Skip to main content
Frontiers in Neuroscience logoLink to Frontiers in Neuroscience
. 2018 Aug 21;12:578. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2018.00578

Cross-Modal Stochastic Resonance as a Universal Principle to Enhance Sensory Processing

Patrick Krauss 1, Konstantin Tziridis 1, Achim Schilling 1, Holger Schulze 1,*
PMCID: PMC6110899  PMID: 30186104

Cross-modal interactions are common in sensory processing, and phenomena reach from changed perception within one modality due to input from another like in the well-known McGurk effect (McGurk and MacDonald, 1976) to misperceptions like synesthesia (Marks, 1975). A recent study demonstrated that electro-tactile stimulation applied to the index finger significantly improves speech perception thresholds (Huang et al., 2017). Here we argue that such cross-modal enhancement can be explained in terms of stochastic resonance (Benzi et al., 1981), a phenomenon that is ubiquitous in nature (Wiesenfeld and Moss, 1995), and especially within the context of neuroscience, receives increasing attention (Douglass et al., 1993; Moss et al., 2004; Faisal et al., 2008; Mino, 2014).

Stochastic resonance refers to a processing principle where signals otherwise sub-threshold for a given sensor can, at least partially, be detected anyway by adding noise of a suitable intensity to the sensor input (Benzi et al., 1981; Collins et al., 1996; Levin and Miller, 1996; Gammaitoni et al., 1998).

In self-adaptive signal detection systems based on stochastic resonance, the optimum noise level is continuously adjusted via a feed-back loop, so that the system response in terms of information throughput remains optimal, even if the properties of the input signal change. For this processing principle the term adaptive stochastic resonance has been coined (Mitaim and Kosko, 1998, 2004; Wenning and Obermayer, 2003). Most objective functions to quantify such information transmission, e.g., mutual information, require knowledge of the signal to be detected (Levin and Miller, 1996; Mitaim and Kosko, 2004; Moss et al., 2004). In a previous study we demonstrated that the autocorrelation of the sensor output, a quantity always accessible and easy to analyze by neuronal nets, can be used to quantify and hence maximize information transmission even for unknown and variable input signals (Krauss et al., 2017). In a further study we demonstrated by implementing a phenomenological computational model that adaptive stochastic resonance based on output autocorrelations might be a major processing principle of the auditory system (Krauss et al., 2016) that serves to partially compensate for acute or chronic hearing loss, e.g., due to cochlear damage (Krauss et al., 2016; Gollnast et al., 2017). In that view, the noise necessary for stochastic resonance to work corresponds to increased spontaneous neuronal firing rates in early processing stages of the auditory brainstem and cortex, a phenomenon that has been observed in animal models and human subjects with central tinnitus (Wang et al., 1997; Ahlf et al., 2012; Tziridis et al., 2015; Wu et al., 2015). In fact we have proposed earlier that the noise necessary for stochastic resonance is the neurophysiological correlate of tinnitus-related enhanced neuronal activity, and that in this view tinnitus is a side effect of an adaptive mechanism within the auditory system whose main purpose is to compensate for hearing loss by constantly optimizing information transmission (Krauss et al., 2016).

Within the auditory system, the dorsal cochlear nucleus (DCN) is the earliest processing stage in the auditory pathway in which acoustic trauma leads to tinnitus-related changes and increased spontaneous firing rates (Kaltenbach et al., 1998, 2004; Kaltenbach and Afman, 2000; Brozoski et al., 2002; Zacharek et al., 2002; Wu et al., 2015). The amount of this increase in spontaneous activity in the DCN has been shown to be correlated with the strength of the behavioral signs of tinnitus (Kaltenbach et al., 2004). Furthermore, this hyperactivity is only found in regions innervated by the damaged parts of the cochlear receptor epithelium (Kaltenbach et al., 2002). Gao and colleagues recently described changes in DCN fusiform cell spontaneous activity after noise exposure that supports our proposed stochastic resonance mechanism. In particular, the time course of spontaneous rate changes shows an almost complete loss of spontaneous activity immediately after loud sound exposure (as no stochastic resonance is needed due to stimulation that is well above threshold), followed by an overcompensation of spontaneous rates to levels well above pre-exposition rates where stochastic resonance is now needed to compensate for acute hearing loss (Gao et al., 2016).

