 | 0.40 | 0.0015 |
Redness, a* | 3,957 | 8 | 0.10 | 0.07 | 0.13 | 0.0020 |
Yellowness, b* | 3,957 | 8 | 0.33 | 0.30 | 0.36 | 0.0016 |
Visual color | 4,108 | 9 | −0.26 | −0.29 | −0.23 | 0.0019 |
Visual marbling | 4,108 | 9 | 0.04 | 0.01 | 0.07 | 0.0022 |
Subjective firmness | 4,108 | 9 | −0.03 | −0.06 | 0.00 | 0.0022 |
Aged chop face | ||||||
Lightness, L* | 4,213 | 11 | 0.28 | 0.25 | 0.30 | 0.0022 |
Redness, a* | 4,213 | 11 | 0.07 | 0.04 | 0.10 | 0.0026 |
Yellowness, b* | 4,213 | 11 | 0.27 | 0.24 | 0.30 | 0.0022 |
Visual color | 3,889 | 9 | −0.23 | −0.26 | −0.20 | 0.0021 |
Visual marbling | 3,889 | 9 | 0.09 | 0.06 | 0.12 | 0.0023 |
Subjective firmness | 3,180 | 7 | 0.00 | −0.04 | 0.03 | 0.0022 |
Sensory estimates | ||||||
Instrumental tenderness2 | 5,075 | 11 | −0.17 | −0.20 | −0.15 | 0.0020 |
Cook loss | 5,073 | 11 | 0.17 | 0.14 | 0.19 | 0.0021 |
Sensory tenderness | 1,556 | 7 | −0.02 | −0.07 | 0.03 | 0.0045 |
Sensory juiciness | 1,556 | 7 | −0.02 | −0.07 | 0.03 | 0.0045 |
Sensory flavor | 1,405 | 6 | 0.00 | −0.05 | 0.05 | 0.0043 |
1Sample-weighted mean correlation.
2Includes Warner–Bratzler and slice shear force evaluated chops cooked to 63 °C through 71 °C.
Early postmortem ventral visual color was moderately correlated with aged ventral lightness = −0.51), aged ventral visual color = 0.50), aged instrumental lightness of the chop face = −0.43), and aged subjective color = 0.43) of the chop face (Table 5). Early postmortem ventral instrumental lightness was not strongly correlated (|| ≥ 0.68) with any instrumental color or visual color, marbling or firmness trait on the aged ventral surface of the loin or the aged but freshly cut chop face.
Table 5.
Item | Total observations | Number of studies | Rho1 | 95% Confidence interval | Sampling variance | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Lower limit | Upper limit | |||||
Aged ventral surface | ||||||
Lightness, L* | 4,110 | 8 | −0.51 | −0.53 | −0.48 | 0.0011 |
Redness, a* | 4,110 | 8 | 0.22 | 0.19 | 0.25 | 0.0018 |
Yellowness, b* | 4,110 | 8 | −0.21 | −0.24 | −0.18 | 0.0018 |
Visual color | 4,260 | 9 | 0.50 | 0.48 | 0.52 | 0.0012 |
Visual marbling | 4,260 | 9 | 0.15 | 0.13 | 0.18 | 0.0020 |
Subjective firmness | 4,260 | 9 | 0.17 | 0.14 | 0.19 | 0.0020 |
Aged chop face | ||||||
Lightness, L* | 4,357 | 11 | −0.43 | −0.46 | −0.41 | 0.0017 |
Redness, a* | 4,357 | 11 | 0.21 | 0.18 | 0.24 | 0.0023 |
Yellowness, b* | 4,357 | 11 | −0.16 | −0.18 | −0.13 | 0.0024 |
Visual color | 4,042 | 9 | 0.43 | 0.41 | 0.46 | 0.0015 |
Visual marbling | 4,040 | 9 | 0.12 | 0.09 | 0.15 | 0.0022 |
Subjective firmness | 3,187 | 7 | 0.04 | 0.00 | 0.07 | 0.0022 |
Sensory estimates | ||||||
Instrumental tenderness2 | 5,223 | 11 | −0.01 | −0.03 | 0.02 | 0.0021 |
Cook loss | 5,223 | 11 | −0.21 | −0.23 | −0.18 | 0.0019 |
Sensory tenderness | 1,552 | 7 | 0.05 | 0.00 | 0.10 | 0.0045 |
Sensory juiciness | 1,552 | 7 | 0.08 | 0.03 | 0.13 | 0.0045 |
Sensory flavor | 1,401 | 6 | 0.09 | 0.04 | 0.14 | 0.0042 |
1Sample-weighted mean correlation.
