Abstract
Risky decision making for others is ubiquitous in our societies. Whereas financial decision making for oneself induces strong concern about the worst outcome (maximin concern) as well as the expected value, behavioral and neural characteristics of decision making for others are less well understood. We conducted behavioral and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) experiments to examine the neurocognitive underpinnings of risky decisions for an anonymous other, using decisions for self as a benchmark. We show that, although the maximin concern affected both types of decisions equally strongly, decision making for others recruited a more risk-neutral computational mechanism than decision making for self. Specifically, participants exhibited more balanced information search when choosing a risky option for others. Activity of right temporoparietal junction (rTPJ, associated with cognitive perspective taking) was parametrically modulated by options’ expected values in decisions for others, and by the minimum amounts in decisions for self. Furthermore, individual differences in self-reported empathic concern modified these attentional and neural processes. Overall, these results indicate that the typical maximin concern is attenuated in a risk-neutral direction in decisions for others as compared to self. We conjecture that, given others’ diverse preferences, deciding as a neutral party may cognitively recruit such risk-neutrality.
Introduction
Humans often have to make decisions on behalf of others, particularly in situations involving financial and social services. Such decisions often require political considerations, as they involve a great deal of uncertainty about consequences that affect the lives of many others whom decision makers do not know personally. However, the neurocognitive bases of such decisions for others are not well understood.
It is well established that decision making for oneself deviates systematically from a simple maximization of expected value, due to the element of risk. Besides the expected value, people are concerned with how variable (variability concern1,2) or how bad (maximizing the minimum payoff, i.e., maximin concern3,4) choice outcomes can be, generally exhibiting risk-averse preferences. On the other hand, research suggests that risky decision making for others may be less affective and more risk tolerant than decision making for self 5. Hsee & Weber6 showed that people tend to think that others are less risk averse than themselves, whether the choices are between options with positive or negative outcomes. According to such predictions, people may focus more strongly on the expected values of choice options when they decide for others than for self. It is also conceivable that, given the relative lack of preference information for others compared to oneself, using the expected value as a normative criterion may be politically most justifiable in risky decision making for others.
Considering the importance of such a potential self-other difference, rigorous behavioral comparisons have been rather sparse (with a few notable exceptions7–9), not to mention research investigating its neural correlates. The difference could be reflected in multiple modalities during decision making, not only in choices per se but also in information search processes, personality traits, and brain activity. Using model-based behavioral and fMRI experiments, this study investigates how risky decisions for others compare to those for self in multiple modalities and how the possible self-other difference may be modulated by individual differences in empathy.
Results
Choice model
In both the behavioral and fMRI experiments, participants made 36 risky choices each for self and for an anonymous other, with the task order counterbalanced. In each trial, they were asked to choose one of two lottery options consisting of three equiprobable outcomes (Fig. 1a left; Table S1). In the behavioral experiment (N = 60), the numerical outcome information, initially hidden behind boxes labeled L, M, or H (low, medium, or high), was displayed only while participants held the pointer over a box (the Mouselab technique10: Fig. 1a middle), allowing us to examine how participants attended to relevant information prior to making choices3.
For the analysis of behavioral choices, we adopted the following economic model (“quasi-maximin model”)11. Applied to the three-outcome (π1, π2, π3) cases, the model posits that the utility of option x for participant i is given by a linear combination of its minimum outcome and the expected value:
1 |
where αi ∈ [0, 1] captures the degree of individual concern about the minimum outcome (hereafter, the maximin weight). Setting α = 0 corresponds to completely risk-neutral preferences, in which the utility of an option is measured solely by its expected value, while setting α = 1 corresponds to completely maximin preferences where the option utility is determined solely by its minimum outcome. If αi is between 0 and 1, individual i is concerned with the minimum outcome as well as the expected value. For each participant, we estimated the maximin weight for Self (αself) and Other (αother) conditions separately by fitting the 36 choices to the model using a logit analysis (see Table S2 for more details of model fitting).
Behavioral experiment
The analysis revealed that the average maximin weights for Self and Other conditions were both significantly greater than zero in the behavioral experiment (Fig. 1b: one-sample t test, Self, t59 = 10.9, P < 0.001; Other, t59 = 8.94, P < 0.001). This means that most participants were risk averse, placing considerable weight on both the minimum outcome and the expected value in evaluating choice options, as suggested by previous research3,4. Participants also took more time to decide for Self than for Other (MSelf = 9.49 s, MOther = 8.41 s; paired t test, t59 = 3.37, P < 0.01). However, no self-other difference in risk preference was detected at the decisional level: the maximin weights in choice preferences were not significantly different between the Self and Other conditions (t59 = 0.51, P = 0.61).
