Abstract
Vocal learning is an important behavior in oscines (songbirds). Some songbird species learn heterospecific sounds as well as conspecific vocalizations. The emergence of vocal mimicry is necessarily tied to the evolution of vocal learning, as mimicry requires the ability to acquire sounds through learning. As such, tracking the evolutionary origins of vocal mimicry may provide insights into the causes of variation in song learning programs among songbirds. We compiled a database of known vocal mimics that comprised 339 species from 43 families. We then traced the evolutionary history of vocal mimicry across the avian phylogeny using ancestral trait reconstruction on a dataset of oscine passerines for which vocalizations have been described. We found that the common ancestor to oscines was unlikely to mimic sounds, suggesting that song learning evolved with mechanisms to constrain learning to conspecific models. Mimicry then evolved repeatedly within the songbird clade, either through relaxation of constraints on conspecific learning or through selection for active vocal mimicry. Vocal mimicry is likely ancestral in only a handful of clades, and we detect many instances of independent origins of mimicry. Our analysis underscores the liability of vocal mimicry in songbirds, and highlights the evolutionary flexibility of song learning mechanisms.
Keywords: vocal mimicry, vocal imitation, vocal learning, songbird, oscine
Impact statement.
Naturalists have long been fascinated by the ability of certain birds to imitate a wide range of sounds such as environmental noises and the vocalizations of other animals, including humans. This ability, often called vocal mimicry, is well known in some species (e.g., parrots, mockingbirds, lyrebirds). However, this ability is more widespread than often recognized, and provides clues to the diversity of song learning strategies within birds. Here, we built a database of 339 known flexible vocal mimics within the oscine passerines, known as ‘songbirds’. Using phylogenetic analysis on data from songbirds with described vocalizations, we traced the evolutionary origins of vocal mimicry. We show that vocal mimicry was probably not ancestral to songbirds, suggesting that the first songbirds likely sang only their own species’ song. This constraint to conspecific learning must have lifted in many clades through relaxation of learning constraints or selection for vocal mimicry for some beneficial function. Our study clearly demonstrates the evolutionary flexibility of vocal mimicry and song learning within songbirds.
Acoustic communication often plays a role in species recognition in animals (e.g., Emlen 1972; Claridge 1985; Hauber et al 2001; Seddon 2005; Percy et al. 2006). For many species, acoustic signals are genetically encoded; individuals can produce species‐specific sounds without the need for learning (Jang and Gerhardt 2006; Jarvis 2006; Janek and Slater 2000). In others, species‐specific vocalizations are learned by first perceiving acoustic information, and then practicing imitating these acquired sounds (Marler 1976). Such vocal learning is a behavior found in select mammals (humans, bats, cetaceans, and elephants; e.g., Jarvis 2006, Crockford et al. 2004; Prat et al. 2015) and birds (hummingbirds, parrots, and songbirds; e.g., Bolhuis and Gahr 2006). Within birds, this ability evolved independently multiple times (Tyack 2007; Jarvis 2007; Slater 1989).
Roughly half of the world's avian species are oscine passerines (songbirds) that learn their song. In many species, young birds listen to the songs of adults (gain an acoustic template), fine‐tune their imitation by comparing their practice songs to the acoustic template, and eventually produce a characteristic song of their own (much simplified; see Marler 1970a,b; Konishi 1965; Marler 1976; Soha 2017). However, there is striking variation in the oscine learning program (e.g., Nottebohm 1972; Soha 2017). Brenowitz and Beecher (2005) identified five dimensions of variation that cause complexity in song learning: timing of learning, number of songs learned, fidelity of imitation, type of exposure, and level of constraint to species‐specific models. The last dimension, level of constraint, ranges from species that learn only species‐specific song elements (highly constrained) to species that incorporate heterospecific and environmental sounds (unconstrained). In other words, a species can be discriminatory or permissive about what sounds it learns and incorporates into vocalizations.
Some highly permissive species, termed vocal mimics, readily learn not only species‐specific sounds but also sounds produced by other species or inanimate objects (Marshall 1950; Kaplan 2003; Kelley et al. 2008). There are many potential functions and definitions of vocal mimicry, which have been the focus of study (reviewed in Marshall 1950; Dobkin 1979; Baylis 1982; Hindmarsh 1986; Kelley et al. 2008; Dalziell et al. 2015; Jamie 2017; see also Dalziell and Welbergen 2016). However, little is known about the roots of mimetic ability in songbirds. Here, we focused on the evolutionary origins of the predilection to mimic heterospecific sounds, combined with the ability to imitate them (‘descriptive definition’ of vocal mimicry; Baylis 1982). Because very little is known about differences in the extent of mimicry across conspecifics, we assumed that mimicry is a species‐level trait to allow us to investigate the evolution of vocal mimicry.
The emergence of vocal mimicry is necessarily tied to the evolution of vocal learning, as mimicry requires the ability to acquire sounds through learning. Therefore, we assume that vocal mimicry could not have evolved before vocal learning. However, there are two broad scenarios in which vocal mimicry could have arisen relative to the emergence of vocal learning. First, if vocal learning evolved due to selection for increased song repertoire (e.g., Nottebohm and Liu 2010), mimicry could have evolved as a mechanism to acquire more song elements or as a by‐product of a broad acoustic template. The ability to mimic heterospecific sounds could then have been lost in lineages that evolved a narrow predisposition to learn only conspecific sounds. In this case, we would expect mimicry to align with the emergence of vocal learning and to be ancestral to all songbirds. Second, in an alternative scenario, vocal learning may have originally evolved with a strong bias toward acquisition of strictly species‐specific sounds. Over time, the perceptual window for sound acquisition could have become more permissive in some lineages, allowing vocal mimicry. The emergence of permissiveness in song learning could have arisen through relaxation of selection for narrow predispositions during song learning, or through strong selection for newly acquired functions (Dalziell et al. 2015).
To determine which of these two scenarios was more likely, we traced the evolutionary history of vocal mimicry across the avian phylogeny using ancestral trait reconstruction. First, we compiled a database of known mimetic species to better understand global patterns of mimicry. Second, we focused on the phylogenetic pattern of the emergence of mimicry. By tracing the history of vocal mimicry on the songbird phylogeny, we determined when the trait is likely to have emerged. Third, we used our phylogenetic approach to suggest the types of questions about mimicry that should be tackled in the future.
