Abstract
Objective:
Dysbiosis of the gut microbiome is implicated in numerous human health conditions. Animal studies have linked microbiome disruption to changes in cognitive functioning, though no study has examined this possibility in neurologically-healthy older adults.
Method:
Participants were 43 community-dwelling older adults (50-85 years) that completed a brief cognitive test battery and provided stool samples for gut microbiome sequencing. Participants performing ≥ 1 SD below normative performance on two or more tests were compared to persons with one or fewer impaired scores.
Results:
Mann Whitney U tests revealed different distributions of Bacteroidetes (p = 0.01), Firmicutes (p = 0.02), Proteobacteria (p = 0.04), and Verrucomicrobia (p = 0.003) between Intact and Impaired groups. These phyla were significantly correlated with cognitive test performances, particularly Verrucomicrobia and attention/executive function measures.
Conclusions:
The current findings suggest that composition of the gut microbiome is associated with cognitive test performance in neurologically-healthy older adults. Future studies are needed to confirm these findings and explore possible mechanisms.
Keywords: Microbiota, Cognition, Attention, Executive Function, Aging, Humans
Introduction
The human intestine supports a diverse and active ecosystem of microorganisms distinct from the human genome, collectively referred to as the gut microbiota (Bäckhed, Ley, Sonnenburg, Peterson, & Gordon, 2005). All three domains of cellular life (i.e., archaea, bacteria, and eukarya) can be found in the microbiota, together outnumbering human cells in the gut by approximately 10 to 1 (Bäckhed, et al., 2005). In healthy hosts, this ecosystem contains mostly symbiotic and commensal organisms that coexist without harm to either the host or to the microorganisms themselves (Bäckhed, et al., 2005). In fact, a healthy gut microbiota benefits its host by supplying essential nutrients and aiding in digestion and immune functions.
However, the composition of the microbiota is influenced by many factors, including intestinal motility, pH value, and bodily secretions, even extending to host lifestyle factors, such as antibiotic usage, diet, stress, and age (Tilg & Kaser, 2011). While some changes to the composition of the microbiota and variance across individuals is normative, dysbiosis of the gut microbiota can become pathogenic. Dysbiosis can result from host or environmental factors and has been linked to a number of disease conditions, including inflammatory bowel disease, some types of cancer, obesity, and metabolic disorders (Tilg & Kaser, 2011).
While research on the connection between the gut microbiome and the brain (i.e., the gut-brain axis) is newly developing and research in humans is limited, a parallel literature demonstrates possible links to both neurocognitive and psychiatric outcomes (Sarkar, Lehto, Harty, Dinan, Cryan, & Burnet, 2016). One such study employed a microbiome depletion/transplantation paradigm. Microbiota isolated from obese C57BL/6 donor mice eating high fat diets were transplanted to nonobese mice. These nonobese, diet-naïve mice showed changes in exploratory and stereotypical behaviors (reflecting increased anxiety), as well as decrements in learning and memory in response to the microbiota transplant (Bruce-Keller, et al., 2015). While specific changes were not thoroughly investigated, investigators did find lower levels of Akkermansia muciniphila (a Verrucomicrobium) and higher levels of Bilophila sp. (a Proteobacterium) in the microbiota of the obese mice on high fat diets than their normal weight, normal diet counterparts (Bruce-Keller, et al., 2015).
Another study examined C57BL/6J mice switched from a normal chow diet to either a high fat or a high sucrose diet. After two weeks of a high-sucrose diet, mice exhibited problems in multiple cognitive abilities (e.g. cognitive flexibility, working and long-term memory), as well as decreases in Bacteroidales and increases in Lactobacillales and Clostridiales (Magnusson, et al., 2015). Those mice on a high fat diet showed decreased cognitive flexibility, increases in Erysipelotrichales and Clostridiales, and decreases in Bacteroidales (Magnusson, et al., 2015).