To examine information-search processes preceding decisions, we divided the decision time used by each participant in each trial into quartiles. As seen in Fig. 1c, participants tended to start information search by viewing the low (“L”) boxes in both conditions, prioritizing attention to the minimum outcomes in the early phase of decision making. Importantly, the degree of such selective attention to the worst outcomes (“L”) over the best outcomes (“H”) in the first view was significantly less evident in the Other condition than in the Self condition (generalized linear mixed model, Z = 2.85, P = 0.004). Furthermore, as the absolute difference in minimum outcomes between two choice options (ΔMin hereafter) was increased, participants viewed the larger-minimum option for longer than the smaller-minimum option in the Self condition (P = 0.008 by linear mixed model; see details in Methods), but not in the Other condition (P = 0.90). These results suggest that attentional bias toward the minimum may be relatively attenuated in risky decision making for others.
Attention to another’s potential misfortune may also be modulated by individual trait differences in empathic concern. Previous research has shown that likelihood of non-utilitarian decisions, which consider the welfare of the worst-off individual over the aggregated utility for the entire group in moral dilemmas, are positively correlated with empathic concern12,13, defined as “other-oriented feelings of sympathy and concern for unfortunate others”14. We measured participants’ empathic concern using the Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI)14. Figure 1d displays the relationship between this self-reported empathic concern and the difference between the maximin weights in the Other and Self conditions (αOther − αSelf). As seen in the figure, participants with higher empathic-concern scores tended to weight the worst possibility more heavily when making risky decisions for others, r = 0.36, P = 0.005 (Fig. 1d). Similarly, these more empathic participants also viewed the low (“L”) boxes more frequently during information search when deciding for others, r = 0.44, P < 0.001 (Fig. 1e). However, participants’ perspective-taking scores (defined as “the tendency to spontaneously adopt the psychological point of view of others” in the IRI14) revealed no similar patterns (Fig. S1). These results may imply that, even though decision making for others generally reduces cognitive focus on the minimums at the group level (Fig. 1c), individual trait differences in affective empathy may modulate the effect.
fMRI experiment
To understand the neural correlates of such self-other differences, we conducted an fMRI experiment (N = 24). The task structure was identical to that used in the behavioral experiment, except that we measured participants’ information search behavior using an eye tracker instead of the Mouselab technique (Fig. 2a).
We first confirmed the behavioral and attentional results above. At the group level, participants exhibited substantive maximin concern in both Self and Other conditions (Fig. 2b: one-sample t test, Self, t23 = 4.64, P < 0.001; Other, t23 = 4.29, P < 0.001), but the self-other difference was not significant (t23 = 1.23, P = 0.23). Participants also took more time to make decisions for Self than for Other (MSelf = 2.59 s, MOther = 2.51 s; paired t test, t23 = 2.39, P < 0.05). As in the behavioral experiment, participants exhibited biased attention to minimum outcomes, viewing the option with the larger minimum more in the Self condition (Fig. 2c: t16 = 2.41, P < 0.05), but this bias was not observed in the Other condition (t16 = 0.23, P = 0.82). Also, parametric differences in the minimum outcomes between two options (ΔMin) significantly predicted longer gaze on the larger-minimum option in the Self condition (P = 0.036 by linear mixed model) but not in the Other condition (P = 0.55). At the individual level, participants with higher empathic-concern scores also weighted the worst possibility more in decisions for others (Fig. 2d: robust correlation, r = 0.41, P < 0.05; to avoid potential undue influence of outliers in our small sample, we adopted robust correlation analysis15,16 throughout the fMRI experiment using the WRS2 package17 for R software18).
Brain activity
Having replicated the behavioral and attentional results, we then examined neural responses. The brain regions of interest were right temporoparietal junction (rTPJ; involved in perspective taking19,20), medial orbitofrontal cortex (mOFC; associated with affective processes during decision making21,22), and ventral striatum (VS; involved in computing utility of choice options23). Although it was not the main concern of this study, we also summarized whole-brain activation in Table S3.
Our recent fMRI study showed that the maximin concern when choosing between risky options for oneself (comparable to the Self condition here) was selectively tracked by neural activity in the rTPJ3. As rTPJ is involved in cognitively prospecting future conditions for oneself (“mental time travel”)24,25, we interpreted these results as participants taking the perspectives of their “future selves” in the worst-possible scenario (i.e., imagining “what if my choice results in the worst outcome?”)3,26. Here we tested whether participants’ cognitive perspective taking may operate in a more risk-neutral manner when they make decisions for others.
