Abstract
Understanding the relationship between people and their soundscapes in an urban context of innumerable and diverse sensory stimulations is a difficult endeavor. What public space users hear and how they evaluate it in relation to their performed or intended activities can influence users’ engagement with their spaces as well as their assessment of suitability of public space for their needs or expectations. While the interaction between the auditory experience and activity is a topic gaining momentum in soundscape research, capturing the complexity of this relationship in context remains a multifaceted challenge. In this paper, we address this challenge by researching the user-soundscape relationships in relation to users’ activities. Building on previous soundscape studies, we explore the role and interaction of three potentially influencing factors in users’ soundscape evaluations: level of social interaction of users’ activities, familiarity and expectations, and we employ affordance theory to research the ways in which users bring their soundscapes into use. To this end, we employ a mixed methods design, combining quantitative, qualitative and spatial analyses to analyze how users of three public spaces in Amsterdam evaluate their soundscapes in relation to their activities. We documented the use of an urban park in Amsterdam through non-intrusive behavioral mapping to collect spatial data on observable categories of activities, and integrated our observations with on site questionnaires on ranked soundscape evaluations and free responses detailing users’ evaluations, collected at the same time from park users. One of our key findings is that solitary and socially interactive respondents evaluate their soundscapes differently in relation to their activities, with the latter offering higher suitability and lower disruption ratings than the former; this points to qualitatively different auditory experiences, analyzed further based on users’ open-ended justifications for their evaluations. We provide a methodological contribution (adding to existing soundscape evaluation methodologies), an empirical contribution (providing insight on how users explain their soundscape evaluations in relation to their activities) and a policy and design-related contribution, offering additional insight on a transferable methodology and process that practitioners can employ in their work on the built environment to address the multisensory experience of public spaces.
Keywords: soundscape evaluation, activity, public space, familiarity, expectation, affordance
Introduction
Research shows that urban sound affects the health and well-being of urbanites in a significant manner, at the same time influencing the use and appreciation of public spaces (Mehta, 2014; Van Kempen et al., 2014). Given this demonstrated importance of sound as part of the urban experience, scientists and practitioners alike have sought to develop strategies to research and influence the relationship between urbanites and their soundscapes, on the one hand to minimize the potential negative effects of sound on urban life, and on the other hand to maximize the opportunities for enjoyment or relaxation that urban sound offers. Whilst extensive attention has been paid to aspects of soundscape evaluation that could potentially feed into effective urban sound policies (Andringa et al., 2013), capturing the complexity of the qualitative urban auditory experience in context (in a real life setting) remains a challenge.
The challenge has both methodological and empirical dimensions, as well as policy and design implications. Strategies focused on how users of various urban spaces evaluate their soundscapes are relatively common in both soundscape research as well as urban policy or practice-related initiatives (see e.g., Axelsson et al., 2010; Booi and van den Berg, 2012; Lercher et al., 2016). However, the conventional methods and tools to study those evaluations are limited in their scope. For example, with regards to public spaces, evaluations are currently mostly collected using questionnaires, largely disregarding other (potentially less invasive) methods that can contribute to a more holistic understanding of the relationship between public space users and their soundscapes, in context. In situ methods like field observation (and behavioral mapping) are still rarely used in soundscape research and are currently in the “experimental” stage of implementation with inconsistent results (see Steele et al., 2016; Aletta et al., 2016b; Bild et al., 2018; Lavia et al., 2018, for different approaches). Furthermore, the questionnaires used as tools to gain insight on users’ soundscape evaluations mostly employ categorical-based assessments and rarely include open-ended questions (see Yang and Kang, 2005; Raimbault, 2006, the work in the “Positive Soundscapes Project”1, Nielbo et al., 2013; Bild et al., 2018 for examples), thus representing a limited understanding of users’ soundscape evaluations. Finally, these methods minimize or do not adequately account for the role of moderating factors, like activity, in influencing how people evaluate what they hear, despite increasing evidence on activity as a moderating activity for users’ soundscapes (e.g., Aspuru et al., 2011; Bild et al., 2015, 2018; Steffens et al., 2015). The challenge has implications for sound-related urban practice and design initiatives, as it affects the adequate and comprehensive collection and implementation of soundscape knowledge in everyday projects.
In this paper we propose to address these shortcomings in a large-scale, multi-sited urban study based in Amsterdam (Netherlands), where we used a mixed methods approach combining fieldwork observations with questionnaires to capture both reported and “enacted” soundscape evaluations (materialized through public space use). In examining users’ evaluations of their soundscapes in urban public outdoor spaces, we rely on users’ activities as a key variable that can influence their evaluations, and, through that, the current and future use of the urban public space (see Nielbo et al., 2013; Steffens et al., 2017; Bild et al., 2018 for comparable approaches). With this in mind, this paper aims to understand the factors that can influence and moderate, both separately and together, how users of three different public spaces evaluate their soundscapes in relation to their on-site activities. Previous soundscape studies indicate three potential factors that can affect the user-soundscape relationship: the level of social interaction of users’ activities, users’ (auditory) expectations and users’ familiarity with the space and with what they hear. To research how the factors interact while influencing users’ evaluations of their soundscapes in relation to their activities, we integrate the concept of affordance (Gibson, 1977, 1979) as a conceptual framework for understanding the user-soundscape relationship, focusing on how people bring their soundscapes “into use” in their everyday life (Ingold, 2000), through their activities.
We aim to answer the following two research questions:
-
simple (1)
To what extent do the level of social interaction of users’ activities, users’ expectations, and users’ familiarity (with the space and with what is heard) influence their soundscape evaluations in relation to their activities, and how are the factors associated?
-
simple (2)
What are the possible reasons for those associations and how do users describe the factors in relation to their soundscapes?
The scientific work that we build on in this paper is detailed in Section “Background.” The data collection and analysis methods are discussed in Section “Materials and Method” and the findings of the analysis are covered in Section “Results.” We discuss the research and practice-related gaps that we address in detail in the following section and in the concluding discussion (see section “Discussion”); in the latter we also outline the three contributions of this study: empirical, methodological and policy and design-oriented.
Background
The evaluation of soundscapes is at the center of efforts of scientists from disciplines as diverse as psychology or anthropology, particularly of those working at the intersection of theoretical and applied research, as they aim to understand how users of various urban public and private spaces engage with and relate to what they hear, and how that influences the quality of their experience. In this section, we review the scientific literature key to the evaluation of soundscapes and develop the analytical model described below guiding the empirical research (Figure 1). First, we discuss studies exploring the role of activity in soundscape evaluations, including the specific effect of level of social interaction of one’s activities. Then we review studies researching the role of other factors influencing soundscape evaluations, like one’s previous experience, as it relates to expectation and familiarity, on these evaluations (discussed below in detail and summarized in the analytical model proposed in Figure 1). Finally, we use the concept of affordance to understand how users of public spaces bring their soundscapes into use through their engagement with and activities performed in their public spaces (Ingold, 2000; Steenson and Rodger, 2015).
Soundscape Evaluations and Activity
Scientific efforts have been made to determine soundscape descriptors and indicators that can help explain or predict users’ soundscape evaluations (Nilsson et al., 2007; Jennings and Cain, 2013; Steenson and Rodger, 2015; Herranz-Pascual et al., 2017), with an eye on operationalizing this knowledge and implementing it in sound-related practices. The dominant approach studies evaluations in relation to sound/soundscape quality (see Schulte-Fortkamp and Fiebig, 2016) and integrates aspects of pleasantness (Raimbault, 2006; Axelsson et al., 2010; Can et al., 2016; Herranz-Pascual et al., 2017, inter alios) and quietness (Pheasant et al., 2008; Booi and van den Berg, 2012; Bloomfield, 2014; Aspuru et al., 2016), usually in contrast with annoyance (see e.g., Lercher and Schulte-Fortkamp, 2003; Andringa and Lanser, 2013).
While the role of users’ activities as a variable potentially influencing their relationship with their soundscapes has been suggested before (Dubois, 2000; Lercher and Schulte-Fortkamp, 2003), the effective and explicit integration of activity in scientific research with a focus on urban public spaces is still in its incipient, exploratory phase (Aletta et al., 2016b; Bild et al., 2016, 2018; Lavia et al., 2016; Steffens et al., 2017). Most of these research projects arise from more practice-oriented questions, either dealing with specific soundscape interventions with some form of behavioral control in mind (see Lavia et al., 2012, 2016), or emphasizing the role users’ soundscapes play in relaxation or rehabilitation activities or in relation to auditory comfort, both indoors and outdoors (Mzali, 2002; Delepaut, 2009; Cerwén et al., 2016; Filipan et al., 2017). Consequently, many questions remain on how best to define and operationalize activity in empirical studies and what methods are suited for researching the relationship between soundscape evaluations and activities in an ecologically valid manner (Guastavino et al., 2005). For example, one laboratory study demonstrated that various soundscape recordings were evaluated as being appropriate2 for different imagined activities by participants in a listening experiment (Nielbo et al., 2013); it, however, remains unclear how we can transfer the outcomes from research performed in a laboratory to research performed on-site. We address this issue by furthering the exploration of the aforementioned relationship with a focus on understanding the role activity plays in influencing public space users’ soundscape evaluations. We base part of our inquiry on preliminary studies in the field, showing that the level of social interaction of users’ activities has an influence on how users evaluate their soundscapes in relation to their activities (Bild et al., 2018) and a marginal effect on their soundscape descriptions. In other words, to what extent does whether users are alone or with others influence how they evaluate their soundscapes in relation to what they were doing (e.g., talking, reading, sunbathing)?