A study using NIH Swiss mice used an antibiotic treatment from weaning onwards to deplete the gut microbiota during a key developmental period. Compared to control mice, those with depleted gut microbiota through development demonstrated reduced anxiety and reduced apparent memory for familiar objects and social learning as adults (Desbonnet, et al., 2015).
Initial research in humans has also linked composition of the gut microbiota to cognitive changes. Bajaj et al. (2016) examined fecal microbiota and cognitive function in patients with and without liver cirrhosis. In persons with cirrhosis, poorer cognitive function was correlated with higher proportions of Lactobacillales and lower proportions of Enterobacteriaceae and Porphyromonadaceae. Additionally, individuals with poorer cognitive test scores exhibited increased inflammation, and initial evidence for an interaction between inflammation, gut dysbiosis, and cognitive function in healthy individuals without liver-cirrhosis was also demonstrated (Bajaj, et al., 2016). The current study sought to further explore the possible association between the composition of the gut microbiome and cognitive function in a sample of healthy older adults.
Methods
Participants
A total of 43 older adults were recruited from a community recreation and wellness center (Table 1). Study participants were English-speaking and 50-85 years of age. Exclusion criteria included: 1) history of neurological, developmental, or severe psychiatric disorder (i.e., conditions known to independently impact cognitive function); 2) MMSE ≥ 24; 3) antibiotic use in the past 30 days; 4) history of significant gastrointestinal surgery; 5) history of alcohol or illicit drug dependence; or 6) history of severe heart, kidney, or liver problems.
Table 1.
Cognitive Group M(SD) or % | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Intact | Impaired | Test | df | p | |
Demographic Variables | |||||
Age | 64.08 ± 6.49 | 64.06 ± 9.37 | 0.01 | 41 | .99 |
Sex (% female) | 32.0% | 33.3% | 0.01 | 11 | .93 |
Years of education | 16.12 ± 1.83 | 14.56 ± 3.22 | 2.02 | 41 | .06 |
Days per month consuming alcohol | 8.24 ± 10.48 | 3.33 ± 6.96 | 1.72 | 41 | .09 |
RAPA aerobic | 6.32 ± 1.25 | 5.67 ± 1.75 | 1.43 | 41 | .16 |
Total kcals consumed daily (g) | 1312.65 ± 336.81 | 1729.49 ± 1260.23 | 1.58 | 41 | .12 |
Carbohydrates consumed daily (g) | 137.48 ± 42.77 | 181.74 ± 154.63 | 1.37 | 41 | .18 |
Sugars consumed daily (g) | 82.11 ± 37.17 | 97.97 ± 82.93 | 0.84 | 41 | .40 |
Fat consumed daily (g) | 55.52 ± 21.43 | 79.55 ±61.80 | 1.81 | 41 | .08 |
Protein consumed daily (g) | 66.90 ± 15.98 | 79.35 ± 38.00 | 1.47 | 41 | .15 |
Cognitive Tests | |||||
MMSE | 29.28 ± 0.98 | 28.00 ± 1.85 | 2.95 | 41 | .005 |
HVLT-R | |||||
Total Learning | 51.66 ± 7.06 | 43.33 ± 17.44 | 2.16 | 41 | .04 |
Delay Recall | 51.84 ± 8.01 | 33.71 ± 21.08 | 3.94 | 41 | <.001 |
Recognition/Discrimination | 52.52 ± 6.60 | 42.72 ± 11.98 | 4.44 | 41 | .001 |
RCFT | |||||
Immediate Recall | 59.96 ± 10.58 | 40.89 ± 18.22 | 4.44 | 41 | <.001 |
Delay Recall | 56.36 ± 10.11 | 38.06 ± 19.15 | 4.07 | 41 | <.001 |
TMT-A | 54.46 ± 10.11 | 42.50 ± 11.41 | 3.10 | 41 | .004 |