Using a Theory-of-Mind localizer27 (Fig. 3a) in a separate fMRI scan after the main experiment, we identified the rTPJ region correlated with cognitive perspective taking for each participant individually (see ROI analysis in Methods). It is important to note that, in contrast to the main task, the Theory-of-Mind localizer involved no elements of money or risk. The average coordinates of identified rTPJ regions were situated in the angular gyrus, corresponding to the canonical “social TPJ”19 (Fig. 3b). We examined whether activity in the identified rTPJ during decision making was modulated by the key parameters of the behavioral-choice model (Eq. 1) – the absolute difference in minimum (ΔMin) and absolute difference in expected value (ΔEV) between the two options. If a participant’s cognitive perspective taking centers on the worst-possible scenario (or the expected value), it is expected that their rTPJ activity will track how the two options compare in terms of minimum outcomes (or the expected values). Figure 3c displays the modulatory effects of the two parameters (ΔMin and ΔEV) on rTPJ activity in the Self and Other conditions. A two-way repeated measures ANOVA with condition (Self or Other) and parameter (ΔMin or ΔEV) revealed a significant interaction, F2,46 = 4.82, P = 0.038. This implies that cognitive perspective taking operated on different dimensions in the Self and Other conditions; specifically, the former centered on the worst-possible outcome (ΔMin) in line with our recent fMRI study3 whereas the latter focused on the risk-neutral, average outcome (ΔEV).
We also examined whether individual differences in empathic concern may modulate cognitive perspective taking of another’s misfortune. Figure 3d shows the relation between self-reported empathic concern and the modulation of rTPJ activity by ΔMin in the Other condition. Although the effect was weak, participants with higher empathic concern exhibited greater rTPJ activity in response to ΔMin (robust correlation, r = 0.37, P = 0.081) in line with the behavioral results (Figs 1d,e and 2d; see also Fig. 1). This may indicate that, even though decision making for others generally recruits cognitive perspective taking via ΔEV (rather than ΔMin) at the group level, individual trait differences in empathic concern also modulate the effect.
Besides cognitive processes, affective processes may also be evoked when participants make risky decisions on behalf of others. The medial orbitofrontal cortex (mOFC) is associated with emotive non-utilitarian decisions for others in moral dilemmas (i.e., sympathizing with the worst-off individual28) and guilty feelings about another’s disadvantaged payoff in economic games29. We thus examined whether activity in the mOFC (see ROI analysis in Methods) was modulated by the two task parameters of the choice model. As seen in Fig. 4a, activity in mOFC tracked ΔMin in the Other condition (t23 = 2.48, P < 0.05 after Bonferroni family-wise error correction). However, mOFC activity in response to ΔMin was not modulated by individual differences in empathic concern (Fig. 4b: robust correlation, r = 0.05, P = 0.80), suggesting that the emotive reaction to another’s (possible) misfortune was relatively common across participants. These results imply that, besides the cognitive perspective taking along the risk-neutral expected value outcome (Fig. 3c), some emotive process is also evoked by the worst-off possibility, affecting risky decision making for others at the group level.
The economic choice-model (Eq. 1) suggests that such neural activities should be integrated into calculations of option utilities. We first confirmed that the ventral striatum (VS), which is associated with comparison of option utilities23, was involved in this process. Using the average maximin weights of all participants (mean αself and mean αother), we calculated the absolute differences in utility between the two options (ΔU) for each choice according to the model. We then examined whether activity in the VS (see ROI analysis in Methods and Fig. S2) was parametrically modulated by ΔU, separately in the Self and Other conditions. As shown in Fig. 4c, the modulatory effect was significant in both conditions (Self, t23 = 3.62, P < 0.01; Other, t23 = 2.37, P < 0.05: both P’s after Bonferroni correction), confirming that the VS encoded the utility difference between two options.
Functional connectivity
We next examined whether the VS was functionally connected with the rTPJ and mOFC, which tracked the key parameters of the choice model (Figs 3c and 4a). We performed a psychophysiological interaction (PPI) analysis with seeds of rTPJ and mOFC and the target ROI of VS during decision-making. The beta estimate of the PPI was weak in the Self condition but significant in the Other condition (Fig. 4d for rTPJ: Self, t23 = 1.84, P = 0.16; Other, t23 = 3.43, P < 0.01; Fig. 4e for mOFC: Self, t23 = 4.50, P < 0.001; Other, t23 = 3.99, P < 0.001: all P’s after Bonferroni correction), suggesting that both cognitive perspective-taking (rTPJ) and affective/emotive processes (mOFC) were involved in utility calculations in risky decision making for others.
