Abstract
Understanding health requires more than knowledge of the genome. Environmental factors regulate gene function through epigenetics. Collectively, environmental exposures have been called the “exposome.” Caregivers are instrumental in shaping exposures in a child's initial years. Maternal dietary patterns, physical activity, degree of weight gain, and body composition while pregnant will influence not only fetal growth, but also the infant's metabolic response to nutrients and energy. Maternal over- or underweight, excess caloric intake, nutrient imbalances, glucose dysregulation, and presence of chronic inflammatory states have been shown to establish risk for many later chronic diseases. During the period from birth to age 3 y, when the infant's metabolic rate is high and synaptogenesis and myelination of the brain are occurring extremely rapidly, the infant is especially prone to damaging effects from nutrient imbalances. During this period, the infant changes from a purely milk-based diet to one including a wide variety of foods. The process, timing, quality, and ultimate dietary pattern acquired are a direct outcome of the caregiver-infant feeding relationship, with potentially lifelong consequences. More research on how meal time interactions shape food acceptance is needed to avoid eating patterns that augment existing disease risk. Traditional clinical trials in nutrition, meant to isolate single factors for study, are inadequate to study the highly interconnected realm of environment-gene interactions in early life. Novel technologies are being used to gather broad exposure data on disparate populations, employing pioneering statistical approaches and correlations applied specifically to the individual, based on their genetic make-up and unique environmental experiences.
Keywords: environment-gene interaction, maternal weight gain, maternal obesity, placenta, brain development, early childhood, childhood obesity
Introduction
Exposures during early life shape health behaviors, but these exposures can also be responsible for influence at a much deeper level. The contemporary view of nutrition in health is one that encompasses environment-gene interconnections. In this concept, each nutrient, through its wide variety of actions at the molecular level, contributes to the total system, keeping it poised to adapt to perturbations and return the body to a state of homeostasis. This casts nutrition in personal terms. It is not whether a dietary pattern or an individual nutrient, such as sodium or sugar, is broadly recommended for all humans, but rather whether it is optimal for helping an individual reach their health goals at this moment in their life. Based on mounting clinical evidence, more recent versions of the Dietary Guidelines for Americans have embraced the broad health benefits of a nutritious dietary pattern. The role of nutrition is to ensure that cellular networks are fully supplied with sufficient energy and nutrients to optimize their flexibility. Nutritional sufficiency, then, is defined by whether the collective physiologic response to the nutrient flow can meet an individual's ever-changing physical and psychologic demands. In order to capture an individual's
genetic-environmental response to health risk, there is a need to overcome the challenges found in molecular nutrition research as well as a need to make assessments on a system-wide basis instead of individual cause-and-effect trials (1).
Around 50 y after the discovery of the structure of DNA, the human genome sequence was fully characterized. Yet, for all its promise, knowledge of the genetic code has afforded only a limited ability to predict the risk of a complex disease. Environmental factors that influence gene function have been shown to be a powerful force, but one that is even more challenging to study. Environmental exposures, ranging from the prenatal to the geriatric phase of life, can alter gene expression and, through it, human health (2). Understanding disease requires identification of the mechanisms through which environmental influences alter cellular responses contributing to pathology (3). Environmental influences occur in several overlapping domains: the broad external environment (community, climate, social, financial), more specific environmental hazards (sun exposure, environmental toxicants, alcohol, tobacco, nutrient deficiencies, lifestyle), and the internal physiologic environment (cardiometabolic activity, stress response, immune and inflammatory systems, hormonal communication, the commensal microbiota). Collectively, these environmental exposures have been termed “the Exposome,” representing the nongenetic counterpart to the genome (2–4). Describing the “exposome” is only the first step in characterizing the system. The fact that lifelong exposome-genome interactions accumulate means that we are faced with the principle of “unique disease” (3, 5). This principle states that any disease or disorder will manifest uniquely in everyone, because of their personal genetic make-up coupled with their history of exposures during critical periods. Although delineating the specific way in which each discrete disease will act in different individuals may seem like a daunting task, it is a line of research that is already well underway. Studies on the epigenome, transcriptome, proteome, metabolome, microbiome, and interactome hold promise for understanding the range of potential reactions that a disease can engender within a population. With these data, disease can be characterized in relation to the external macroenvironment alongside the cellular microenvironment.
The dichotomy between genes and environment is now recognized to be an ever-changing complex interplay of nature via nurture. The developing fetus, neonate, and infant are especially susceptible to environmental influences, and it is here that the gene × environment interaction is in full display. The physical and emotional environments that surround and engage the mother directly mold her child's lifelong potential. The preconception period within the human life course is the first to manifest environment-gene interactions, and these affect the molecular on-off switches that control DNA expression. Nongenetic factors that control gene expression in a stable manner are termed “epigenetic.” Epigenetic mechanisms are fundamental to expressing phenotypically different states from a single genetic blueprint, as typified by lineage commitment and differentiation from pluripotent stem cells. Epigenetic mechanisms also serve as an interface between environmental and exogenous stimuli and cellular responses, and eventually the phenotypes they confer. Epigenetic forces may involve alterations in the DNA methylation patterns at cytosine, post-transcriptional modifications of histones, around which the DNA is wrapped and via regulation of noncoding RNA. These chemical changes alter the type, timing, and extent of protein production by genes. Maternal diet and health condition, as well as the quality and balance of the child's diet during the first years after birth, contribute not only to the child's physical habitus and well-being, but also fuels a unique neurocognitive expansion on which their future academic, social, and behavioral success will be based (6). Given these important outcomes, research has sought specific ways to ensure optimal environments and experiences for the developing child. In this article, we will examine examples of what is known and unknown in terms of the biological and environmental nutritional forces that shape the fetus, neonate, infant, and child in the early years (Table 1) (7, 8). Illustrations of the way that early experiences affect lifelong health risk will be presented in 4 areas: in utero and developmental programming of metabolism, neurodevelopment and cognition, postnatal behaviors around food choice, and a “Future Vision of the Exposome.”
TABLE 1.
Effects of maternal nutrition and weight on fetal development |
Specific molecular factors involved in fetal programming |
The influence of maternal dietary pattern on the offspring health |
Diet quality and individual micro- and/or macronutrients during critical windows in pregnancy on offspring health outcomes |
The impact of obesity in the preconception phase vs. from excess gestational weight gain on glucose-insulin axis perturbations |
The effect the inflammatory response has in programming cardiometabolic risks |
Early nutrition, brain structure, and cognitive development |
The impact of diet quality on cognitive development in infants and toddlers |
The identification of epigenetic mechanisms by which acute changes in dietary behavior affect cognitive function in children and teens |
The identification of key factors that may protect the developing child and turn early cognitive “potential” into lifelong social, emotional, and academic skills |
Early life environmental exposures and eating behavior |
Longitudinal studies on the acquisition of dietary habits in the first years of life are needed to establish tailored interventions |
Data documenting successful strategies to help inform about when and how less nutritious eating patterns become entrenched |
Deeper understanding of child development and developmental norms in response to food behavior (neophobia, food jags, increasing selectivity with age, strong preferences for the same foods at each meal, and shifting food preferences) |
Microbiome for childhood health |
Characterization of the exposome |
The role of the microbiome on metabolic and physiologic processes that comprise the maternal-infant feeding relationship |
A vision of the future |
Methods to translate personal longitudinal health data into practical personal dietary advice |
Effects of Maternal Nutrition and Weight on Fetal Development
The co-occurrence of obesity, hunger, and malnutrition around the world complicates public health policy. Worldwide, the overweight population now exceeds the underweight population (9). Cardiometabolic diseases, such as heart disease, dyslipidemia, type 2 diabetes, metabolic syndrome, and fatty liver are diagnosed not only in adults and seniors, but also in children and adolescents. Unraveling the intersecting circumstances that result in obesity will require a better understanding of how an individual's environmental exposures create or mitigate risk. Increasingly, a systems perspective has been invoked that examines layered exposures to food quality, to agricultural practices, to community and social factors, and to personal and family lifestyle (10). The identification of “epigenetic” forces—environmentally triggered DNA modulations that adapt gene expression to the environment—has expanded the role of genes in the development of obesity and its complications to include a more complex perspective (11).