It is well-known that the DCN receives not only input from the cochlea, but also from the somatosensory system (Ryugo et al., 2003; Shore and Zhou, 2006; Dehmel et al., 2012; Zeng et al., 2012). Therefore, in a previous paper we proposed the possibility that the neuronal noise which is crucial for stochastic resonance to work may be injected into the auditory system via somatosensory projections (Krauss et al., 2016). This idea is supported by a number of papers.

For example, it is well-known that jaw movements lead to a modulation of tinnitus sensation in patients (Pinchoff et al., 1998). This may easily be explained within our model, as jaw movements alter somatosensory input to the DCN. Since this somatosensory input would correspond to the noise which is crucial for stochastic resonance, auditory input to the DCN is modulated through this mechanism and the altered noise level would be perceived as modulated tinnitus (Krauss et al., 2016). Tang et al. demonstrated that somatosensory (noise) input and hence tinnitus sensation may also be modified by serotonergic regulation of excitability of principal cells of the DCN (Tang and Trussell, 2015, 2017).

In addition, the finding that DCN responses to somatosensory stimulation are enhanced after noise-induced hearing loss (Shore et al., 2008) supports our idea that stochastic resonance plays a key role in auditory processing and actually takes place in the DCN. After hearing-loss, the auditory input to the DCN is decreased. By that, information transmission is reduced leading to an increase of internally generated neuronal noise which is crucial for adaptive stochastic resonance to work and partially restore hearing thresholds.

Finally, the finding that electro-tactile stimulation applied to the index finger significantly improves speech perception thresholds (Huang et al., 2017) further supports our interpretation that somatosensory input to the DCN corresponds to the noise input for stochastic resonance. Again, electro-tactile stimulation increases somatosensory input which is equivalent to increased neuronal noise, which in turn improves detection thresholds for auditory stimuli via stochastic resonance.

In a previous paper we already discussed the possibility of superseding the internally generated neuronal noise by adding external acoustic noise, thereby suppressing the tinnitus percept (Krauss et al., 2016). Remarkably, a recent publication demonstrated that exposure to moderate levels of white noise after acoustic trauma prevented most individuals from developing tinnitus in a mouse model (Sturm et al., 2017). Along the same line, Marks and coworkers found that auditory-somatosensory bimodal stimulation reduces tinnitus in guinea pigs and humans (Marks et al., 2018). Within the framework of our modal one can argue that simultaneous auditory and somatosensory stimulation corresponds to a combination of the two aforementioned studies (Huang et al., 2017; Sturm et al., 2017).

Based on these observations we here speculate that stochastic resonance in one sensory modality driven by input from another modality may be a general principle, namely multisensory integration causing stochastic resonance like cross-modal enhancement. For example, it is known that the concept of stochastic resonance via internal noise applies also to visual perception (Aihara et al., 2008). Furthermore, analogous to the above discussed somatosensory enhancement of auditory perception, visual perception at threshold can be enhanced by auditory stimulation (Caclin et al., 2011) and that even visual below threshold stimuli may be perceived through spatially converging audiovisual inputs (Bolognini et al., 2005). Such audiovisual cross-modal improvement of detection thresholds has been demonstrated in a broad range of different species, e.g., the ferret (Hollensteiner et al., 2015) or the chicken (Verhaal and Luksch, 2016).

In addition to the here proposed neuronal mechanism of somatosensory-driven stochastic resonance in the auditory system explaining the better hearing in the CI patients during finger tapping (Huang et al., 2017), alternative explanations are discussed: First, an attentional effect modulating the sensory threshold has been proposed (for Review see Sarter et al., 2005). Second, due to the fact that patients are dealing with a multimodal integration task, the interaction of both stimuli can mathematically be explained by the “optimal observer” model of sensory integration based on maximum-likelihood estimation theory as already shown in visual and auditory (e.g., Battaglia et al., 2003) and visual and somatosensory interaction (Hollensteiner et al., 2015). Nevertheless, both alternative interpretations would most likely interact on the level of cortical representations of the sensory modalities. Hence, to proof or falsify our hypothesis proposed here that auditory-somatosensory interaction takes place at the level of the dorsal cochlear nucleus, neuronal recordings in the dorsal cochlear nucleus should be performed in search of a somatosensory driven modulation of auditory neuron spontaneous activity in combination with lowered neuronal and/or behavioral thresholds, Nevertheless, finding such interaction within the DCN would not disproof that cross-modal interactions between other modalities and/or in other contexts might take place at the level of sensory cortex.