2Includes Warner–Bratzler and slice shear force evaluated chops cooked to 63 °C through 71 °C.
No instrumental or subjective color parameters were moderately (|| = 0.36 to 0.67) or strongly correlated (|| ≥ 0.68) with traits associated with eating experience (instrumental tenderness, cook loss, sensory tenderness, juiciness, or flavor).
Marbling/extractable lipid.
Extractable lipid was moderately correlated with aged ventral marbling = 0.62) and strongly correlated with aged visual marbling of the chop face ( = 0.69, Table 6). Extractable lipid was not moderately or strongly correlated (|| ≤ 0.31) with any instrumental color or visual color or subjective firmness trait on the aged ventral surface of the loin or the aged but freshly cut chop face.
Table 6.
Item | Total observations | Number of studies | Rho1 | 95% Confidence interval | Sampling variance | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Lower limit | Upper limit | |||||
Aged ventral surface | ||||||
Lightness, L* | 4,097 | 8 | 0.17 | 0.14 | 0.20 | 0.0018 |
Redness, a* | 4,097 | 8 | 0.10 | 0.07 | 0.13 | 0.0019 |
Yellowness, b* | 4,097 | 8 | 0.21 | 0.18 | 0.23 | 0.0018 |
Visual color | 4,250 | 9 | 0.01 | −0.02 | 0.04 | 0.0021 |
Visual marbling | 4,250 | 9 | 0.62 | 0.60 | 0.63 | 0.0008 |
Subjective firmness | 4,250 | 9 | 0.25 | 0.22 | 0.28 | 0.0019 |
Aged chop face | ||||||
Lightness, L* | 3,543 | 10 | 0.15 | 0.12 | 0.18 | 0.0027 |
Redness, a* | 3,543 | 10 | 0.23 | 0.19 | 0.26 | 0.0025 |
Yellowness, b* | 3,543 | 10 | 0.31 | 0.28 | 0.34 | 0.0023 |
Visual color | 3,225 | 8 | −0.02 | −0.06 | 0.01 | 0.0025 |
Visual marbling | 3,223 | 8 | 0.69 | 0.67 | 0.71 | 0.0007 |
Subjective firmness | 2,377 | 6 | 0.14 | 0.10 | 0.18 | 0.0024 |
Sensory estimates | ||||||
Instrumental tenderness2 | 4,407 | 10 | −0.13 | −0.16 | −0.10 | 0.0022 |
Cook loss | 4,406 | 10 | −0.07 | −0.10 | −0.04 | 0.0023 |
Sensory tenderness | 1,555 | 7 | 0.10 | 0.05 | 0.15 | 0.0044 |
Sensory juiciness | 1,555 | 7 | 0.03 | −0.02 | 0.08 | 0.0045 |
Sensory flavor | 1,401 | 6 | 0.07 | 0.02 | 0.13 | 0.0043 |
1Sample-weighted mean correlation.
2Includes Warner–Bratzler and slice shear force evaluated chops cooked to 63 °C through 71 °C.
Early postmortem ventral visual marbling was moderately correlated with aged ventral marbling = 0.63) and aged visual marbling of the chop face = 0.56, Table 7). Early postmortem ventral visual marbling was not strongly correlated (|| ≥ 0.68) with any instrumental color or visual color, marbling or firmness trait on the aged ventral surface of the loin or the aged chop face.
Table 7.