The first awareness of the role of maternal nutritional status on adult health began with studies on the effects of malnutrition during pregnancy. In the late 20th century, the developmental origins of the health and disease hypothesis were advanced based on the finding that undernutrition during critical periods in utero permanently altered the development of the fetus, including structure, function, weight, and metabolism (12–14). Since those initial studies, a wide variety of environmental factors occurring during pregnancy have been identified, ranging from toxins and chemicals to diseases and depressive symptoms, all of which raised the risk for health and mental health disorders in adulthood.
Among the most keenly studied and reproducible outcomes have been the mechanisms that link maternal body composition to the child's risk for future obesity (15–17). In utero exposure to maternal obesity and high-fat diets in rodent models has illuminated the extent to which preconception maternal body habitus, excess gestational weight gain, and dietary imbalances can lead to a “hyper-responsive infant”; that is, an infant prone to accumulate excess body fat, fatty liver, and metabolic derangement when exposed to postnatal high-calorie or high-fat feeding (18, 19). This risk is associated with alterations in a multitude of metabolic processes, including impaired glucose homeostasis and insulin sensitivity (20, 21), lower skeletal muscle mass (22), altered appetite and food preferences (23), and epigenetic changes in DNA methylation patterns that favor adipogenesis (24). In experimental models, accumulation of ectopic lipids in the liver (25, 26), increased lipogenesis in white adipose tissue (24), and a greater tendency for stem cells to differentiate into adipocytes (24) have all been noted. Similarly, alterations in pathways regulating lipid oxidation and basal energy expenditure (such as PPAR-α, adenosine monophosphate protein kinase and insulin signaling) in metabolically important tissues such as liver, muscle, and adipose tissue create a milieu that favors storage over oxidation (27–29).
Human studies, similarly, have shown that babies born to obese mothers, even in the absence of glycemic disturbance, have greater adiposity and a higher risk of insulin resistance (30, 31). With >60% of women in the United States overweight at the time of conception (32) and with steadily rising rates of weight gain and gestational diabetes during pregnancy, poor nutrition puts both the fetus and the mother at risk. In the face of excess energy, the composition of the fetus changes quickly, with it developing a higher fat mass, insulin resistance, and excess growth (33). Meta-analysis has confirmed the association between maternal BMI and the risk for high gestation–weight babies (34), which, in turn, is associated with higher cesarean delivery rates and complications during delivery. As early as 2–4 y of age, obesity risk is doubled, and this continues into adolescence and young adulthood, as shown in several studies (30, 35, 36). Likewise, cardiometabolic risk markers for dyslipidemia, glucose-insulin dysregulation, and diminished hunger-satiety control are also closely associated with maternal obesity and gestational diabetes.
In the Beginnings Study, a USDA-funded prospective observational study tracking the growth and development of children from 2 mo to 6 y of age, 325 infants born to normal weight, overweight, or obese mothers were followed longitudinally (37). By school age, significantly higher BMI z-scores and percentage of body fat were seen in children of obese mothers. Differences became apparent after age 2–3 y, with boys showing a more striking response than girls. Another study showed the relation between maternal adiposity and child obesity in children born to morbidly obese women before and after they had undergone bariatric surgery. Those infants before the intervention had a significantly higher risk for later obesity than did their siblings born after the surgery-induced maternal weight loss (38, 39).
What are the mechanisms underlying fetal programming of obesity? Maternal obesity, diet, and the resulting metabolic milieu, including the lipids and nutrients delivered to the developing fetus, can alter gene expression. During critical windows of development, external events directly shape every cell, tissue, and organ under development. In total, these adjustments reset the fetal—and later child and adult—metabolic response. Gene expression responds to environmental cues very quickly. Shifting the maternal diet toward one that is high in fat intake upregulates fetal inflammatory markers as early as the oocyte and embryo stages in animal models (40). The placenta, which senses nutrient flow, adjusts to the needs of the fetus (41). The placenta has also shown a capacity not only to monitor nutrients, but also to produce inflammatory compounds and lipotoxic mediators in response to the type and quantity of circulating nutrients, which further modifies the uterine environment (42–44). It is possible that a proinflammatory environment and pathways sensitive to excess nutrition in the peri- and postconception environments serve to regulate systems that promote adiposity in the offspring. The specific molecular factors involved are yet to be fully elucidated.
The specific dietary choices of the mother may influence epigenetic mechanisms. Most of the available evidence comes from rodent and nonhuman primate studies on the effects of a few dietary components, including folic acid, fructose, long-chain PUFAs, and protein content. However, the mechanisms that underpin cardiometabolic risk are being targeted in several human studies, as well. The Healthy Start Study has shown a strong association between maternal insulin resistance and late-term circulating glucose concentrations with the development of the neonatal fat mass, independent of maternal preconception BMI (45, 46). The ongoing USDA-funded Glowing Study has been designed to follow 320 human maternal-infant pairs from early pregnancy through to age 2 y, specifically examining the interconnected nature of maternal weight, body composition, dietary intake, and energy expenditure on adiposity, weight gain trajectory, and fat oxidation in the child.
Despite steady progress in understanding pivotal links between maternal and offspring health, several knowledge gaps need to be addressed in this area. These include a better understanding of the influence of maternal dietary patterns, diet quality, and individual micronutrients and/or macronutrients during critical windows in pregnancy on offspring health outcomes. How does obesity in the preconception phase differ from excess gestational weight gain in terms of impact on glucose-insulin axis perturbations? Is fat distribution a key factor? After birth, are there postnatal factors that could be manipulated to lower risk, such as the effect of the microbiome on metabolic and physiologic processes, or the social-emotional-behavioral conditions that comprise the maternal-infant feeding relationship? What is the combined effect of diet quality, nutrient components, physical activity, and fitness? To what extent is the inflammatory response central to programming cardiometabolic risks?
Early Nutrition, Brain Structure, and Cognitive Development
In utero and early life factors also program and impact systems beyond adiposity, including the brain and neurocognitive development. Mental health and wider health risks resulting from gene-epigene interactions accumulate over time. As in pregnancy, postnatal early life exposures and experiences are the foundations on which a child's future social, emotional, cognitive, and motor skills rest (47). Like other major organs of the body, the brain and neurodevelopment are strongly affected by maternal nutrition during pregnancy (48, 49). Neural tube formation, neurogenesis, neuron migration, programmed cell death, the expansion of the nervous system, and the structural coordination between regions of the brain occur sequentially over the 40 wk of gestation (50, 51). By parturition, the neonate has developed >85 billion neurons, which, with the exception of 2 areas (the hippocampus and the cerebellum), amount to the infant's total for life (52–54). Brain imaging studies have shown that maternal obesity is associated with decreases in the white matter content of the central nervous system in several different regions of the neonatal brain (55). Besides obesity, nutrient deficiencies from an unbalanced maternal diet, a persistent high-risk metabolic and inflammatory state, and many other deleterious exposures that occur during sensitive periods can negatively influence fetal brain structure and function. For instance, not only diet quality (56, 57), regular physical activity (58), and body composition (59) affect the fetal brain; so, too, do psychological stresses (60), social interactions (61), daily routines (62, 63), sleep (64, 65), chemical toxins (66), emotional and sensory stimulation (61, 67), financial status (68), family structure (69), and the child's experiences with acute and chronic diseases (70–72).
