Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2019 Sep 1.
Published in final edited form as: J Head Trauma Rehabil. 2018 Sep-Oct;33(5):E16–E23. doi: 10.1097/HTR.0000000000000420

An Exploratory Study of Mild Cognitive Impairment of Retired Professional Contact Sport Athletes

John G Baker 1,2, John J Leddy 1, Andrea L Hinds 3, Jennifer Shucard 1, Tania Sharma 1, Sergio Hernandez 3, Joel Durinka 5, Robert Zivadinov 4, Barry S Willer 3
PMCID: PMC6126937  NIHMSID: NIHMS964706  PMID: 30080798

Abstract

Objective

To test the hypothesis that Mild Cognitive Impairment (MCI) rates are higher among retired professional contact sport athletes compared to non-contact athlete controls and compare history of contact sports to other MCI risk factors.

Setting

University Concussion Management Clinic

Participants

21 retired National Football League (NFL) and National Hockey League (NHL) players and 21 aged matched non-contact athlete controls.

Designs

Case-control

Main Measures

Comprehensive Criteria was used to assess MCI based on Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System, Trail Making Part A and B, Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Third Edition subtests, Neuropsychological Assessment Battery Memory Module List Learning, Story Learning, and Naming subtests, and Controlled Oral Word Association Test. The Wide Range Achievement Test was used as a proxy measure for IQ.. Atherosclerotic cardiovascular disease risk factors were self-reported and blood cholesterol were measured. Depression was measured by Beck Depression Inventory-II (BDI).

Results

Eight contact sport athletes (38%) and three non-contact athletes (14%) met MCI criteria (p=0.083). Contact sport athletes’ scores were significantly worse on Letter Fluency and List B Immediate Recall. Contact athletes were more obese, had more vascular risk factors, and had higher scores on the BDI than Controls.

Conclusion

Athletes with a history of playing professional contact sports had more vascular risk factors and higher depression scores. MCI rates were somewhat higher, though not significant.

Keywords: cerebral concussion, athletics, mild cognitive impairment, cognitive reserve, neuropsychology tests, risk factors

INTRODUCTION

Much attention has been given to Chronic Traumatic Encephalopathy (CTE) as a cause of behavioral, mood, and cognitive changes among retired professional contact sport athletes. Other competing hypotheses for these changes have received less attention. Vascular, mood, and other disease etiologies leading to Mild Cognitive Impairment (MCI) may explain some of the cognitive changes among aging professional athletes. A study using post-mortem analysis shows that up to 99% of retired NFL players had chronic traumatic encephalopathy (CTE) with 71% having severe CTE and it is suggested that MCI is present in CTE.1 The definition of MCI has changed since its inception more than twenty years ago.2 It is distinguishable from normal aging and may be a precursor to dementia.3 A patient with MCI may be aware of cognitive difficulties and able to function independently.4 The DSM-V diagnostic criteria for Mild Neurocognitive Disorder (a broader term which includes MCI) states that it includes a modest impairment in cognitive performance, preferably documented by standardized neuropsychological testing.5 Some studies of retired NFL players have not found evidence for cognitive, mood, and behavioral changes that would be consistent with CTE. Randolph et al.6 studied a subsample of 513 retired football players over 50 whose spouse completed a screening interview for MCI. They identified players with probable MCI and compared this group to age-matched healthy controls and a sample diagnosed with MCI. The cognitive profile of the NFL MCI group was milder although similar to the profile of the clinical MCI group.. There was no association between years of play and cognitive scores. They concluded that their results supported diminished cognitive reserve (process of adapting to deterioration by using cognitive processing resources to compensate for deficits) rather than CTE as an explanation for MCI.

Alosco et al.7 studied 25 professional football players (mean age at death = 65 years) with autopsy confirmed stage III or IV CTE using next-of-kin interviews. They found that all 25 had cognitive symptoms and their age of cognitive decline was inversely related to their cognitive reserve. Unfortunately, this study did not compare the football players with a control population. Hart et al. 88 studied 34 retired football players, aged 41 to 79 years (mean 61.8), and found that 14 of 34 (41%) demonstrated cognitive deficits. Eight of 34 were identified with MCI which the authors note is slightly higher than the proportion in the general population, but not significant. McMillan et al.9 studied mental health and cognitive functioning in 52 retired professional rugby players aged 53.5 years (26 – 79) and compared them to age-matched healthy controls. Despite a high number of concussions in the rugby players, no differences in mental health or cognitive functioning were found later in life.