TMT-B | 56.21 ± 8.63 | 42.61 ± 19.91 | 3.12 | 41 | .003 |
SCWT | |||||
Word | 51.25 ± 5.00 | 44.28 ± 6.28 | 3.96 | 41 | <.001 |
Color | 50.92 ± 5.30 | 43.89 ± 6.43 | 3.88 | 41 | <.001 |
Color Word | 56.21 ± 8.63 | 44.00 ± 11.10 | 3.96 | 41 | <.001 |
FAB | 50.64 ± 13.48 | 34.78 ± 15.51 | 3.57 | 41 | .001 |
FAS | 57.44 ± 10.39 | 50.83 ± 13.14 | 1.84 | 41 | .07 |
Animal Naming | 55.76 ± 9.52 | 56.03 ± 10.60 | 0.09 | 41 | .93 |
Medical History | |||||
Type 2 Diabetes | 4.0% | 16.7% | 1.99 | 1 | .16 |
Cardiovasc Dis. | 12.0% | 5.5% | 0.52 | 1 | .47 |
Hypertension | 40.0% | 38.8% | 2.29 | 1 | .32 |
Sleep Apnea | 12.0% | 11.1% | 0.83 | 1 | .66 |
Smoked in past week | 0.0% | 5.5% | 1.42 | 1 | .23 |
Microbiome | |||||
Euryarchaeota | 1.92 × 10−4 ± 6.49 × 10−4 | 2.81 × 10−4 ± 8.90 × 10−4 | 177.00 | - | .79 |
Actinobacteria | 1.03 × 10−2 ± 1.37 × 10−2 | 7.17 × 10−3 ± 1.13 × 10−2 | 151.00 | - | .35 |
Bacteroidetes | 2.58 × 10−1 ± 1.39 × 10−1 | 3.82 × 10−1 ± 1.24 × 10−1 | 95.00 | - | .01 |
Chloflexi | 2.37 × 10−6 ± 1.14 × 10−5 | 0.00 | 176.00 | - | .40 |
Cyanobacteria | 2.91 × 10−3 ± 5.71 × 10−3 | 4.59 × 10−5 ± 1.26 × 10−5 | 126.00 | - | .06 |
Elusimicrobia | 2.66 × 10−4 ± 1.28 × 10−3 | 0.00 | 176.00 | - | .40 |
Firmicutes | 6.48 × 10−1 ± 1.89 × 10−1 | 5.38 × 10−1 ± 1.4 × 10−1 | 102.00 | - | .02 |
Lentisphaerae | 6.07 × 105 ± 1.35 × 10−4 | 5.62 × 10−5 ± 1.87 × 10−4 | 168.00 | - | .50 |
Proteobacteria | 3.37 × 10−2 ± 8.20 × 10−2 | 6.24 × 10−2 ± 7.32 × 10−2 | 113.00 | - | .04 |
Spirochaetes | 1.18 × 10−6 ± 5.67 × 10−6 | 0 | 176.00 | - | .40 |
Synergistetes | 8.53 × 10−5 ± 3.82 × 10−4 | 2.17 × 10−5 ± 7.87 × 10−5 | 174.00 | - | .65 |
TM7 | 1.29 × 10−5 ± 2.32 × 10−5 | 1.47 × 10−6 ± 3.04 × 10−5 | 182.00 | - | .94 |
Tenericutes | 1.14 × 10−3 ± 2.32 × 10−3 | 6.20 × 10−4 ± 1.92 × 10−3 | 133.00 | - | .08 |
Verrucomicrobia | 4.49 × 10−2 ± 8.79 × 10−2 | 9.25 × 10−3 ± 2.21 × 10−2 | 80.00 | - | .003 |
p < 0.05
p < 0.01
List of Abbreviations: RAPA aerobic = Rapid Assessment of Physical Activity Aerobic, MMSE = Mini Mental State Exam, HVLT-R = Hopkins Verbal Learning Test – Revised, RCFT = Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Task, TMT = Trail Making Test, SCWT = Stroop Color Word Test, FAB = Frontal Assessment Battery, FAS = Verbal Association Fluency
To facilitate analyses with the modest sample, participants were grouped into Intact vs. Impaired groups based on cognitive test performance. Participants with test scores falling one or more standard deviations below normative test performance on two or more tests (i.e. Impaired) were compared to those with one or fewer impaired test scores (i.e. Intact). This approach accounts for the high prevalence of individuals obtaining a single impaired test score in this age range (>70%, Palmer, Boone, Lesser, & Wohl, 1998) and corresponds with the median number of impaired test performances within the sample (median = 1.0; Intact group 0.60 ± 0.50 impaired tests vs. Impaired group 4.33 ± 2.03 impaired tests). Intact (N = 25) and Impaired (N = 18) groups did not differ on key demographic, medical, or lifestyle characteristics (including dietary habits) and no covariates were employed for analyses (See Table 1).