Methods
DEFINITION OF VOCAL MIMICRY
For the purposes of this study, we use a general definition of vocal mimicry, which encompasses imitation of all types of non‐conspecific sounds: other animals, anthropogenic (e.g., dog whistle, chainsaw), and environmental (e.g., water drip, leaves rustling) noises. We chose this definition to allow analysis of the evolution of the ability to learn and produce ‘mimicked’ sounds. While information on vocalizations is available for many species, determining the functions of mimicry requires careful experimental studies that have only been conducted on a few species. Here, we focused on the evolution of mimicry in species that utilize imitated sounds frequently and extensively, regardless of function.
COMPILING THE DATABASE
Data were compiled from a variety of primary and secondary sources. A preliminary search was done on Google Scholar. We then expanded the search to various websites using search terms “mimic‐”, “imitate‐”, and “copy‐”. Field guides and handbooks, including all volumes of the Handbook of the Birds of the World, were browsed manually for mention of vocal imitation or copying behavior. Sources without peer‐review were verified whenever possible with an extensive search for corroborative scientific publications on each species on Google Scholar. If an account could not be verified, the species in question was not included in analysis. Scientific and common names were standardized using the IOC Bird List version 5.4 (Gill and Donsker 2015).
We compiled data on 557 oscine species to create our database of vocal mimicry (Supporting Information Table S1). We divided these species into three classifications corresponding to different extents of vocal imitation: “flexible”, “incidental”, and “unknown”. Under our classification scheme, “flexible mimics” were species that frequently imitate a wide variety of sounds, often having plastic repertoires and an extended period of song learning. In these species, mimetic ability is found in most individuals and is readily observed under natural conditions. In contrast, species that mimic on specific or rare occasions were classified as “incidental mimics”. This “incidental mimic” designation was also applied to mimicry in brood parasites that learn the calls or songs of their host species (17 species of Vidua finches; e.g., DaCosta and Sorenson 2014). Although brood parasites are flexible in which species they imitate (Langmore et al. 2008; Madden and Davies 2006), in this case, they have merely shifted their learning template from a conspecific to heterospecific tutor, and do not imitate a wider range of sounds (Kelley et al. 2008; DaCosta and Sorenson 2014). Other species considered “incidental mimics” were species of which individuals may have imitated a heterospecific in unnatural circumstances, such as when kept in captivity (e.g., bullfinches, zebra finches). Any accounts of a single individual imitating heterospecific song were also considered incidental mimicry, as this imitation most likely resulted from a learning mistake. The classification of “unknown mimic” was used for species reported to mimic but for which we found no details as to extent of imitation. In total, we classified 339 species as “flexible”, 120 as “incidental”, and 98 as “unknown” mimics.
We then classified oscine species for which we had found no accounts of mimicry as “non‐mimics”. However, to avoid scoring unstudied, potentially mimetic species as “non‐mimics”, we did not include species for which there were no data on vocalizations (based on accounts in the Handbook of the Birds of the World). Of the 5004 oscine species described, we found no information on the vocalizations of 594 species, and excluded these from all analyses. We therefore had a final dataset of 4410 oscine species (557 mimics and 3853 non‐mimics) for analysis.
To explore the robustness of our classification scheme, we treated each species as mimic or non‐mimic based on two alternative criteria (Supporting Information Figure S1). First, under our more conservative interpretation, we only considered flexible mimics as mimics, and treated incidental and unknown mimics as non‐mimics. We refer to this as the “flexible mimics” dataset. Second, we repeated our analyses using a more relaxed interpretation in which all three mimic classifications were treated as mimics. We refer to this as the “all mimics” dataset. We considered the first, more conservative, definition of mimicry (flexible mimics dataset) to be more reliable for evolutionary analysis of the use of mimicked sounds in vocal communication. However, using the second, more relaxed definition of mimicry (all mimics dataset) allowed us to test the robustness of our conclusions. In total, 339 flexible mimic species were considered mimics in the flexible mimic analysis, and an additional 218 species (for a total of 557 species) were scored as mimics in the analysis of all mimics.
DESCRIPTIVE ANALYSIS
We calculated proportions of mimetic species within each avian family, as well as a global proportion, using the flexible and all mimics datasets. We were especially interested in patterns of mimicry within families, and determined whether mimics were clumped within, or dispersed throughout, a family. As we used published accounts of mimicking species, we expected under‐sampling of certain regions (Asia, Africa, and South America) compared to well‐studied ones (North America, Europe, and Australia). To investigate this further, we also calculated proportions of mimics based on geographic region. We used bird checklists from Avibase (excluding all accidental and introduced species) as the source for the total number of resident oscine species to allow comparison between regions (Lepage 2017).
PHYLOGENETIC ANALYSIS
We conducted phylogenetic analyses using our compiled database to estimate the probability of mimicry being the ancestral state of songbirds. We first conducted analyses using alternate methods to assign species as vocal mimics. We downloaded the global phylogeny of birds accompanying Jetz et al. (2012). The tree source was Hackett All Species: a set of 1000 trees with 9993 operational taxonomic units (OTUs) each. As these trees included all avian species, we first pruned the trees by removing non‐oscine species. We then removed the 594 oscine species for which we found no song data in the Handbook of the Birds of the World. Our final phylogeny therefore included 4410 species. In the first analysis, we used the flexible mimics dataset and assigned a vocal mimic score of 1 to only the flexible mimic species, treating the incidental and unknown mimics as non‐mimics (score of 0). In our second analysis, we used our all mimics dataset in which flexible, incidental, and unknown mimics were all treated as vocal mimics (given a score of 1).
We compared the probability of mimicry being ancestral to oscines, and to each oscine family, based on these two analyses. The discrete character of mimicry was mapped onto the phylogeny as a basic binary value (present or absent). We then reconstructed the ancestral state for the basal ancestor of all species in the phylogeny, as well as the basal ancestor of each family. We reconstructed discrete ancestral states using the equal rates model, which estimates the marginal ancestral states based upon Bayesian likelihood. We also used stochastic character mapping to estimate the number of state changes across the phylogeny, also using the equal rates model. All analyses were done on the set of 1000 pruned trees using the R package phytools version 0.5‐38 (Revell 2012; Supporting Information).