Influencing Factor: Expectation
Bruce and Davies (2014) relate soundscape expectations to Truax’s concept of “soundscape competence” (2001), referring to the ability of users of a space to interpret and make sense of what they hear, based on previous experience, and framing soundscape expectations for future situations. Filipan et al. (2017) suggest that the presence or absence of certain expected sounds in a context (like a park) can affect users’ evaluations of their soundscapes in terms of, e.g., tranquility. Along similar lines, Bruce et al. (2009, p. 6) argued that a user’s soundscape “becomes an issue when it does not conform to subjects ‘perceived’ sense of normality or interferes with information […] transfer,” thus not conforming to the users’ expectations. The complexity of expectations in relation to one’s experience has been explored extensively apropos music (see e.g., Huron, 2006) and only recently has it been researched explicitly in relation to soundscape (Bruce et al., 2009, 2015; Bruce and Davies, 2014). We build on the conclusions of the latter research avenue, particularly their preliminary findings on the effect of users’ expectations from the space and what they hear on their soundscape evaluations, as well as what users refer to as “expected activities” within the space, as influenced by their soundscapes (Bruce and Davies, 2014).
Influencing Factor: Familiarity
Familiarity is understood as “how usual or common a stimulus is in the subject’s realm of experience” (Marcell et al., 2000, p. 834), referring to the previous experience of the user with their space, which includes their frequency of use of a space as well as activities performed in the space (Kogan et al., 2017). Particularly for the auditory domain, “familiarity” is one of the three factors that influence the “identifiability” of sounds along with “complexity” (Marcell et al., 2000) and “pleasantness,” as well as one of the three features or perceptual attributes that Axelsson found to be most relevant for users’ evaluations of their soundscapes (third after pleasantness and eventfulness – Axelsson et al., 2010). Axelsson found that variance in familiarity ratings tends to be low for urban respondents sharing a similar cultural framework, thus implying a limited applicability of the feature for design initiatives (Axelsson et al., 2010). We nonetheless consider that users’ reported familiarity both with the space and with what they hear provides valuable insight into users’ evaluations of their soundscapes in relation to their intended or performed activities; familiarity is essential in relation to aspects of expectations, and failure or success to meet them, as it relies on users’ previous knowledge and experience.
Soundscape and Affordance
Considering the activity-centered approach we take in this paper, we integrate the concept of (auditory) affordances in a public space context. In Gibson’s formulation, affordances are defined as the qualities of an object or an environment that allow for the performance of an activity (Gibson, 1977). Turvey (1992, p. 174) describes an auditory affordance as a way to “provide a description of the environment that was directly relevant to behavior.”. Affordances have been discussed and used previously in auditory research particularly in relation to music, in reference to what music can afford to a listener (see DeNora, 2000; Clarke, 2005; Reybrouck, 2012, inter alios). There have also been proposals and strategies for integrating the concept in soundscape research (Thibaud, 1998; Pecqueux, 2012; Nielbo et al., 2013; Nielbo, 2015; Steenson and Rodger, 2015) to more accurately address the complexities of user-soundscape relationships and articulate the role that users’ soundscapes play in guiding or informing their public space experiences and uses. We follow Steenson and Rodger’s reading of Gibson in relation to the auditory domain, suggesting that “auditory information is formed relationally, emerging with the situated activity of the agent” (2015, p. 181). We build on the work of Pecqueux (2012), who expands on the idea of affordances elicited by sounds in urban settings (p. 221) and demonstrates the relevance of an activity-centered strategy to researching the urban auditory experience, with implications for design practice. In our approach, we also extend on the idea of “actualization of affordances” (Kyttä, 2002; Stoffregen, 2003), that is, turning possibilities for action into actual activities, focusing on understanding how sounds are brought into use in an urban context (Steenson and Rodger, 2015). We articulate the notion that, by affording users’ activities, users’ soundscapes can enable or impede their activities.
Proposed Analytical Model
Figure 1 summarizes the various strands of soundscape research to understand the individual and interaction effect of three factors over users’ soundscape evaluations in relation to their performed activities: (1) the level of the social interaction of users’ performed activities (i.e., solitary vs. socially interactive), (2) expectation (including expectation from the space and from what is heard), and (3) familiarity (with a focus on familiarity with the space and with what is heard). The analytical model informs our mixed methods approach to the evaluation of soundscapes in relation to activity detailed in the next section.
Materials and Methods
To research to what extent the level of social interaction of users’ activities, users’ expectations, and users’ familiarity (with the space and with what is heard) influence their soundscape evaluations in relation to their activities, and how these factors are associated, we combined quantitative, qualitative and spatial methods in the collection and analysis stages as part of a mixed methods approach (see Creswell and Plano Clark, 2011). Mixed methods approaches are common in soundscape research, as the complexity of people’s urban experiences cannot be fully grasped in mono-method studies (Bloomfield, 2014; Aletta et al., 2016a; Herranz-Pascual et al., 2017; Bild et al., 2018). They are conducive to a more nuanced, situated and integrated exploration of the relationship between users of public spaces and their soundscapes, in context (see also Knigge and Cope, 2006 with respect to the integration of qualitative and quantitative data).
In our research, we relied on a combination of on-site data collection methods, including self-completion questionnaires with randomly selected public space users, and non-participant observation of activities performed in the selected public spaces. The questionnaires included both soundscape evaluations/ratings as well as open-ended questions asking respondents to reflect on their ratings. We collected different types of data suited for both quantitative and qualitative analyses, ultimately contributing to a multi-layered understanding of users’ on-site experience in relation to their activity as follows. The quantitative analysis allowed us to measure potential differences in soundscape ratings between public space users performing different activities and to test the role of various factors in influencing these ratings; the qualitative analysis offered a more nuanced understanding of users’ ratings as well as an in-depth exploration of the reasons behind the aforementioned potential differences between user groups. The non-participant observation of activities was done to situate users’ auditory experiences and their soundscape evaluations in a spatial and behavioral context. In the following sections, we first describe the data collection, including the research design, fieldwork locations and data collection methods, and then elaborate on the data analysis methods.
Data Collection
We employed a mixed methods research design relying on parallel data gathering. Building on previous pilot studies (Steele et al., 2016; Bild et al., 2018), we combined field observations with on-site questionnaire data collection in a multi-sited field research. We collected data in three public spaces (two large urban public parks and one small urban “plein”/square) over the summer of 2016. 208 self-completion questionnaires were collected with Dutch public space users in similar weather conditions (sunny, warm, and dry), during two data collection sessions per space. Two types of data were collected at the same time: (1) using questionnaires, the ratings and open-ended responses on users’ experiences, and, (2) using field observation, the patterns of occupancy of the public space by solitary and socially interactive users (including the spatial position of users who completed the questionnaire).
Fieldwork Locations
The fieldwork was conducted in various areas of three different locations (Figure 2): two traditional urban parks (Oosterpak and Sarphatipark) and one smaller square-park hybrid (Frederiksplein). The spaces are located in central Amsterdam and were selected due to their heavy use for leisure purposes. They represent typical Dutch urban public spaces that can be split in smaller areas bordered by paths and greenery, and are designed with diverse amenities encouraging mixed use and users (see Table 1 below).
Table 1.
Location | Description | Amenities |
---|---|---|
Oosterpark | Large urban park | Large green fields Benches Pond with waterfront green areas Paths Gray/built open area with benches and other sitting possibilities |
Sarphatipark | Large urban park | Large green fields Benches Pond with waterfront green areas Paths Water fountain |
Frederiksplein | Smaller-sized square-park hybrid Transition space (from center to adjoining neighborhood) | Gray/built open area Benches Paths Water fountain with benches surrounding it Tram tracks cutting through the space |
Questionnaire Data Collection
Questionnaire design
The aim of the questionnaire was to understand whether soundscapes were evaluated as affording users’ activities in a public space context by researching users’ soundscape evaluations in relation to their activities. Questionnaires used in previous research on soundscape evaluations tend to address experiences of spaces in relation to perceptions of pleasantness or eventfulness (e.g., Axelsson et al., 2010; Herranz-Pascual et al., 2017), rarely going in depth on the relationship between use of space and soundscape evaluation. These lines of questioning usually rely on semantic scales and seldom employ additional open-ended questions asking respondents to expand on their evaluations, effectively limiting their applicability in practice (Raimbault, 2006; Nielbo et al., 2013). Current standardized protocols (e.g., the “Soundscape Quality Protocol” – SSQP, Axelsson et al., 2010) might prove insufficient to collect insight useful for both urban researchers aiming to understand the user-soundscape relationship as well as city makers interested in developing spaces with sound in mind, as they would not offer substantial insight into what in users’ soundscapes is perceived as disrupting or suitable for their activities or purposes of use of space. We addressed these two challenges in the design of the questionnaire by focusing on users’ soundscape evaluations in relation to their activity and by combining Likert items with open-ended questions in one questionnaire to understand how users reflect on the effect of their soundscapes on their activities and explain potential discrepancies in their evaluations (see Table 2).