Procedures
All procedures were approved by the Kent State University Institutional Review Board. Participants were recruited from a local community recreation and wellness center. After providing written informed consent, participants completed the neuropsychological test battery, administered by an examiner and using paper and pencil, in a quiet room. After testing, participants received the self-report questionnaire packet and stool sample kit, with instructions to complete them at home and return by mail. The stool samples were collected in kits prearranged by Ubiome (www.ubiome.com). Samples were mailed directly to Ubiome in sterile, capped tubes containing proprietary buffer. 16S rRNA amplicon sequencing was done following protocols from the Human Microbiome Project (Human Microbiome Project Consortium, 2012).
Measures
Cognitive Test Battery
A brief neuropsychological test battery assessed cognitive functioning. Measures were chosen for their known sensitivity to cognitive dysfunction in an older adult population and all raw scores were transformed into t-scores using normative data.
Global Cognitive Function:
The Mini Mental State Exam (MMSE) examines global cognitive functioning by briefly assessing various cognitive domains. Total scores range from 0 to 30 with lower scores indicating poorer performances (Folstein, Folstein, & McHugh, 1975). Low MMSE scores were used as an exclusion criterion and were not used as measures of cognitive function in analyses.
Attention/Executive Function:
The Frontal Assessment Battery (FAB) is a composite measure of executive function and comprised of six subtests, incorporating aspects of conceptualization, mental flexibility, inhibitory control, and sensitivity to interference (Dubois, Slachevsky, Litvan, & Pillon, 2000). Trail Making Test A (TMT-A) has individuals draw lines to quickly connect numbered circles in ascending order and provides a measure of complex visual scanning and psychomotor speed, while Trail Making Test B (TMT-B) has individuals alternate connecting numbers and letters (e.g., 1 – A – 2 – B, etc.) and provides a measure of set-shifting (Dikmen, Heaton, Grant, & Temkin, 1999). The Stroop Color Word Test (Golden & Freshwater, 2002) requires that individuals indicate the color ink in which a word is printed, while ignoring the word itself, which is a different color word (e.g., “blue” printed in red ink). It is a commonly-used measure of mental flexibility and selective attention.
Memory:
The Hopkins Verbal Learning Test-Revised (HVLT-R; Brandt, 1991) is a test of verbal memory requiring learning, recall, and recognition of a 12-item word list. The Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure task (RCFT) measures memory for complex visual information by asking participants to copy a complicated geometric figure (learning task), immediately draw the figure again from memory (immediate recall), and later draw the figure from memory (delayed recall; Loring, Martin, Meador, & Lee, 1990).
Language:
A measure of verbal fluency (FAS) asks participants to name as many words beginning with a specific letter as possible in 60 seconds (Lezak, Howieson, Bigler, & Tranel, 2012), and the Animal Naming task asks participants to list different animals for 60 seconds (Lezak, Howieson, Bigler, & Tranel, 2012).
Questionnaires
Self-report questionnaires were used to clarify possible group differences. A brief Medical History Questionnaire asked participants to indicate histories of medical conditions, such as hypertension, type 2 diabetes, sleep apnea, and gastrointestinal disorders, among others. The EPIC-Norfolk Food Frequency Questionnaire (FFQ; Mulligan, et al., 2014) quantifies usual food intake over the past 30 days. The Rapid Assessment of Physical Activity (RAPA) offers a valid assessment of physical activity among older adults (Topolski, et al., 2006).