We supplemented our phylogenetic analysis with two additional analyses to further explore the robustness of our results (see Supporting Information Methods for details). Both supplementary analyses incorporated only flexible mimics as mimics. One analysis restricted our dataset to the 817 oscines from regions with a long history of birdsong research (USA, Canada, Europe, Australia, and New Zealand). This regional analysis was meant to ensure that no species were mistakenly categorized as mimics or non‐mimics, as the vocalizations of every oscine from these regions have been described. The other analysis was on a dataset of 3550 avian species, including 65 non‐oscines and 3485 oscines. We included this analysis as a relatively unrestricted comparison that incorporated representatives from all avian orders.
Results
DESCRIPTIVE ANALYSIS
Of the roughly 5004 extant species and 115 families in the suborder Passeri (songbirds), 339 species (6.8%) from 43 families (37.4%) were classified as flexible mimics (Supporting Information Table S2). When considering only those 4410 oscine species for which we had vocalization data in the total, the percentage of mimetic oscine species becomes 8.9%. Songbird families vary greatly in mimetic ability and number of mimicking species (Fig. 1A and B). Using our dataset of flexible mimics, mimicry was rare (i.e., proportion of flexible mimics ≤ .10) in 95 of the 115 (82.6%) songbird families. In contrast, 16 families (13.9%) had a proportion of mimetic species between 0.1 and 0.5, while four songbird families (3.5%; Dicruridae, Nicatoridae, Ptilonorhynchidae, Menuridae) had a proportion higher than 0.5. In all cases, mimicry was spread across a family such that mimetic and non‐mimetic species were often each other's closest living relatives.
Figure 1.
Phylogenetic tree of songbirds. (A) Presence of mimicry overlaid on phylogeny. Mimicry is represented by white marks in the dark ring. (B) Stochastic character mapping of estimated emergence and loss of mimetic ability. Mimicry is represented in red. Vocal mimicry evolved independently at least 237 times and was lost at least 52 times.
When using our more relaxed definition of mimicry (including incidental mimics and mimics with unknown extent of imitation), thirteen additional oscine families had at least one mimetic species, bringing the total to 56 families (48.7%). The number of families with proportions of mimicry between 0.1 and 0.5 jumped to 24 (20.9%), and the proportion of mimicking species in two additional families—Viduidae and Atrichornithidae—were above 0.5. The estimated global percentage of mimics based on this less restrictive analysis ranged from 11.0% (of the number of extant oscines) to 14.5% (of the pruned number of oscines included in analysis).
The proportion of oscine species that mimic varied regionally as well. In Europe, 31.6% of oscine species were flexible mimics, while other regions had 4–9% mimicking species (Table 2). These numbers nearly doubled when including all mimics, with percentages of mimics in North America, Australasia, and Africa rising to 10–15%, Europe rising to 60%, and other regions reaching 7%.
Table 2.
Proportion of mimics on each continent
Region | Total oscine species | Number of flexible mimics (proportion of total) | Number of all mimics (proportion of total) |
---|---|---|---|
North America | 762 | 54 (0.071) | 85 (0.112) |
Europe | 215 | 68 (0.316) | 129 (0.600) |
Australasia | 778 | 58 (0.075) | 87 (0.112) |
Asia | 2004 | 90 (0.045) | 137 (0.068) |
Africa | 1442 | 132 (0.092) | 222 (0.154) |
South America | 825 | 32 (0.039) | 61 (0.074) |
Central America | 531 | 26 (0.049) | 41 (0.077) |
PHYLOGENETIC ANALYSIS
Based on our conservative phylogenetic analysis constrained to flexible mimics, the most recent common ancestor of all songbirds most likely did not mimic (probability of presence = 0.129 ± 0.002; Table 1). When we use our more relaxed classification for mimics (all mimics dataset), our estimate of ancestral mimetic ability increases but remains unlikely (probability of presence = 0.220 ± 0.002). Our results remain qualitatively similar in two other versions of the phylogenetic analysis. The probability that the common ancestor of songbirds was mimetic is low when we constrain our analysis to geographic regions in which birdsong has been particularly well‐studied (probability of presence = 0.167 ± 0.39), and when we include all songbirds (probability of presence = 0.185 ± 0.003).
Table 1.
Probability of mimicry as the ancestral trait in oscines based on classification scheme
Dataset | Classifications scored as mimic | Number of mimics | Total number of species | Probability of ancestral mimicry |
---|---|---|---|---|
Flexible mimics | Flexible | 339 | 4410 | 0.129 ± 0.002 |
All mimics | Flexible, incidental, unknown | 557 | 4410 | 0.220 ± 0.002 |
Regional | Flexible | 339 | 817 | 0.167 ± 0.39 |
Unrestricted | Flexible | 339 | 3550 | 0.185 ± 0.003 |
When using our flexible mimic dataset, we estimate that vocal mimicry evolved independently at least 237 times across oscine taxa (Fig. 1B) and was lost 52 times on average. These values become 401 and 100, respectively, when using the all mimics database. The ancestors of three songbird families—Mimidae, Dicruridae, Menuridae—were likely vocal mimics (probability ≈ 0.75 or greater; Table 3). It is unclear whether mimicry was ancestral to Atrichornithidae, Ptilonorhynchidae, and Nicatoridae (probability ≈ 0.5). All other oscine families did not have ancestors that actively mimicked (P < 0.05, Supporting Information Table S2). Results were similar in the analysis using all mimics, although under this relaxed definition mimicry was likely ancestral to Atrichornithidae and Ptilonorhynchidae as well.
Table 3.