Discussion
This study investigated multimodal processes underlying risky decision making for others. Although deciding matters on behalf of others is common across many financial, social, and political situations, the neurocognitive underpinnings of such decisions are not well understood. Here we have addressed this question by setting risky decisions for self as a benchmark for comparison3. The self-other comparison has revealed a systematic pattern across multiple modalities.
First, in line with previous studies3,4, participants showed a strong maximin concern across the behavioral and fMRI experiments, weighting the worst (minimum) possibility in risky decision making for others as well as for self (Figs 1b and 2b). Although the operation of maximin concern at the decisional level was indistinguishable between the Self and Other conditions, decision making for others recruited a more risk-neutral computational mechanism than decision making for self. Specifically, information search in the behavioral experiment (Fig. 1c) and gaze pattern in the fMRI experiment (Fig. 2c) both showed that attentional bias to the worst outcomes was attenuated in the Other condition compared to the Self condition. The brain-imaging results also showed that rTPJ activity was parametrically modulated by the difference in minimums (ΔMin) in the Self but not in the Other condition. Instead, rTPJ activity in the Other condition was modulated by the difference in expected value (ΔEV; Fig. 3c). It should be emphasized that the Theory-of-Mind localizer that we used to identify ROI of rTPJ for each participant (Fig. 3a) was purely cognitive, with no affective, risk, or monetary components. Taken together, these results imply that participants’ cognitive perspective taking operated in a more risk-neutral manner when deciding on behalf of others, mainly occurring along the dimension of expected value.
Simultaneously, the activity of mOFC, which relates to affective, non-utilitarian decisions for others28,29, was modulated by the worst possible outcome (ΔMin) when deciding for others (Fig. 4a). This suggests that, when deciding on behalf of others, people are emotionally concerned with the worst-off possibility, while cognitively focusing on the risk-neutral average scenario (ΔEV) as a neutral third party. The PPI results confirmed that both the cognitive and affective elements were integrated in utility calculations for risky decision making for others at VS (Fig. 4d,e), which may explain why the maximin concern was indistinguishable at the decisional level between the Self and Other conditions (Figs 1b and 2b).
Finally, this study revealed robust individual differences associated with empathy across multiple modalities. More empathic participants displayed greater maximin concern for others at the decisional level (Figs 1d and 2d), and this pattern was also observed at the attentional (Figs 1e and 2c) and neural (Fig. 3d) levels. These results prompt the question of whether these highly empathic participants may be particularly sensitive to potential harm to others, as suggested by previous research on neural responses to others’ pain30,31. Future neurocognitive research addressing the modulatory role of empathy in calculating values for self and for others is highly important, as empathy constitutes a core dimension of human sociality32.
By using multimodal measurements, we have shown that risky decision making for others engages more risk-neutral cognitive processes as compared to that for self. Whereas the maximin concern (which is likely to be an evolutionary adaptation for survival3,33) also affected final decisions for others through affective routes, its cognitive operation during decision making was attenuated. Given the diverse preferences of others, perhaps using expected value as a normative criterion for decision making would be politically most justifiable. In other words, deliberating about decisions for others as a “neutral third party” may entail cognitive risk neutrality; this would seem to have important implications for many financial and welfare decisions in our societies.
Methods
Overview
This study consists of two experiments. The first is a behavioral experiment using the Mouselab technique10 to investigate information search during decision making. The second is an fMRI experiment to reveal neural correlates of risky decisions for others as compared to those for self. In the fMRI experiment, participants’ eye movements during decision-making were recorded by an eye-tracker to examine whether the information search process was consistent with that identified by the Mouselab technique in the behavioral experiment. At the end of each experiment, each participant’s empathic concern was measured using a seven-point Likert-scale questionnaire (Davis’s Interpersonal Reactivity Index: IRI)14. Empathic concern is one of the subscales of IRI, assessing “other-oriented” feelings of sympathy and concern for unfortunate others14. After the experiment, participants received the base payment plus the result of a lottery, randomly drawn from their 36 choices for self. Both experiments were approved by and carried out in accordance with the guidelines and regulations of the ethical committee of the Center for Experimental Social Sciences of Hokkaido University.
Participants
Sixty Hokkaido University students participated in the behavioral (Mouselab) experiment (34 males and 26 females; age 18–21, mean = 19.2 years, s.d. = 0.80). In the fMRI experiment, we scanned 24 right-handed Hokkaido university students with no history of neurological or psychiatric illness (14 males and 10 females; aged 18–21, mean = 19.2 years, s.d. = 1.06) and no overlap with participants in the behavioral experiment. All participants had normal or corrected-to-normal visual acuity. All participants gave written informed consent, approved by the ethical committee, prior to the experiments.