Statistical Analyses
All microbiome data were represented as proportions of total microbiome and then centered prior to all analyses. Due to small sample size and non-normal distributions, Mann-Whitney U tests were used to compare composition of the microbiome between Intact and Impaired groups. Spearman correlations examined the association between specific phyla and performance on individual cognitive tests. Mean imputation was used to confirm group assignment for a participant with missing test data on a single subtest (Stroop Color Word Test – Color) using regression analyses from demographic (e.g. age, education, sex) and remaining cognitive test scores.
Results
Between-group Differences in Gut Microbiome Composition
Mann-Whitney U tests employing a 95% confidence interval revealed a different distribution for Bacteroidetes (p = 0.01), Firmicutes (p = 0.02), Proteobacteria (p = 0.04), and Verrucomicrobia (p =0.003) for Intact relative to Impaired groups. Intact persons exhibited a lower proportion of Bacteroidetes and Proteobacteria and higher proportions of Firmicutes and Verrucomicrobia than the Impaired group. See Table 1. Mann-Whitney U tests employing a 95% confidence interval also revealed non-significant trends for differences in distributions for Cyanobacteria (p = .06) and Tenericutes (p = 0.08).
Correlation between Specific Phyla and Cognitive Test Performance
Spearman correlations examined possible associations between the significant phyla in the above analyses and specific cognitive tests. Analyses showed a number of significant associations, most commonly between Verrucomicrobia and measures of attention, executive function, and memory. Specifically, larger proportions of Verrucomicrobia were significantly correlated with better scores on HVLT-R Total Learning, TMT-A, TMT-B, SCWT Word, and SCWT Color (r ranging from 0.37 to 0.39, p < 0.05). Greater proportions of Firmicutes were significantly correlated with higher scores on the CFT Immediate (r = 0.39, p < 0.05) and Delayed Recall tasks (r = 0.35, p < 0.05), while higher proportions of Bacteroidetes were correlated with poorer performances on the CFT Immediate (r = −0.34, p < 0.05) and Delayed Recall tasks (r = −0.33, p < 0.05). Higher proportions of Proteobacteria were significantly correlated with poorer scores on HVLT-R Recognition/Discrimination, FAB, and FAS (r ranging from −0.34 to −0.41, p < 0.05). See Table 2.
Table 2.
Bacteroidetes | Firmicutes | Proteobacteria | Verrucomicrobia | |
---|---|---|---|---|
HVLT-R Total Learning | −0.03 | .12 | −.29 | .39* |
HVLT-R Delay Recall | −.16 | .26 | −.22 | .23 |
HVLT-R Recognition/Discrimination | −.23 | .23 | −.41* | .22 |
CFT Immediate Recall | −.34* | .39* | −.14 | .02 |
CFT Delay Recall | −.33* | .35* | −.14 | .07 |
TMT-A | −.22 | .16 | −.06 | .37* |
TMT-B | −.22 | .13 | .04 | .37* |
SCWT Word | −.19 | .06 | −.15 | .38* |
SCWT Color | −.27 | .21 | −.16 | .38* |
SCWT Color Word | −.33 | .21 | −.13 | .29 |
FAB | −.19 | .26 | −.40* | .29 |
FAS | −.17 | .11 | −.34* | .31 |
Animal Naming | −.04 | −.02 | .15 | .13 |
p < 0.05
List of Abbreviations: HVLT-R = Hopkins Verbal Learning Test – Revised, RCFT = Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Task, TMT = Trail Making Test, SCWT = Stroop Color Word Test, FAB = Frontal Assessment Battery, FAS = Verbal Association Fluency
Discussion
The findings from the current study are consistent with past work highlighting an association between the gut microbiome and cognitive function, though findings vary somewhat across studies. Generally, the presently observed correlations indicate that higher proportions of Firmicutes and Verrucomicrobia may be in some way beneficial or protective for cognitive function, while higher proportions of Bacteroidetes and Proteobacteria may be less beneficial, or possibly deleterious. Such findings are similar to past work in a mouse model using a microbiome depletion/transplantation paradigm, in which proportions of Verrucomicrobium were positively associated and proportions of Proteobacterium were negatively associated with learning and memory performance (Bruce-Keller, et al., 2015). Likewise, previous work in humans also found negative correlations between proportion of Proteobacteria and Bacteroidetes with cognitive impairment in persons with severe liver dysfunction (Bajaj et al, 2011).