There are sensitive periods between birth and 3 y of age during which the brain's structure and function greatly expands, becoming more coordinated and refined (57, 73). From birth, the brain doubles in size by year 1 and triples in size by year 3, nearly to complete adult size, as a result of increasing myelination and synaptic connections, spurred on by the infant's sensory-motor experiences (74, 75). Play, nonverbal expression, acquisition of verbal language, and routine problem-solving stimulate the formation of synaptic linkages. These form the basis for all future cognitive, social, and behavioral performance. Synaptic connections are formed at 700 connections/s during this crucial period. Arborization reaches a lifetime maximum around age 3 y, after which weak or unused synapses are “pruned back,” leaving only 50% by young adulthood (75). For the child in a stimulating, nurturing, and healthful environment, this results in a well-structured, highly responsive brain built on early exploration and expanded through the child's experiences. However, for those children who are not raised in such a stimulating and supportive environment, those who lack the kind of constant verbal and emotional exchange that anchors basic language and literacy skills, or those who face persistent or severe adverse events in early life, the brain displays a very different, attenuated capacity that can potentially leave the child at a cognitive disadvantage for life (73, 76).
Scientists have used 2 primary methods for studying brain structure and function noninvasively. Brain imaging (MRI) procedures are used to quantify brain structural integrity and connectivity, and functional MRI is used for observing brain activation during the performance of specific tasks (77). Imaging procedures require the absence of movement to obtain acceptable data, making such procedures difficult to use successfully in active infants and young children. Consequently, electroencephalogram measures, which are more tolerant to movement, have been the primary source of information describing brain-behavior relations in early life. During the infancy-early childhood period there is rapid development of processes involving attention, arousal, sensory-motor skills, emotion, working memory, social orientation, and language acquisition. Optimal operation, maintenance, and repair of the growing brain require a steady stream of nutrients and energy.
Nutrition continues to be a central environmental factor throughout childhood. Infants and children have a metabolic rate >2.5 times higher than in the adult (78, 79). Their metabolic demands make them sensitive to variations in energy or nutrient flow during even short periods of fasting. Recognition of this vulnerability has emphasized the importance of breakfast, since this meal follows the longest naturally occurring fast that occurs daily. Academic performance among school-age children is better in those who regularly eat breakfast (80, 81), but there have been few studies using measures of brain activity—and none using imaging procedures—to describe the neural correlates associated with eating or skipping breakfast in young children. Electroencephalogram studies have shown changes in brain activity, indicating increased attention and memory processes, resulting in faster decision making and more efficient mental math in children fed compared with those who skipped breakfast (82, 83). Decision making and mental arithmetic rely on processes known as “executive functions.” These functions, which are largely controlled by the frontal lobe (84–86), involve processes contributing to self-regulation and mental flexibility, such as attention, emotional control, and memory (87).
Sensitivity of executive functions to even transient variations in morning nutrition suggest that the cognitive deficits resulting from meal skipping or chronic poor diet are likely to be far worse for the obese child (88). Imaging studies have shown that both gray and white matter are diminished among obese children (89), and that frontal brain activation is reduced when the obese child is given tasks involving memory, attention, and verbal and motor skills that challenge the prefrontal cortex (90, 91). The obese population also shows impaired outcomes on a variety of tests measuring executive function (92). Physical exercise, similar to breakfast consumption, has been shown to be capable of improving these executive skills (93–95), even in obese children (90, 91). Such studies illustrate how one's prior “programmed” risks associated with obesity may interact with more proximate environmental exposures to alter the immediate cardiometabolic state and affect cognitive function.
More research on the environment-gene relations that result from nutritional status and body composition is needed. The individual and collective components of diet, along with the benefits of regular physical activity, may present ways to alleviate the detrimental effects of weight, adiposity, or other programmed risk. Studies on diet quality and cognitive development in infants and toddlers are just beginning. Identification of the epigenetic mechanisms by which acute changes in dietary behavior affect cognitive function in children and teens is needed. An integrated approach that combines studies of brain structure, function, environmental exposures, and personal variables will be necessary to identify which key factors may protect the developing child and turn early cognitive potential into lifelong social, emotional, and academic skills.
Early Life Environmental Exposures and Eating Behavior
Diet quality in early childhood is also an important epigenetic force, given the intense increases in height and weight, ongoing organic development, and the extreme expansion of the brain during this time. Just as the maternal diet and obesity can affect the offspring's weight and brain development, so, too, can it start to shape food preferences and dietary habits, influencing the child's health and development well into the future. Establishing nutrient-rich dietary habits has proved challenging considering the complexity of human behavior, which often is resistant to change despite proven benefits. Research has shown the importance of initial experiences with food on later food preferences and dietary habits (96, 97). Typically, nonmilk foods offered during the second 6 mo of life represent the child's most intensive period of exposure to the many tastes, textures, and colors of foods and beverages. But it is not the first exposure. Studies show that both the fetus and the neonate can experience flavors through swallowed amniotic fluid and through breastmilk, both of which reflect components of the mother's diet, which are hypothesized to influence the infant food acceptance and preference (96, 98). However, given that most adults in the United States fail to achieve the recommended intake of fruit and vegetables, particularly low-income pregnant and lactating women, the potential for maternal diets to positively influence infant diet quality is not maximized (99, 100). Parental diet quality becomes an even more crucial factor in the second year of a child's life, when the transition from breastmilk- or formula-based feeding to family foods occurs.
Food preferences established in the first 3 y of life tend to persist (97, 101). Although children are more open to new food experiences during the first years, the daily parent-child negotiation around meals and snacks often prevents the opportunity to promote varied dietary preferences. After instilling an acceptance of fruit and vegetable intake using infant food during the first year, intake of both falls quickly in the second year, not only in total consumption, but also in variety (102). Nearly one-third of toddlers and preschoolers eat no vegetables regularly (103). Irrespective of race, ethnicity, or socioeconomic status, intake in the United States consistently fails to achieve the recommendations of the Dietary Guidelines for Americans for vegetables and fruits (104). Snacks often introduce high-calorie, low-nutrient products that become strongly preferred by children already primed to like sweet, salty, and fatty items—a preference that has been honed throughout human evolution (104, 105). Conversely, bitter and sour, the attributes of vegetables, are more likely to be resisted (98, 106). These less-nutritious dietary patterns that do not meet dietary recommendations, especially for the food groups that are encouraged (fruits, vegetables, whole wheat, and dairy), put our population at an increased risk for consuming suboptimal amounts of essential nutrients, which in turn increases the risk for developing diet-related health concerns.
What is the pathway to broad food acceptance that establishes a lifelong high-quality dietary pattern? There are several factors in play, some individual to the child, such as neophobia (i.e., the extent to which children display resistance to trying new foods) (107), temperament (108), and intensity of response to bitter (109), and others related to the environment, such as education, income, food insecurity, and family norms (110). Children prefer what they know; that is, they tend to accept what is familiar and routine (111). Taste can be “trained” through familiarity or mere exposure (112–116). This seemingly simple principle requires that caregivers show patience and persistence in their efforts to shape food preferences and in their willingness to continue to offer and model consumption of previously rejected foods. The number of exposures to induce acceptance varies in the literature, with reports ranging from 8 to >15 times (117). Recent studies have shown a greater likelihood of establishing food acceptance through repeated exposure in children <24 mo old than in older children, emphasizing early life as a pivotal period (118). Pairing preferred flavors with new flavors can encourage acceptance, but the evidence for the effectiveness of this strategy is inconsistent (115, 116, 119, 120). Current resources have not been successful at guiding parental understanding of appropriate portion sizes, ways to meet recommended servings per day, or use of nutrient-rich options from each of the 5 food groups to improve children's intake at meals and in snacks. Repeated exposure takes patience, something that may arise from the parent's level of understanding of the developmental norms matching the child's age and a trust in the child's ability to eat sufficiently to meet the energy needs for appropriate growth.