Probabilities of the presence of vocal mimicry in the ancestor of select songbird families
Oscine family | Species (in analysis) | Species (total) | Proportion of species (in analysis) | Mimic species (flexible) | Proportion flexible mimic (of total) | Mimic species (all) | Proportion all mimic (total) | Probability ancestor was mimic (flexible) | Probability ancestor was mimic (all) |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Atrichornithidae | 2 | 2 | 1 | 1 | 0.5 | 2 | 1 | 0.574 ± 0.005 | 0.92 ± 0.003 |
Dicruridae | 21 | 25 | 0.84 | 13 | 0.52 | 17 | 0.68 | 0.854 ± 0.006 | 0.831 ± 0.005 |
Menuridae | 2 | 2 | 1 | 2 | 1 | 2 | 1 | 0.949 ± 0.002 | 0.926 ± 0.003 |
Mimidae | 34 | 34 | 1 | 16 | 0.47 | 17 | 0.5 | 0.742 ± 0.004 | 0.635 ± 0.003 |
Nicatoridae | 3 | 3 | 1 | 2 | 0.67 | 2 | 0.67 | 0.417 ± 0.007 | 0.48 ± 0.006 |
Ptilonorhynchidae | 17 | 20 | 0.85 | 14 | 0.7 | 17 | 0.85 | 0.576 ± 0.008 | 0.748 ± 0.005 |
Discussion
With high likelihood, mimetic ability was not ancestral to songbirds. Instead, vocal mimicry evolved numerous times within oscines and is currently widespread among extant species. One‐third of songbird families contain at least one mimicking species, and there are a few families in which most species are mimetic. Our phylogenetic analysis indicates that mimicry may have emerged at the base of some families, multiple times within other families, or never emerged in still other families. Mimicry appears to have been ancestral to two families (Dicruridae and Menuridae), with probabilities of presence greater than 0.75. These families include species in which functions of mimicry have been well studied. For example, greater racket‐tailed drongos, Dicrurus paradiseus, may use mimicked alarm calls of heterospecific flock members to demonstrate aggressive intent or to steal food (e.g., Satischandra et al. 2010). In superb lyrebirds, Menura novaehollandiae, mimetic accuracy may be used by females to choose between males (Dalziell and Magrath 2012; Coleman et al. 2007). However, although mimicry clearly serves a function in certain clades, it is unclear whether such functions alone can account for the widespread emergence of vocal mimicry across oscines.
Our global estimate of flexible mimics ranges from approximately 7 to 9%. However, there is strong geographic variation in the prevalence of vocal mimicry. For example, while 31.6% of European oscines were categorized as flexible mimics, all other regions only had an average of 6% flexible mimetic species. Furthermore, the pattern changes within regions. While the percentage of flexible mimics in Australia and New Zealand was 14.9% (see also Chisholm 1932; Marshall 1950), this percentage became 7.5% for all Australasian islands. Similarly, the percentage of North American mimics increases from 7.1 to 14.2% when excluding Mexican species. Part of this difference may be explained by a dearth of publications on the vocalizations of understudied avifauna in certain regions. However, although we expect that our knowledge of mimics is more complete for certain regions, this cannot fully explain the difference. For example, although our number of mimicking North American species is relatively small, we believe this number to be accurate, as North American oscines have been studied extensively. Although we have no reason a priori for expecting Europe to have a large percentage of mimics, further study is needed to understand this pattern.
There has long been debate over the definition of vocal mimicry (reviewed in Dalziell et al. 2015). Here, we focused on evidence for flexible use of imitated sounds (e.g., heterospecific, anthropogenic, environmental) based on existing literature. However, we acknowledge that there is uncertainty associated with how to categorize and define mimetic species. To account for this uncertainty, we also assessed the prevalence of species with records of incidental mimicry (e.g., evidence of heterospecific copying in captivity or other limited contexts) as well as species for which accounts of vocal mimicry did not specify the extent of flexibility. Many of these species are unlikely to be considered vocal mimics under any definition, but including incidental mimics provided a maximum estimate of vocally mimicking species. When including these putative mimics (‘all mimics’ in Tables 1 and 2), we estimate that approximately 11–15% of oscine species imitate non‐conspecific sounds.
However, we (and most others to date) have treated vocal mimicry as a species‐level trait. Our literature review revealed little information on within‐species variation in vocal mimicry. Given the evolutionary lability of this trait, it is likely that the extent of vocal mimicry varies within some species. Although we cannot account for any within‐species variation in our analysis, understanding the variation in extent and function of vocal mimicry within species would yield further understanding of the evolutionary origins of vocal mimicry.
Our phylogenetic analysis indicates that vocal mimicry was likely not ancestral to oscines. As song learning probably evolved at some point early in the evolution of oscines (Nottebohm 1972; Nottebohm and Liu 2010), this implies that mimetic ability did not evolve concurrently with the origin of song learning. Instead, our analysis supports the hypothesis that the ancestral songbird had a restricted song template that excluded non‐species‐specific sounds. Vocal flexibility may have been limited by constraints on template acquisition facilitating the learning of only conspecific sounds, and/or by restrictive sound production mechanisms. These restrictions and constraints on vocal mimicry would have lessened repeatedly and independently within the songbird clade.
Given our results, the question becomes why and how restrictions on sensory recognition (song template) and/or sound production (syringeal function) became relaxed in some lineages. Here, we propose two hypotheses. In the first hypothetical scenario, species‐specificity in both song recognition and production slowly relaxed over time in the absence of selection. At some point after song learning evolved, mimicry became possible and the imitation of heterospecific sounds became commonplace in many species, eventually gaining functional significance. Alternatively, permissiveness in imitation may have undergone repeated positive selection after the evolution of song learning. A proposed mechanism driving the evolution of vocal learning is mate choice based on song complexity or novelty (Nottebohm 1972; Jarvis 2004) as females of many species appear to prefer males singing more complex repertoires (e.g., canaries, Draganoiu et al 2002; starlings, Mountjoy and Lemon 1996; Gentner and Hulse 2000; chaffinches, Leitao et al 2005), and learning enhances complexity (Nottebohm 1972; Jarvis 2006). Similarly, Laiolo et al. (2011) suggest that mimicry increases song complexity and serves as an honest signal. As such, selection for vocal repertoire complexity or plasticity in vocal performance may have led to a less restricted song learning template. As imitation became increasingly plastic, vocal learning would have broadened to include mimicry. Of the mimicking species used in our analysis, more than 90% are likely using mimicry solely in song. It therefore seems likely that sexual selection played a role in the emergence of mimicry.