Table 2.
Variable | Question/statement (translation EN) | Question/statement (original NL) | Type of response |
---|---|---|---|
Activity (including level of social interaction) | Think back on the activities you perform in this park and describe them in as much detail as possible. | Denk terug aan uw activiteiten in dit park vandaag en beschrijf deze zo uitgebreid mogelijk. | Open-ended response. Coded for level of social interaction |
Disruption | The performance of my activities was disrupted by what I heard. | Het uitvoeren van mijn activiteiten werd verstoord door wat ik hoorde. | Likert item: 1–5 (1, “I completely disagree” to 5, “I completely agree”) |
Stimulation | The performance of my activities was stimulated by what I heard. | Het uitvoeren van mijn activiteiten werd gestimuleerd door wat ik hoorde. | Likert item: 1-5 |
Explanation of disruption or stimulation ratings | In what ways did what you hear disrupt or stimulate the activities you performed? | Op welke manieren werd het uitvoeren van uw activiteiten verstoord dan wel gestimuleerd door wat u hoorde? | Open-ended response |
Suitability | What I heard was suitable for the activities that I performed. | Dat wat ik hoorde was toepasselijk voor de activiteiten die ik uitvoerde. | Likert item: 1–5 |
Expectations | Did you have expectations about this park before you came here? If yes, what were they? | Had u verwachtingen over het park voordat u hier kwam? Zo ja, wat verwachtte u? | Open-ended response |
Satisfaction of expectations | In what respect did what you hear match (or not) your expectations? | In hoeverre voldeed wat u hoorde aan uw verwachtingen? | Open-ended response |
Familiarity with what is heard | I am familiar with what I heard during the activities I performed. | Ik was bekend met wat ik hoorde tijdens het uitvoeren van mijn activiteiten. | Likert item: 1–5 |
Familiarity with space (i.e., frequency of use of space) | How often do you visit this park? | Hoe vaak bezoekt u dit park? | Categorical scale: 1–4 (from 1, “Once a week,” to 4, “It is my first time here”) |
Age | What is your age? | Wat is uw leeftijd? | Continuous |
Original Dutch and English translation.
Based on the analytical model outlined in Figure 1, we aimed to research the potential influence of three factors on users’ soundscape evaluations in relation to activity, i.e., the level of social interaction of their activity, their familiarity (with what is heard and with the space), and their expectation, and whether these three factors interact for a stronger effect. Additionally, as indicated by literature, we also explored the potential effect of age and gender to influence auditory experiences and potentially the aforementioned evaluations (see e.g., Yang and Kang, 2005).
To understand whether their soundscapes afforded their on-site activities, we asked users to evaluate their soundscapes from three perspectives: in terms of disruption, stimulation and overall suitability; we afterwards asked for detailed explanations of their evaluations (see Table 2 below). Stimulation is a common term used in relation to soundscapes and particularly in soundscape evaluation usually used as an adjective (Axelsson et al., 2010; Botteldooren et al., 2015), but we use it as an active verb (“to stimulate”). While some authors prefer “to disturb” (and “disturbance”) to convey a similar message (see e.g., Lercher et al., 2016), we selected “to disrupt” as an antonym for “stimulate,” due to its nature as a transitive verb as well as its common use in relation to activity (e.g., Truax, 2001). We did not introduce the concept of “soundscape” in the questionnaire, as we wanted to ensure the statements were phrased in a “natural,” everyday language, allowing respondents to focus on their experience rather than on relating to a new concept.
Questionnaire data collection protocol
We approached park users who were usually seated (not in transit), and were willing to engage with the data collector and complete the questionnaire; the questionnaires were completed by native Dutch speakers. Park users were handed clipboards and pens, and were invited to fill out the questionnaires themselves. The data collector offered clarifications when needed. We gathered 188 questionnaires in the three fieldwork locations (Oosterpark: 81 questionnaires, Sarphatipark: 83, Frederiksplein: 24), as part of two data collection sessions per location (one in the weekend and one during the week).
Non-participant Observation
To situate the questionnaire data on users’ soundscape evaluations in relation to their activities in a spatial and behavioral context, we also relied on systematic non-participant observation as a fieldwork method, more specifically, behavioral mapping (see e.g., Cosco et al., 2010; Goličnik and Thompson, 2010; Bild et al., 2018). Field observation (Aletta et al., 2016b; Lavia et al., 2016) has been increasingly integrated in urban soundscape research, particularly to document the effects of certain acoustic interventions on the ways in which people engage with and act in their public spaces3. Documenting public space use is crucial for on-site studies, as it shows how users relate to and behave in their physical (built) environments and how this relationship can further connect with their soundscape evaluations. By spatially mapping and situating the evaluations of users and their engagement with its amenities and with each other, we can explore how their physical environments and their soundscapes may interact to influence their urban experience in relation to their activities.
In this paper, using a behavioral mapping application4, we gathered data on the level of social interaction of activities performed by public space users (individual, in pairs or in groups), in parallel with the collection of questionnaires, as part of hour-long sessions throughout the research period. The behavioral mapping resulted in a total of 665 distinct data points, referring to both individual users and users in groups in all three locations, in selected areas of each location. The 665 points include the 188 questionnaire respondents/public space users5, and are marked on the resulting behavioral maps (Figure 3 in section Contextual Maps”).
Data Analysis
To answer our two research questions, we analyzed the questionnaire data using a sequential approach, first statistically analyzing the responses to the closed-ended questions and afterwards qualitatively analyzing the responses to the open-ended questions. The quantitative analysis served to establish potential patterns in the ways in which solitary and socially interactive respondents evaluate their soundscapes in relation to their activities and the role of, e.g., familiarity as a factor in influencing the evaluation. The qualitative analysis, for which we transcribed and combined the open-ended responses of the questionnaire from all three public spaces, helped to interpret the potential inter-group differences in evaluation and to provide richer, more nuanced knowledge on soundscapes as affordances for respondents’ activities, including exploring the role of expectation as a further factor influencing the evaluation. We created contextual maps to situate users’ questionnaire responses in a spatial and behavioral context.
Quantitative Analysis
The variables used in the quantitative analysis are described in Table 3 below.
Table 3.
Dependent variables | |
Disruption | 5-point Likert item |
Stimulation | 5-point Likert item |
Suitability | 5-point Likert item |
Independent variables | |
Level of social interaction | Binary variable (“solitary” and “socially interactive”) |
Familiarity with what is heard∗ | Ordinal variable (“low and medium familiarity,” “high familiarity,” “very high familiarity”) |
Frequency of use of public space | Ordinal variable (“this is my first visit,” “a few times per year,” “at least once a month,” “at least once a week”) |
Location | Categorical variable; three distinct locations (“Sarphatipark,” “Oosterpark,” “Frederiksplein”) |
Age∗∗ | Binary variable: “35 or younger” and “older than 35” |
Gender | Binary variable: “male,” and “female” |
∗The original five categories were collapsed in three for group comparison: “low and medium familiarity” (including “very low,” “low,” and “medium familiarity”), “high familiarity” and “very high familiarity.” ∗∗The original continuous “age” variable was collapsed in two for group comparison.
The three dependent variables in the analysis – disruption, stimulation and suitability were measured on a 5-point ordinal scale and in our data were non-normally distributed, so we relied on two non-parametric tests for our analysis (Ruxton, 2006). First, using the Kruskal–Wallis test, we tested whether there are statistically significant differences between the categories of the independent variables on each of the three soundscape evaluations. Second, we applied the Mann–Whitney U test to investigate whether soundscape evaluations differed significantly between activity types (solitary or socially interactive) according to frequency of use, familiarity with what is heard, location, age and gender. We considered relationships with p< 0.05 as statistically significant. We also discussed cases where p< 0.1 to indicate trends in the data, given the limited sample and number of variables we had at our disposal. The quantitative analysis was performed with the help of statistics software (SPSS version 19).
Qualitative Analysis
We performed an in-depth analysis of responses to the open-ended questions that respondents provided when asked to explain how their soundscape stimulated or disrupted their activities (if at all), focusing on what was disrupting/stimulating (cause) and what was disrupted/stimulated (effect). We also analyzed how respondents articulated their (auditory) expectations and whether they were met during their time in the space. Our thematic coding approach was inspired by previous work on soundscape and place expectations (Bruce and Davies, 2014), focusing on respondents’ expectations from the space itself, their auditory expectations (namely expected sounds), what they expected to experience in the space as well as expectations from others present in the space. We contrasted the answers of respondents performing solitary activities with those performing socially interactive activities.