Item | Total observations | Number of studies | Rho1 | 95% Confidence interval | Sampling variance | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Lower limit | Upper limit | |||||
Aged ventral surface | ||||||
Lightness, L* | 4,103 | 8 | −0.09 | −0.12 | −0.06 | 0.0019 |
Redness, a* | 4,103 | 8 | 0.03 | 0.00 | 0.06 | 0.0020 |
Yellowness, b* | 4,103 | 8 | −0.04 | −0.07 | −0.01 | 0.0019 |
Visual color | 4,253 | 9 | 0.21 | 0.18 | 0.24 | 0.0019 |
Visual marbling | 4,253 | 9 | 0.63 | 0.61 | 0.64 | 0.0008 |
Subjective firmness | 4,253 | 9 | 0.26 | 0.23 | 0.29 | 0.0018 |
Aged chop face | ||||||
Lightness, L* | 4,352 | 11 | −0.08 | −0.11 | −0.05 | 0.0025 |
Redness, a* | 4,352 | 11 | 0.12 | 0.09 | 0.15 | 0.0025 |
Yellowness, b* | 4,352 | 11 | 0.06 | 0.03 | 0.09 | 0.0025 |
Visual color | 4,037 | 9 | 0.23 | 0.20 | 0.26 | 0.0020 |
Visual marbling | 4,035 | 9 | 0.56 | 0.54 | 0.58 | 0.0011 |
Subjective firmness | 3,186 | 7 | 0.12 | 0.09 | 0.16 | 0.0021 |
Sensory estimates | ||||||
Instrumental tenderness2 | 5,216 | 11 | −0.09 | −0.11 | −0.06 | 0.0021 |
Cook loss | 5,216 | 11 | −0.14 | −0.17 | −0.11 | 0.0020 |
Sensory tenderness | 1,549 | 7 | 0.12 | 0.07 | 0.16 | 0.0044 |
Sensory juiciness | 1,549 | 7 | 0.04 | −0.01 | 0.09 | 0.0045 |
Sensory flavor | 1,398 | 6 | 0.00 | −0.06 | 0.05 | 0.0043 |
1Sample-weighted mean correlation.
2Includes Warner–Bratzler and slice shear force evaluated chops cooked to 63 °C through 71 °C.
Firmness.
Early postmortem ventral firmness was not strongly (|| ≥ 0.68) or moderately (|| = 0.36 to 0.67) correlated with any instrumental color or visual color, visual marbling or subjective firmness trait on the aged ventral surface of the loin or the aged chop face (Table 8). Nor was early postmortem ventral firmness strongly (|| ≥ 0.68) or moderately (|| = 0.36 to 0.67) correlated with any traits associated with eating experience (Table 7).
Table 8.
Item | Total observations | Number of studies | Rho1 | 95% Confidence interval | Sampling variance | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Lower limit | Upper limit | |||||
Aged ventral surface | ||||||
Lightness, L* | 4,107 | 8 | −0.16 | −0.19 | −0.13 | 0.0018 |
Redness, a* | 4,107 | 8 | 0.01 | −0.02 | 0.04 | 0.0020 |
Yellowness, b* | 4,107 | 8 | −0.06 | −0.09 | −0.03 | 0.0019 |
Visual color | 4,257 | 9 | 0.21 | 0.18 | 0.24 | 0.0019 |
Visual marbling | 4,257 | 9 | 0.28 | 0.25 | 0.31 | 0.0018 |
Subjective firmness | 4,257 | 9 | 0.26 | 0.23 | 0.28 | 0.0019 |
Aged chop face | ||||||
Lightness, L* | 4,354 | 11 | −0.13 | −0.16 | −0.10 | 0.0024 |
Redness, a* | 4,354 | 11 | 0.04 | 0.01 | 0.07 | 0.0025 |
Yellowness, b* | 4,354 | 11 | −0.03 | 0.00 | −0.06 | 0.0025 |
Visual color | 4,039 | 9 | 0.19 | 0.16 | 0.22 | 0.0021 |
Visual marbling | 4,037 | 9 | 0.24 | 0.21 | 0.27 | 0.0020 |
Subjective firmness | 3,185 | 7 | 0.09 | 0.06 | 0.13 | 0.0022 |
Sensory estimates | ||||||
Instrumental tenderness2 | 5,220 | 11 | −0.06 | −0.09 | −0.03 | 0.0021 |
Cook loss | 5,220 | 11 | −0.15 | −0.18 | −0.12 | 0.0020 |
Sensory tenderness | 1,552 | 7 | 0.12 | 0.07 | 0.17 | 0.0044 |
Sensory juiciness | 1,552 | 7 | 0.08 | 0.03 | 0.13 | 0.0045 |
Sensory flavor | 1,401 | 6 | 0.07 | 0.02 | 0.13 | 0.0043 |
1Sample-weighted mean correlation.