Other hypotheses for MCI in retired contact-sport athletes have received less attention. Willeumier et al.10 studied 38 over-weight and 38 healthy-weight retired National Football League (NFL) players (mean age = 57 years, range 25 – 82) and showed that players with higher body mass had significantly worse neuropsychological test scores compared to the healthy-weight players. Hart et al. 8 showed that differences in regional blood flow using arterial spin labeling in retired NFL players corresponded to regions associated with impaired cognitive performance. Other etiologies, like depression, have been associated with MCI in the non-athlete population and may explain some of the cognitive changes among aging former professional athletes.11,12 Due to the controversy present in the literature, we wanted to study a sample of retired professional contact sport athletes and compare them to non-contact sport athlete controls on rates of MCI, cognitive scores, and other etiologies of MCI like vascular disease and depression. We hypothesized that the contact sport athletes would have higher rates of MCI, lower neurocognitive scores, and more vascular and depression risk factors.

METHODS

The current case-control study was completed as part of a larger study of retired athletes (cite overview paper) at the University at Buffalo. Approval was obtained prior to the study from the University at Buffalo IRB committee.

Study Participants

The contact sport athlete group was composed of former NFL and National Hockey League (NHL) players who were contacted and ultimately recruited through their respective local alumni associations (N=21, mean age 56.7). The non-contact athlete control group was composed of people who participated in non-contact sports (N=21, mean age 55.4) and were contacted through associations of athletic clubs that included older athletes on their roster. Detailed inclusion and exclusion criteria are presented in the overview paper.

Measures

Neurocognitive Measures

Executive function was assessed with the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST)13 and the Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System (D-KEFS).14 Five subtests for the WCST (perseverative, non-perseverative, and conceptual level responses, perseverative and total errors) and two subtests for the DKEFS (inhibit and switch) were selected based on their relevance to the long term effects of multiple concussions. Attention was assessed with the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, Third Edition (WAIS-III) using the Standardization Sample norms for Scaled Scores for the subtest of Digit Span15 and the Trail Making Test Part A.16 Memory was assessed with the Neuropsychological Assessment Battery (NAB) Memory Module including the four primary scores for immediate and delayed recall from the List Learning test and immediate and delayed recall for phrases for the Story Learning test, using the same parallel form across participants.17 Language was assessed using Controlled Oral Word Association Test (COWAT) and NAB Naming Test.1820 For the COWAT, the FAS form was used for phonemic fluency and the Animal Naming task was used to measure category or semantic fluency.

Finally, the domain of perceptual motor skills was assessed with the WAIS-III Scaled Scores for the Digit Symbol subtest15 and Trail Making, Part B.16,21 IQ was estimated using standard scores from a proxy measure for IQ, the WRAT-4 Word Reading achievement test.21,22 Each of the scores for the tests noted above were converted to T-scores in order to provide a common metric for the subtests of each of the measures. All of the tests were performed under the supervision of the same experienced neuropsychologist.

Criteria for MCI

To define MCI we used the Jak. et al23 publication which provided several approaches to quantifying cognitive impairment that use test scores in multiple cognitive domains. The cognitive domains are based on norm-referenced neuropsychological test scores in attention, memory, executive function, language, and visuospatial domains. They recommend the Comprehensive criteria to classify individuals as MCI or normal. The criteria include (1) two test scores below 1 standard deviation (SD) in one domain or (2) at least one test score below 1 SD in three domains. Subtypes of MCI have been based on test scores below 1 or 1.5 standard deviations in one versus multiple domains and in memory versus non-memory domains. The four currently recognized subtypes include: memory, one domain; memory, multiple domains; non-memory, one-domain; and non-memory, multiple domains.24,25 At least two neuropsychology tests were included for each domain. For tests with more than one score per test, we included 2 to 5 primary scores for that test.

Depression

Beck Depression Inventory-II (BDI-II) was used to assess depression in both groups. It is a validated 21-item instrument that rates depression in the following categories: none, mild, moderate, and severe.26

Vascular Risk Factors

Participants reported on vascular risk factors, including a history of: high blood pressure, diabetes, smoking, elevated cholesterol, and obesity. One or more vascular risk factors were compared to no vascular risk factors. Serum cholesterol levels and supine blood pressures were obtained. The Framingham Heart Study atherosclerotic cardiovascular disease (ASCVD) risk calculator was used to estimate the 10-year primary risk of ASCVD for people aged 40 to 79.27 Patients are considered to be at “elevated” risk if the Pooled Cohort Equations predicted risk is ≥ 7.5%.28 The factors used in calculating risk include: age, gender, race, total cholesterol, high density lipoproteins, systolic blood pressure, diastolic blood pressure, history of high blood pressure, history of diabetes, and smoking.