Contextual Maps
We visualized the data collected through behavioral mapping for the three fieldwork locations using GIS-based methods to situate the data on soundscape evaluation in a spatial setting. The resulting maps show the spatial distribution of questionnaires and are accompanied by an overview of patterns of occupancy for each observed location, in relation to the level of social interaction of users’ activities, illustrating the social interaction context within which the questionnaire responses were collected.
Results
Contextual Maps
We begin with the analysis of the maps resulting from the behavioral mapping process (Figure 3) as they play a descriptive role, that is, to illustrate the larger context in which the questionnaires were filled out in terms of patterns of use based on the level of social interaction of the activities performed. The maps for each public space are an aggregation of the data collected during the two sessions per space and visualize the use of space exclusively in the areas where the behavioral mapping was carried out (marked with light gray in the resulting maps); the other areas have not been observed due to practical reasons, yet they were also consistently frequented by users.
The maps clearly show that socially interactive users are dominant in the space, throughout all three locations. The main observed physical factors influencing the distribution of use and subsequent concentration of users were: the surface materials (i.e., pavement or grass), presence/absence of shade (influenced by trees and other greenery), location and presence of conventional seating amenities (i.e., benches) or other elements that could used as seating amenities (e.g., other built structures), points of attraction (e.g., water fountains), proximity to bodies of water (i.e., ponds), and proximity to foot/bicycle paths.
The less dense, more spread out occupancy of the large open area in, e.g., the Western part of Oosterpark or all of Sarphatipark was influenced by the existence of conventional seating amenities mainly along the foot/bicycle paths, with large open grass fields in between. We observed the clustering of users both in Eastern Oosterpark and throughout Frederiksplein. This could be due to the lack of grass where users could sit on and the dominance of various seating amenities (users, mostly socially interactive, also sat on the side of the fountain in Frederiksplein, and on round, elevated built structures in Oosterpark). The users closest to the body of water in Easter Oosterpark, largely performing socially interactive activities, were facing the water while sitting on grass, whereas solitary users mostly faced the water from a larger distance, while sitting on benches. The clear dominance and clustering of socially interactive users in the NE section of Sarphatipark was due to three separate birthday celebrations taking place at the same time, bringing together large groups of users.
The location of the completed questionnaires document in Figure 3 demonstrates that the sample of users approached to complete our questionnaires is representative for the distribution of users in space in the timeframe and the locations where we conducted our research, with socially interactive users dominant across spaces, usually occupying the larger grass fields (generally in the sun), and solitary users equally distributed between the open fields and seating amenities closer to the paths (the latter generally in the shade).
Quantitative Results: Statistical Analyses of Soundscape Evaluations
The sample distribution according to the main variables (Table 4) shows that, for the dependent variables, the vast majority of respondents (86%) evaluated their soundscapes as having low or very low disruption values, while no respondent evaluated them as being very disrupting. The sample was split rather evenly for stimulation ratings (low, medium and high stimulation), with around 30% of respondents each. The majority of respondents (67%) evaluated their soundscapes as highly or very highly suitable for their activities. Most of the respondents were participating in socially interactive activities. The sample was divided rather evenly also by frequency, with 47% visiting the locations at least once a month. The vast majority of respondents (90%) stated to be highly or very highly familiar with their soundscapes. 76% were 35 or younger and a slight majority of the sample identified as female.
Table 4.
Variable | Values | N (%) |
---|---|---|
Level of social interaction | Socially interactive | 127 (67.6%) |
Solitary | 61 (32.4%) | |
Frequency of use of space | This is my first visit | 30 (16%) |
A few times per year | 70 (37.2%) | |
At least once a month | 58 (30.9%) | |
At least once a week | 30 (16%) | |
Familiarity with what is heard | Low and medium familiarity | 18 (9.6%) |
High familiarity | 57 (30.3%) | |
Very high familiarity | 113 (60.1%) | |
Location | Sarphatipark | 83 (44.1%) |
Oosterpark | 81 (43.1%) | |
Frederiksplein | 24 (12.8%) | |
Age | 35 or younger | 143 (76.1%) |
Older than 35 | 45 (23.9%) | |
Gender | Male | 85 (45.2%) |
Female | 103 (54.8%) | |
Disruption | Very low disruption | 111 (59%) |
Low disruption | 58 (30.9%) | |
Medium disruption | 11 (5.9%) | |
High disruption | 8 (4.3%) | |
Very high disruption | 0 | |
Stimulation | Very low stimulation | 29 (15.4%) |
Low stimulation | 29 (15.4%) | |
Medium stimulation | 68 (36.2%) | |
High stimulation | 47 (25%) | |
Very high stimulation | 15 (8%) | |
Suitability | Very low suitability | 11 (5.9%) |
Low suitability | 11 (5.9%) | |
Medium suitability | 41 (21.8%) | |
High suitability | 80 (42.6%) | |
Very high suitability | 45 (23.9%) |
(N = 188).
The distribution of soundscape ratings split by level of social interaction is presented in Figure 4. For disruption, a larger share of solitary respondents evaluated their soundscapes as having very high levels of disruption than socially interactive users; 11% of solitary respondents evaluated their soundscapes as highly or very highly disruptive, compared to 1% of socially interactive respondents. For stimulation, a larger share of solitary users evaluated their soundscape as having very low or low levels of stimulation: 36% compared to 28% of socially interactive users. Finally, for suitability, a smaller share of solitary respondents evaluated their soundscapes as highly or very highly suitable (54% of respondents compared to 72% of socially interactive respondents).
The Kruskal–Wallis test was conducted to evaluate the differences between the categories of the five independent variables (level of social interaction, frequency of use of space, familiarity with what is heard, location and age) on the three soundscape evaluations (disruption, stimulation and suitability). The results (Table 5) showed that there was a significant difference in suitability rating for all independent variables, albeit a weak significance for level of social interaction; there was also a significant difference in disruption ratings between the three locations. These differences demonstrate the relevance of the independent variables in influencing the extent to which respondents’ soundscapes are perceived to afford/be suitable for their on-site activities. Overall, the tests showed that the independent variables included in this research are related mainly with suitability ratings and minimally disruption ratings. The independent variables do not significantly relate with stimulation ratings. This suggests that “suitability” is the clearest construct for respondents to grasp, while “stimulation,” and to an extent “disruption,” are somewhat more challenging to assess.
Table 5.
Variables | Disruption | Stimulation | Suitability |
---|---|---|---|
Level of social interaction | 1.668 | 0.739 | 3.523∗ |
Frequency of use of space | 2.270 | 5.849 | 13.066∗∗ |
Familiarity with what is heard | 4.027 | 2.222 | 11.230∗∗ |
Location | 17.897∗∗ | 2.543 | 9.474∗∗ |
Age | 0.416 | 0.547 | 4.547∗∗ |
Chi-square values reported. P-value significance: ∗∗ for p < 0.05, ∗ for p < 0.1 (trend of significance). N = 188.
To further understand the relationships identified above, we used the Mann–Whitney U test to calculate whether soundscape ratings, grouped by the level of social interaction of respondents’ activities, differ among categories of the independent variables (Table 6).
Table 6.
Variable | Values | Disr. SI (mean rank) | Disr. Sol (mean rank) | Disr. (sig.) Mann–Whitney U | Stim. SI (mean rank) | Stim. Sol (mean rank) | Stim. (sig.) Mann–Whitney U | Suitability SI (mean rank) | Suitability Sol (mean rank) | Suitability (sig) Mann–Whitney U |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Frequency of use of space | This is my first visit | 14.39 | 19.14 | 55.000 | 17.50 | 8.93 | 34.500∗∗ | 17.50 | 8.93 | 34.500∗∗ |
A few times per year | 34.69 | 33.97 | 388.500 | 35.95 | 33.97 | 407.500 | 36.85 | 30.94 | 359.000 | |
At least once a month | 28.13 | 31.59 | 354.500 | 32.26 | 25.30 | 306.000 | 30.37 | 28.17 | 372.000 | |
At least once a week | 14.93 | 16.07 | 104.000 | 12.93 | 18.07 | 74.000∗ | 17.23 | 13.77 | 86.500 | |
Familiarity with what is heard | Low and medium familiarity | 9.58 | 9.33 | 35.000 | 9.92 | 8.67 | 31.000 | 10.29 | 7.92 | 26.500 |
High familiarity | 26.98 | 35.85 | 197.000∗ | 29.41 | 27.62 | 268.000 | 30.00 | 25.62 | 242.000 | |
Very high familiarity | 55.18 | 60.07 | 1362.000 | 58.01 | 55.29 | 1419.000 | 60.96 | 50.30 | 1209.500 | |
Location | Sarphatipark | 39.10 | 51.15 | 447.000∗∗ | 44.15 | 35.23 | 494.500 | 45.36 | 31.43 | 418.500∗∗ |
Oosterpark | 41.64 | 39.79 | 708.000 | 40.12 | 42.66 | 695.500 | 41.88 | 39.34 | 695.500 | |
Frederiksplein | 12.68 | 12.35 | 69.500 | 13.18 | 11.92 | 64.000 | 12.23 | 12.73 | 68.500 | |
Age | 35 or younger | 70.93 | 74.86 | 1916.500 | 72.02 | 71.94 | 2025.500 | 75.52 | 62.62 | 1662.000 |
Older than 35 | 20.22 | 25.91 | 189.000∗ | 26.30 | 19.55 | 177.000∗ | 25.83 | 20.05 | 188.000 | |
Gender | Male | 41.39 | 45.09 | 810.500 | 44.86 | 40.58 | 798.500 | 45.40 | 39.89 | 773.000 |
Female | 50.46 | 57.08 | 826.000 | 52.51 | 50.33 | 908.000 | 54.41 | 44.08 | 758.000 |
Mean rank and Mann–Whitney U values reported (the higher the mean rank, the higher the rating). Dependent variables: disruption (“disr.”), stimulation (“stim.”), and suitability (“suit.”). Level of social interaction: solitary (“Sol”) and socially interactive (“SI”). P-value significance (“sig.”): ∗∗ for p < 0.05, ∗ for p < 0.1 (trend of significance). N = 188.