2Includes Warner–Bratzler and slice shear force evaluated chops cooked to 63 °C through 71 °C.
Ultimate pH.
Early postmortem ultimate pH was moderately correlated with aged ventral lightness = −0.43), aged ventral yellowness = −0.43), aged ventral visual color = 0.36), and aged visual color of the chop face = 0.36, Table 9). Ultimate pH was not moderately (|| = 0.36 to 0.67) or strongly correlated (|| ≥ 0.68) to traits associated with eating experience.
Table 9.
Item | Total observations | Number of studies | Rho1 | 95% Confidence interval | Sampling variance | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Lower limit | Upper limit | |||||
Aged ventral surface | ||||||
Lightness, L* | 3,739 | 8 | −0.43 | −0.45 | −0.40 | 0.0014 |
Redness, a* | 3,739 | 8 | −0.19 | −0.23 | −0.16 | 0.0020 |
Yellowness, b* | 3,739 | 8 | −0.43 | −0.46 | −0.40 | 0.0014 |
Visual color | 3,889 | 9 | 0.36 | 0.34 | 0.39 | 0.0017 |
Visual marbling | 3,889 | 9 | 0.20 | 0.16 | 0.23 | 0.0021 |
Subjective firmness | 3,889 | 9 | 0.10 | 0.07 | 0.13 | 0.0023 |
Aged chop face | ||||||
Lightness, L* | 4,155 | 11 | −0.35 | −0.37 | −0.32 | 0.0021 |
Redness, a* | 4,155 | 11 | −0.15 | −0.18 | −0.12 | 0.0025 |
Yellowness, b* | 4,155 | 11 | −0.28 | −0.31 | −0.25 | 0.0023 |
Visual color | 3,840 | 9 | 0.36 | 0.33 | 0.39 | 0.0018 |
Visual marbling | 3,838 | 9 | 0.15 | 0.12 | 0.19 | 0.0022 |
Subjective firmness | 3,095 | 7 | −0.01 | −0.05 | 0.03 | 0.0023 |
Sensory estimates | ||||||
Instrumental tenderness2 | 4,855 | 11 | −0.05 | −0.08 | −0.02 | 0.0023 |
Cook loss | 4,855 | 11 | −0.29 | −0.32 | −0.26 | 0.0019 |
Sensory tenderness | 1,398 | 7 | 0.21 | 0.16 | 0.26 | 0.0046 |
Sensory juiciness | 1,398 | 7 | 0.12 | 0.07 | 0.17 | 0.0049 |
Sensory flavor | 1,247 | 6 | 0.05 | −0.01 | 0.10 | 0.0048 |
1Sample-weighted mean correlation.
2Includes Warner–Bratzler and slice shear force evaluated chops cooked to 63 °C through 71 °C.
Predictive Ability of Early Postmortem Loin Quality Parameters on Purchasing Intent and Eating Experience
The combination of traits usually associated with pork quality that can easily be evaluated on the fabrication line of commercial processing facilities (e.g., visual color, visual marbling, subjective firmness, and muscle pH) was not predictive (R2 ≤ 0.13) of sensory tenderness or juiciness. The combination of color, marbling, firmness, and pH was moderately predictive (R2 = 0.28) of sensory flavor (Table 10). On the other hand, pork quality traits that can easily be evaluated on the fabrication line of commercial processing facilities were moderately predictive of traits associated with purchasing intent (e.g., aged chop color and aged chop marbling). The combination of the color, marbling, firmness, pH, and extractable lipid was moderately predictive of aged chop face visual color (R2 = 0.26) and aged chop face visual marbling (R2 = 0.28) when chemical extractable lipid was not included as a candidate predictor variable. Including chemical extractable lipid along with early color, marbling, firmness, and pH was moderately predictive (R2 = 0.52) of aged visual chop face marbling (Table 10). The discrepancy in predictive ability of aged chop marbling by including or excluding extractable lipid is likely due to the skill level of the technician evaluating visual marbling.