Even though multisensory integration leading to improved perception has been reported earlier (Stein and Stanford, 2008), we here provide a new mechanistic explanation of how this enhancement might work. We hypothesize that this mechanism represents a universal principle of neural computation.

Author contributions

PK and HS wrote the manuscript. PK, HS, KT, and AS discussed possible interpretations of the cited studies and developed the hypothesis presented in this paper.

Conflict of interest statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest. The reviewer SV and the handling editor declared their shared affiliation.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG-grant SCHU 1272/12-1).

References

  1. Ahlf S., Tziridis K., Korn S., Strohmeyer I., Schulze H. (2012). Predisposition for and prevention of subjective tinnitus development. PLoS ONE 7:e44519. 10.1371/journal.pone.0044519 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Aihara T., Kitajo K., Nozaki D., Yamamoto Y. (2008). Internal noise determines external stochastic resonance in visual perception. Vis. Res. 48, 1569–1573. 10.1016/j.visres.2008.04.022 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Battaglia P. W., Jacobs R. A., Aslin R. N. (2003). Bayesian integration of visual and auditory signals for spatial localization. J. Opt. Soc. Am. A. Opt. Image Sci. Vis. 20, 1391–1397. 10.1364/JOSAA.20.001391 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Benzi R., Sutera A., Vulpiani A. (1981). The mechanism of stochastic resonance. J. Phys. A Math. Gen. 14:L453. [Google Scholar]
  5. Bolognini N., Frassinetti F., Serino A., Làdavas E. (2005). “Acoustical vision” of below threshold stimuli: interaction among spatially converging audiovisual inputs. Exp. Brain Res. 160, 273–282. 10.1007/s00221-004-2005-z [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Brozoski T. J., Bauer C. A., Caspary D. M. (2002). Elevated fusiform cell activity in the dorsal cochlear nucleus of chinchillas with psychophysical evidence of tinnitus. J. Neurosci. 22, 2383–2390. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-06-02383.2002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Caclin A., Bouchet P., Djoulah F., Pirat E., Pernier J., Giard M. H. (2011). Auditory enhancement of visual perception at threshold depends on visual abilities. Brain Res. 1396, 35–44. 10.1016/j.brainres.2011.04.016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Collins J. J., Imhoff T. T., Grigg P. (1996). Noise-enhanced information transmission in rat SA1 cutaneous mechanoreceptors via aperiodic stochastic resonance. J. Neurophysiol. 76, 642–645. 10.1152/jn.1996.76.1.642 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Dehmel S., Pradhan S., Koehler S., Bledsoe S., Shore S. (2012). Noise overexposure alters long-term somatosensory-auditory processing in the dorsal cochlear nucleus–possible basis for tinnitus-related hyperactivity? J. Neurosci. 32, 1660–1671. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4608-11.2012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Douglass J. K., Wilkens L., Pantazelou E., Moss F. (1993). Noise enhancement of information transfer in crayfish mechanoreceptors by stochastic resonance. Nature 365, 337–340. 10.1038/365337a0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Faisal A. A., Selen L. P., Wolpert D. M. (2008). Noise in the nervous system. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 9, 292–303. 10.1038/nrn2258 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Gammaitoni L., Hänggi P., Jung P., Marchesoni F. (1998). Stochastic resonance. Rev. Mod. Phys. 70:223 10.1103/RevModPhys.70.223 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  13. Gao Y., Manzoor N., Kaltenbach J. A. (2016). Evidence of activity-dependent plasticity in the dorsal cochlear nucleus, in vivo, induced by brief sound exposure. Hear. Res. 341, 31–42. 10.1016/j.heares.2016.07.011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Gollnast D., Tziridis K., Krauss P., Schilling A., Hoppe U., Schulze H. (2017). Analysis of audiometric differences of patients with and without Tinnitus in a large clinical database. Front. Neurol. 8:31. 10.3389/fneur.2017.00031 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Hollensteiner K. J., Pieper F., Engler G., König P., Engel A. K. (2015). Crossmodal integration improves sensory detection thresholds in the ferret. PLoS ONE 10:e0124952. 