The Mann–Whitney U test indicated that for those visiting the locations for the first time, socially interactive respondents have significantly higher stimulation ratings than solitary respondents (U = 34.500, p < 0.05), and higher suitability ratings, albeit with a weak significance (U = 34.500, p < 0.05). A possible explanation could be that the locations researched here are more geared toward group activities. Groups were especially dominant in those spaces on sunny days, usually engaged in various – likely audible – interactive activities throughout the observed areas (as seen in the contextual maps in Figure 3). Also among respondents who visit at least weekly, evaluations of stimulation were higher – although weakly significant – for socially interactive respondents than for solitary respondents (U = 74.000, p < 0.1).
For respondents with high familiarity ratings, there is a weakly significant difference between socially interactive and solitary respondents, with the former having lower disruption ratings than the latter (U = 197.000, p < 0.1).
In the particular case of Sarphatipark, socially interactive respondents have significantly lower disruption and significantly higher suitability ratings than solitary respondents (U = 447.000, p < 0.05, and U = 418.5000, p < 0.05, respectively). This suggests that Sarphatipark is uniquely perceived as affording socially interactive activities rather than solitary ones in a significant manner, when compared to the other two locations.
For users older than 35, socially interactive respondents have lower disruption and higher stimulation ratings than solitary respondents, albeit weakly significant (U = 189.000, p < 0.1, and U = 177.000, p < 0.1, respectively). Finally, no significant differences between socially interactive users and solitary ones were found for males and females.
Qualitative Results
The quantitative analysis partially confirmed our literature-driven expectations on the role of age, the level of social interaction of respondents’ activities and of respondents’ familiarity, both with the space and with what they hear, on their soundscape ratings in relation to their activity. The location in which the research was conducted was also identified as having an effect on soundscape ratings, particularly for Sarphatipark. The findings provided little detail on the respondents’ experience that could guide, for example, design interventions, e.g., what they find disrupting or stimulating or what their expectations were from their space and their soundscapes, thus leaving much to speculation. To address this, we relied on qualitative insights from an in-depth analysis of respondents’ explanations of their disruption and stimulation ratings, as well as their expectations, to better understand what specifically in their space and their soundscapes affords (or discourages) their activities. We first grouped the responses of all three spaces together, and categorized respondents’ descriptions of their expectations according to: the type of space they were expecting to find, its amenities, what they expected to hear and how they expected others to use the space. Considering that Sarphatipark stood out in the quantitative analysis as a space evaluated as particularly affording of respondents’ activities, we investigated whether the responses in the park differed from those in the other two fieldwork locations. However, no particular differences were observed, so below we report only on the aggregated data from the three spaces.
Explanation of Disruption and Stimulation Ratings
Respondents described how their soundscapes disrupted and/or stimulated their activities, with a particular emphasis on what in their soundscapes they considered to be disrupting or stimulating.
Disruption
The main source of disruption was, for both solitary and socially interactive respondents, the sounds of others in the space, especially the sounds of loud conversations and of children crying; surprisingly, the sounds of traffic (and public transportation) were mentioned only in passing as a source of disruption, the focus remaining on other public space users and their sound-producing activities. Solitary respondents also tended to cite more holistic reasons for their disturbance (e.g., “city sounds,” “all sounds,” “racket”6) than socially interactive respondents.
Both solitary and socially interactive respondents focused on the disturbing/distracting effect that some sounds had over their own activity: in the case of solitary respondents, what they heard disturbed their thought process or their ability to unwind, whereas for socially interactive respondents, their conversation was interrupted or they had to adjust their speaking levels to be able to understand each other.
Stimulation
While for sources of disruption, there was quite some consensus on which sources are considered disrupting (see above) and a relatively small number of sounds were listed, there was a comparatively larger array of sources of stimulation mentioned by both categories of users. Socially interactive respondents stood out by listing comparatively more aspects of their auditory experience that they considered stimulating, including not only sounds but also using more holistic descriptions like “coziness” (“gezelligheid”). The sources of stimulation were, to relatively equal extents, nature-related sounds (i.e., fountain, birds, water, with socially interactive respondents putting an emphasis on the sound of wind through the leaves of trees) and human activity-related sounds.
Both solitary and socially interactive respondents focused on how what they heard stimulated the “atmosphere” in their space and the effect it had over users, particularly in relation to a relaxing7 effect or to a “holiday feeling”: “the buzz/murmur contributes to a pleasant atmosphere”8. Interestingly enough, solitary respondents focused particularly on how what they heard stimulated hypothetical conversations (“if I hear other people talk, it is also easier for me to talk”9) or doing what they wanted (“I’m stimulated to do what I like”10), e.g., fall asleep (“calming sounds allow me to fall asleep”11). Comparatively, socially interactive respondents further emphasized the importance of the presence of others for coziness and cheerfulness: “The fact that you can hear life around you makes it pleasant and cozy. In either way, it makes [this] pleasant and cozy”.12
Solitary respondents focused more on the effect of what they heard had on their intended or current activities, whereas socially interactive respondents were more embedded in and engaged with their soundscapes, emphasizing not only the quiet dimension of their experience, but also the dynamism generated by the presence of others.
One socially interactive respondent (offering a low disruption rating and a high stimulation rating) summarized the complexity of their relationship with their soundscape: “music offers an atmosphere, so does the water and people. The tram is a bit disturbing but it is allowed here in the city”13.
Not all respondents that offered explanations to their disruption or stimulation ratings identified particular sounds that affected their evaluation. Some respondents focused only on one or two disrupting sounds, stating that “the rest” is neither stimulating nor disruptive. Others stated that some sounds were “distracting,” but that in general they were neither stimulated nor stimulated by what they heard; a sub-group of respondents stated that they were too focused on their activity to be aware of their soundscape: “I was very busy with my own activity so I was not very aware of the ambient sound”14 .
Users’ Expectations From Their On-Site Experiences
A count of occurrences showed that the majority of respondents reported that their expectations were met in all three spaces during their activities; however, only a slight majority of solitary respondents felt their expectations were met, compared to slightly over three quarters of socially interactive respondents. The subtle differences in expectations between solitary and socially interactive respondents indicate slightly different auditory experiences for those who use the public spaces alone or with others. As indicated by Bruce and Davies (2014), public space users tend to expect a limited number of sounds in an urban environment, especially for leisure-related uses and in relation to urban parks. Both categories of respondents expected the sound of fountain and water (due to two of the three public spaces being designed with large water fountains around which users tended to cluster, as shown in “Contextual Maps,” and visualized in Figure 3) as well as “city sounds,” However, socially interactive respondents also expected to hear the sounds of birds, which solitary respondents did not mention in their responses: “quiet environment with a fountain and birds”15. Furthermore, only socially interactive respondents stated they expected to hear the sounds of people and traffic-related sounds: “many people because of the nice weather. Tram + car also expected because we are close to the road. Oosterpark is not so big”16.
Solitary respondents were more likely than socially interactive respondents to expect quietness first, with crowdedness mentioned second; the latter placed crowdedness first in their list of expectations, followed by quietness and, equally important, atmosphere (whatever it entailed for respondents, usually in relation to coziness). Not surprisingly, in relation to the expected behavior of others in the public space, both groups of users expected the presence of others; however, while solitary respondents referred only marginally to the expected behavior of others, a large proportion of socially interactive respondents specifically mentioned they expected the presence of others when they decided to use the public spaces. Furthermore, they emphasized the expected level of interaction and dynamism of the activities that others would be performing: “crowdedness, many groups of people, young men playing football”17.
Finally, in relation to expectations from the public spaces themselves, both categories of respondents stated that they expected a city park (“a park, just like any other park in Amsterdam”18), which comes with its assumptions in terms of patterns of use (shown in Figure 3) and, of course, audible sounds. This is particularly interesting for the case of Frederiksplein, not a traditional large urban park but rather a small urban square – park hybrid (a “plein”).