Table 10.
Dependent variable (trait) | Regression equation1 | Model R2 | Independent variables—Partial R2 | Model P-value | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Extractable lipid | pH | Subjective firmness | Visual color | Visual marbling | ||||
Eating experience | ||||||||
Tenderness | y = −5.61 + 0.37a + 2.13b + 0.29c + 0.15d | 0.13 | 0.07 | 0.05 | 0.02 | 0.00 | – | <0.0001 |
Juiciness | y = −0.61 + 0.22a + 1.20b + 0.16c + 0.16e + 0.15d | 0.09 | 0.05 | 0.02 | 0.01 | 0.00 | 0.01 | <0.0001 |
Flavor | y = 22.00 − 2.70b + 1.04e − 0.83a − 0.99d + 0.20c | 0.28 | 0.09 | 0.07 | 0.00 | 0.05 | 0.06 | <0.0001 |
Purchasing intent | ||||||||
Aged chop color | y = −0.67 + 0.34d + 0.46b + 0.07c + 0.06e − 0.04a | 0.26 | −0.04 | 0.46 | 0.07 | 0.34 | 0.06 | <0.0001 |
Aged chop marbling | y = −1.46 + 0.40a + 0.16e + 0.10d + 0.29b + 0.05c | 0.52 | 0.46 | 0.00 | 0.00 | 0.01 | 0.04 | <0.0001 |
Aged chop marbling2 | y = −0.46 + 0.34e + 0.13c + 0.24b − 0.04d | 0.28 | – | 0.24 | 0.13 | −0.04 | 0.34 | <0.0001 |
1a = extractable lipid; b = ultimate pH; c = ventral firmness; d = ventral color; e = ventral marbling.
2Prediction of chop marbling without extractable lipid included as a candidate variable.
DISCUSSION
A meta-analysis of correlations is also referred as the validity generalization procedure (Schmidt and Hunter, 2014). It was originally developed as a means to test the notion that cognitive ability predicted job performance equally well for all persons in all situations. However, a parallel point of view existed that instead stated that the validity of employment tests was not generalizable but instead were specific to each scenario. This position was referred to as the “situational specificity hypothesis.” Based on that, employment tests would need to be validated each time they were used because job tasks and environmental interactions were unique to each job. The situational specificity hypothesis was based on empirical validation studies that reported the magnitude of validity coefficients to vary among job situations. This argued that validity generalization was impossible and empirical validation was required for each specific test. In other words, a meta-analysis of correlations as a means to generalize the relationship between early postmortem quality traits and aged quality traits was not possible. Validity generalization techniques, such as the one used in this meta-analysis, were developed to test the situational specificity hypothesis. The premise behind a validity generalization technique uses results of previous studies as the sample to summarize (generalize) the true relationship represented from the original studies. Validity generalization states that the evidence for situational specificity is artefactual in nature (Schmidt and Hunter, 2014). In other words, observed differences in relationships from one study to another are the results of sampling errors and not true differences. Therefore, if the residual variance is nearly 0 after the variance due to these errors are removed, then the situational specificity hypothesis is rejected and the validity generalization is accepted. The sampling variance for each correlation in the meta-analysis is <0.01. Therefore, based on the recommendations of Schmidt and Hunter (2014), the results of the meta-analysis are justified and the variation due to differences in methodologies (e.g., instrumental tenderness differences) was accounted for in the analysis.