In contrast to the present findings, in a study using high fat or high sugar diets to manipulate the gut microbiota in mouse models Bacteroidetes were positively correlated and Firmicutes were negatively correlated with cognitive function (Magnusson, et al., 2015). Full interpretation of such findings is difficult, as the many factors influencing the composition of the gut microbiome are just beginning to be understood and very few studies have directly examined its possible link to neurological function. Similarly, the appropriate level of analyses of gut-brain relationships is unclear at this time (e.g. phyla, genera, etc.) and requires clarification through future work in large samples.
Despite this early stage of the literature, there are several likely mechanisms that warrant evaluation, though investigation of these mechanisms was outside the scope of the current, preliminary study. One such possibility is the known relationship between gut dysbiosis and greater inflammation (Sarkar, et al., 2016). Chronic inflammation has long been viewed as a contributor to cognitive decline (Trollor, et al., 2012) and there is some evidence that dysbiosis – rather than a specific phylum of bacteria – may lead to greater inflammation (Tilg & Kaser, 2011). Other possible mechanisms have also been suggested for the link between the gut microbiome and neurocognitive outcomes, including communication through the vagus nerve, production of bacterial metabolites, and enteroendocrine signaling (as reviewed by Sherwin et al., 2016). Each of these pathways has the potential to influence a wide range of physiological processes within the brain. Other work shows that the gut microbiota can impact other known contributors to cognitive outcomes, including glycemic control (Tilg & Kaser, 2011). Identification and elucidation of these possible pathways is much needed, particularly as they may lead to novel intervention targets that promote neurological health through manipulation of the gut microbiota.
Findings from this preliminary study are limited in several important ways. First, the small sample was comprised of healthy community-dwelling older adults and future studies should include patient samples to examine a wider range of cognitive functioning. Second, the present study found a cross-sectional relationship between the gut microbiome and cognitive test scores, but does not provide insight to possible mechanisms or trajectories of such a relationship. Prospective studies that include pre-/pro-biotic supplementation will shed light on the relationship between the gut microbiome and cognitive functioning, as they have been proposed as possible therapeutics for psychiatric conditions (Sarkar et al., 2016) and may provide similar benefits for neurological deficits. Additionally, antibiotics are known to affect the microbiome, and the 30 day exclusion criterion used here may not capture persons on longer or more frequent courses of antibiotics and may influence the observed findings. Finally, our preliminary study used self-report measures to assess dietary habits and physical activity levels, which may account for the lack of association found between diet and performance on cognitive testing. Studies that more precisely measure dietary intake, particularly feeding studies or the use of a more detailed diet log to log food intake over time, and physical activity, such as actigraphy, will help clarify the observed association between the gut microbiome and cognitive function (as dietary habits and physical activity are primary contributors to gut microbiome composition) and may assist in disentangling the often inconsistent relationships between these factors and cognitive outcomes.
In brief summary, the present study found an association between the composition of the gut microbiome and cognitive functioning in a sample of healthy older adults. Such findings raise the possibility that the gut microbiota is a potential contributor to neurological outcomes in late life and future studies may lead to novel intervention targets.
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest and Sources of Funding: None.