Despite the variety in expectations, a majority of both solitary and socially interactive respondents stated their expectations were mostly or fully met, with some respondents explaining that their expectations were influenced by their previous uses of the park: “I was here before so I knew what I could expect”19.
Discussion
This paper employed a mixed methods approach to study the user-soundscape relationship in a public space context, with an emphasis on users’ activities; we further investigated how the level of social interaction of users’ activities, individually or interacting with other factors, influence users’ evaluations of their soundscapes, following the analytical model introduced earlier. We thus sought to demonstrate the relevance of considering the relationship between activity and soundscape evaluations when designing spaces for specific uses and interactions, rather than exclusively for generic goals like restoration. Through a mixed methods approach, we tested a number of factors that soundscape literature suggested to be likely to influence the user-soundscape relationship in a specific context, given the users’ activities. We framed our research questions in relation to affordance theory, which helped to explain how public space users refer and evaluate the relationship between what they hear and what they do, i.e., do they evaluate their soundscapes as disruptive, stimulating or overall suitable for their activity.
We make three contributions to help address the challenge detailed in the introduction, and discuss each one below:
-
simple (1)
A methodological contribution, adding to existing soundscape evaluation methodologies, reflecting at the same time on the limitations and ways of improving current methods,
-
simple (2)
An empirical contribution, providing insight on how users explain their soundscape evaluations in relation to their activities and building on previous use of affordance theory to further the idea of a strong relationship between users’ soundscape evaluations and their activities
-
simple (3)
A policy and design-related contribution, offering additional insight on a transferable methodology/process that practitioners can employ in their work on the built environment.
Methodological Contribution and Reflection
We used a mixed methods approach for a multi-layered analysis of the user-soundscape relationship in a public space setting, by (1) integrating users’ activities as a key variable framing evaluations, (2) exploring the individual and interaction effect of additional factors on this relationship, (3) combining Likert items with open-ended responses where users can explain their soundscape ratings, and (4) combining questionnaires with on-site behavioral mapping to situate users’ soundscape evaluations in a public space context.
The behavioral mapping was used to integrate a spatial dimension and to situate the data collected through questionnaires, not only in a physical environment, but also in a behavioral setting of others performing activities, that can offer additional insight into user evaluations and that cannot sufficiently be grasped via questionnaires alone. The quantitative method was used to collect categorical data on public space users’ evaluations of their soundscapes in order to compare the ratings between users engaged in activities with different levels of social interaction, as well as across various factors that might influence the user-soundscape relationship. The qualitative analysis was used to offer more depth to the statistical findings; as the quantitative findings show a similar trend in soundscape ratings, the qualitative insight helped to understand the subtle differences in ratings and offer an interpretation of the findings. As people continue using the public spaces despite some (albeit low) level of reported disruption, only an in-depth approach could allow researchers and practitioners to understand, e.g., what are the sources of disruption and what makes users apparently accept them. Open-ended responses were encouraged through open-ended questions, which meant offering users the space to reflect on their experience and their subsequent evaluations. While analyzing such responses is time-consuming, it allows researchers and practitioners alike to make sure that they understand what the users of spaces are experiencing, focusing on and, ultimately, evaluating; simply asking users if they “like” what they hear in a space or if they find it “pleasant” is insufficient, as responses to such questions can potentially lead the data collector (designer, planner, researcher, etc.) to resort to a top-down interpretation of what the users evaluated. The knowledge collected through open-ended responses was thus essential in understanding what users focused on and referred to in their evaluation, as well as grasping the specific aspects in their experience that disrupted or stimulated their use of spaces, thus allowing for an exploration of sounds and soundscapes as affordances for users’ activities on site.
In this paper, we provide practitioners and researchers with an example of an insightful research process and with methods and tools to observe, ask and engage with users (actual or potential) of public spaces in relation to their multisensory experience. The methodology we put forward can also be used to research and document other aspects of the built environment, without being restricted to the auditory experience. We thus do not put forward a one-size-fits-all model, but rather a qualitative user-centered process that must be adapted to the specific and unique needs of each case, but that can provide a wealth of knowledge on what disrupts or stimulates users’ activities in a public space. However, one minor limitation of this study method is that single Likert items were the variables analyzed (disruption, suitability, and stimulation); a future approach would be to substitute these with validated multi-item Likert scales as variables instead. For example, considering that suitability is indicated as the most useful/robust rating for users’ soundscapes, it would be worth it in future studies to formulate a “suitability scale” based on multiple items, which may incorporate disruption and stimulation as well as other variables as the ones explored in this paper.
While time consuming and heavily reliant on users’ willingness to participate, the methodology is nevertheless valuable for understanding what types of activities users’ soundscapes and physical environments afford. A limitation of our mixed methods approach is that it questions and observes current users of mostly green parks, that are therefore less likely to have negative evaluations of their environment and their experience (as seen in the largely positive soundscape evaluations of both solitary and socially interactive respondents). Furthermore, asking users to reflect on what they hear through questionnaires encourages them to actively focus on their soundscapes, which results in responses that might not fully reflect their on-site everyday auditory experiences. This shows the need for further improving our methodology to elicit auditory knowledge in more creative, but systematic ways. The study described here is a first effort to research this topic on site and the questions raised by the results of our data can be used as opportunities for guiding or improving future research. For example, further attention could be paid to developing additional protocols to analyze the responses to the open-ended question on activity or on testing hypotheses on specific auditory affordances that soundscapes “create” for public space users, in terms directly relevant to users’ activities or behavior. Furthermore, given the responses of, e.g., first-time solitary respondents, who evaluated their soundscapes as less suitable for their activities than first-time socially interactive respondents, an additional line of inquiry could be focused on their likeliness to return in the future to the public space in the future (or would prefer a different space). This could provide insight into whether there are aspects of their auditory experience in that particular location that have failed to meet the needs of various solitary respondents on multiple occasions. Finally, to better benefit from behavioral mapping as a method, more complex data could be collected on space users and uses, for example more detailed insight on the type of social interaction they are engaged in, e.g., families with children, pairs, and more sophisticated spatial statistics could be employed to analyze the relationship between characteristics of the space, its patterns of use and users’ soundscape evaluations.
Empirical Contribution
The behavioral mapping showed the patterns of use of the three fieldwork locations by both solitary and socially interactive users during the research period, as a context in which the questionnaire respondents provided their soundscape evaluations. The quantitative analysis indicated that the level of social interaction of users’ activities had an association with their suitability ratings, albeit weakly significant. It also showed that familiarity levels, both with what was heard and with the space (frequency of use of space) differed significantly for suitability ratings. The experience of first time visitors was of particular interest, as there was a significant difference between stimulation and suitability ratings for solitary and socially interactive respondents (with the latter being having higher ratings than the former for both ratings). For frequent users, a weakly significant association was shown in relation to stimulation ratings, with socially interactive users having higher stimulating evaluations than solitary users. Location also had a statistically significant association with disruption and suitability ratings, particularly for Sarphatipark, where solitary respondents reported significantly lower suitability and higher disruption ratings than socially interactive respondents. This difference in ratings also holds true for users older than 35 across locations for disruption and stimulation ratings, for whom the differences between solitary and socially interactive are weakly significant.
The qualitative analysis confirmed that solitary and socially interactive respondents differed slightly both in terms of sources of disruption and stimulation, as well as in the particular expectations (auditory and otherwise) from their experience. The sounds of people were considered as the main source of both disruption and stimulation for both groups; while conversations and the sounds of others in general were referred to as stimulating, loud conversations and children crying were disrupting. Surprisingly, the sounds of traffic were not mentioned as a main source of disruption; unsurprisingly, “natural” sounds were mentioned as a main source of stimulation (with only socially interactive respondents mentioning birds among stimulating sources). While solitary respondents were more likely to include holistic sounds (e.g., “city sounds”) among sources of disruption, socially interactive respondents were more likely to include such sounds among sources of stimulation, thus affording their activities (e.g., “atmosphere,” “buzz/murmur”). In terms of expectations, both solitary and socially interactive respondents reported that their expectations were largely met, which explains the soundscape ratings in relation to their activities reported on in the previous section, i.e., overall low disruption ratings and high stimulation and suitability ratings. Socially interactive respondents tended to focus not only on the presence of others, but also on their activities as well as the others’ levels of social interaction. They were also more likely to emphasize the importance of the general atmosphere/ambiance in their expectations, whereas solitary respondents focused on their expectations in relation to quietness. In terms of sounds expected, socially interactive respondents tended to expect a larger variety of “natural sounds” (including birds, wind in the trees, etc.), as well as more traffic and street-life related sounds (e.g., cars, tram, “the street”). The presence of others, not only as a source of disruption but rather as an affordance that both helps in the “creation of atmosphere” and encourages one’s own engagement with the space is thus essential when discussing/addressing auditory concerns in relation to public space use.