Postmortem aging of pork loins has benefits for the consumer because most pork loins become more tender during a postmortem aging period (Dilger et al., 2010). This tenderization is partially due proteolysis of myofibrils and associated proteins (Koohmaraie et al., 2002). However, meat color is expected to deteriorate with postmortem aging. Meat color stability is largely a function of oxygen consumption and metmyoglobin reduction during storage (Mancini and Hunt, 2005). During postmortem aging, myoglobin oxidizes and fresh meat becomes discolored. Quantifying the differences in color from early postmortem aging times to later aging times is important as these times represent the 2 different instances where meat purveyors (early) and consumers (late) make quality observations. Due to the expected changes that occur with postmortem aging, it is important to determine whether meat purveyors and consumers are observing loins to be similar in color. Based on previous reports (Klehm et al., 2018), it was not surprising to note that based on data from the meta-analyses, ventral instrumental color changed during postmortem aging. What is important to note is that although instrumental lightness values increased (loins became lighter), the magnitude of the change was 3.99 L* units. A change of this magnitude represents less than 1 color score difference in visual color based on NPPC visual color. The magnitude of difference between NPPC visual color scores is 6 L* units (NPPC, 1999). In addition to becoming lighter, with postmortem aging, both a* (redness) and b* yellowness increased. This increase in redness may explain why, while instrumentally loins become lighter, in terms of NPPC color score, they were perceived as “darker.” Visual color scores increased during postmortem aging, but the magnitude of the difference only increased 0.21 NPPC visual color units. Although statistically significant, the magnitude of these changes suggests that meat purveyors and consumers would perceive similar loins to be similar despite the time spent in postmortem aging.
Although the change in color during postmortem aging was expected, it was not known whether this change would proceed in a predictable fashion in all loins. If all loins, regardless of initial quality traits, experienced the same pattern and magnitude of change during postmortem aging, the correlation between early and aged ventral loin traits would be expected to be high. In terms of pork quality grading systems, this would be ideal as “high quality” loins based on color observations at the time of quality evaluation in the processing facility would continue to appear, within a population, as “high quality” to a consumer. Within single traits (e.g., early ventral L* to aged ventral L*), correlations were weak (firmness, b*) or moderate (L*, a*, color, marbling). This suggests some similarity in the ways that these traits change with aging, but not perfect, or even strong, correlation between the 2 observation times.
Further complicating this relationship between meat purveyors and consumer evaluations of quality is the difference between the surfaces being evaluated. At early postmortem times, intact loins are observed, but consumers are more likely to observe quality on the cut surface (chop face) of loins. Therefore, the strength of the relationship between observations on the ventral surface at early times postmortem with those on the aged chop surface must be established. Again, within single traits (e.g., early ventral L* to aged chop L*), correlations were weak (firmness, b*) or moderate (L*, a*, color, marbling). This suggests that loins judged “dark” using the ventral side at early postmortem times are likely to appear “dark” to consumers after aging and when observing the chop face. Although not perfect, these correlations appear consistent enough to allow for ventral loin observations to be used at early postmortem aging times.
The emphasis on visual observations of color and marbling is important because consumers use fresh color to make purchasing decisions (Brewer et al., 1999, 2001). However, ultimate quality of a purchase is determined based on eating experience. To have utility in categorizing loins based on quality, early observations of loin quality traits should be predictive of eating experience traits like tenderness, juiciness, and flavor. However, correlations between eating experience traits and single early ventral loin quality traits are weak at best. No single correlation coefficient exceeded an absolute value of 0.17, and many confidence intervals around these correlation coefficients contained zero. This indicates the overall weakness of these correlations. In terms of predictive ability, no combination of traits that are routinely evaluated at the processing facility during early postmortem evaluation was able to account for more than 13% of the variation in either sensory tenderness or juiciness. Using all available traits, only 28% of the variability in pork flavor was accounted for. Thus, the ability of early postmortem ventral loin observations to predict eating experience may not be reliable.
CONCLUSION
Historically, marbling has been considered important for positive eating experiences, but according to the meta-analysis, no measure of color or marbling would be a reliable sorting tool to determine loins that would provide a favorable or less favorable eating experience. Furthermore, the combination of pork quality traits routinely evaluated on the loin fabrication line during early postmortem evaluations is poor predictors of eating experience. Even though color changed during postmortem aging, the magnitude of change is likely not great enough to change the overall perception of the product, therefore, making color and marbling suitable parameters for sorting pork chops based on consumer purchasing preferences.