Vocal mimicry requires both a broadly permissive song template (or lack of template) and the morphological ability to produce imitations. Yet, the imitation of heterospecific sounds has evolved frequently, suggesting that relatively simple mechanisms may separate non‐mimetic and mimetic species. This also suggests that some species may have the capability to mimic but only express the trait under certain circumstances. Social context may be one important factor driving incidental mimicry of heterospecifics, at least in captive conditions (as with starlings, West and King 1990; bullfinches, Nicolai 1959; budgerigars, Gramza 1970). In our database, the incidental mimic classification includes species that imitated when kept without conspecific social partners, but are not known to imitate in the wild (e.g., zebra finch and Oregon junco, Bertram 1970; house sparrow, Conradi 1905). Similarly, another factor influencing incidental mimicry could be similarity between vocalizations of conspecifics and heterospecifics, such that learning of heterospecific song can occur by accident (Kelley et al. 2008; e.g., short‐toed and Eurasian treecreepers, Garamszegi et al. 2007; Thielcke 1962, 1972). These examples underscore the diversity of forms vocal mimicry can take, suggesting that mimicry is not a discrete trait, but rather a continuum within the spectrum of vocal flexibility. Therefore, one further way to explore the evolution of vocal mimicry may be to study transitions between incidental and flexible mimics. Similarly, studying pairs of sister species, one mimetic and one non‐mimetic, may improve our understanding of vocal mimicry. More generally, we suggest that phylogenetic and comparative approaches to vocal flexibility can continue to provide insights into the evolution of avian song learning programs.
DATA ACCESSIBILITY
The full database, methods, and supplemental R script can be found in Supporting Information Materials.
Associate Editor: A. Goswami
Supporting information
Appendix I.
Figure S1
Table S1
Table S2
Supporting information
AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS
M.G. and D.S. conceived the project. M.G. compiled the database and ran analyses. M.G. and D.S. wrote and revised the manuscript.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
We would like to thank Dan Gates, Chad Eliason, Benny Goller, and Liam Revell for help with phylogenetics in R. We would also like to thank Franz Goller for helpful insights about song learning and mimicry.
LITERATURE CITED
- Alexander. 1927. The birds of Latium, Italy. Ibis 3(12):245–271. [Google Scholar]
- Archawaranon, M. 2005. Vocal imitation in hill mynahs Gracula religiosa: factors affecting competency. Int. J. Zool. Res. 1(1):26–32. [Google Scholar]
- Areta, J.I. , and Repenning M.. 2011. Systematics of the tawny‐bellied seedeater (Sporophila hypoxantha). I. Geographic variation, ecology, and evolution of vocalizations. Condor 113(3):664–677. [Google Scholar]
- Armstrong, E.A. 1963. A study of bird song. Oxford Univ Press, Oxford, UK. [Google Scholar]
- Atkinson, E.C. 1997. Singing for your supper: acoustical luring of avian prey by northern shrikes. Condor 99:203–206. [Google Scholar]
- Baldwin, J. 2013. Large‐footed finch (Pezopetes capitalis), version 1.0 in Schulenberg T. S., (Ed.) Neotropical Birds Online. Cornell Lab of Ornithology, Ithaca, NY, USA: 10.2173/nb.laffin1.01 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Baptista, L.F. 1972. Wild house finch sings white‐crowned sparrow song. Z. Tierpsychol. 30:266–270. [Google Scholar]
- Baylis, J.R. 1982. Avian vocal mimicry: its function and evolution. Acoustic Communication in Birds (2):51–80. [Google Scholar]
- Bent, A.C. 1948d. Northern mockingbird. Smithsonian Institution US National Museum Bulletin 195:295–315. [Google Scholar]
- Bent, A.C. 1949. Blue‐gray gnatcatcher. Smithsonian Institution US National Museum Bulletin 196:344–364. [Google Scholar]
- Bent, A.C. 1953a. Common yellowthroat. Smithsonian Institution United States National Museum Bulletin 203:542–565. [Google Scholar]
- Bent, A.C. 1953b. Yellow‐breasted chat. Smithsonian Institution US National Museum Bulletin 203:587–599. [Google Scholar]
- Bertram, B. 1970. The vocal behaviour of the indian hill mynah, Gracula religiosa. Anim. Behav. Monogr. 3:81–192. [Google Scholar]
- BirdLife International . 2018a. Species factsheet: Acrocephalus paludicola. Accessible at: http://datazone.birdlife.org/species/factsheet/aquatic-warbler-acrocephalus-paludicola/text.
- BirdLife International . 2018b. Species factsheet: Gracula religiosa. Accessible at: http://datazone.birdlife.org/species/factsheet/common-hill-myna-gracula-religiosa/text
- BirdLife International . 2018b. Species factsheet: Microptilotis analogus. Accessible at: http://datazone.birdlife.org/species/factsheet/mimic-honeyeater-microptilotis-analogus/text
- BirdLife International . 2018c. Species factsheet: Mimus graysoni. Accessible at: http://datazone.birdlife.org/species/factsheet/socorro-mockingbird-mimus-graysoni/text.
- Blasé, B. 1960. Ethology die Lautäußerungen des Neuntöters (Lanius c. collurio), Freilandbeobachtungen und Kaspar‐hauser‐versuche. Ethology 17:293–344. [Google Scholar]
- Bolhuis, J.J. , and Gahr M. 2006. Neural mechanisms of birdsong memory. Neuroscience 7:347–357. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bourke, P.A. 1947. Notes on the Horsfield bush‐lark. Emu 47:1–7. [Google Scholar]
- Bourke, P.A. , and Austin, A.F. 1947. The Atherton tablelands and its avifauna. Emu 47:87–116. [Google Scholar]
- Bradley, J.M. 1987. Vocal behaviour and annual cycle of the western bowerbird Chlamydera guttata. Aust. Bird Watcher 12:83–90. [Google Scholar]
- Brady, S. 2010. Azure Jay (Cyanocorax caeruleus), version 1.0. In Neotropical Birds Online (T. S. Schulenberg, Editor). Cornell Lab of Ornithology, Ithaca, NY, USA. 10.2173/nb.azujay1.01 [DOI]
- Brenowitz, E.A. , and Beecher M.D. 2005. Song learning in birds: diversity and plasticity, opportunities and challenges. Trends Neurosci. 28:127–132. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Campbell, A.J. 1901. Nests and eggs of Australian birds including the geographical distribution of the species and popular observations thereon. Pawson and Brailsford, Sheffield.
- Carnaby, T. 2010. Beat about the bush: birds. Jacana Media.