Policy/Design Implications
The policy and design implications are twofold, based on the empirical findings, as well as our methodological approach. On the one hand, the empirical insights demonstrate the added value of considering users’ soundscapes in relation to their activities (with a focus on whether the activities performed are solitary or socially interactive) when considering new policy or design initiatives; it also showed the potential of including the analytical framework developed in the background section to help unpacking the complexity of the auditory experience. For example, tools like questionnaires commonly used by various practitioners should include activity questions in soundscape-related queries, such as asking users about their activity and whether they were by themselves or with others at the time of the completion of, for example, noise exposure surveys.
On the other hand, the methodological approach described in this paper, and the resulting research process described above can be used by policy makers and designers to gain contextual insights in users’ experiences. For example, integrating open-ended questions in current questionnaires for soundscape evaluations can help with verifying the suitability or relevance of commonly (and uncritically) used terms like “annoyance” or “pleasantness” by accessing the everyday sound-related vocabulary of urbanites, which can in turn feed into and help adjust existing tools used by local, regional and national authorities.
Overall, in this paper, we provided both empirical and methodological insights that researchers and practitioners alike can adjust and employ in their own investigations of urban auditory complexity to contribute to the creation of spaces that afford a large array of activities. For future research and practice, it would be interesting to explore whether using richer descriptions or evaluations brings designers and policy makers to different kinds of interventions.
Ethics Statement
This study was carried out in accordance with the guidelines outlined by the Ethics Committee (AIEC) of the University of Amsterdam. The participation in the study was voluntary and the responses were fully anonymized; subjects were informed that, if they completed the questionnaires, their answers would be used in a scientific study. By completing the written questionnaires, they confirmed that they provided consent to participate in the research.
Author Contributions
EB performed the data collection and analyses and wrote the majority of the paper. KP, MC, and LB provided extensive comments and textual edits on previous versions of the manuscript. OR contributed to the quantitative analysis in this manuscript. The doctoral project this research is part of was initiated by MC, as part of INCAS, and further elaborated jointly by EB, KP, MC, and LB.
Conflict of Interest Statement
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Acknowledgments
We would like to thank Gideon Hamburger for his help collecting questionnaires. We would like to thank Daniel Steele for providing his statistical insights on the quantitative analysis and Ate Poorthuis for making the behavioral mapping app available. Finally, we would also like to thank the two “Frontiers in Psychology” reviewers for the careful reading of previous drafts and for their suggestions to improve this manuscript.
Funding. This research was partially funded by INCAS as part of a doctoral scholarship.
For a review of papers published in this project, check the project URL at: https://www.salford.ac.uk/research/sirc/research-groups/acoustics/psychoacoustics/positive-soundscapes-project [Accessed May 2nd 2018].
The term “appropriateness” has been used to measure users’ evaluation of their soundscapes in relation to their settings, defined either as their performed or imagined activity – appropriateness for activity (Nielbo et al., 2013; Bild et al., 2018) or, more commonly, the geographical setting they find themselves in – appropriateness in/for “a place” (Lavia et al., 2012; Aletta et al., 2016a). In this paper, we focus on the former understanding of the term.
For another application of this method as part of a mixed methods approach, see Steele et al. (2016).
The method has been tested and implemented in two previous smaller scale studies (viz. Bild et al., 2018, for a detailed explanation of the behavioral mapping method and tools; see also Steele et al., 2016).
We hereinafter refer to the public space users who completed our questionnaire as “respondents.”
Original in Dutch: “lawaai.”
Original in Dutch: “rustgevend.”
Original in Dutch: “het geroezemoes draagt bij aan gemoedelijke sfeer.”
Original in Dutch: “als ik andere mensen hoor praten, dan is het voor mij ook makkelijk om te praten.”
Original in Dutch: “gestimuleerd om met te doen wat ik leuk vind.”
Original in Dutch: “rustgevende geluiden laten me in slaap vallen.”
Original in Dutch: “dat je leven om je heen hoort, maakt het aangenaam en gezellig. In dit geval maakt het hier erg gemoedelijk en gezellig.”
Original in Dutch: “muziek geeft sfeer, water er mensen ook. Tram verstoord beetje maar mocht erbij hier in de stad.”
Original in Dutch: “was teveel bezig met mijn eigen activiteit dat ik me weinig bewust was van het omgevings geluid.”
Original in Dutch: “rustige omgeving met een fontein en vogels.”
Original in Dutch: “veel mensen want het is mooi weer. Tram + auto ook verwacht omdat we vlakbij de weg zitten. Oosterpark is niet zo groot.”
Original in Dutch: “drukte, veel groepjes mensen, voetballende jongens.”
Original in Dutch: “een park, zoals elk ander park in A’dam.”
Original in Dutch: “ik ben hier vaker geweest dus wist wat ik kon verwachten.”
References
- Aletta F., Kang J., Axelsson Ö. (2016a). Soundscape descriptors and a conceptual framework for developing predictive soundscape models. Landsc. Urban Plan. 149 65–74. 10.1016/j.landurbplan.2016.02.001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Aletta F., Lepore F., Kostara-Konstantinou E., Kang J., Astolfi A. (2016b). An experimental study on the influence of soundscapes on people’s behaviour in an open public space. Appl. Sci. 6:276 10.3390/app6100276 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Andringa T. C., Lanser J. J. L. (2013). How pleasant sounds promote and annoying sounds impede health: a cognitive approach. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 10 1439–1461. 10.3390/ijerph10041439 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Andringa T. C., Weber M., Payne S. R., Krijnders J. D., Dixon M. N., Linden R. V., et al. (2013). Positioning soundscape research and management. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 134 2739–2747. 10.1121/1.4819248 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aspuru I., Garcia I., Bartalucci C., Borchi F., Carfagni M., Governi L., et al. (2016). LIFE+ 2010 QUADMAP project: a new methodology to select, analyze and manage quiet urban areas defined by the European Directive 2002/49/EC. Noise Mapp. 3 120–129. 10.1515/noise-2016-0009 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Aspuru I., Garcia I., Herranz-Pascual K., Garcia-Borreguero I. (2011). Understanding soundscape as a specific environmental experience: highlighting the importance of context relevance. Proc. Mtgs. Acoust. 14:015004 10.1121/1.3676685 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Axelsson Ö., Nilsson M. E., Berglund B. (2010). A principal components model of soundscape perception. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 128 2836–2846. 10.1121/1.3493436 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bild E., Coler M., Dubois D., Pfeffer K. (2015). “A pilot experiment on effects of motor and cognitive activities on memories of soundscapes,” in Proceedings of the Euronoise, Maastricht 1529–1533. [Google Scholar]
- Bild E., Coler M., Pfeffer K., Bertolini L. (2016). Considering sound in planning and designing public spaces: a review of theory and applications and a proposed framework for integrating research and practice. J. Plan. Lit. 31 419–434. 10.1177/0885412216662001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Bild E., Steele D., Pfeffer K., Bertolini L., Guastavino C. (2018). “Activity as a mediator between users and their auditory environment in an urban pocket park: a case study of Parc du Portugal (Montreal, Canada),” in Handbook of Research on Perception-Driven Approaches to Urban Assessment and Design eds Aletta F., Jiao X. (Hershey, PA: IGI Global; ) 100–125. 10.4018/978-1-5225-3637-6.ch005 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Bloomfield A. (2014). Good Practice Guide on Quiet Areas. EEA Technical Report No. 4/2014. Copenhagen: European Environment Agency. [Google Scholar]
- Booi H., van den Berg F. (2012). Quiet areas and the need for quietness in Amsterdam. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 9 1030–1050. 10.3390/ijerph9041030 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Botteldooren D., Andringa T., Aspuru I., Brown A. L., Dubois D., Guastavino C., et al. (2015). “From sonic environment to soundscape,” in Soundscape and the Built Environment eds Kang J., Schulte-Fortkamp B. (Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press; ) 17–42. 10.1201/b19145-3 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Bruce N., Condie J., Henshaw V., Payne S. R. (2015). Analysing Olfactory and Auditory Sensescapes in English Cities: Sensory Expectation and Urban Environmental Perception. Ambiances. Available at: http://ambiances.revues.org/560 [Google Scholar]
- Bruce N. S., Davies W. J. (2014). The effects of expectation on the perception of soundscapes. Appl. Acoust. 85 1–11. 10.1016/j.apacoust.2014.03.016 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Bruce N. S., Davies W. J., Adams M. D. (2009). “Expectation as a factor in the perception of soundscapes,” in Proceedings of the Euronoise, Edinburgh. [Google Scholar]
- Can A., Aumond P., Michel S., Coensel B. D. E., Ribeiro C., Lavandier C. (2016). “Comparison of noise indicators in an urban context,” in Proceedings of the INTER-NOISE and NOISE-CON Congress Vol. 253 (Reston, VA: Institute of Noise Control Engineering; ) 775–783. [Google Scholar]