References
- 1.Bäckhed F, Ley RE, Sonnenburg JL, Peterson DA, Gordon JI (2005). Host-bacterial mutualism in the human intestine. Science, 307(5717), 1915–1920. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Bajaj JS, Ahluwalia V, Steinberg JL, Hobgood S, Boling PA, Godschalk M, Wade JB (2016). Elderly patients have an altered gut-brain axis regardless of the presence of cirrhosis. Scientific Reports, 6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Brandt J (1991). The Hopkins Verbal Learning Test: Development of a new memory test with six equivalent forms. Clinical Neuropsychologist, 5, 125–142. [Google Scholar]
- 4.Bruce-Keller AJ, Salbaum JM, Luo M, Blanchard E, Taylor CM, Welsh DA, Berthoud HR (2015). Obese-type gut microbiota induce neurobehavioral changes in the absence of obesity. Biological Psychiatry, 77(7), 607–615. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Desbonnet L, Carke G, Traplin A, O’Sullivan O, Crispie F, Moloney RD, Cryan JF (2015). Microbiota depletion from early adolescence in mice: Implications for brain and behaviour. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity, 48, 165–173. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Dikmen SS, Heaton RK, Grant I, & Temkin NR (1999). Test-retest reliability and practice effects of expanded Halstead-Reitan Neuropsychological Test Battery. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 5, 346–356. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Dubois B, Slachevsky A, Litvan I, Pillon B (2000). The FAB: a Frontal Assessment Battery at bedside. Neurology, 55(11), 1621–1626. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Folstein MF, Folstein SF, & McHugh PR (1975). “Mini-Mental State”: A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12(3), 189–198. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Golden JC, Freshwater SM (2002). Stroop Color and Word Test: Revised examiner’s manual. Wood Dale, IL: Stoelting Co. [Google Scholar]
- 10.Human Microbiome Project Consortium. (2012). A framework for human microbiome research. Nature, 486(7402), 215–221. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Lezak MD, Howieson DB, Bigler ED, Tranel D (2012). Neuropsychological Assessment (5th ed.). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- 12.Loring DW, Martin RC, Meador KJ, Lee GP Psychometric construction of the Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure: Methodological considerations and interrater reliability. (1990). Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 5(1), 1–14. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Magnusson KR, Hauck L, Jeffrey BM, Elias V, Humphrey A, Nath R, Bermudez LE (2015). Relationships between diet-related changes in the gut microbiome and cognitive flexibility. Neuroscience, 300, 128–140. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Mulligan AA, Luben RN, Bhaniani A, Parry-Smith DJ, O’Conner L, Khawaja AP, ... Khaw K (2014). A new tool for converting food frequency questionnaire data into nutrient and food group values: FETA research methods and availability. BMJ Open, 4(3), :e004503. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Palmer BW, Boone KB, Lesser IM, & Wohl MA (1998). Base rates of “impaired” neuropsychological test performance among healthy older adults. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 13(6), 503–511. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Sarkar A, Lehto SM, Harty S, Dinan TG, Cryan JF, & Burnet PW (2016). Psychobiotics and the Manipulation of Bacteria–Gut–Brain Signals. Trends in Neurosciences, 39(11), 763–781. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Sherwin E, Sandhu KV, Dinan TG, & Cryan JF (2016). May the force be with you: the light and dark sides of the microbiota–gut–brain axis in neuropsychiatry. CNS Drugs, 30(11), 1019–1041. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Tilg H, Kaser A (2011). Gut microbiome, obesity, and metabolic dysfunction. Journal of Clinical Investigation, 121(6), 2126–2132. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Topolski TD, LoGerfo J, Patrick DL, Williams B, Walwick J, Patrick MMB (2006). The Rapid Assessment of Physical Activity (RAPA) Among Older Adults. Preventing Chronic, 3(4), A118. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Trollor JN, Smith E, Agars E, Kuan SA, Baune BT, Campbell L, Brodaty H (2012). The association between systemic inflammation and cognitive performance in the elderly: the Sydney Memory and Ageing Study. Age, 34, 1295–1308. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]