- Chisholm, A.H. 1932. XXXVII. Vocal mimicry among Australian birds. Ibis 74:605–624. [Google Scholar]
- Chisholm, A.H. , and Chaffer N. 1956. Observations on the golden bower‐bird. Emu 56:1–39. [Google Scholar]
- Claridge, M.F. 1985. Chapter 5: Acoustic behavior of leafhoppers and planthoppers: species problems and speciation In ‘Offprints from the leafhoppers and planthoppers’ (ed. Nault L.R. and Rodriguez J.G.). University College, Wales. John Wiley & Sons, Inc. [Google Scholar]
- Clement, P. , and Rose C.. 2015. Robins and chats. Helm Identification Guides, Bloomsbury. [Google Scholar]
- Coleman, S.W. , Patricelli G.L., Coyle B., Siani J., and Borgia G.. 2007. Female preferences drive the evolution of mimetic accuracy in male sexual displays. Biol. Lett. 3:463–466. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Conradi, E. (1905). Song and call‐notes of English sparrows when reared by canaries. Am. J. Psychol. 16(2):190–198. [Google Scholar]
- Cooper, W.T. , and Forshaw J.M.. 1977. The birds of paradise and bower birds. Collins.
- Corwin, P. 2012. Yellow‐rumped Cacique (Cacicus cela), version 1.0. In Neotropical Birds Online (T. S. Schulenberg, Editor). Cornell Lab of Ornithology, Ithaca, NY, USA. 10.2173/nb.yercac1.01 [DOI]
- Costa, L.M. , Freitas G., Pena J.C.C., and Rodrigues M.. 2012. O comportamento de brincar de um gaviao‐miudo (Accipiter striatus) perseguindo um bando de gralha‐canca (Cyanocorax cyanopogon). Rev. Bras. Ornitol. 20(1):40–43. [Google Scholar]
- Crockford, C. , Herbinger I., Vigilant L., and Boesch C.. 2004. Wild chimpanzees produce group‐specific calls: a case for vocal learning? Ethology 110:221–243. [Google Scholar]
- DaCosta, J.M. and Sorenson M.D.. 2014. An experimental test of host song mimicry as a species recognition cue among male brood parasitic indigobirds (Vidua spp.). Auk 131(4):549–558. [Google Scholar]
- Dalziell, A.H. and Magrath R.D.. 2012. Fooling the experts: accurate vocal mimicry in the song of the superb lyrebird, Menura novaehollandiae . Anim. Behav. 83:1401–1410. [Google Scholar]
- De Marsico, M.C. , Gantchoff M.G., and Reboreda J.C.. 2012. Host‐parasite coevolution beyond the nestling stage? Mimicry of host fledglings by the specialist screaming cowbird. Proc. R. Soc. B. 279:3401–3408. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dobkin, D.S. 1979. Functional and evolutionary relationships of vocal copying phenomena in birds. Ethology 50(4):348–363. [Google Scholar]
- Dalziell, A.H. , and Welbergen J.A.. 2016. Mimicry for all modalities. Ecol. Lett. 19:609–619. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dalziell, A.H. , Welbergen J.A., Igic B., and Magrath R.D.. 2015. Avian vocal mimicry: a unified conceptual framework. Biol. Rev. Camb. Philos. Soc. 90:643–668. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davis, D.E. 1940. Social nesting habits of the smooth‐billed ani. Auk 57:179–218. [Google Scholar]
- del Hoyo, J. , Elliott A., and Christie D.. 2011. Handbook of the birds of the world.
- Diamond, J.M. 1982. Rediscovery of the yellow‐fronted gardener bowerbird. Science 216:431–434. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Diamond, J.M. 1987. Bower building and decoration by the bowerbird Amblyornis inornatus . Ethology 74:177–204. [Google Scholar]
- Dickey, D.R. 1922. The mimetic aspect of the mocker's song. Condor 24:153–157. [Google Scholar]
- Dowsett‐Lemaire, F. 1979. The imitative range of the song of the marsh wren Acrocephalus palustris, with special reference to imitations of African birds. Ibis 121:453–468. [Google Scholar]
- Emlen, S.T. 1972. An experimental analysis of the parameters of bird song eliciting species recognition. Behaviour 41:130–171. [Google Scholar]
- Ferguson, J.W.H. , van Zyl A., and Delport K.. 2002. Vocal mimicry in African Cossypha robin chats. J. Ornithol. 143:319–330. [Google Scholar]
- Ferry, C. 1977. The mapping method applied to species problems. Interspecific territoriality of Hippolais icterina and H. polyglotta . Pol. Ecol. Stud. 3:145–146. [Google Scholar]
- Flower, T. 2011. Fork‐tailed drongos use deceptive mimicked alarm calls to steal food. Proc. Biol. Sci. 278:1548–1555. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Flower, T.P. , Gribble M., and A.R, Ridley . 2014. Deception by flexible alarm mimicry in an African bird. Science 344:513–516. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Forbush, E.H. 1929. Birds of Massachusetts and other New England states. Berwick and Smith.
- Fraga, R.M. 1987. Vocal mimicry in the epaulet oriole. Condor 89(1):133–137. [Google Scholar]
- Frith, C.B. , and Frith D.W.. 1990. Notes on the nesting biology of the great bowerbird Chlamydera nuchalis (Ptilonorhynchidae). Australian Bird Watcher 13(5):137–148. [Google Scholar]
- Frith, C.B. , and Frith D.W.. 1992. Annotated list of birds in western Tari Gap, Southern Highlands, Papua New Guinea, with some nidification notes. Australian Bird Watcher 14:262–276. [Google Scholar]
- Frith, C.B. , and Frith D.W.. 2000. Attendance levels and behaviour at bowers by male golden bowerbirds, Prionodura newtoniana (Ptilonorhynchidae). Memoirs of the Queensland Museum 45(2):317–342. [Google Scholar]
- Frith, C.B. , and Frith D.W.. 2004. The bowerbirds. Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Frith, C.B. , and McGuire M.. 1996. Visual evidence of vocal avian mimicry by male tooth‐billed bowerbirds Scenopoeetes dentirostris (Ptilonorhynchidae). Emu 96(1):12–16. [Google Scholar]
- Frith, C. , Borgia G., and Frith D.. 1996. Courts and courtship behaviour of Archbold's bowerbird Archboldia papuensis in Papua New Guinea. Ibis 138:204–211. [Google Scholar]
- Fry, C.H. , Keith S., and Urban E.K. 1992‐2004. The birds of Africa, Volumes IV‐VII: Passeriformes. Academic Press, London. [Google Scholar]
- Gammon, D.E. , and Lyon R.P.. 2017. An acoustic comparison of mimetic and non‐mimetic song in northern mockingbirds Mimus polyglottos. Ardea 105(1):1–6. [Google Scholar]
- Garamszegi, L.Z. , Eens M., Pavlova D.Z., Aviles J., and Moller A.P.. 2007. A comparative study of the function of heterospecific vocal mimicry in European passerines. Behavioral Ecology 18:1001–1009. [Google Scholar]
- Gentner, T.Q. , and Hulse S.H.. 2000. Female European starling preference and choice for variation in conspecific male song. Animal Behaviour 59:443–458. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gill F. and Donsker D. (eds). 2015. IOC World Bird List (v. 5.4). Accessible at: http://www.worldbirdnames.org/.