Parents who label their child a “picky eater” report halting presentation of rejected foods after only 3–5 tries, which may be insufficient to give the child enough experience to overcome their initial neophobic response (121, 122). They also tend to attribute their child's limited food acceptance to inheritance rather than to malleable behavior (123). Bribing or pressuring the child to eat, along with a permissive feeding style of catering to the child, fosters rejection, as does failure to create a supportive emotional milieu around mealtimes (124). Conversely, successful behavioral strategies include encouragement, task-centered praise, and monitoring of eating progress (125–128). Setting high expectations, establishing clear structure, the presence of family rules, and modeling by family members all have reinforcing benefits (129, 130).
The emotional environment surrounding the parent-child feeding relationship may be important in shaping feeding behavior, but remains largely unstudied (130, 131). Breastfeeding promotes a close physical-emotional attachment that transcends simply the supplying of food (132, 133). This feeding principle, termed “responsive feeding,” embodies a reciprocal relationship between the child and the caregiver in which the child's cues are promptly met with contingent caregiver responses that support the child's development of healthy eating behaviors (133, 134).
In the Totality of Early Environmental Exposures: The Body's Other Biome
A recent corollary to the epigenetic manipulation of gene expression in animals and humans has been the expanding literature on the ways that our diet, diseases, and exposures act as inputs to alter the gut microbiome. Our microbiome serves as another epigenetic influence on a child's genetic make-up. In the first years of life, the human gut is colonized by >1000 bacterial species, which encode as many as 5–10 million genes, compared with our own 20,000 genes. The microbiome illustrates a classic symbiotic relationship, one that closely intertwines the collective physiology of the bacterial population with our own. The presence of the microbiome greatly amplifies the complexity of a system-wide or exposome-wide approach to human health. The adaptive flexibility of the microbiome to rebound from environmental perturbations mirrors many of our own body's adaptations. Consider the sum of the body's response to a single invading enteric pathogen, which triggers reactions across the highly interconnected processes that control inflammation, immunity, nervous system function, and epithelial absorptive capacity in the gut. The resulting vomiting, diarrhea, loss of appetite, and dehydration dramatically diminish the amount of bacteria in the gut. Prolonged illness or complications might impede homeostasis, particularly if antibiotics are prescribed. Adaptations within the microbial system might be detrimental, chronically altering the gut milieu. Opportunistic overgrowth of other pathogens, such as Clostridia difficile, may further compromise the stability of the microbiome as well as the integrity of the gastrointestinal tract itself, causing a cascade of adaptations across physiologic and metabolic systems.
Can both systems, ours and the microbiome's, be strengthened through diet to enhance our health, defined as our ability to resist stresses and maintain homeostasis? To approach this question, we need to use novel 21st-entury technologies and innovations that can characterize and model the highly complicated relationships that involve humans within the many environments in which we live and grow. Complete characterizations of the human genome and the gut microbial genome have been undertaken successfully. Characterization of the exposome, although complex, is a pressing research endeavor (2).
The Search for Novel Methods to Study the Exposome-Genome Interaction
Scientists are designing projects to lay the groundwork. Among the most established are those examining environmental chemicals, occupational hazards, and their effect on health. Multiple agencies acting in concert are pooling large data sets and building platforms for analysis in such projects as Exposure21, Health and Environment-wide Associations based on Large population Surveys (HEALS), and the Exposomics Consortium. A forward-looking report on exposure science was published by the National Academies of Sciences and funded by the National Institute for Environmental Health Services and the US Environmental Protection Agency, titled “Exposure Science in the 21st Century: A Vision and A Strategy” (8). The report called for multiagency cooperation to gather pooled toxicity testing data, build platforms to handle massive data waves, develop novel statistical approaches, and design analysis paradigms that are publicly available to encourage information sharing. Many emerging research technologies have been cited as building blocks for such an undertaking, including microfluidics, nanotechnologies, mass spectrometry, capillary lab-on-a-chip technology for chemical analysis, LC/tandem MS to measure protein adducts (chemicals bonded to protein structures), high-tech sensor technology, global positioning systems, genomic techniques, and informatics. Many will provide the biomarkers needed to quantify chemical exposures. The report from the National Academy of Sciences also urged that the diverse information gleaned from these large-scale toxicology studies be applied not only to health care, but also to environmental regulation, urban and ecosystem planning, and disaster management.
Among the several nascent studies being launched on the exposome, early life exposures and their resulting health outcomes are being taken up in the Human Early-Life Exposome (HELIX) project, centered in Barcelona, but comprised of 6 birth cohort studies in Europe (135). The project will follow >32,000 mother-child pairs to examine chemical and physical environmental factors. A subset of 1200 dyads will have repeated biomarkers assessed, using smart phones to quantify mobility, physical activity, and personal exposures, coupled with -omics technologies to provide molecular profiles of each subject. Advanced statistical methods will correlate fetal and child growth, exercise, obesity, neurodevelopment, health, and respiratory outcomes (e.g., asthma) to begin to characterize exposure-response relations.
The intensity or persistence of certain stress events can impose permanent changes in the body, which can change the way that our body subsequently reacts to adversity. Significant environmental challenges have the capacity to alter not only gene expression, but also metabolic, neuronal, immunologic, and hormonal responses, which can alter the trajectory of an individual's life. That is clear from the findings on the maternal-child relationship in gestation. Life-changing exposures are also well documented in studies on the effects of severe adverse events in early childhood, permanently altering the capabilities of the rapidly growing infant brain (136). Adult outcomes as disparate as emotional behavior, responsiveness to stress, resilience, academic success, and peer social interactions have been associated with adverse childhood events accumulated during the critical window of early life.
If the concept of health is represented by the flexibility, robustness, and durability of the body and mind, then a fundamental research question can now be stated: can we enhance a system's adaptability to environmental exposures by challenging it (137)? That is, are there ways of “exercising” the body's homeostatic networks to increase their resilience to perturbations of all kinds? Mounting evidence suggests that many different types of stimuli (antigen, pathogen, metabolite, chemical) can serve to stimulate and benefit existing physiologic processes (138, 139). It is conceivable that adjustments in the child's socio-ecological connectedness, their diet quality, hygiene, and physical activity might be manipulated to improve their system's “flexibility,” with significant practical consequences for the growing fetus and child.
Complex Diseases in the Light of Gene × Exposome Interaction
The Human Genome Project was an investment of billions, but it in turn has reaped strong economic benefits. So, too, does the Exposome Project offer enormous economic and societal return on investment, but will require a similar financial and scientific commitment (7). Researchers are already compiling longitudinal and extensive data from individuals via the expanding electronic health records system, which has the capacity to outline the variations of “normal” during times of health and disease. Creating a screening tool derived from these data that compares one's personal biomarkers as well as those of the microbiome against that of the healthy population, for instance, offers an opportunity to identify deviations and potentially to adjust them. Consider assaying an individual's microbiome, comparing it with population norms, and, by undertaking specific adjustments to the dietary pattern, augmenting the symbiotic relationship with the human organism. A similar example might be a tool that quantifies the responsiveness of one's inflammatory system to a variety of stimuli. Such challenges might illuminate ways to modulate the inflammatory function for the prevention of disease or its rapid mitigation, allowing faster healing.