10.1371/journal.pone.0124952 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Huang J., Sheffield B., Lin P., Zeng F. G. (2017). Electro-tactile stimulation enhances cochlear implant speech recognition in noise. Sci. Rep. 7:2196. 10.1038/s41598-017-02429-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Kaltenbach J. A., Afman C. E. (2000). Hyperactivity in the dorsal cochlear nucleus after intense sound exposure and its resemblance to tone-evoked activity: a physiological model for tinnitus. Hear. Res. 140, 165–172. 10.1016/S0378-5955(99)00197-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Kaltenbach J. A., Godfrey D. A., Neumann J. B., McCaslin D. L., Afman C. E., Zhang J. (1998). Changes in spontaneous neural activity in the dorsal cochlear nucleus following exposure to intense sound: relation to threshold shift. Hear. Res. 124, 78–84. 10.1016/S0378-5955(98)00119-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Kaltenbach J. A., Rachel J. D., Mathog T. A., Zhang J., Falzarano P. R., Lewandowski M. (2002). Cisplatin-induced hyperactivity in the dorsal cochlear nucleus and its relation to outer hair cell loss: relevance to tinnitus. J. Neurophysiol. 88, 699–714. 10.1152/jn.2002.88.2.699 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Kaltenbach J. A., Zacharek M. A., Zhang J., Frederick S. (2004). Activity in the dorsal cochlear nucleus of hamsters previously tested for tinnitus following intense tone exposure. Neurosci. Lett. 355, 121–125. 10.1016/j.neulet.2003.10.038 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Krauss P., Metzner C., Schilling A., Schütz C., Tziridis K., Fabry B., et al. (2017). Adaptive stochastic resonance for unknown and variable input signals. Sci. Rep. 7:2450. 10.1038/s41598-017-02644-w [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Krauss P., Tziridis K., Metzner C., Schilling A., Hoppe U., Schulze H. (2016). Stochastic resonance controlled upregulation of internal noise after hearing loss as a putative cause of tinnitus-related neuronal hyperactivity. Front. Neurosci. 10:597. 10.3389/fnins.2016.00597 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Levin J. E., Miller J. P. (1996). Broadband neural encoding in the cricket cercal sensory system enhanced by stochastic resonance. Nature 380, 165–168. 10.1038/380165a0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Marks K. L., Martel D. T., Wu C., Basura G. J., Roberts L. E., Schvartz-Leyzac K. C., et al. (2018). Auditory-somatosensory bimodal stimulation desynchronizes brain circuitry to reduce tinnitus in guinea pigs and humans. Sci. Transl. Med. 10:eaal3175. 10.1126/scitranslmed.aal3175 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Marks L. E. (1975). On colored-hearing synesthesia: cross-modal translations of sensory dimensions. Psychol. Bull. 82:303. 10.1037/0033-2909.82.3.303 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. McGurk H., MacDonald J. (1976). Hearing lips and seeing voices. Nature 264, 746–748. 10.1038/264746a0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Mino H. (2014). The effects of spontaneous random activity on information transmission in an auditory brain stem neuron model. Entropy 16, 6654–6666. 10.3390/e16126654 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  28. Mitaim S., Kosko B. (1998). Adaptive stochastic resonance. Proc. IEEE 86, 2152–2183. 10.1109/5.726785 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  29. Mitaim S., Kosko B. (2004). Adaptive stochastic resonance in noisy neurons based on mutual information. IEEE Trans. Neural Netw. 15, 1526–1540. 10.1109/TNN.2004.826218 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Moss F., Ward L. M., Sannita W. G. (2004). Stochastic resonance and sensory information processing: a tutorial and review of application. Clin. Neurophysiol. 115, 267–281. 10.1016/j.clinph.2003.09.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Pinchoff R. J., Burkard R. F., Salvi R. J., Coad M. L., Lockwood A. H. (1998). Modulation of tinnitus by voluntary jaw movements. Otol. Neurotol. 19, 785–789. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Ryugo D. K., Haenggeli C. A., Doucet J. R. (2003). Multimodal inputs to the granule cell domain of the cochlear nucleus. Exp. Brain Res. 153, 477–485. 