Statistical Analysis

According to the systematic review and meta-analysis by Karr et al.29, an estimated effect size of 0.80 was used for the power analysis. Based on the computed power analysis, the results indicated that in order to achieve a power of 0.80 with a one sided test at level 0.05, a total of 20 participants in each group was required. Independent two sample t-tests were used to compare neuropsychology test scores in each of five domains between the contact sport athletes and non-contact sport athletes with statistic, p-value, Cohen’s d effect size and 95% confidence interval is reported. Most of the normative reference scores for the neuropsychology tests included adjustment for education with the exception of one measure in each of the attention, executive function, and visuospatial domains. Since IQ and education (and therefore cognitive reserve) was significantly different between contact sport athletes and non-contact sport athletes, we compare neuropsychology test scores in each of the five domains in a full model containing IQ and group as explanatory variables and a reduced model only containing group as the explanatory variable with test score as response. The full model and reduced model were compared using a partial F-test. Similarly, two sample t-tests (unequal variation) were conducted to examine vascular risk factors, BDI scores, and MCI in Athletes and Controls. All analyses were performed using the R programming language.30

RESULTS

The neurocognitive test results are summarized in Table 1. Using a proxy measure of IQ, the WRAT-4 Word Reading achievement test standard score, the contact sport athletes scored significantly lower on this measure of estimated IQ (p<0.001). Contact sport athletes did not significantly differ from non-contact sport athletes on most of the primary scores in the five domains. Only the Letter Fluency, NAB Naming, and List B Immediate Recall scores were significant, but after adjusting for IQ, only Letter Fluency (p=0.01) and List B Immediate Recall (p=0.03) were significant.

Table 1.

Sub-Domains of Neurocognitive Measures Associated with Mild Cognitive Impairment using T-scores

Domain Primary Variable Mean Contact Sport Athletes T-score mean (SD) Mean Non-Contact Sport Athletes T-score mean (SD) T-test score p-value Cohen effect (95% CI) Intercept Shift in Contact Athletes relative to Controls p-value2
Proxy IQ WRAT Word Reading1 achievement test 49.29 (±6.76) 57.57 (±8.82) 3.44 0.01 1.06 (3.43, 13.2)
Language Letter Fluency (FAS Total Score)* 52.95 (±10.98) 47.43 (±6.83) 1.96 0.06 0.6 (−0.03,1.24) 8.9742 0.01
Animal Naming Total* 49.95 (±11.59) 49.14 (±10.39) 0.24 0.81 0.07 (−0.55,0.70) −2.3015 0.55
NAB3 Naming** 49.33 (±8.49) 53.33 (±1.68) −2.12 0.05 −0.65 (−1.29,−0.01) −2.7477 0.21
Visual-spatial WAIS-III Digit Symbol*** 55 (±9.98) 53.38 (±8.38) 0.57 0.57 0.18 (−0.45,0.80) 2.6053 0.43
Trails B**** 48.52 (±23.06) 53.38 (±9.95) −0.89 0.38 −0.27 (−0.9,0.35) −5.2625 0.41
Attention WAIS-III Digit Span*** 54.9 (±11.67) 57.33 (±9.65) −0.73 0.47 −0.23 (−0.85,0.40) 0.5543 0.88
Trails A**** 55.52 (±13.12) 54.9 (±10.2) 0.17 0.87 0.05 (−0.57,0.68) 2.8804 0.49
Executive Function DKEFS Color-Word Interference4 Inhibitioh*** 57.67 (±4.64) 57.19 (±8.29) 0.23 0.82 0.07 (−0.55,0.69) 2.5044 0.28
DKEFS Color-Word Interference Inhibition/Switching*** 59.14 (±5.86) 56.48 (±8.1) 1.22 0.23 0.38 (−0.25,1.01) 4.6449 0.06
WCST5 Total Errors*** 49.43 (±9.89) 52.45 (±6.3) −1.17 0.25 −0.36 (−1, 0.27) −2.9522 0.33
WCST Perseverative Responses**** 49.95 (±9.46) 51.3 (±6.06) −0.55 0.59 −0.17 (−0.8,0.46) −1.2879 0.66
WCST Perseverative Errors**** 49.95 (±9.82) 51.75 (±6.25) −0.70 0.49 −0.22 (−0.85,0.42) −1.5971 0.59
WCST Non-Perseverative Errors**** 48.29 (±9.74) 51.65 (±6.76) −1.29 0.21 −0.4 (−1.04,0.24) −4.1909 0.17
WCST Conceptual Level Responses**** 49.71 (±10.35) 52.7 (±6.33) −1.12 0.27 −0.35 (−0.98,0.29) −3.1447 0.32
Memory List A Immediate Recall** 54.48 (±10.21) 56.71 (±8.99) −0.75 0.46 −0.23 (−0.86, 0.39) −2.6561 0.44
List B Immediate Recall** 50.9 (±9.93) 57.24 (±10.44) −2.01 0.05 −0.62 (−1.26,0.02) −8.2536 0.03
List A Short Delay** 53.48 (±10.85) 57.33 (±8.3) −1.29 0.2 −0.4 (−1.03,0.23) −4.2227 0.23
List A Long Delay** 56.1 (±11.48) 59.38 (±8.12) −1.07 0.29 −0.33 (−0.96,0.30) −2.2336 0.53
STL6 Phrase Immediate Recall** 41.71 (±9.72) 45.57 (±9.66) −1.29 0.2 −0.4 (−1.03, 0.23) −3.9058 0.26
STL Phrase Delayed Recall** 45.1 (±8.81) 48.19 (±8.43) −1.16 0.25 −0.36 (−0.99,0.27) −2.2176 0.47
1