Supplementary Material
Footnotes
Funding, wholly or in part, was provided by The National Pork Board. Mention of trade names, proprietary products, or specified equipment does not constitute a guarantee or warranty by the United States Department of Agriculture (USDA) and does not imply approval to the exclusion of other products that may be suitable. The USDA is an equal opportunity provider and employer.
Present address: Department of Animal Sciences, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL 32611
LITERATURE CITED
- AMSA 2012. AMSA meat color measurement guidelines http://www.meatscience.org/docs/default-source/publications-resources/Hot-Topics/2012_12_meat_clr_guide.pdf?sfvrsn=0 (Accessed 20 October 2017).
- Arthur W., Bennett W., and Huffcutt A. I.. 2001. Meta-analysis of correlations. In: Arthur W., editor. Conducting meta-analysis using SAS. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc., Publishers, Mahwah, NJ: p. 57–116. [Google Scholar]
- Brewer M.S., and McKeith F. K.. 1999. Consumer-rated quality characteristics are related to purchase intent of fresh pork. J. Food Sci. 64:171–174. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2621.1999.tb09885.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Brewer M. S., Zhu L. G., and McKeith F. K.. 2001. Marbling effects on quality characteristics pork loin chops: consumer purchase intent, visual and sensory characteristics. Meat Sci. 59:153–163. doi: 10.1016/S0309-1740(01)00065-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dilger A. C., Rincker P. J., Eggert J. M., McKeith F. K., and Killefer J.. 2010. Pork tenderness and postmortem tenderization: correlations with meat quality traits and the impact of sire line. J. Muscle Foods. 21:529–544. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-4573.2009.00201.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Huff-Lonergan E., Baas T. J., Malek M., Dekkers J. C., Prusa K., and Rothschild M. F.. 2002. Correlations among selected pork quality traits. j. Anim. Sci. 80:617–627. doi: 10.2527/2002.803617x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Klehm B. J., King D. A., Dilger A. C., Shackelford S. D., and Boler D. D.. 2018. Effect of packaging type during postmortem aging and degree of doneness on pork chop sensory traits of loins selected to vary in color and marbling. J. Anim. Sci. 96:1736–1744. doi:10.1093/jas/sky084 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koohmaraie M., Kent M. P., Shackelford S. D., Veiseth E., and Wheeler T. L.. 2002. Meat tenderness and muscle growth: is there any relationship?Meat Sci. 62:345–352. doi: 10.1016/S0309-1740(02)00127-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mancini R. A. and Hunt M. C.. 2005. Current research in meat color. Meat Sci. 71:100–121. doi: 10.1016/j.meatsci.2005.03.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Norman J. L., Berg E. P., Heymann H., and Lorenzen C. L.. 2003. Pork loin color relative to sensory and instrumental tenderness and consumer acceptance. Meat Sci. 65:927–933. doi: 10.1016/S0309-1740(02)00310-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- NPPC 1991. Procedures to evaluate market hogs. 3rd ed Natl. Pork Prod. Counc, Des Moines, IA. [Google Scholar]
- NPPC 1999. Official color and marbling standards. Natl. Pork Prod. Counc, Des Moines, IA. [Google Scholar]
- Rosen G. D. 2006. Holo-analysis. Poult. Sci. 85:957–959. doi: 10.1093/ps/85.6.957 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sauvant D., Schmidely P., Daudin J. J., and St-Pierre N. R.. 2008. Meta-analyses of experimental data in animal nutrition. Animal 2:1203–1214. doi: 10.1017/S1751731108002280. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schmidt F. L., Hunter J. E.. 2014. Meta-analysis of correlations corrected individually for artifacts. In: Barrett L., editor, Methods of meta-analysis: correcting error and bias in research findings. 3rd ed Sage publishing, Los Angeles, CA: p. 87–164. [Google Scholar]
- Taylor R. 1990. Interpretation of the correlation coefficient. A basic review. J. Diagn. Med. Sonor. 6:35–39. doi: 10.1177/875647939000600106 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- USDA 2017. U.S. grade standards: pork carcasses https://www.regulations.gov/document?D=AMS_FRDOC_0001-1640 (Accessed 16 November 2017).
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.