A Vision of Future Nutrition and Health
Rather than a one-size-fits-all dietary recommendation such as those of the past, a comprehensive health model, complete with challenges and biomarker-based assays, holds promise for creating individualized dietary recommendations that can help consumers develop an ideal dietary pattern to promote their personal health. New initiatives have begun to recruit consumers into “health data cooperatives,” which are legal entities pooling system-wide health data from dozens of disparate sources, ranging from doctors and insurance files to government, school, and health service provider records (140). What makes this type of amalgamated data repository unique is that the individual citizen-consumers own their own data. These are the first seeds of truly personalized health care. When a child is born later in the 21st century, each parent may be handed a personal biological passport that, unlike the traditional infant book or electronic health supervision visit handout, not only gathers all the child's health information in one place, but also guides parental decisions, based on their child's gene-environment responsiveness and current health trajectory, proposing specific ways to use nutrition and other variables to “nourish” their child's optimal health for life.
Acknowledgements
The authors’ responsibilities were as follows— KS, RTP, SLJ, BvO, ED, and RM: wrote the article; RM: had primary responsibility for final content; and all authors: read and approved the final manuscript.
Notes
Published in a supplement to Current Developments in Nutrition. Presented at the conference, “Childhood Nutrition Research,” held in Little Rock, AR on 27–28 January 2016. The conference was organized by the National Dairy Council and was co-sponsored by the National Dairy Council and Arkansas Children's Nutrition Center. Supplement Coordinators for the supplement publication were Elieke Demmer and Emily Radlowski. Supplement coordinator disclosures: Both Dr. Demmer and Dr. Radlowski are employed by the National Dairy Council.
Supported by USDA-ARS Project 6026-51000-010-05S (to KS and RTP) and by The Cardinal Health Foundation (to RM).
Author disclosures: KS, RTP, BvO: no conflicts of interest. SLJ is funded for research by USDA, NIH, and The Sugar Association, and received honoraria from The Academy for Nutrition and Dietetics, the Idaho Academy of Nutrition and Dietetics, National Dairy Council. ED is an employee of National Dairy Council. RM is a consultant to Hass Avocado, Egg Nutrition Board, National Dairy Council, Sabra Dipping Co., and Dannon Co. Speaker's Bureau for Abbott Nutrition and American Dairy Association affiliates.
References
- 1. van Ommen B, Cavallieri D, Roche HM, Klein UI, Daniel H. The challenges for molecular nutrition research 4: the “nutritional systems biology level.” Genes Nutr 2008;3(3-4):107–13. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2. Miller GW, Jones DP. The nature of nurture: refining the definition of the exposome. Toxicol Sci 2014;137(1):1–2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3. Ogino S, Lochhead P, Chan AT, Nishihara R, Cho E, Wolpin BM, Meyerhardt JA, Meissner A, Schernhammer ES, Fuchs CS, et al. Molecular pathological epidemiology of epigenetics: emerging integrative science to analyze environment, host, and disease. Mod Pathol 2013;26(4):465–84. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4. Miller GW. The exposome: a primer. Amsterdam: Elsevier; 2013. [Google Scholar]
- 5. Patel CJ, Chen R, Kodama K, Ioannidis JP, Butte AJ. Systematic identification of interaction effects between genome-and environment-wide associations in type 2 diabetes mellitus. Hum Genet 2013;132(5):495–508. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6. National Scientific Council on the Developing Child. Early experiences can alter gene expression and affect long-term development. Working Paper No. 10. Available from: www.developingchild.harvard.edu.
- 7. Smith MT, Rappaport SM. Building exposure biology centers to put the E into “G x E” interaction studies. Environ Health Perspect 2009;117(8):A334. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8. Lioy PJ, Smith KR. A discussion of exposure science in the 21st century: a vision and a strategy. Environ Health Perspect 2013;121(4):405. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9. NCD Risk Factor Collaboration. Trends in adult body-mass index in 200 countries from 1975 to 2014: a pooled analysis of 1698 population-based measurement studies with 19·2 million participants. Lancet 2016;387(10026):1377–96. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10. Hammond RA. Complex systems modeling for obesity research. Prev Chronic Dis 2009;6(3):A97. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11. Waterland RA. Epigenetic mechanisms affecting regulation of energy balance: many questions, few answers. Ann Rev Nutr 2014;34:337–55. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12. Kubota T, Miyake K, Hariya N, Mochizuki K. Understanding the epigenetics of neurodevelopmental disorders and DOHaD. J Dev Orig Health Dis 2015;6(02):96–104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13. Barker DJ, Osmond C, Winter P, Margetts B, Simmonds SJ. Weight in infancy and death from ischaemic heart disease. Lancet 1989;334(8663):577–80. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14. Hales C, Barker D, Clark P, Cox L, Fall C, Osmond C, Winter PD. Fetal and infant growth and impaired glucose tolerance at age 64. BMJ 1991;303(6809):1019–22. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15. Dabelea D, Crume T. Maternal environment and the transgenerational cycle of obesity and diabetes. Diabetes 2011;60(7):1849–55. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16. Catalano PM. Obesity and pregnancy: the propagation of a viscous cycle? J Clin Endocrinol Metab 2003;88(8):3505–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17. King JC. Maternal obesity, metabolism, and pregnancy outcomes. Annu Rev Nutr 2006;26:271–91. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18. Heerwagen MJ, Miller MR, Barbour LA, Friedman JE. Maternal obesity and fetal metabolic programming: a fertile epigenetic soil. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol 2010;299(3):R711–R22. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19. Li M, Sloboda D, Vickers M. Maternal obesity and developmental programming of metabolic disorders in offspring: evidence from animal models. Exp Diabetes Res 2011;2011, doi: 10.1155/2011/592408. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20. Shankar K, Harrell A, Liu X, Gilchrist JM, Ronis MJ, Badger TM. Maternal obesity at conception programs obesity in the offspring. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol 2008;294(2):R528–R38. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21. Samuelsson A-M, Matthews PA, Argenton M, Christie MR, McConnell JM, Jansen EH, et al. Diet-induced obesity in female mice leads to offspring hyperphagia, adiposity, hypertension, and insulin resistance. A novel murine model of developmental programming. J Hypertens 2008;51(2):383–92. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22. Du M, Yan X, Tong JF, Zhao J, Zhu MJ. Maternal obesity, inflammation, and fetal skeletal muscle development. Biol Reprod 2010;82(1):4–12. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23. Bayol SA, Simbi BH, Stickland NC. A maternal cafeteria diet during gestation and lactation promotes adiposity and impairs skeletal muscle development and metabolism in rat offspring at weaning. J Physiol 2005;567(3):951–61. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24. Borengasser SJ, Zhong Y, Kang P, Lindsey F, Ronis MJ, Badger TM, Gomez-Acevedo H, Shankar K. Maternal obesity enhances white adipose tissue differentiation and alters genome-scale DNA methylation in male rat offspring. Endocrinology 2013;154(11):4113–25. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25. Oben JA, Mouralidarane A, Samuelsson A-M, Matthews PJ, Morgan ML, Mckee C, Soeda J, Fernandez-Twinn DS, Martin-Gronert MS, Ozanne SE, et al. Maternal obesity during pregnancy and lactation programs the development of offspring non-alcoholic fatty liver disease in mice. J Hepatol 2010;52(6):913–20. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26. McCurdy CE, Bishop JM, Williams SM, Grayson BE, Smith MS, Friedman JE, Grove KL. Maternal high-fat diet triggers lipotoxicity in the fetal livers of nonhuman primates. J Clin Investig 2009;119(2):323–35. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27. Borengasser SJ, Lau F, Kang P, Blackburn ML, Ronis MJ, Badger TM, Shankar K. Maternal obesity during gestation impairs fatty acid oxidation and mitochondrial SIRT3 expression in rat offspring at weaning. PloS one 2011;6(8):e24068. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28. Borengasser SJ, Kang P, Faske J, Gomez-Acevedo H, Blackburn ML, Badger TM, Shankar K. High fat diet and in utero exposure to maternal obesity disrupts circadian rhythm and leads to metabolic programming of liver in rat offspring. PloS one 2014;9(1):e84209. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29. Shankar K, Kang P, Harrell A, Zhong Y, Marecki JC, Ronis MJ, Badger TM. Maternal overweight programs insulin and adiponectin signaling in the offspring. Endocrinology 2010;151(6):2577–89. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30. Lawlor DA, Smith GD, O'Callaghan M, Alati R, Mamun AA, Williams GM, Najman JM. Epidemiologic evidence for the fetal overnutrition hypothesis: findings from the mater-university study of pregnancy and its outcomes. Am J Epidemiol 2007;165(4):418–24. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31. Catalano PM, McIntyre HD, Cruickshank JK, McCance DR, Dyer AR, Metzger BE, Lowe LP, Trimble ER, Coustan DR, Hadden DR, et al. The Hyperglycemia and Adverse Pregnancy Outcome Study Associations of GDM and obesity with pregnancy outcomes. Diabetes Care 2012;35(4):780–6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32. Ogden CL, Carroll MD, Flegal KM. Prevalence of obesity in the United States. J Am Med Assoc 2014;312(2):189–90. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33. Catalano PM, Presley L, Minium J, Hauguel-de Mouzon S. Fetuses of obese mothers develop insulin resistance in utero. Diabetes Care 2009;32(6):1076–80. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34. Yu Z, Han S, Zhu J, Sun X, Ji C, Guo X. Pre-pregnancy body mass index in relation to infant birth weight and offspring overweight/obesity: a systematic review and meta-analysis. PloS one 2013;8(4):e61627. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35. Whitaker RC. Predicting preschooler obesity at birth: the role of maternal obesity in early pregnancy. Pediatr 2004;114(1):e29–36. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36. Gale CR, Javaid MK, Robinson SM, Law CM, Godfrey KM, Cooper C. Maternal size in pregnancy and body composition in children. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 2007;92(10):3904–11. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37. Andres A, Hull HR, Shankar K, Casey PH, Cleves MA, Badger TM. Longitudinal body composition of children born to mothers with normal weight, overweight, and obesity. Obesity (Silver Spring) 2015;23(6):1252–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38. Smith J, Cianflone K, Biron S, Hould F, Lebel S, Marceau S, Lescelleur O, Biertho L, Simard S, Kral JG, et al. Effects of maternal surgical weight loss in mothers on intergenerational transmission of obesity. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 2009;94(11):4275–83. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39. Guénard F, Deshaies Y, Cianflone K, Kral JG, Marceau P, Vohl M-C. Differential methylation in glucoregulatory genes of offspring born before vs. after maternal gastrointestinal bypass surgery. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2013;110(28):11439–44. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40. Robker RL, Akison LK, Bennett BD, Thrupp PN, Chura LR, Russell DL, Lane M, Norman RJ. Obese women exhibit differences in ovarian metabolites, hormones, and gene expression compared with moderate-weight women. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 2009;94(5):1533–40. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41. Díaz P, Powell TL, Jansson T. The role of placental nutrient sensing in maternal-fetal resource allocation. Biol Reprod 2014;91(4):82. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42. Challier J, Basu S, Bintein T, Minium J, Hotmire K, Catalano P, et al. Obesity in pregnancy stimulates macrophage accumulation and inflammation in the placenta. Placenta 2008;29(3):274–81. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43. Basu S, Haghiac M, Surace P, Challier JC, Guerre‐Millo M, Singh K, Waters T, Minium J, Presley L, Catalano PM, et al. Pregravid obesity associates with increased maternal endotoxemia and metabolic inflammation. Obesity (Silver Spring) 2011;19(3):476–82. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44. Saben J, Lindsey F, Zhong Y, Thakali K, Badger T, Andres A, Gomez-Acevedo H, Shankar K. Maternal obesity is associated with a lipotoxic placental environment. Placenta 2014;35(3):171–7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45. Shapiro AL, Schmiege SJ, Brinton JT, Glueck D, Crume TL, Friedman JE, Dabelea D. Testing the fuel-mediated hypothesis: maternal insulin resistance and glucose mediate the association between maternal and neonatal adiposity, the Healthy Start study. Diabetologia 2015;58(5):937–41. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46. Crume TL, Shapiro AL, Brinton JT, Glueck DH, Martinez M, Kohn M, Harrod C, Friedman JE, Dabelea D. Maternal fuels and metabolic measures during pregnancy and neonatal body composition: the Healthy Start Study. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 2015;100(4):1672–80. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47. Fox NA, Rutter M. Introduction to the special section on the effects of early experience on development. Child Dev 2010;81(1):23–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48. Georgieff MK. Nutrition and the developing brain: nutrient priorities and measurement. Am J Clin Nutr 2007;85(2):614S–20S. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49. Geraghty AA, Lindsay KL, Alberdi G, McAuliffe FM, Gibney ER. Nutrition during pregnancy impacts offspring's epigenetic status—evidence from human and animal studies. Nutr Metab Insights 2015;8(Suppl 1):41. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50. Lenroot RK, Giedd JN. The structural development of the human brain as measured longitudinally with magnetic resonance imaging. In: Coch D, Dawson G, Fischer KW, editors. Human Behavior, Learning, and the Developing Brain: Atypical Development. Hove: Guilford Press; 2007. p. 50–73. [Google Scholar]
- 51. Stiles J, Jernigan TL. The basics of brain development. Neuropsychol Rev 2010;20(4):327–48. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52. Azevedo FA, Carvalho LR, Grinberg LT, Farfel JM, Ferretti RE, Leite RE, Lent R, Herculano-Houzel S. Equal numbers of neuronal and nonneuronal cells make the human brain an isometrically scaled‐up primate brain. J Comp Neurol 2009;513(5):532–41. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53. Knoth R, Singec I, Ditter M, Pantazis G, Capetian P, Meyer RP, et al. Murine features of neurogenesis in the human hippocampus across the lifespan from 0 to 100 years. PloS one 2010;5(1):e8809. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54. Walton R. Postnatal neurogenesis of mice, men, and macaques. Vet Pathol 2012;49(1):155–65. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55. Ou X, Thakali KM, Shankar K, Andres A, Badger TM. Maternal adiposity negatively influences infant brain white matter development. Obesity (Silver Spring) 2015;23(5):1047–54. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56. Fuglestad AJ, Kroupina MG, Johnson DE, Georgieff MK. Micronutrient status and neurodevelopment in internationally adopted children. Acta Paediatr 2016;105(2):e67–76. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57. Georgieff MK, Brunette KE, Tran PV. Early life nutrition and neural plasticity. Dev Psychopathol 2015;27(02):411–23. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58. Segalowitz SJ. Exercise and pediatric brain development: a call to action. Pediatr Exerc Sci 2016;28(2):217. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59. Galván M, Uauy R, López-Rodríguez G, Kain J. Association between childhood obesity, cognitive development, physical fitness and social-emotional wellbeing in a transitional economy. Ann Hum Biol 2014;41(2):101–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60. Blair C, Raver CC. Poverty, stress, and brain development: new directions for prevention and intervention. Acad Pediatr 2016;16(3):S30–S6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61. Marshall PJ, Fox NA, editors. The development of social engagement: neurobiological perspectives. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2005. [Google Scholar]