- Gonzales, K. , Richart C.H., and Burns K.J.. 2015. Fawn‐breasted Tanager (Pipraeidea melanonota), version 1.0 In Neotropical Birds Online (Schulenberg T. S., Editor). Cornell Lab of Ornithology, Ithaca, NY, USA: 10.2173/nb.fabtan1.01 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Goodale, E. and Kotagama S.W.. 2006. Context‐dependent vocal mimicry in a passerine bird. Proc. R. Soc. B. 273:875–880. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goodwin, D. 1956. Further observations on the behaviour of the jay Garrulus glandarius. Ibis 98:186–219. [Google Scholar]
- Gould, J. 1865. Birds of Australia. Melbourne, Lansdowne. [Google Scholar]
- Gramza, A.F. 1970. Vocal mimicry in captive budgerigars (Melopsittacus undulates). Ethology 27:971–983. [Google Scholar]
- Gwinner, E. , and Dorka, V. 1965. Beobachtungen an Zilpzalp‐Fitis‐Mischsängern. Vogelwelt 86:146–151. [Google Scholar]
- Haavie, J. , Borge T., Bures S., Garamszegi L.Z., Lampe H.M., Moreno J., Qvarnström A., Török J., and Saetre G‐P.. Flycatcher song in allopatry and sympatry ‐ convergence, divergence and reinforcement. Journal of Evolutionary Biology. 17:227–237. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hamao, S. , and Eda‐Fujiwara H.. 2004. Vocal mimicry by the black‐browed reed warbler Acrocephalus bistrigiceps: objective identification of mimetic sounds. Ibis 146:61–68. [Google Scholar]
- Hansen, P. 1975. Interspecific vocal imitation and the formation of new dialects. Biophon 3:2–5. [Google Scholar]
- Harcus, J.L. 1977. The functions of mimicry in the vocal behaviour of the Chorister Robin. Ethology 44:178–193. [Google Scholar]
- Hardy, J.W. , and Parke T.A.. 1985. Voices of the New World Thrushes. Audio‐cassette tape with insert notes. ARA Records, Gainesville, Florida.
- Hardy, J.W. , and Parker T.A. III. 1997. The nature and probable function of vocal copying in Lawrence's thrush, Turdus lawrenceii. Ornithol. Monogr. 48:307–320. [Google Scholar]
- Hauber, M.E. , Russo S.A., and Sherman P.W.. 2001. A password for species recognition in a brood‐parasitic bird. Proc. R. Soc. B 268:1041–1048. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heinroth, O. , and Heinroth K.. 1958. Ch.18: How birds communicate with each other. Univ. of Michigan Press, Ann Arbor, 133–135. [Google Scholar]
- Hindmarsh, A.M. 1986. The functional significance of vocal mimicry in song. Behaviour 99:87–100. [Google Scholar]
- Hindwood, K.A. 1955. Long use of nest by lyrebird. Emu 55(4):257–258. [Google Scholar]
- Hopkins, N. 1974. Some observations of the great bowerbird. Sunbird 5(1):10–15. [Google Scholar]
- Howard, R.D. 1974. The influence of sexual selection and interspecific competition on mockingbird song (Mimus polyglottos). Evolution 28(3):428–438. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jackson, S.W. 1912. Haunts of the spotted bowerbird (Chlamydera maculata). Emu: “Birds of a feather” 12:65–104. [Google Scholar]
- Jamie, G.A. 2017. Signals, cues and the nature of mimicry. Proc. R. Soc. B 284:1–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Janek, V.M. , and Slater P.J.. 2000. The different roles of social learning in vocal communiation. Anim. Behav. 60:1–11. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jang, Y. , and Gerhardt H.C.. 2006. Divergence in the calling songs between sympatric and allopatric populations of the southern wood cricket Gryllus fultoni (Orthoptera: Gryllidae). J. Evol. Biol. 19:459–472. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jarvis, E.D. 2004. Learned birdsong and the neurobiology of human language. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1016:749–777. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jarvis, E.D. 2006. Selection for and against vocal learning in birds and mammals. Ornithol. Sci. 5:5–14. [Google Scholar]
- Jarvis, E.D. 2007. Neural systems for vocal learning in birds and humans: a synopsis. J. Ornithol. 148:35–44. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jetz, W. , Thomas, G.H. , Joy, J.B. , Hartmann, K. , and Mooers, A.O . 2012. The global diversity of birds in space and time. Nature 491: 444–448. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kaplan, G. 2003. Magpie mimicry. Nat. Austr. 27(10):60. [Google Scholar]
- Kelley, L.A. , Coe R.L., Madden J.R., and Healy, S. D. 2008. Vocal mimicry in songbirds. Anim. Behav. 76:521–528. [Google Scholar]
- Konishi, M. 1965. The role of auditory feedback in the control of vocalization in the white‐crowned sparrow. Ethology 22(7):770–783. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Laiolo, P. , Obeso J.R., and Roggia Y.. 2011. Mimicry as a novel pathway linking biodiversity functions and individual behavioural performances. Proc. R. Soc. B 278:1072–1081. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Langmore, N.E. , Maurer G., Adcock G.J., and Kilner R.M.. 2008. Socially acquired host‐specific mimicry and the evolution of host races in Horsfield's bronze‐cuckoo Chalcites basalis . Evolution 62:1689–1699. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lanyon, W.E. 1957. The comparative biology of the meadowlarks (Sturnella) in Wisconsin. Nuttall Ornithological Club, Cambridge, Massachusetts. [Google Scholar]
- Lepage, D. 2017. Regional checklists of the birds of the world. Database, https://avibase.bsc-eoc.org/checklist.jsp?region=index&lang=EN&list=clements.