Tasks
Behavioral (Mouselab) Experiment
Participants performed 36 choice-trials in both Self and Other conditions, 72 trials in total. In each trial, two options were presented horizontally on the upper and lower positions of the display (Fig. 1a left). Each option included three equiprobable outcomes (High, Middle, and Low). Spatial ordering of the three outcome amounts was alternated (High/Middle/Low or Low/Middle/High) every 6 trials. On the screen, numerical outcome information was hidden behind boxes labeled L, M, or H (Fig. 1a middle). When the mouse pointer was held over a box, its numerical information was displayed, and when the pointer was moved away, the information was hidden again, so that participants could only view one box at a time. Participants had to make choices within 30 seconds in each trial, during which they were free to view any boxes in any order. Participants were asked to choose one option by clicking the check mark to the left of the option (Fig. 1a right). In the Other condition, participants were instructed to make choices for an anonymous student who would receive the monetary outcome from the chosen lottery as a compensation for participating in another experiment.
We used a laptop computer with a 15.6-inch display (PC-GN256FSG8, NEC co., Japan) and a set of PsychoPy34 scripts to present stimuli and record responses. As compensation for participating in this experiment, participants received the result of a lottery, randomly selected from the 36 choices in the Self condition, in addition to a show-up fee of 200 JPY in cash. Participants were instructed that if they made no choice within the trial’s time limit, a penalty of 50 JPY would be imposed each time; however, this occurred only once throughout the behavioral experiment.
fMRI experiment
Participants performed the same lottery choice task as in the behavioral experiment (Fig. 2a). The instructions for the Self and Other conditions were identical to those used in the behavioral experiment, except that participants were also asked to choose the option with larger arithmetic average in separate filler trials (Comp condition). There were 36 trials for each of the three conditions, 108 trials in total. The conditions alternated every 12 trials in an order that was counterbalanced across participants.
In each trial, a condition cue was presented for 1 s, followed by a stimulus onset asynchrony of 2, 4, or 6 s (Fig. 2a) with the fixation cross. Then, two options were presented vertically on the left and right side of the fixation cross at the center of the display. Each lottery option had three equiprobable outcomes. Spatial ordering of the three outcome amounts (High/Middle/Low or Low/Middle/High, listed top to bottom) was alternated every 6 trials. Participants were asked to choose one of the two options by pressing a corresponding button within 5 s. The options were presented on the screen for the entire 5 s regardless of when the participant responded. Inter-trial interval was 2, 4, or, 6 s with a fixation cross.
After the lottery-choice task, participants were also scanned while performing a Theory-of-Mind task (Fig. 3a) to identify the right temporoparietal junction (rTPJ) associated with perspective taking27. Participants were asked to infer the beliefs of others (Belief condition) or the states of places and physical objects (e.g., a fountain) as a control (Photo condition). There were 10 trials in each condition. In each trial, the background explanation was presented for 10 s, followed by the statement related to the inference. Participants were asked to answer whether the statement was true or false by pressing a corresponding button within 4 s, during which time the question remained on the screen. Then the fixation cross was presented for 12 s until the next trial began.
Stimuli were presented on an MRI-compatible 32-inch LCD display (NordicNeuroLab, Norway) placed at back of the MR bore. An MRI-compatible response pad (Current Designs, USA) was used to record responses. To examine each participant’s information search process, we also recorded their eye movements during each trial (see the Eye-tracking in fMRI section below). We used a PsychoPy34 script to present stimuli and record responses and eye-tracking data during the fMRI experiment. Participants received a fixed compensation of 4,000 JPY plus the result of a lottery, randomly selected from the 36 choices in the Self condition, all in cash.
During the fMRI experiment, eye movements were recorded using an MRI-compatible eye-tracker operating at 500 Hz (EyeLink CL, SR Research Ltd.). Before scanning, calibration was performed using a sequence of 9-point fixations. To classify saccades and fixations in each trial, we used the following parameters: saccade velocity threshold >30°/s, saccade acceleration threshold >9500°/s2, and fixation duration threshold >100 ms. Fixations and saccades were correctly measured in 17 out of 24 participants; the remaining 7 participants were excluded from this analysis because of technical problems (e.g. reflection from eyeglasses or contact lenses, partial occlusion of pupil by eyelashes).
Computational modeling of choice behavior
As described in the main text, our previous study showed that the “quasi-maximin model”11 (Eq. 1) provided the best fit to the lottery choices for self3. The model posits the utility function as a weighted combination of the minimum amount and the expected value of the option. The choice probability was modeled using a softmax function of the option utilities. We used the Broyden-Fletcher-Goldfarb-Shanno method for numerical estimation of the maximin weight α for each participant in each task, using R software18 for the behavioral experiment and Matlab software (www.mathworks.com) for the fMRI experiment.