- 62. Jones BL, Fiese BH, Team TSK. Parent routines, child routines, and family demographics associated with obesity in parents and preschool-aged children. Front Psychol 2014;5, doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00374. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63. Zimmermann LK. The influence of chronotype in the daily lives of young children. Chronobiol Int 2016;33(3):268–79. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64. Kim TW, Jeong JH, Hong SC. The impact of sleep and circadian disturbance on hormones and metabolism. Int J Endocrinol 2015;2015, 10.1155/2015/591729. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65. Teff K, Silva C. Waking up to the importance of sleep and circadian rhythms for metabolic health: the need for in-depth phenotyping. Sleep 2014;38(12):1847–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66. Suades-González E, Gascon M, Guxens M, Sunyer J. Air pollution and neuropsychological development: a review of the latest evidence. Endocrinology 2015;156(10):3473–82. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67. Lickliter R. The integrated development of sensory organization. Clin Perinatol 2011;38(4):591–603. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68. Lawson GM, Duda JT, Avants BB, Wu J, Farah MJ. Associations between children's socioeconomic status and prefrontal cortical thickness. Dev Sci 2013;16(5):641–52. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69. American Academy of Pediatrics. Family pediatrics report of the task force on the family. Pediatrics 2003;111(Supplement 2):1541–71. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70. Augustine JM, Crosnoe RL, Gordon R. Early child care and illness among preschoolers. J Health Soc Behav 2013;54(3):315–34. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71. Fisher E, Law E, Palermo TM, Eccleston C. Psychological therapies (remotely delivered) for the management of chronic and recurrent pain in children and adolescents. Cochrane Database Syst Rev 2015;3, doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD011118.pub2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72. Rennick JE, Dougherty G, Chambers C, Stremler R, Childerhose JE, Stack DM, Harrison D, Campbell-Yeo M, Dryden-Palmer K, Zhang X, et al. Children's psychological and behavioral responses following pediatric intensive care unit hospitalization: the caring intensively study. BMC Pediatr 2014;14(1):276. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73. Werker JF, Hensch TK. Critical periods in speech perception: new directions. Annu Rev Psychol 2015;66(1):173. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74. Sanchez CE, Richards JE, Almli CR. Neurodevelopmental MRI brain templates for children from 2 weeks to 4 years of age. Dev Psychobiol 2012;54(1):77–91. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75. Tau GZ, Peterson BS. Normal development of brain circuits. Neuropsychopharmacology 2010;35(1):147–68. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76. Christensen DL, Schieve LA, Devine O, Drews-Botsch C. Socioeconomic status, child enrichment factors, and cognitive performance among preschool-age children: results from the Follow-Up of Growth and Development Experiences study. Res Dev Disabil 2014;35(7):1789–801. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77. Isaacs EB. Neuroimaging, a new tool for investigating the effects of early diet on cognitive and brain development. Front Hum Neurosci 2013;7, doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00445. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78. Chugani HT. A critical period of brain development: studies of cerebral glucose utilization with PET. Prev Med 1998;27(2):184–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79. Kennedy C, Sokoloff L. An adaptation of the nitrous oxide method to the study of the cerebral circulation in children; normal values for cerebral blood flow and cerebral metabolic rate in childhood. J Clin Invest 1957;36(7):1130. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80. Kleinman RE, Hall S, Green H, Korzec-Ramirez D, Patton K, Pagano ME, Murphy JM. Diet, breakfast, and academic performance in children. Ann Nutr Metab 2002;46(Suppl. 1):24–30. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81. Lopez-Sobaler A, Ortega R, Quintas M, Navia B, Requejo A. Relationship between habitual breakfast and intellectual performance (logical reasoning) in well-nourished schoolchildren of Madrid (Spain). Eur J Clin Nutr 2003;57:S49–53. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82. Pivik RT, Tennal KB, Chapman SD, Gu Y. Eating breakfast enhances the efficiency of neural networks engaged during mental arithmetic in school-aged children. Physiol Behav 2012;106:548–55. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83. Pivik R, Dykman R. Event-related variations in alpha band activity during an attentional task in preadolescents: effects of morning nutrition. Clin Neurophysiol 2007;118(3):615–32. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84. Fassbender C, Murphy K, Foxe J, Wylie G, Javitt D, Robertson I, Garavan H. A topography of executive functions and their interactions revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Cogn Brain Res 2004;20(2):132–43. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85. Lewis MD, Stieben J. Emotion regulation in the brain: conceptual issues and directions for developmental research. Child Dev 2004;75(2):371–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86. Schultz W. Predictive reward signal of dopamine neurons. J Neurophysiol 1998;80(1):1–27. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87. Diamond A. Executive functions. Ann Rev Psychol 2013;64:135. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88. Maffeis C, Fornari E, Surano M, Comencini E, Corradi M, Tommasi M, Fasan I, Cortese S. Breakfast skipping in prepubertal obese children: hormonal, metabolic and cognitive consequences. Eur J Clin Nutr 2012;66(3):314–21. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89. Ou X, Andres A, Pivik R, Cleves MA, Badger TM. Brain gray and white matter differences in healthy normal weight and obese children. J Magn Reson Imag 2015;42(5):1205–13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90. Davis CL, Tomporowski PD, McDowell JE, Austin BP, Miller PH, Yanasak NE, Allison JD, Naglieri JA. Exercise improves executive function and achievement and alters brain activation in overweight children: a randomized, controlled trial. Health Psychol 2011;30(1):91. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91. Krafft CE, Schwarz NF, Chi L, Weinberger AL, Schaeffer DJ, Pierce JE, Rodrigue JE, Yanasak NE, Miller PH, Tomporowski PD, et al. An 8‐month randomized controlled exercise trial alters brain activation during cognitive tasks in overweight children. Obesity (Silver Spring) 2014;22(1):232–42. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92. Smith E, Hay P, Campbell L, Trollor J. A review of the association between obesity and cognitive function across the lifespan: implications for novel approaches to prevention and treatment. Obes Rev 2011;12(9):740–55. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 93. Chaddock L, Hillman CH, Pontifex MB, Johnson CR, Raine LB, Kramer AF. Childhood aerobic fitness predicts cognitive performance one year late. J Sport Sci 2012;30(5):421–30. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 94. Colcombe S, Kramer AF. Fitness effects on the cognitive function of older adults a meta-analytic study. Psychol Sci 2003;14(2):125–30. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 95. Tomporowski PD, Lambourne K, Okumura MS. Physical activity interventions and children's mental function: an introduction and overview. Preventive medicine 2011;52:S3–S9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 96. Beauchamp GK, Mennella JA. Early flavor learning and its impact on later feeding behavior. J Pediatr Gastroenterol Nutr 2009;48:S25–30. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 97. Nicklaus S. Development of food variety in children. Appetite 2009;52(1):253–5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 98. Mennella JA. Ontogeny of taste preferences: basic biology and implications for health. Am J Clin Nutr 2014;99(3):704S–11S. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 99. Mardis AL, Anand R. A look at the diet of pregnant women. Fam Econ Nutr Rev 2001;13(2):55. [Google Scholar]