- Madden, J.R. , and Davies N.B.. 2006. A host‐race difference in begging calls of nestling cuckoos Cuculus canorus develops through experience and increases host provisioning. Proc. R. Soc. B 273:2343–2351. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marler, P. 1970a. A comparative approach to vocal learning: Song development in white‐crowned sparrows. J. Compar. Physiol. Psychol. 71(2):1–25. [Google Scholar]
- Marler, P. 1970b. Birdsong and speech development: could there be parallels? There may be basic rules governing vocal learning to which many species conform, including man. Am. Sci. 58(6):669–673. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marler, P. 1976. An ethological theory of the origin of vocal learning. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 280:386–395. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marshall, A.M. 1950The function of vocal mimicry in birds. Emu 60:5–16. [Google Scholar]
- Mountjoy, D.J. , and Lemon R.E.. 1996. Female choice for complex song in the European starling: a field experiment. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 38:65–71. [Google Scholar]
- Nicolai, J. 1959. Familientradition in der Gesangsentwicklung des Gimpels (Pyrrhula pyrrhula). J. Ornithol. 100:39–46. [Google Scholar]
- Nottebohm, F. 1972. The origins of vocal learning. Am. Nat. 106:116–140. [Google Scholar]
- Nottebohm, F. , and Liu W‐C.. 2010. The origins of vocal learning: new sounds, new circuits, new cells. Brain Lang. 115(1):3–17. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Percy, D.M. , Taylor G.S., and Kennedy M.. 2006. Psyllid communication: acoustic diversity, mate recognition and phylogenetic signal. Invertebrate Systematics 20:431–445. [Google Scholar]
- Peterson, R.T. , and Chalif E.L. (eds). 1973. A field guide to Mexican birds: Mexico, Guatemala, Belize, El Salvador. Houghton Mifflin Company. [Google Scholar]
- Pollard, J. 1930. Whisper song. Emu 30:62–63. [Google Scholar]
- Prat, Y. , Taub M., and Yovel Y.. 2015. Vocal learning in a social mammal: demonstrated by isolation and playback experiments in bats. Science Advances 1:e1500019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Satischandra, S.H.K. , Kodituwakku P., Kotagama S.W., and Goodale E.. 2010. Assessing “false” alarm calls by a drongos (Dicrurus paradiseus) in mixed‐species bird flocks. Behav. Ecol. 21:396–403. [Google Scholar]
- Seddon, N. 2005. Ecological adaptation and species recognition drives vocal evolution in neotropical suboscine birds. Evolution 59:200–215. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Slater, P . 1989. Bird song learning: causes and consequences. Ethology Ecology and Evolution 1: 19–46. [Google Scholar]
- Soha, J. 2017. The auditory template hypothesis: a review and comparative perspective. Anim. Behav. 124:247–254. [Google Scholar]
- Thielcke, G. 1962. Gesangsgeographische Variation des Gartenbaumläufers (Certhia brachydactyla) im Hinblick auf das Artbildungsproblem. Ethology 22:542–566. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thielcke, G. 1972. Waldbaumläufer (Certhia familiaris) ahmen artfremdes Signal nach und reagieren darauf. J. Orn. 113:287–296. [Google Scholar]
- Thorogood, H. 1941. Emu 40(4):328. [Google Scholar]
- Thorpe. 1969. The significance of vocal imitation in animals with special reference to birds. Acta Biol. Exp. 29:251–269. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Townsend, C.W. 1924. Mimicry of voice in birds. Auk 41:541–552. [Google Scholar]
- Tretzel, E. 1965. Imitation und Variation von Schäferpfiffen durch Haubenlerchen (Galerida c. cristata): ein Beispiel für spezielle Spottmotiv‐prädisposition. Ethology 22:784–809. [Google Scholar]
- Tretzel, E. 1966. Spottmotiv Predisposition und akustische Abstraction bei Gartengrasmücken (Sylvia borin borin). Zool. Anz. Suppl. 30:333–343. [Google Scholar]
- Tretzel, E. 1967. Imitation und Transposition menschlicher Pfiffe durch Amseln (Turdus m. meruhi): Ein weiterer Nachweis relative Lernens und akustischer Abstraktion bei Vögeln. Ethology 24:137–161. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tubaro, P.L. 1991. Can Troglodytes aedon in Argentina "mimic" the songs of Thryomanes bewickii? Condor 93:443–445. [Google Scholar]
- Turner, E.L. 1929. Stray leaves from nature's notebook. Country Life, London, 39–43. [Google Scholar]
- Tyack, P.L. 2007. Convergence of calls as animals form social bonds, active compensation for noisy communication channels, and the evolution of vocal learning in mammals. J. Comp. Psychol. 122(3):319–331. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- West, M.J. , and King A.P.. 1990. Mozart's starling. Am. Sci. 78:106–114. [Google Scholar]
- Weydemeyer, W. 1929. An unusual case of mimicry by a catbird. Condor 32:124–125. [Google Scholar]
- Whitman, E. 2016. Yellow‐throated Euphonia (Euphonia hirundinacea), version 1.0 In Neotropical Birds Online (Schulenberg T. S., Editor). Cornell Lab of Ornithology, Ithaca, NY, USA: 10.2173/nb.yeteup1.01 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Whittle, C.L. 1922. Additional data regarding the famous Arnold Arboretum mockingbird. Auk 39:496–506. [Google Scholar]
- Wootton, J.T. 1996. Purple finch (Haemorhous purpureus) In Poole A., ed. The Birds of North America. Cornell Laboratory of Ornithology, Ithaca; retrieved from The Birds of North America: https://birdsna.org. [Google Scholar]
- Wright, H.W. 1921. The mockingbird in the Boston region and in New England and Canada. Auk 38(3):382–432. [Google Scholar]
- Xenocanto. 2018. Naturalis Biodiversity Center. Audio archive, http://www.xeno-canto.org. See Appendix I for list of relevant recordings.
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Appendix I.
Figure S1
Table S1
Table S2
Supporting information
Data Availability Statement
The full database, methods, and supplemental R script can be found in Supporting Information Materials.