Analysis of information search in the behavioral experiment
We analyzed the information search process using the percentages of viewed lottery outcome amounts in each quartile of the decision time used by each participant in each trial. Based on our previous research3, we hypothesized that participants would show greater attention to the minimum amounts during the early phase of decision making. Therefore, the percentage of viewing low amounts (“L” in Fig. 1a) first was compared with that of the high amounts (“H”) using a generalized linear mixed model with the logit link (first views on the middle amounts [“M”] were excluded here, because the middle amounts were generally viewed much less often in both conditions; see Fig. 1c). We also analyzed differences between viewing durations of the larger- and smaller-minimum options using a linear mixed model, with the absolute differences in the minimum amounts (ΔMin) and in the average amounts (ΔEV) between the two options in each pair as independent variables. In both analyses, participants were treated as random effects, and the display order of the amounts (L/M/H or H/M/L from left to right: Fig. 1a) was also modeled as a dummy variable in the model. For these analyses, we used the lme4 package35 for R software18.
Analysis of eye movements in the fMRI experiment
Analyzing gaze on individual monetary amounts was not possible, because the size of the monitor in the MRI did not allow us to display the amounts with adequate spatial separation for such an analysis. Instead, we analyzed participants’ gaze duration on the two presented lottery options, assuming that biased attention to the minimum outcomes would result in longer view duration on the option with larger minimum amount. Gaze durations from the option display onset to the response were examined in the Self and Other conditions (Fig. 2c). We further investigated which task parameter of the choice set affected the longer gaze on the option with larger minimum. To do this, we used a linear mixed model of differences in gaze duration between the larger- and smaller-minimum options, with the parametric differences in the minimum amounts (ΔMin) and in the average amounts (ΔEV) of the two options as the independent variables. The participants were treated as random effects, and the display order of the amounts (L/M/H or H/M/L from the top to the bottom: Fig. 2a) was also modeled as a dummy variable. For this analysis, we used the Matlab Statistics and Machine Learning Toolbox (www.mathworks.com/products/statistics).
Image acquisition of fMRI
A 3T MR scanner (Prisma; Siemens Medical Systems, Erlangen, Germany) equipped for echo-planar imaging (EPI) was used to acquire functional magnetic resonance images. A 64-channel head-neck coil was used for radio frequency signal reception. Mild cushioning minimized participant’s head movement. Thirty-eight slices of functional images were acquired using blood oxygenation level dependent imaging (192 mm × 192 mm × 142.5 mm, in-plane resolution = 64 × 64, pixel size = 3 mm × 3 mm, thickness = 3 mm, 25% distance factor, TR = 2.0 s, TE = 25 ms). The slices were rotated 30° from the AC-PC line to the forehead to minimize artifacts due to the air in the sinus. The images covered the entire cerebrum after the rotation. We acquired 255 scans in each fMRI run of the lottery choice task and 260 scans in the functional localizer for rTPJ (see below). The first three scans in each fMRI run were discarded to ensure magnetization equilibrium.
Image analyses
Pre-processing
We used SPM8 (Wellcome Department of Cognitive Neurology, University College London) working on Matlab to process the scanned images. We performed slice-timing correction using the middle slice as a reference, scan-to-scan realignment, normalization to the EPI template of SPM, resampling the images with a voxel size of 3 mm × 3 mm × 3 mm, and spatial smoothing (FWHM of Gaussian kernel = 8 mm isotropic). A high-pass filter of 128 s was used to remove low frequency noise in the main and control experiments.
General linear model analysis
The first-level analysis using a general linear model (GLM) included the three choice conditions (i.e. Self, Other, and Comp), the condition of task cue, and the condition of response. The onsets of choice condition were the presentation of options. The duration of these conditions was measured as the response time in each trial. The onset of the task cue condition was the appearance of the cue, and its duration was 1 s. All three task cues were collapsed into one condition. Responses were also collapsed into a single condition with duration zero. The blood oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) signal was modeled using the canonical hemodynamic response function included in the SPM software. Realignment parameters reflecting head motion estimated in preprocessing were used as nuisance regressors. In addition to these effects on the brain activation, we examined the modulatory effects of ΔU (GLM1) and of ΔMin and ΔEV (GLM2) in choice period.