Wide Range Achievement Test-4 Word Reading;

2

Adjustment for Estimated IQ;

3

Neuropsychological Assessment Battery;

4

Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System;

5

Wisconsin Card Sorting Test;

6

Story Learning Trial;

*

Age, Education, and Ethnicity Normative T-Scores;

**

Age, Education, and Gender Normative T-Scores;

***

Age Normative T-Scores;

****

Age and Education Normative T-Scores

Table 2 presents contact sport athletes and non-contact athletes who qualified as MCI along with self-reported vascular risk factors, BDI categories, and number of years played (only for the contact sport athlete group). Eight contact sport athletes (38%) versus 3 non-contact controls (14%) met the criteria for MCI (p=0.083). Although close, this did not reach significance. Of the 8 contact sport athletes who met the Comprehensive criteria for MCI; 7 were considered memory MCI, multiple domains; and 1 was considered memory MCI, one domain. Of the 3 non-contact sport athletes who met the criteria; 2 were considered memory MCI, multiple domains; and 1 was considered non-memory MCI, one domain. In a sub-analysis of contact athletes, the number of playing years was not associated with meeting the MCI criteria (p=0.66).

Table 2.

MCI Classification, Reported VRF, and AHA Risk in Contact Sport Athletes and Non-Contact Sport Controls

Contact Sport Athletes (n=21) Non-Contact Athletes (n=21)
Case # Sport Age MCI1 VRF2 BDI3 Years Played Sport Age MCI VRF BDI
1 Football 62 X X - 2 Running 72 X X -
2 Hockey 64 X X - 13 Running 49 X - -
3 Football 64 X X 1 9 Cycling 61 X - -
4 Hockey 71 X X - 6 Running 57 - X -
5 Hockey 58 X X 2 2 Running 42 - - -
6 Hockey 52 X - 3 18 T & F 72 - - -
7 Hockey 36 X - - 2 Running 64 - X -
8 Football 69 X X - 2 Running 59 - - -
9 Hockey 45 - X - 19 Cycling 47 - - -
10 Football 72 - - - 5 Cycling 61 - X -
11 Hockey 45 - - - 16 Running 67 - X -
12 Football 71 - X - 2 Running 65 - X -
13 Hockey 50 - X - 9 Cycling 62 - - -
14 Hockey 66 - X - 15 Running 61 - X -
15 Hockey 59 - - - 13 Triathlete 61 - - -
16 Football 57 - X 1 5 Running 59 - X -
17 Football 57 - X - 6 Running 55 - X 1
18 Hockey 52 - X - 18 Cycling 45 - - -
19 Football 51 - X - 4 Cycling 45 - - -
20 Hockey 41 - X 2 4 Triathlete 44 - - -
21 Hockey 40 - - - 12 Running 43 - - -
1

Comprehensive criteria: at least 2 test scores in 1 cognitive domain or 1 test score in 3 domains fall 1 SD below normative reference values, X = Yes, - = No;

2

VRF’s = Vascular risk factors, presence of one or more of BMI ≥ 30 (criteria for obesity), history of high blood pressure, diabetes, smoking, or high cholesterol, X = Yes, - = No;

3

BDI = Becks Depression Inventory, - = minimal, 1 = mild, 2 = moderate, 3 = severe

Table 3 presents the vascular risk factors and depression scores for contact sport athletes and non-contact sport athletes. The ASCVD risk for one contact sport athlete could not be calculated because he was 36 years old. BMI (p<0.001) and one or more reported vascular risk factors (p=0.005) was significantly higher in contact sport athletes and HDL Cholesterol (p=0.017) was significantly higher in non-contact sport controls. No other vascular risk factors were significantly different. Table 3 also presents the depression scores of the contact athlete and non-contact control groups as assessed by the BDI. The contact sport athlete group scored significantly higher (p=0.04) than the non-contact control group in the BDI raw scores, but were not significantly different (p=0.078) when comparing minimal risk for depression (13 or below on BDI) to mild depression or higher (14 or above on BDI).