- 100. Stallings TL, Gazmararian JA, Goodman M, Kleinbaum D. Prenatal and postnatal fruit and vegetable intake among US women: associations with WIC participation. Matern Child Health J 2016;20(8):1598–606. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 101. Cox DR, Skinner JD, Carruth BR, Moran J III, Houck K. A Food Variety Index for Toddlers (VIT): development and application. J Am Diet Assoc 1997;97(12):1382–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 102. Nicklaus S, Boggio V, Chabanet C, Issanchou S. A prospective study of food variety seeking in childhood, adolescence and early adult life. Appetite 2005;44(3):289–97. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 103. Fox MK, Condon E, Briefel RR, Reidy KC, Deming DM. Food consumption patterns of young preschoolers: are they starting off on the right path? J Am Diet Assoc 2010;110(12):S52–S9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 104. Ford CN, Slining MM, Popkin BM. Trends in dietary intake among US 2-to 6-year-old children, 1989–2008. J Acad Nutr Diet 2013;113(1):35–42. e6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 105. Blaine RE, Fisher JO, Taveras EM, Geller AC, Rimm EB, Land T, Perkins M, Davison KK. Reasons low-income parents offer snacks to children: how feeding rationale influences snack frequency and adherence to dietary recommendations. Nutrients 2015;7(7):5982–99. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 106. Dinehart M, Hayes J, Bartoshuk L, Lanier S, Duffy V. Bitter taste markers explain variability in vegetable sweetness, bitterness, and intake. Physiol Behav 2006;87(2):304–13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 107. Pliner P, Loewen ER. Temperament and food neophobia in children and their mothers. Appetite 1997;28(3):239–54. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 108. Vereecken C, Rovner A, Maes L. Associations of parenting styles, parental feeding practices and child characteristics with young children's fruit and vegetable consumption. Appetite 2010;55(3):589–96. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 109. Johnson SL, Davies PL, Boles RE, Gavin WJ, Bellows LL. Young children's food neophobia characteristics and sensory behaviors are related to their food intake. J Nutr 2015;145(11):2610–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 110. Ebenegger V, Marques-Vidal P, Nydegger A, Laimbacher J, Niederer I, Bürgi F, Giusti V, Bodenmann P, Kriemler S, Puder JJ. Independent contribution of parental migrant status and educational level to adiposity and eating habits in preschool children. Eur J Clin Nutr 2011;65(2):210–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 111. Aldridge V, Dovey TM, Halford JC. The role of familiarity in dietary development. Dev Rev 2009;29(1):32–44. [Google Scholar]
- 112. Zajonc RB. Mere exposure: a gateway to the subliminal. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 2001;10(6):224–8. [Google Scholar]
- 113. Birch LL, Marlin DW. I don't like it; I never tried it: effects of exposure on two-year-old children's food preferences. Appetite 1982;3(4):353–60. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 114. Johnson SL, Bellows L, Beckstrom L, Anderson J. Evaluation of a social marketing campaign targeting preschool children. Am J Health Behav 2007;31(1):44–55. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 115. Anzman-Frasca S, Savage JS, Marini ME, Fisher JO, Birch LL. Repeated exposure and associative conditioning promote preschool children's liking of vegetables. Appetite 2012;58(2):543–53. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 116. Hausner H, Olsen A, Møller P. Mere exposure and flavour–flavour learning increase 2–3year-old children's acceptance of a novel vegetable. Appetite 2012;58(3):1152–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 117. Wardle J, Herrera ML, Cooke L, Gibson EL. Modifying children's food preferences: the effects of exposure and reward on acceptance of an unfamiliar vegetable. Eur J Clin Nutr 2003;57(2):341–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 118. Mennella JA, Nicklaus S, Jagolino AL, Yourshaw LM. Variety is the spice of life: strategies for promoting fruit and vegetable acceptance during infancy. Physiol Behav 2008;94(1):29–38. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 119. Ahern SM, Caton SJ, Blundell P, Hetherington MM. The root of the problem: increasing root vegetable intake in preschool children by repeated exposure and flavour flavour learning. Appetite 2014;80:154–60. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 120. Wild V, Graaf C, Jager G. Efficacy of repeated exposure and flavour–flavour learning as mechanisms to increase preschooler's vegetable intake and acceptance. Pediatr Obes 2015;10(3):205–12. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 121. Carruth BR, Skinner JD. Revisiting the picky eater phenomenon: neophobic behaviors of young children. J Am Coll Nutr 2000;19(6):771–80. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 122. Skinner JD, Carruth BR, Houck KS, Bounds W, Morris M, Cox DR, Moran J, Coletta F. Longitudinal study of nutrient and food intakes of white preschool children aged 24 to 60 months. J Am Diet Assoc 1999;99(12):1514–21. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 123. Russell CG, Worsley A. Why don't they like that? And can I do anything about it? The nature and correlates of parents’ attributions and self-efficacy beliefs about preschool children's food preferences. Appetite 2013;66:34–43. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 124. Galloway AT, Lee Y, Birch LL. Predictors and consequences of food neophobia and pickiness in young girls. J Am Diet Assoc 2003;103(6):692–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 125. de Lauzon-Guillain B, Jones L, Oliveira A, Moschonis G, Betoko A, Lopes C, Moreira P, Manios Y, Papadopoulos NG, Emmett P, et al. The influence of early feeding practices on fruit and vegetable intake among preschool children in 4 European birth cohorts. Am J Clin Nutr 2013;98(3):804–12. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 126. Farrow C, Blissett J. Stability and continuity of parentally reported child eating behaviours and feeding practices from 2 to 5 years of age. Appetite 2012;58(1):151–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 127. Gregory JE, Paxton SJ, Brozovic AM. Maternal feeding practices predict fruit and vegetable consumption in young children. Results of a 12-month longitudinal study. Appetite 2011;57(1):167–72. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 128. Shim JE, Kim J, Mathai RA, Team SKR. Associations of infant feeding practices and picky eating behaviors of preschool children. J Am Diet Assoc 2011;111(9):1363–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 129. Rodgers RF, Paxton SJ, McLean SA, Campbell KJ, Wertheim EH, Skouteris H, Gibbons K. Maternal negative affect is associated with emotional feeding practices and emotional eating in young children. Appetite 2014;80:242–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 130. Blissett J. Relationships between parenting style, feeding style and feeding practices and fruit and vegetable consumption in early childhood. Appetite 2011;57(3):826–31. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 131. Rhee K. Childhood overweight and the relationship between parent behaviors, parenting style, and family functioning. Ann Am Acad Polit Soc Sci 2008;615(1):11–37. [Google Scholar]
- 132. Ziv Y, Cassidy J. Maternal responsiveness and infant irritability: the contribution of Crockenberg and Smith's “Antecedents of mother–infant interaction and infant irritability in the first 3 months of life.” Infant Behav Dev 2002;25(1):16–20. [Google Scholar]
- 133. DiSantis K, Hodges EA, Johnson S, Fisher JO. The role of responsive feeding in overweight during infancy and toddlerhood: a systematic review. Int J Obes 2011;35(4):480–92. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 134. Black MM, Aboud FE. Responsive feeding is embedded in a theoretical framework of responsive parenting. J Nutr 2011;141(3):490–4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 135. Vrijheid M, Slama R, Robinson O, Chatzi L, Coen M, Van den Hazel P, Thomsen C, Wright J, Athersuch TJ, Avellana N, et al. The human early-life exposome (HELIX): project rationale and design. Env Health Perspect 2014;122(6):535–44. 2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 136. Shonkoff JP, Garner AS, Siegel BS, Dobbins MI, Earls MF, McGuinn L, Pascoe J, Wood DL, Committee on Psychosocial Aspects of Child and Family Health, and Committee on Early childhood, Adoption, and Dependant Care. The lifelong effects of early childhood adversity and toxic stress. Pediatrics 2012;129(1):e232–e46. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 137. Mattson MP. Challenging oneself intermittently to improve health. Dose-Response 2014;12(4):dose-response. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 138. Lee J, Jo D-G, Park D, Chung HY, Mattson MP. Adaptive cellular stress pathways as therapeutic targets of dietary phytochemicals: focus on the nervous system. Pharmacol Rev 2014;66(3):815–68. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 139. Rook GA, Lowry CA, Raison CL. Hygiene and other early childhood influences on the subsequent function of the immune system. Brain Res 2015;1617:47–62. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 140. Hafen E, Kossmann D, Brand A. Health data cooperatives—citizen empowerment. Methods Inf Med 2014;53(2):82–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]