ROI analysis
Using a separate fMRI scan after the main experiment with the Theory-of-Mind localizer27, we individually identified the rTPJ, associated with taking another’s perspective19,20,27. The average coordinates of the peak of identified rTPJ (57, −50, 20) were located in the posterior subdivision of rTPJ. This is consistent with results from a previous study indicating that the posterior rTPJ is involved in cognitive perspective-taking, using anatomical and functional connectivity36. We examined the modulatory effect of the key parameters in the quasi-maximin model (i.e. ΔMin and ΔEV) on the activation of rTPJ, as well as whether the individual variability in empathic personality trait influenced the effect of parametric modulation.
We hypothesized that the mOFC would be associated with affective/emotive evaluation of choice options. We used the anatomical mOFC ROI defined in AAL237 for analysis. Beta estimates in this ROI were extracted and examined. We also hypothesized that the activation in VS would be associated with utility calculations (ΔU) of the quasi-maximin model. For this purpose, an anatomical ROI of VS was defined using MarsBaR38. The striatum ROI of the automated anatomical labeling equipped in MarsBaR within the range from −12 to 0 of MNI z coordinate of caudate ROI was used as the VS (Fig. S1). To calculate the option utility based on the quasi-maximin model (Eq. 1) for this analysis, we used the maximin weight (α) averaged across all participants in each condition.
Functional connectivity analysis
We hypothesized that the VS would be functionally connected with the rTPJ and mOFC. First, we examined the functional connectivity between the rTPJ and the VS using a generalized psychophysiological interaction (gPPI)39. The time series of brain activity in the rTPJ was extracted, and the interactions between the first Eigenvariate of extracted time series and task conditions (Self/Other) were entered into a GLM analysis for gPPI. The beta estimates (i.e. functional connectivity) of the interactions in Self and Other conditions in the ROI of VS were extracted using MarsBaR and tested statistically. Similarly, we examined the functional connectivity between the mOFC and the VS in Self and Other conditions.
Electronic supplementary material
Acknowledgements
We thank the faculty and staff of the Center for Experimental Research in Social Sciences at Hokkaido University, who kindly allowed us to use their laboratory resources. We also thank Aiko Murata, Yoshimatsu Saito, and Shotaro Hori for their support on the fMRI experiment. This work was supported by Japan Society for the Promotion of Science Grant-in-Aid for Scientific Research JP16K16076 to A.O. and JP16H06324 and JP25118004 to T.K. and Japan Science and Technology Agency CREST Grant JPMJCR17A4 (17941861) to T.K. Support from CiSHub at the University of Tokyo is also appreciated.
Author Contributions
The study was designed by A.O., A.U. and T.K. The data was collected by A.O., A.U. and K.I. Data analysis was performed by A.O. and A.U. The manuscript was written by A.O., A.U. and T.K. T.K. supervised the entire process of the study.
Competing Interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher's note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Electronic supplementary material
Supplementary information accompanies this paper at 10.1038/s41598-018-31308-6.
References
- 1.Markowitz H. Portfolio selection. J. Finance. 1952;7:77–91. [Google Scholar]
- 2.Preuschoff K, Bossaerts P, Quartz SR. Neural differentiation of expected reward and risk in human subcortical structures. Neuron. 2006;51:381–390. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.06.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Kameda T, et al. Rawlsian maximin rule operates as a common cognitive anchor in distributive justice and risky decisions. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2016;113:11817–11822. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1602641113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Wang XT, Johnson JG. A tri-reference point theory of decision making under risk. J. Exp. Psychol.: Gen. 2012;141:743–756. doi: 10.1037/a0027415. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Loewenstein GF, Weber EU, Hsee CK, Welch N. Risk as feelings. Psychol. Bull. 2001;127:267–286. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.127.2.267. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Hsee CK, Weber EU. A fundamental prediction error: self-others discrepancies in risk preference. J. Exp. Psychol.: Gen. 1997;126:45–53. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.126.1.45. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Mengarelli F, Moretti L, Faralla V, Vindras P, Sirigu A. Economic decisions for others: an exception to loss aversion law. Plos One. 2014;9:e85042. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0085042. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Polman E. Self-other decision making and loss aversion. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 2012;119:141–150. doi: 10.1016/j.obhdp.2012.06.005. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Jung D, Sul S, Kim H. Dissociable neural processes underlying risky decisions for self versus other. Front. Neurosci. 2013;7:15. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2013.00015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Payne, J. W., Bettman, J. R. & Johnson, E. J. The Adaptive Decision Maker (Cambridge Univ. Press, 1993).