Table 3.

Contact Sport Athletes and Non-Contact Sport Athletes vs Vascular Risk Factors and Depression

Contact Athletes (n=21) Non-Contact Athletes (n=21) p-value Cohen effect (95% CI)
AHA/ACC Risk
 Mean (SD) 14.11 (±10.60) 10.36 (±8.44) 0.223 0.39 (−9.89, 2.37)
 Elevated Risk1 70% (n = 14/20*) 62% (n = 13/21) 0.747 0.10 (−0.35, 0.26)
Body Mass Index
 Mean (SD) 29.67 (±3.64) 24.52 (±2.55) <0.001 1.63 (−7.10, −3.19)
 Obese (BMI > 30) 42.8% (n = 9) 0% (n = 0) <0.001 1.19 (−0.65, −0.21)
One or More Reported VRF2 71.4% (n = 15) 42.9% (n = 9) 0.005 0.57 (−0.59, 0.017)
History of High Blood Pressure 38.0% (n = 8) 19.0% (n = 4) 0.18 0.41 (−0.47, 0.092)
History of Diabetes - - - -
History of High Cholesterol 9.5% (n = 2) 19.0% (n = 4) 0.39 0.25 (−0.13, 0.32)
History of Smoking 33.3% (n = 7) 23.8% (n = 5) 0.506 0.19 (−0.38, 0.19)
Total Cholesterol (SD) 189.4 (±30) 182.8 (±25) 0.44 0.24 (−23.8, 10.5)
 LDL3 Cholesterol (SD) 106.9 (±39) 103.3 (±33) 0.753 0.09 (−26.0, 19.0)
 HDL4 Cholesterol (SD) 42.6 (±8) 49.8 (±11) 0.017 0.76 (1.37, 13.1)
Beck Depression Inventory
 Mean Score (SD) 10.2 (±8.1) 3.9 (±5.3) 0.004 0.92 (−10.6, −2.09)
 Mild Depression or Higher5 23.8% (n = 5) 4.8% (n = 1) 0.078 0.59 (−0.78, 0.025)
*

One participant < 40 years of age;

1

AHA/ACC Risk ≥ 7.5;

2

Vascular Risk Factors;

3

Low Density Lipoprotein;

4

High Density Lipoprotein;

5

Fourteen or higher on Beck Depression Inventory

DISCUSSION

The purpose of our study of retired athletes was to extensively evaluate the cognitive and behavioral characteristics of athletes who had professional careers playing contact sports that may have left them vulnerable to CTE.3139 The research on CTE and extensive media attention, have given the impression that most former contact sport athletes will experience early onset dementia marked by cognitive impairment, even though the high rate of findings of CTE occurred in a very selected population of athletes involved in contact sports. The former NFL and NHL athletes we recruited and evaluated in this study were convinced that early signs of dementia had already happened, or was about to happen, which was their primary reason for participating in our study.

Contrary to expectations, our results provide little evidence of early onset dementia. The control group, with whom the contact sport athletes were compared, were healthier due in part to their continued athletic activities, better education level, had a higher estimated IQ, and a reduced likelihood of dementia.40,41 Despite the substantial advantage, we did not find many differences in cognitive abilities (except those that could easily be explained by the differences in education and IQ).

The rates of MCI were higher in the contact sport athlete group but this did not reach significance. We found that 38% of our contact sport athletes had MCI vs 14% of our non-contact sport controls which is similar to earlier studies,6,8,9 but lower than studies with similar sample sizes. Tremblay et al.42 found reduced semantic verbal fluency and altered episodic memory on both delayed recall and recognition among a sample of 15 retired contact sport athletes aged 51 to 75. De Beaumont et al.43 found decreased episodic memory and response inhibition performance that correlated with slowed motor speed and delayed event-related potentials among 19 retired athletes compared to 21 controls. Stern et al.44 conducted a retrospective post-mortem analysis of 36 athletes with confirmed CTE (mostly former NFL athletes and a few former NHL athletes). Three were asymptomatic, 11 had recognizable cognitive dysfunction, 13 had behavior alterations that gradually became mood disturbance, and 10 were diagnosed with dementia. Guskiewicz et al.45 found that more than 3 reported concussions resulted in a five-fold increase in MCI among their sample of retired NFL players. Larger studies from the Boston University group have identified the range of pathologies of CTE. Post-mortem analysis of professional NFL players suggest that almost every individual who plays contact sports will have cognitive impairment and develop CTE, but they also provide a cautionary note.1 Simply stated, one cannot expect that an autopsy based case series is representative of the total population of athletes that play contact sports since families of individuals with cognitive deficits are far more likely to donate the brain of their deceased family member for study.