10.1007/s00221-003-1605-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Sarter M., Hasselmo M. E., Bruno J. P., Givens B. (2005). Unraveling the attentional functions of cortical cholinergic inputs: interactions between signal-driven and cognitive modulation of signal detection. Brain Res. Rev. 48, 98–111. 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2004.08.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Shore S. E., Koehler S., Oldakowski M., Hughes L. F., Syed S. (2008). Dorsal cochlear nucleus responses to somatosensory stimulation are enhanced after noise-induced hearing loss. Eur. J. Neurosci. 27, 155–168. 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2007.05983.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Shore S. E., Zhou J. (2006). Somatosensory influence on the cochlear nucleus and beyond. Hear. Res. 216, 90–99. 10.1016/j.heares.2006.01.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Stein B. E., Stanford T. R. (2008). Multisensory integration: current issues from the perspective of the single neuron. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 9, 255–266. 10.1038/nrn2331 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Sturm J. J., Zhang-Hooks Y. X., Roos H., Nguyen T., Kandler K. (2017). Noise trauma-induced behavioral gap detection deficits correlate with reorganization of excitatory and inhibitory local circuits in the inferior colliculus and are prevented by acoustic enrichment. J. Neurosci. 37, 6314–6330. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0602-17.2017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Tang Z. Q., Trussell L. O. (2015). Serotonergic regulation of excitability of principal cells of the dorsal cochlear nucleus. J. Neurosci. 35, 4540–4551. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4825-14.2015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Tang Z. Q., Trussell L. O. (2017). Serotonergic modulation of sensory representation in a central multisensory circuit is pathway specific. Cell Rep. 20, 1844–1854. 10.1016/j.celrep.2017.07.079 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Tziridis K., Ahlf S., Jeschke M., Happel M. F., Ohl F. W., Schulze H. (2015). Noise trauma induced neural plasticity throughout the auditory system of Mongolian gerbils: differences between tinnitus developing and non-developing animals. Front. Neurol. 6:22. 10.3389/fneur.2015.00022 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Verhaal J., Luksch H. (2016). Multimodal integration in the chicken. J. Exp. Biol. 219, 90–95. 10.1242/jeb.129387 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Wang J., Powers N. L., Hofstetter P., Trautwein P., Ding D., Salvi R. (1997). Effects of selective inner hair cell loss on auditory nerve fiber threshold, tuning and spontaneous and driven discharge rate. Hear. Res. 107, 67–82. 10.1016/S0378-5955(97)00020-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Wenning G., Obermayer K. (2003). Activity driven adaptive stochastic resonance. Phys. Rev. Lett. 90:120602. 10.1103/PhysRevLett.90.120602 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Wiesenfeld K., Moss F. (1995). Stochastic resonance and the benefits of noise: from ice ages to crayfish and SQUIDs. Nature 373, 33–36. 10.1038/373033a0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Wu C., Stefanescu R. A., Martel D. T., Shore S. E. (2015). Tinnitus: maladaptive auditory–somatosensory plasticity. Hear. Res. 334, 20–29. 10.1016/j.heares.2015.06.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Zacharek M. A., Kaltenbach J. A., Mathog T. A., Zhang J. (2002). Effects of cochlear ablation on noise induced hyperactivity in the hamster dorsal cochlear nucleus: implications for the origin of noise induced tinnitus. Hear. Res. 172, 137–144. 10.1016/S0378-5955(02)00575-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Zeng C., Yang Z., Shreve L., Bledsoe S., Shore S. (2012). Somatosensory projections to cochlear nucleus are upregulated after unilateral deafness. J. Neurosci. 32, 15791–15801. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2598-12.2012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Frontiers in Neuroscience are provided here courtesy of Frontiers Media SA

RESOURCES