- 11.Charness G, Rabin M. Understanding social preferences with simple tests. Q. J. Econ. 2002;117:817–869. doi: 10.1162/003355302760193904. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Decety J, Yoder KJ. Empathy and motivation for justice: cognitive empathy and concern, but not emotional empathy, predict sensitivity to injustice for others. Soc. Neurosci. 2016;11:1–14. doi: 10.1080/17470919.2015.1029593. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Gleichgerrcht E, Young L. Low levels of empathic concern predict utilitarian moral judgment. Plos One. 2013;8:e60418. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0060418. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Davis MH. Measuring individual differences in empathy: evidence for a multidimensional approach. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1983;44:113–126. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.44.1.113. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Pernet CR, Wilcox RR, Rousselet GA. Robust correlation analyses: false positive and power validation using a new open source Matlab toolbox. Front. Psychology. 2013;3:606. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00606. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Crockett MJ, Siegel JZ, Kurth-Nelson Z, Dayan P, Dolan RJ. Moral transgressions corrupt neural representations of value. Nat. Neurosci. 2017;20:879–885. doi: 10.1038/nn.4557. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Mair, P. & Wilcox, R. WRS2: Wilcox robust estimation and testing, https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/WRS2/ (2018).
- 18.R Core Team. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, https://www.R-project.org/ (2017).
- 19.Carter RM, Huettel SA. A nexus model of the temporal-parietal junction. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2013;17:328–336. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.05.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Koster-Hale J, Saxe R. Theory of mind: a neural prediction problem. Neuron. 2013;79:836–848. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.08.020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Bechara A, Damasio H, Damasio AR. Emotion, decision making and the orbitofrontal cortex. Cereb. Cortex. 2000;10:295–307. doi: 10.1093/cercor/10.3.295. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Rolls ET, Grabenhorst F. The orbitofrontal cortex and beyond: from affect to decision-making. Prog. Neurobiol. 2008;86:216–244. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2008.09.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Strait CE, Sleezer BJ, Hayden BY. Signatures of value comparison in ventral striatum neurons. Plos Biol. 2015;13:e1002173. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1002173. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Buckner RL, Carroll DC. Self-projection and the brain. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2007;11:49–57. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2006.11.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Suddendorf T, Corballis MC. The evolution of foresight: what is mental time travel, and is it unique to humans? Behav. Brain Sci. 2007;30:299–313. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X07001975. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Rawls, J. A Theory of Justice (Harvard Univ. Press, 1971).
- 27.Dodell-Feder D, Koster-Hale J, Bedny M, Saxe R. fMRI item analysis in a theory of mind task. Neuroimage. 2011;55:705–712. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.12.040. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Koenigs M, et al. Damage to the prefrontal cortex increases utilitarian moral judgements. Nature. 2007;446:908–911. doi: 10.1038/nature05631. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Krajbich I, Adolphs R, Tranel D, Denburg NL, Camerer CF. Economic games quantify diminished sense of guilt in patients with damage to the prefrontal cortex. J. Neurosci. 2009;29:2188–2192. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5086-08.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Singer T, et al. Empathy for pain involves the affective but not sensory components of pain. Science. 2004;303:1157–1162. doi: 10.1126/science.1093535. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Singer T, et al. Empathic neural responses are modulated by the perceived fairness of others. Nature. 2006;439:466–469. doi: 10.1038/nature04271. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.de Waal FBM. Putting the altruism back into altruism: the evolution of empathy. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2008;59:279–300. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093625. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Kameda T, Takezawa M, Hastie R. Where do social norms come from? the example of communal sharing. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2005;14:331–334. doi: 10.1111/j.0963-7214.2005.00392.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Peirce JW. PsychoPy - psychophysics software in Python. J. Neurosci. Methods. 2007;162:8–13. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2006.11.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Bates B, Maechler M, Bolker B, Walker S. Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. J. Stat. Softw. 2015;67:1–48. doi: 10.18637/jss.v067.i01. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Mars RB, et al. Connectivity-based subdivisions of the human right ‘temporoparietal junction area’: evidence for different areas participating in different cortical networks. Cereb. Cortex. 2012;22:1894–1903. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr268. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Rolls ET, Joliot M, Tzourio-Mazoyer N. Implementation of a new parcellation of the orbitofrontal cortex in the automated anatomical labeling atlas. Neuroimage. 2015;122:1–5. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.07.075. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Brett, M., Anton, J.-L., Valabreque, R. & Poline, J.-B. Region of interest analysis using an SPM toolbox. In 8th International Conference on Functional Mapping of the Human Brain (Sendai, Japan, 2002).
- 39.McLaren DG, Ries ML, Xu GF, Johnson SC. A generalized form of context-dependent psychophysiological interactions (gPPI): a comparison to standard approaches. Neuroimage. 2012;61:1277–1286. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.03.068. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.