When comparing other risk factors for developing MCI, we found that BMI, presence of one or more vascular risk factors, and depression score differences approached significance. HDL cholesterol was higher in the non-contact sport athletes. The higher BMI is easily explained by the fact that contact sport athletes were much heavier builds,46 and our non-contact athlete controls were currently active in aerobic activities. Both the contact sport athletes and non-contact sport athletes controls had vascular risk factors (71% vs 43%), but the contact athletes were significantly higher risk because of obesity. Although not significantly different, both contact sport athletes and non-contact sport athletes had a high risk of a cardiovascular event according to the Framingham Heart Study risk calculator. This could be due to the older ages of both groups, which is a strong modifier of vascular risk. HDL Cholesterol is considered to be the “good” cholesterol and is associated with lower BMI and exercise, which was higher in the non-contact control group.47

Our rate of MCI among the athletes using the Comprehensive criteria (8/21 = 38%) was higher than that found in the Hart et al8 study (8/34 = 24%), which the authors note was slightly higher than the general population.

There are a number of limitations of the current study. Most notable, we were unable to ascertain with confidence, the number of concussions experienced by each athlete. The contact sport athlete group studied did play a substantial amount of time in their respective professional leagues with an average career length of 8.7 years. Several of these athletes are hall of fame members. The years played does not account for the number of concussive or sub-concussive hits directly nor does it account for the years of contact and potential head injuries sustained in junior and college playing years. All members of the non-contact athlete group continue to remain physically active in their sports of cycling, running or swimming. The continued physical activity is important to note as physical activity has been shown to reduce the risk of developing dementia.40,41 They also had higher education levels which is an indicator of higher cognitive reserve. Also, the impact of head injuries on cognitive reserve is related to education48 and our non-contact athlete control group had much higher education, on average.

We realize that studies that report no significant differences between groups are often underpowered to make such claims. However, when we began this study we too were influenced by the media reports and reports of high rates of CTE among former NFL and NHL athletes. We hypothesized that these former contact sport athletes would be significantly impaired cognitively compared with an above average non-contact group of athletes and concluded that the likelihood of type II error would be reduced. Still, our sample size is small and the study results need to be replicated. Future studies of retired contact sport athletes can continue to examine alternate influences on cognition with aging. Examples could include nutrition, exercise/activity, chronic pain, sleep disturbance, current and previous substance abuse, and a history of performance enhancing drugs.

CONCLUSION

According to the results of our study, contact sport athletes do not have a significantly higher rate of mild cognitive impairment when compared to age-matched, non-contact sport control athletes. Other risk factors for mild cognitive impairment, such as vascular risk factors, depression, and lower cognitive reserve, are present in contact sport athletes and could explain some of the cognitive issues thought to be present in retired athletes due to chronic traumatic encephalopathy.

Acknowledgments

Source of Funding: Research reported in this publication was supported by the National Center for Advancing Translational Sciences at the National Institutes of Health under award number UL1TR001412. We also wish to thank the following organizations for financial support: The Robert Rich Family Foundation, and Ralph and Mary Wilson Foundation. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not reflect the official views of the NIH or any Foundation. We also want to thank the research team at Boston University, in particular Dr. Robert Stern, for their willingness to share their protocol for evaluation of neurocognitive performance.

Footnotes

Conflicts of Interest: Robert Zivadinov received personal compensation from EMD Serono, Novartis, Celgene, Genentech, Claret Medical and Sanofi-Genzyme for speaking and consultant fees. R. Zivadinov received financial support for research activities from Biogen Idec, Teva Pharmaceuticals, Sanofi-Genzyme, Novartis, Claret Medical and Coherus-Intekrin. Other investigators report no conflicts.