- Cerwén G., Pedersen E., Pálsdóttir A. M. (2016). The role of soundscape in nature-based rehabilitation: a patient perspective. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 13:1229. 10.3390/ijerph13121229 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clarke E. F. (2005). Ways of Listening: An Ecological Approach to the Perception of Musical Meaning. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195151947.001.0001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Cosco N. G., Moore R. C., Islam M. Z. (2010). Behavior mapping: a method for linking preschool physical activity and outdoor design. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 42 513–519. 10.1249/MSS.0b013e3181cea27a [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Creswell J. W., Plano Clark V. L. (2011). Designing and Conducting Mixed Methods Research 2nd Edn. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications, Inc. [Google Scholar]
- Delepaut G. (2009). “Questionner: comment et pourquoi? Le choc des questions, le poids des mots!,” in Le Sentir et le Dire. Concepts et Méthodes en Psychologie et Linguistique Cognitives ed. Dubois D. (Paris: L’Harmattan; ) 157–188. [Google Scholar]
- DeNora T. (2000). Music in Everyday Life. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 10.1017/CBO9780511489433 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Dubois D. (2000). Categories as acts of meaning: the case of categories in olfaction and audition. Cogn. Sci. Q. 1 35–68. [Google Scholar]
- Filipan K., Boes M., De Coensel B., Lavandier C., Delaitre P., Domitrović H., et al. (2017). The personal viewpoint on the meaning of tranquility affects the appraisal of the urban park soundscape. Appl. Sci. 7:91 10.3390/app7010091 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Gibson J. J. (1977). “The theory of affordances,” in Perceiving, Acting, and Knowing eds Shaw R. E., Bransford J. (Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; ) 67–82. [Google Scholar]
- Gibson J. J. (1979). The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin. [Google Scholar]
- Goličnik B., Thompson C. W. (2010). Emerging relationships between design and use of urban park spaces. Landsc. Urban Plan. 94 38–53. 10.1016/j.landurbplan.2009.07.016 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Guastavino C., Katz B. F., Polack J. D., Levitin D. J., Dubois D. (2005). Ecological validity of soundscape reproduction. Acta Acust. United Acust. 91 333–341. [Google Scholar]
- Herranz-Pascual K., García I., Diez I., Santander A., Aspuru I. (2017). Analysis of field data to describe the effect of context (Acoustic and Non-Acoustic Factors) on urban soundscapes. Appl. Sci. 7:173 10.3390/app7020173 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Huron D. B. (2006). Sweet Anticipation: Music and the Psychology of Expectation. Cambridge: MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
- Ingold T. (2000). The Perception of the Environment: Essays on Livelihood, Dwelling and Skill. Hove: Psychology Press; 10.4324/9780203466025 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Jennings P., Cain R. (2013). A framework for improving urban soundscapes. Appl. Acoust. 74 293–299. 10.1016/j.apacoust.2011.12.003 24073684 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Knigge L., Cope M. (2006). Grounded visualization: integrating the analysis of qualitative and quantitative data through grounded theory and visualization. Environ. Plan. A 38 2021–2037. 10.1068/a37327 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Kogan P., Turra B., Arenas J. P., Hinalaf M. (2017). A comprehensive methodology for the multidimensional and synchronic data collecting in soundscape. Sci. Total Environ. 580 1068–1077. 10.1016/j.scitotenv.2016.12.061 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kyttä M. (2002). Affordances of children’s environments in the context of cities, small towns, suburbs and rural villages in Finland and Belarus. J. Environ. Psychol. 22 109–123. 10.1006/jevp.2001.0249 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Lavia L., Dixon M., Witchel H. J., Goldsmith M. (2016). “Applied soundscape practices,” in Soundscape and the Built Environment eds Kang J., Schulte-Fortkamp B. (Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press; ) 243–301. 10.1201/b19145-11 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Lavia L., Easteal M., Close D., Witchel H., Axelsson Ö., Ware M.et al. (2012). “Sounding Brighton: practical approaches towards better soundscapes,” in Proceedings of the INTER-NOISE and NOISE-CON Congress New York, NY. [Google Scholar]
- Lavia L., Witchel H. J., Aletta F., Steffens J., Fiebig A., Kang J., et al. (2018). “Non-participant observation methods for soundscape design and urban panning,” in Handbook of Research on Perception-Driven Approaches to Urban Assessment and Design eds Aletta F., Jiao X. (Hershey, PA: IGI Global; ) 73–99. 10.4018/978-1-5225-3637-6.ch004 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Lercher P., Schulte-Fortkamp B. (2003). “The relevance of soundscape research to the assessment of noise annoyance at the community level,” in Proceedings of the Eighth International Congress on Noise as a Public Health Problem Rotterdam: 225–231. [Google Scholar]
- Lercher P., Van Kamp I., Von Lindern E., Botteldooren D. (2016). “Perceived soundscapes and health-related quality of life, context, restoration, and personal characteristics,” in Soundscape and the Built Environment eds Kang J., Schulte-Fortkamp B. (Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press; ) 89–132. 10.1201/b19145-6 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Marcell M. M., Borella D., Greene M., Kerr E., Rogers S. (2000). Confrontation naming of environmental sounds. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 22 830–864. 10.1076/jcen.22.6.830.949 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mehta V. (2014). Evaluating public space. J. Urban Des. 19 53–88. 10.1080/13574809.2013.854698 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Mzali M. (2002). Perception de L’ambiance Sonore et Évaluation du Confort Acoustique des Trains. Doctoral dissertation, Paris, Université Paris; 6. [Google Scholar]
- Nielbo F. L. (2015). “From soundscape to meaningscape,” in Proceedings of the EuroNoise Edinburgh. [Google Scholar]
- Nielbo F. L., Steele D., Guastavino C. (2013). “Investigating soundscape affordances through activity appropriateness,” in Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics ICA2013 (London: ASA; ). 10.1121/1.4800502 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Nilsson M., Botteldooren D., De Coensel B. (2007). “Acoustic indicators of soundscape quality and noise annoyance in outdoor urban areas,” in Proceedings of the 19th International Congress on Acoustics Madrid. [Google Scholar]
- Pecqueux A. (2012). Les affordances des événements: des sons aux événements urbains. Communications 90 215–227. 10.3917/commu.090.0215 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Pheasant R. J., Horoshenkov K., Watts G., Barret B. T. (2008). The acoustic and visual factors influencing the construction of tranquil space in urban and rural environments tranquil spaces-quiet places? J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 123 1446–1457. 10.1121/1.2831735 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Raimbault M. (2006). Qualitative judgements of urban soundscapes: questionning questionnaires and semantic scales. Acta Acust. United Acust. 92 929–937. [Google Scholar]
- Reybrouck M. (2012). Musical sense-making and the concept of affordance: an ecosemiotic and experiential approach. Biosemiotics 5 391–409. 10.1007/s12304-012-9144-6 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Ruxton G. D. (2006). The unequal variance t-test is an underused alternative to Student’s t-test and the Mann–Whitney U test. Behav. Ecol. 17 688–690. 10.1093/beheco/ark016 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Schulte-Fortkamp B., Fiebig A. (2016). “Impact of soundscape in terms of perception,” in Soundscape and the Built Environment eds Kang J., Schulte-Fortkamp B. (Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press; ) 69–88. 10.1201/b19145-5 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Steele D., Bild E., Cynthia T., Irene M., Cubero J., Guastavino C. (2016). “A comparison of soundscape evaluation methods in a large urban park,” in Proceedings of International Congress on Acoustics Montreal. [Google Scholar]
- Steenson C. J., Rodger M. W. (2015). Bringing sounds into use: thinking of sounds as materials and a sketch of auditory affordances. Open Psychol. J. 8 174–182. 10.2174/1874350101508010174 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Steffens J., Steele D., Guastavino C. (2015). “New insights into soundscape evaluations using the experience sampling method,” in Proceedings of the Euronoise Maastricht: 1496–1500. [Google Scholar]
- Steffens J., Steele D., Guastavino C. (2017). Situational and person-related factors influencing momentary and retrospective soundscape evaluations in day-to-day life. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 141 1414–1425. 10.1121/1.4976627 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stoffregen T. A. (2003). Affordances as properties of the animal-environment system. Ecol. Psychol. 15 115–134. 10.1207/S15326969ECO1502_2 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Thibaud J. P. (1998). “The acoustic embodiment of social practice,” in Proceedings of the Conference Stockholm, Hey Listen Stockholm. [Google Scholar]
- Truax B. (2001). Acoustic Communication Vol. 1 Westport, CT: Greenwood Publishing Group [Google Scholar]
- Turvey M. T. (1992). Affordances and prospective control: an outline of the ontology. Ecol. Psychol. 4 173–187. 10.1207/s15326969eco0403_3 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Van Kempen E., Devilee J., Swart W., Van Kamp I. (2014). Characterizing urban areas with good sound quality: development of a research protocol. Noise Health 16 380–387. 10.4103/1463-1741.144416 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yang W., Kang J. (2005). Acoustic comfort evaluation in urban open public spaces. Appl. Acoust. 66 211–229. 10.1016/j.apacoust.2004.07.011 [DOI] [Google Scholar]