References

  • 1.Mez J, Daneshvar DH, Kiernan PT, et al. Clinicopathological Evaluation of Chronic Traumatic Encephalopathy in Players of American Football. Jama. 2017;318(4):360–370. doi: 10.1001/jama.2017.8334. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Lopez OL. Mild cognitive impairment. Continuum: Lifelong Learning in Neurology. 2013;19(2 Dementia):411. doi: 10.1212/01.CON.0000429175.29601.97. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Petersen RC. Mild cognitive impairment as a diagnostic entity. Journal of internal medicine. 2004;256(3):183–194. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2796.2004.01388.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Knopman DS, Petersen RC. Mild cognitive impairment and mild dementia: a clinical perspective. Paper presented at: Mayo Clinic Proceedings; 2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Association AP. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-5®) American Psychiatric Pub; 2013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Randolph C, Karantzoulis S, Guskiewicz K. Prevalence and characterization of mild cognitive impairment in retired national football league players. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 2013;19(08):873–880. doi: 10.1017/S1355617713000805. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Alosco ML, Mez J, Kowall NW, et al. Cognitive Reserve as a Modifier of Clinical Expression in Chronic Traumatic Encephalopathy: A Preliminary Examination. The Journal of neuropsychiatry and clinical neurosciences. 2017;29(1):6–12. doi: 10.1176/appi.neuropsych.16030043. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Hart J, Kraut MA, Womack KB, et al. Neuroimaging of Cognitive Dysfunction and Depression in Aging Retired National Football League PlayersA Cross-sectional StudyNeurobiology of Aging NFL Players. JAMA neurology. 2013;70(3):326–335. doi: 10.1001/2013.jamaneurol.340. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.McMillan T, McSkimming P, Wainman-Lefley J, et al. Long-term health outcomes after exposure to repeated concussion in elite level: rugby union players. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry. 2016 doi: 10.1136/jnnp-2016-314279. jnnp-2016-314279. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Willeumier K, Taylor D, Amen D. Elevated body mass in National Football League players linked to cognitive impairment and decreased prefrontal cortex and temporal pole activity. Translational psychiatry. 2012;2(1):e68. doi: 10.1038/tp.2011.67. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Ganguli M, Fu B, Snitz BE, Hughes TF, Chang C-CH. Mild cognitive impairment Incidence and vascular risk factors in a population-based cohort. Neurology. 2013;80(23):2112–2120. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e318295d776. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Geda YE, Roberts RO, Mielke MM, et al. Baseline neuropsychiatric symptoms and the risk of incident mild cognitive impairment: a population-based study. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2014;171(5):572–581. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2014.13060821. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Heaton RK. Wisconsin card sorting test: computer version 2. Odessa: Psychological Assessment Resources; 1993. [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Delis DC, Kaplan E, Kramer JH. Delis-Kaplan executive function system (D-KEFS) Psychological Corporation; 2001. [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Wechsler D. The Psychological Corporation. 1997. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale. [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Tombaugh TN. Trail Making Test A and B: normative data stratified by age and education. Archives of clinical neuropsychology. 2004;19(2):203–214. doi: 10.1016/S0887-6177(03)00039-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Stern RA, White T. NAB, Neuropsychological Assessment Battery: Administration, Scoring, and Interpretation Manual. Psychological Assessment Resources; 2003. [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Loonstra AS, Tarlow AR, Sellers AH. COWAT metanorms across age, education, and gender. Applied neuropsychology. 2001;8(3):161–166. doi: 10.1207/S15324826AN0803_5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Rodriguez-Aranda C, Martinussen M. Age-related differences in performance of phonemic verbal fluency measured by Controlled Oral Word Association Task (COWAT): a meta-analytic study. Developmental neuropsychology. 2006;30(2):697–717. doi: 10.1207/s15326942dn3002_3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Tombaugh TN, Kozak J, Rees L. Normative data stratified by age and education for two measures of verbal fluency: FAS and animal naming. Archives of clinical neuropsychology. 1999;14(2):167–177. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Strauss E, Sherman EMS, Spreen O. A compendium of neuropsychological tests: Administration, norms, and commentary. Oxford University Press; 2006. [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Wilkinson GS, Robertson G. Wide Range Achievement Test 4 (WRAT4) Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc; Lutz, FL: 2006. [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Jak AJ, Bondi MW, Delano-Wood L, et al. Quantification of five neuropsychological approaches to defining mild cognitive impairment. The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry. 2009;17(5):368–375. doi: 10.1097/JGP.0b013e31819431d5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Clark LR, Delano-Wood L, Libon DJ, et al. Are empirically-derived subtypes of mild cognitive impairment consistent with conventional subtypes? Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 2013;19(06):635–645. doi: 10.1017/S1355617713000313. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Busse A, Hensel A, Gühne U, Angermeyer M, Riedel-Heller S. Mild cognitive impairment long-term course of four clinical subtypes. Neurology. 2006;67(12):2176–2185. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000249117.23318.e1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Beck AT, Steer RA, Brown GK. Beck depression inventory-II. San Antonio. 1996;78(2):490–498. [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Goff DC, Lloyd-Jones DM, Bennett G, et al. 2013 ACC/AHA guideline on the assessment of cardiovascular risk: a report of the American College of Cardiology/American Heart Association Task Force on Practice Guidelines. Journal of the American College of Cardiology. 2014;63(25_PA) doi: 10.1016/j.jacc.2013.11.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Stone NJ, Blum CB, Eckel FRH, et al. 2013 ACC/AHA Guideline on the Treatment of Blood Cholesterol to Reduce Atherosclerotic Cardiovascular Risk in Adults. 2015 [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Karr JE, Garcia-Barrera MA, Areshenkoff CN. Executive functions and intraindividual variability following concussion. Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology. 2014;36(1):15–31. doi: 10.1080/13803395.2013.863833. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Team RC. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2013. URL http://www.R-project.org. [Google Scholar]
  • 31.McKee AC, Cantu RC, Nowinski CJ, et al. Chronic traumatic encephalopathy in athletes: progressive tauopathy after repetitive head injury. J Neuropathol Exp Neurol. 2009;68(7):709–735. doi: 10.1097/NEN.0b013e3181a9d503. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.McKee AC, Stein TD, Kiernan PT, Alvarez VE. The neuropathology of chronic traumatic encephalopathy. Brain pathology (Zurich, Switzerland) 2015;25(3):350–364. doi: 10.1111/bpa.12248. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.McKee AC, Stern RA, Nowinski CJ, et al. The spectrum of disease in chronic traumatic encephalopathy. Brain: a journal of neurology. 2013;136(Pt 1):43–64. doi: 10.1093/brain/aws307. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Omalu BI, Bailes J, Hammers JL, Fitzsimmons RP. Chronic traumatic encephalopathy, suicides and parasuicides in professional American athletes: the role of the forensic pathologist. Am J Forensic Med Pathol. 2010;31(2):130–132. doi: 10.1097/PAF.0b013e3181ca7f35. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Riley DO, Robbins CA, Cantu RC, Stern RA. Chronic traumatic encephalopathy: Contributions from the Boston University Center for the Study of Traumatic Encephalopathy. Brain injury. 2015;29(2):154–163. doi: 10.3109/02699052.2014.965215. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Stamm JM, Bourlas AP, Baugh CM, et al. Age of first exposure to football and later-life cognitive impairment in former NFL players. Neurology. 2015;84(11):1114–1120. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000001358. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Seichepine DR, Stamm JM, Daneshvar DH, et al. Profile of self-reported problems with executive functioning in college and professional football players. Journal of neurotrauma. 2013;30(14):1299–1304. doi: 10.1089/neu.2012.2690. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Stern RA, Daneshvar DH, Baugh CM, et al. Clinical presentation of chronic traumatic encephalopathy. [Accessed 13, 81];Neurology. 2013 :1122–1129. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3182a55f7f. Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/23966253. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 39.Gavett BE, Stern RA, McKee AC. Chronic traumatic encephalopathy: a potential late effect of sport-related concussive and subconcussive head trauma. Clin Sports Med. 2011;30(1):179–188. xi. doi: 10.1016/j.csm.2010.09.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Elwood P, Galante J, Pickering J, et al. Healthy lifestyles reduce the incidence of chronic diseases and dementia: evidence from the Caerphilly cohort study. PloS one. 2013;8(12):e81877. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0081877. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Larson EB, Wang L, Bowen JD, et al. Exercise is associated with reduced risk for incident dementia among persons 65 years of age and older. Annals of internal medicine. 2006;144(2):73–81. doi: 10.7326/0003-4819-144-2-200601170-00004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Tremblay S, De Beaumont L, Henry LC, et al. Sports Concussions and Aging: A Neuroimaging Investigation. Cerebral Cortex. 2012 doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs102. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.De Beaumont L, Theoret H, Mongeon D, et al. Brain function decline in healthy retired athletes who sustained their last sports concussion in early adulthood. Brain: a journal of neurology. 2009;132(3):695–708. doi: 10.1093/brain/awn347. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Stern RA, Daneshvar DH, Baugh CM, et al. Clinical presentation of chronic traumatic encephalopathy. Neurology. 2013;81(13):1122–1129. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3182a55f7f. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Guskiewicz KM, Marshall SW, Bailes J, et al. Association between recurrent concussion and late-life cognitive impairment in retired professional football players. Neurosurgery. 2005;57(4):719–726. doi: 10.1093/neurosurgery/57.4.719. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Jonnalagadda SS, Skinner R, Moore L. Overweight athlete: fact or fiction? Current sports medicine reports. 2004;3(4):198–205. doi: 10.1249/00149619-200408000-00005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Gordon B, Chen S, Durstine JL. The effects of exercise training on the traditional lipid profile and beyond. Current sports medicine reports. 2014;13(4):253–259. doi: 10.1249/JSR.0000000000000073. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES